Abstract
Monogamy is deeply rooted in most Western societies, shaping how people construe and behave in romantic relationships. These normative views facilitate the emergence of negative perceptions and evaluations when people choose not to adhere to mononormativity. Even though people in consensual non-monogamous (CNM) relationships are targets of stigmatization, research shows a dichotomy between these negative views and the relational experiences of CNM people. Indeed, people in CNM and monogamous relationships have comparable relationship functioning and quality and struggle with similar relationship problems. One of the differences is that CNM relationships afford people to explore their sexuality and fulfill their needs with multiple partners, without agreed-upon extradyadic behavior being perceived as infidelity or having deleterious consequences to relationship maintenance. These positive experiences notwithstanding, CNM people are continuously pressured by mononormativity and stigmatization, increasing the risk of internalized CNM negativity and worse personal and relational outcomes. One possible way to counteract CNM stigmatization and improve the lives of CNM people is by changing discourses surrounding non-monogamy and improving acceptance, not only in professional settings but also in the general population. Another strategy is to understand how the relationship beliefs and scripts of younger generations can help promote more inclusive and diverse societies.
Keywords: Consensual non-monogamy, Mononormativity, Stigmatization, Relationship functioning, Internalized negativity
Introduction
Non-monogamy is among the most frequent sexual fantasies reported by American people (Lehmiller, 2018) and has been a popular topic of online search queries among the general public over time (Moors, 2017). Likewise, researchers across different areas have been increasingly interested in non-monogamy (Balzarini & Muise, 2020; Scoats & Campbell, 2022). Some studies suggest that about 3–7% of adults may be in a consensual non-monogamous (CNM) relationship and up to 25% may have had past experiences with consensual non-monogamy (Haupert et al., 2017a; Levine et al., 2018; Rubin et al., 2014; Séguin et al., 2017; Træen & Thuen, 2022). However, the prevalence of a monogamous norm in Western societies implies that people who depart from mononormativity are at risk of negative appraisals and struggle to accept their identity. Indeed, research has consistently shown that CNM people are socially stigmatized and perceived as having unadjusted relationships, despite ample evidence that relationship processes do not significantly differ between monogamous and CNM partners (Mogilski et al., 2023).
This narrative review offers an overview of research examining the dichotomy between the negative societal view of CNM relationships and the positive relational experiences reported by CNM people. Establishing a parallel with other sexual minorities, it also explores the personal and relational consequences of internalized negativity and offers ways to counteract CNM stigmatization. References were drawn from the consensual non-monogamies literature list, one of the education and outreach initiatives from the American Psychological Association Division 44 Committee on CNM (https://www.div44cnm.org), as well as from searches on academic databases.
Monogamy Norms
Imagine that Jo and Sam are in a stable and committed romantic relationship. They are together for some time and then decided to have a conversation about opening their relationship to include other people. At some point, they meet Alex and decide to open their relationship. This illustrates a CNM agreement, whereby two (or more) partners agree on the possibility of having sexual encounters and/or romantic relationships with other people (e.g., open relationships, swinging, or polyamorous relationships; Conley et al., 2017; Rubin et al., 2014). This scenario, however, departs from the prototypical romantic relationship. Indeed, there is a shared belief that romantic relationships should be monogamous and partners should be sexually and emotionally exclusive to each other, conveying monogamy as better than any other type of agreement (Conley et al., 2012b). These beliefs are imposed through socialization (Conley et al., 2013; Henrich et al., 2012; Ryan & Jetha, 2012), shared in political, public, and religious discourses (Cardoso et al., 2021), and even shared by some experts and professionals (Grunt-Mejer & Chańska, 2020; Herbitter et al., 2021). This belief is so pervasive that partners often assume monogamy (Muise et al., 2021) and rely on their partner’s exclusivity (Conley et al., 2017; Ziegler et al., 2015). And yet, most partners fail to address the topic of monogamy in their relationships. For example, Badcock et al. (2014) found that more than 96% of participants expected their partner and themselves not to have sex with other people, but only about 30% explicitly addressed these expectations with their partner.
This lack of clear communication about monogamy expectations opens the possibility that partners have different conceptions about extradyadic behaviors and infidelity. For example, people can engage in behaviors perceived as infidelity by their partners (e.g., watching pornography alone), even though they consider otherwise and believe their relationship to be monogamous (Liu & Zheng, 2019). In their study with users from a dating website, Rodrigues et al. (2017a) found that even though users reported being monogamous, more than 88% registered alone on the website and more than 66% had sex with another user. Aligned with the argument that monogamy expectations shape infidelity beliefs, the authors also found that users who have enacted extradyadic sex believed that ambiguous (e.g., talking with another person in secret) and explicit behaviors (e.g., sexual intercourse) were less indicative of infidelity when compared to users who did not enact extradyadic sex. No difference between groups was observed in the perception of deceptive behaviors (e.g., lying to the partner). These different conceptions and behaviors are likely to result in relational conflicts and problems.
Regardless of what monogamy expectations imply for behaviors and relationship quality, the negative perceptions and reactions that monogamous people have when they are faced with infidelity (de Visser et al., 2020; Kruger et al., 2015; Previti & Amato, 2004) parallel those shared by most Western societies. Indeed, extradyadic behaviors (particularly those involving sexual activity) and people who enact these behaviors tend to be socially condemned, regardless of whether or not romantic partners consensually agree upon those behaviors.
Consensual Non-Monogamy Stigmatization
Research has consistently shown a negative appraisal of non-monogamy. In a series of studies, Conley et al. (2013) asked about the benefits of having a monogamous relationship. Participants indicated relational benefits (e.g., committed relationships, trusting another person, having a meaningful connection with another person, or having a family) but also health and moral benefits. When asked to make judgments about monogamous and CNM relationships, participants reported more negative perceptions about the relational dynamics of CNM relationships (e.g., less comforting, trusting, and intimate relationships; partners have sex with each other less frequently), and at the same time perceived CNM partners more negatively in arbitrary traits (e.g., less likely to floss daily; less invested in taking care of others; less caring, reasonable, and satisfied with life). In another study, Grunt-Mejer and Campbell (2016) compared different relationship structures and found that monogamous partners were perceived as the most satisfied with their relationship, the most moral, and with the highest cognitive abilities (e.g., more intelligent). In contrast, monogamous partners who wanted to maintain their relationships but enacted extradyadic behaviors (named “cheating” by the authors), received the most negative appraisals. The negative halo surrounding non-monogamy has been replicated in other studies. For example, Balzarini et al. (2018) found that monogamous participants reported wanting more social distance from partners who were in open, swinging, or polyamorous relationships. In another study, Rodrigues et al. (2022) found that monogamous partners were perceived as the most trustworthy, moral, committed, and sexually satisfied, whereas CNM partners were perceived as the most promiscuous and likely to have sexually transmitted infections.
The stigmatization of non-monogamy has also been extended to other phenomena with potentially more severe consequences. For example, there is evidence that CNM people are dehumanized (Rodrigues et al., 2018, 2021b). Broadly, dehumanization occurs when people are deprived of certain attributes that are uniquely human and not shared with objects or animals (Haslam, 2006). This phenomenon is observed among people from different countries, different ethnic groups, or different social groups (Haslam & Loughnan, 2014). People dehumanize others by perceiving them to lack secondary emotions that are uniquely human and require a higher level of processing (e.g., love, embarrassment), and instead experience mostly primary emotions that are shared with other animals and require a lower level of processing (e.g., anger, happiness; Demoulin et al., 2004; Leyens et al., 2000, 2001; Vaes et al., 2012). Dehumanization has been associated with negative psychological and physical consequences, such that dehumanized people tend to be victims of verbal and physical abuse in different contexts (e.g., Rai et al., 2017), including in romantic relationships (Pizzirani & Karantzas, 2019; Pizzirani et al., 2019). In a study that included samples from Portugal, Italy, and Croatia, Rodrigues et al. (2018) found that participants attributed more secondary (vs. primary) emotions to monogamous partners, and more primary (vs. secondary) emotions to CNM partners. Interestingly, these findings were independent of whether partners were described as heterosexual or gay (see also Moors et al., 2013; Rodrigues et al., 2022), suggesting that departures from mononormativity were more salient in determining stigmatization, so long partners were committed to a monogamous relationship. In a follow-up study, Rodrigues et al. (2021b) extended these findings by showing that dehumanization occurred because participants perceived CNM (vs. monogamous) partners as more immoral and less committed to their relationship. Taken together, these findings indicate that the stigmatization of CNM people (and their relationships) is mostly anchored on departures from socially conveyed norms of emotional exclusivity and morality. The stigma experienced by CNM people sharply contrasts with their relational experiences.
Personal Experiences with Non-Monogamy
Research has highlighted some a priori demographic differences between monogamous and CNM people (e.g., gender, sexual orientation; Balzarini et al., 2019b; Haupert et al., 2017b; Moors et al., 2021a; Stults, 2019). Regardless, other individual variables have been shown to shape how people pursue their affective and sexual needs. For example, people with a more unrestricted sociosexuality (i.e., predisposed to have multiple sex partners) in a monogamous relationship are more likely to experience relationship distress (e.g., Webster et al., 2015), enact extradyadic behaviors (e.g., Barta & Kiene, 2005; Rodrigues & Lopes, 2017; Rodrigues et al., 2017b) and have their relationship end (e.g., French et al., 2019). Although there is some degree of assortative mating in sociosexuality (Manning, 2006), this does not mean that romantic partners with unrestricted sociosexuality are at odds with relationship failure. For example, Rodrigues and Lopes (2017) found that partners with a more unrestricted sociosexuality were more likely to have enacted extradyadic sex in their current relationship if they reported lower (but not higher) relationship commitment. Using a dyadic approach, Markey and Markey (2013) found that partners with a more restricted sociosexuality were the most committed, followed by partners with a more unrestricted sociosexuality. In contrast, the lowest relationship commitment was reported by partners with unmatched sociosexuality. Hence, having a partner with similar predispositions in sexual behavior and desires might help to discuss alternative ways to accommodate affective and sexual needs. Unsurprisingly, then, CNM (vs. monogamous) partners tend to have a more unrestricted sociosexuality (Mogilski et al., 2020), and those who act upon their unrestricted sociosexuality tend to be more satisfied and committed in their relationship, and report better quality of life (Rodrigues et al. 2016, 2017b, 2019b).
Research has also suggested that CNM people may be better equipped to express intimacy and feel closer to their partners. For example, Cohen (2016) and Wood et al. (2021) found that CNM people highlight the ability to experience new things, the freedom to explore sexuality and sexual satisfaction, how close they feel to their partner, and need fulfillment as some of the positive experiences and motives for engaging in CNM relationships. In contrast, having to deal with social stigma, jealousy, and trust issues were among the negative aspects related to a CNM relationship. Some of these benefits are unique to non-monogamy (e.g., personal growth, need fulfillment), whereas others are shared with monogamy (e.g., stable relationship, love; Moors et al., 2017). Aligned with this, Murphy et al. (2021) found that partners who decided to engage in non-monogamy experienced significant increases in sexual satisfaction later on, despite not reporting changes in relationship quality. In other words, CNM partners likely fulfilled specific needs with other partners that were being unmet in their current relationship.
When negotiating non-monogamy, CNM partners also rely on open communication to mutually establish and clarify the boundaries of their agreement (Andersson, 2022; Cohen, 2016; Wood et al., 2021). Open communication promotes perceptions of equity in the relationship, trust in one another, and commitment (Hangen et al., 2020; McLean, 2004; Moors et al., 2015, 2017), helps to work around jealousy issues (de Visser & McDonald, 2007), and may help CNM partners who are not comfortable to take a step back and discuss the terms of the agreement (e.g., Philpot et al., 2018). When comfortable with their agreement, CNM partners report levels of affection, eroticism, and relationship functioning (e.g., satisfaction, commitment, trust, intimacy) that are comparable (if not higher) than those reported by monogamous partners (Balzarini et al., 2019c; Lecuona et al., 2021; Mogilski et al., 2017; Rodrigues et al., 2017b; Rubel & Bogaert, 2015). CNM (vs. monogamous) partners are also likely to use more positive strategies to solve relational issues (e.g., more problem solving; less withdrawal in conflicts) and report more well-being (Brooks et al., 2022). Other studies have shown that CNM people are equally or more sexually satisfied than monogamous partners (Conley et al., 2018; Parsons et al., 2012; Rodrigues et al., 2021a), particularly when both CNM partners fulfill and are responsive to each other’s needs (Muise et al., 2019). They also tend to experience relatively low levels of jealousy and can even feel good (i.e., compersion) when their partner has extradyadic relationships (Balzarini et al., 2021; Barker, 2005; Ritchie & Barker, 2006). And even though CNM people tend to be more focused on their sexual novelty, pleasure, and sexual satisfaction when compared to monogamous people, emotional motives are similar in both relationships (Mitchell et al., 2020; Wood et al., 2018). Still, CNM people tend to be particularly careful with their own and their partners’ sexual health, especially when compared to monogamous people engaged in infidelity (Conley et al., 2012a; Lehmiller, 2015), arguably because CNM people perceive to have more self-control in sex (Rodrigues et al., 2019a, 2019c).
Consensual non-monogamous people do not necessarily perceive extradyadic behaviors as infidelity insofar as both partners stick to their agreement, unlike monogamous people. For example, Cohen (2016) found that CNM people consider behaviors such as lying or withholding information from the partner as more indicative of infidelity and tend to be more lenient about explicit behaviors (e.g., extradyadic sex), which are typically conceived as infidelity by monogamous people. Like monogamous people, however, CNM people experience negative emotions if their agreement is crossed. For example, Mogilski et al. (2019) found that CNM participants were more distressed and experienced greater jealousy after imagining their partner establishing an emotional bond with a person outside of the boundaries mutually agreed upon. However, these similarities may be particularly true when CNM people are considering their primary partners. Indeed, the authors found that CNM people were more confident that their primary (vs. secondary) partner would not engage in infidelity, were more distressed when thinking about that possibility, and were more protective of their primary relationship. In another study, Mogilski et al. (2017) found no differences between CNM and monogamous people in relationship satisfaction, but only if CNM people were considering their primary partner. CNM people had also been in a relationship with their primary (vs. secondary) partner for a longer period, viewed this partner as a more desirable long-term mate, and were more likely to discuss and downplay extradyadic sexual experiences with them. Extending these findings to include social perceptions, Balzarini et al. (2017, 2019a, 2019c) found that polyamorous partners with a primary-secondary relationship structure reported greater more relationship quality toward primary partners (e.g., more commitment, satisfaction, love, and attraction; better communication) and perceived their primary relationships to be more socially accepted (e.g., by friends and family). In contrast, polyamorous partners spent more time on sexual activity with secondary partners, but they were also more secretive about these secondary relationships. Despite being smaller in magnitude, these differences also emerged among polyamorous partners who rejected hierarchical labels in their relationship. In some ways, then, it seems that societal views may restrict and determine how CNM people perceive and behave in their relationships.
Challenges for Consensually Non-Monogamous People
Even though CNM people have adjusted and functioning relationships, they still must cope with the constant exposure to monogamous values and expectations. Exposure to normative views can lead to the internalization of such norms, create internal conflicts, and have consequences for relationships, health, and well-being. This reasoning follows research framed by the minority stress model (Meyer, 1995), which has shown that people from sexual minorities (e.g., LGBTQIA + people) face unique stressors in response to context cues (e.g., normative pressure to conform), which can result in internalized negativity (Rostosky & Riggle, 2017) and poorer health (Dürrbaum & Sattler, 2020). For example, Torres and Rodrigues (2022) found that Portuguese and Turkish gay men who endorsed more heteronormativity beliefs also reported more internalized homonegativity. Such negative experience has been associated with a negative self-identity (Riggle et al., 2014), discomfort with one’s sexual orientation and fear of coming out (e.g., identity concealment; Dyar et al., 2018), worse relationship functioning (Doyle & Molix, 2021), and riskier behaviors in sex (Burton et al., 2020).
Much like other sexual minorities, CNM people are met with stigmatization daily, either by being continuously exposed to monogamy expectations, exposed to the stigmatization of other CNM people, or directly confronted for their non-adherence to monogamy after assuming their relationship configuration. Being confronted with stigmatization leads CNM people to question whether or not to disclose their CNM identity and relationship configuration to others (Valadez et al., 2020), trust the healthcare system to address their specific health needs (Vaughan et al., 2019), or maintain their therapeutic relationship after seeking for psychological help (Schechinger et al., 2018). This stigmatization can also result in internalized negativity and worse outcomes (e.g., psychological distress; Mahar et al., 2022). Aligned with this reasoning, Moors et al. (2021a) found that CNM people who were uncomfortable with non-monogamy (i.e., endorsed more internalized CNM negativity) were less satisfied with their current relationship agreement, and reported being less satisfied and committed to their primary partner. Extending these findings, Rodrigues et al. (2023) found that CNM people who endorsed more mononormative beliefs reported more internalized CNM negativity, had a more negative view about themselves (i.e., reported experiencing negative emotions more often and positive emotions less often), and perceived their partners as more immature, unrefined, exploitable, and emotionless (i.e., were more likely to dehumanize their partner).
There is still a restricted understanding of the consequences of internalized CNM negativity on health and well-being, particularly its pervasiveness in the CNM community and the extensiveness of its effects over time. Still, researchers can consider variables that may help counter stigmatization and improve the experiences of CNM people.
A Brighter Future
Different strategies can help improve intergroup relations and decrease intergroup bias (Boin et al., 2021; Eisenberg et al., 2010; Gonzalez et al., 2015; Visintin et al., 2020). One possibility is to improve knowledge among professionals and the general public. On the one hand, therapists with more informed, affirming, and inclusive approaches to non-monogamy are better equipped to work with, and to be perceived as helpful by their CNM patients (Schechinger et al., 2018). On the other hand, people with more positive CNM attitudes are less likely to stigmatize CNM partners (Rodrigues et al., 2021b). For example, Rodrigues et al. (2022) showed that having more positive CNM attitudes was associated with less stigmatization, because participants perceived CNM (vs. monogamous) partners to be more open to change. In contrast, having more negative CNM attitudes was associated with more stigmatization because participants perceived CNM (vs. monogamous) partners to be less conservative and more open to change. In other words, favorable CNM views can improve acceptance by attributing strength to mononormativity departures, whereas unfavorable CNM views can foster negative appraisals through the lens of traditionalism and mononormative disruption.
Perceptions of CNM relationships may also benefit from changes in the way multi-partner relationships are perceived and enacted by younger adults (Hamilton & Winward, 2022), particularly among those who consider this to be a period of exploration and trying new things (Olmstead & Anders, 2022). For example, Sizemore and Olmstead (2018) found that one in four young adults was willing or open-minded about the possibility of having a CNM experience, and Stephens and Emmers-Sommer (2020) found that 48% of younger people were in a CNM relationship (i.e., monogamist, negotiable, open relationship, swinging, polyamorous, or other). Possibly, experimenting with alternative relationship structures and openly talking about their experiences with close others (e.g., friends, prospective partners) may be increasingly recurring among younger generations. Aligned with this reasoning, younger adults are more open to experimenting with sexuality and sexual relationships, and more predisposed to have multiple casual sexual relationships with different partners (Alvarez et al., 2023; Claxton & van Dulmen, 2013; James-Kangal et al., 2018; McMahan & Olmstead, 2021; Sizemore & Olmstead, 2018). Similar to the perceived benefits and behaviors reported by CNM partners, younger people engage in different casual sexual relationships (e.g., friends with benefits, fuck buddies, one-night stands) because these relationships afford them the freedom to explore sexuality, excitement, and novelty, provide sexual and/or affective intimacy, and help them fulfill multiple needs with distinct partners, without discarding the importance of sexual health and protection (Alvarez et al., 2021; Luz et al., 2022). If younger generations are becoming less attached to the mononormative views of romantic relationships, then they should be more open to defying mononormativity as the prevalent social norm and be more accepting of alternative relationship configurations.
Conclusion and Implications
This narrative review highlighted some of the ways through which mononormativity fosters CNM stigmatization while contributing to a more comprehensive understanding of relationship science. From theoretical and methodological standpoints, the evidence herein reviewed can have implications for the way researchers think about relationship processes (Hammack et al., 2019), particularly because monogamy tends to be assumed by several theoretical perspectives and the assessment of relationship configuration is often overlooked in studies. For instance, both the interdependence theory (Arriaga, 2013) and the investment model (Rusbult et al., 2012) assume monogamy when discussing the implications of perceived comparison levels and the perceived quality of potential alternative partners for relational outcomes. Self-expansion theory (Aron et al., 2022) assumes monogamy when discussing the implications of developing a common self with the partner for relationship stability and quality. Likewise, the sexual communal strength framework (Muise & Impett, 2016) assumes monogamy when examining the implications of being responsive to the partner’s sexual needs. By contrasting social views and personal experiences, this review can potentially contribute to revising and extending established theoretical frameworks to include CNM relationships (Impett et al., 2020; Lee & O’Sullivan, 2019; MacDonald et al., 2021; Wood et al., 2021) or inform the extension of theoretical perspectives already acknowledging sexual and gender diversity in partnered sexuality (Abed et al., 2019).
Moving forward, researchers should strive to acknowledge how, why, and under which conditions relationship agreements and configurations are negatively perceived and shape the way people navigate their lives and relationships. These efforts can potentially contribute to open discussions and changing discourses toward the acceptance, affirmation, and celebration of relationship diversity.
Authors' contributions
DLR conceptualized the article, reviewed the empirical evidence, and wrote and edited the paper.
Funding
Open access funding provided by FCT|FCCN (b-on). This work was funded by a grant awarded by Fundação para a Ciência e a Tecnologia (Ref.: 2020.00523.CEECIND), awarded to DLR.
Availability of data and material
Not applicable.
Declarations
Conflict of interests
Not applicable.
Footnotes
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- Abed EC, Schudson ZC, Gunther OD, Beischel WJ, van Anders SM. Sexual and gender diversity among sexual and gender/sex majorities: Insights via sexual configurations theory. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2019;48(5):1423–1441. doi: 10.1007/s10508-018-1340-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alvarez M-J, Pereira CR, Godinho CA, Luz R. Clear-cut terms and culture-sensitive characteristics of distinctive casual sexual relationships in Portuguese emerging adults. Sexuality & Culture. 2021;25(6):1966–1989. doi: 10.1007/s12119-021-09859-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Alvarez M-J, Pegado A, Luz R, Amaro H. Still striving after all these years: Between normality of conduct and normativity of evaluation in casual relationships among college students. Current Psychology. 2023;42(13):10645–10655. doi: 10.1007/s12144-021-02344-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Andersson C. Drawing the line at infidelity: Negotiating relationship morality in a Swedish context of consensual non-monogamy. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2022;39(7):1917–1933. doi: 10.1177/02654075211070556. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Aron A, Lewandowski G, Branand B, Mashek D, Aron E. Self-expansion motivation and inclusion of others in self: An updated review. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2022;39(12):3821–3852. doi: 10.1177/02654075221110630. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Arriaga, X. B. (2013). An interdependence theory analysis of close relationships. In J. A. Simpson & L. Campbell (Eds.). The Oxford handbook of close relationships (pp. 39–65). Oxford University Press. 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780195398694.001.0001
- Badcock PB, Smith AMA, Richters J, Rissel C, de Visser RO, Simpson JM, Grulich AE. Characteristics of heterosexual regular relationships among a representative sample of adults: The second Australian Study of Health and Relationships. Sexual Health. 2014;11:427–438. doi: 10.1071/SH14114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balzarini RN, Campbell L, Kohut T, Holmes BM, Lehmiller JJ, Harman JJ, Atkins N. Perceptions of primary and secondary relationships in polyamory. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(5):e0177841. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0177841. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balzarini RN, Dharma C, Kohut T, Campbell L, Lehmiller JJ, Harman JJ, Holmes BM. Comparing relationship quality across different types of romantic partners in polyamorous and monogamous relationships. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2019;48(6):1749–1767. doi: 10.1007/s10508-019-1416-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balzarini RN, Dharma C, Kohut T, Holmes BM, Campbell L, Lehmiller JJ, Harman JJ. Demographic comparison of American individuals in polyamorous and monogamous relationships. Journal of Sex Research. 2019;56(6):681–694. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2018.1474333. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balzarini RN, Dharma C, Muise A, Kohut T. Eroticism versus nurturance: How eroticism and nurturance differs in polyamorous and monogamous relationships. Social Psychology. 2019;50(3):185–200. doi: 10.1027/1864-9335/a000378. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Balzarini RN, McDonald JN, Kohut T, Lehmiller JJ, Holmes BM, Harman JJ. Compersion: When jealousy-inducing situations don’t (just) induce jealousy. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2021;50(4):1311–1324. doi: 10.1007/s10508-020-01853-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Balzarini RN, Muise A. Beyond the dyad: A review of the novel insights gained from studying consensual non-monogamy. Current Sexual Health Reports. 2020;12(4):398–404. doi: 10.1007/s11930-020-00297-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Balzarini RN, Shumlich EJ, Kohut T, Campbell L. Dimming the “halo” around monogamy: Re-assessing stigma surrounding consensually non-monogamous romantic relationships as a function of personal relationship orientation. Frontiers in Psychology. 2018;9:894. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00894. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barker M. This is my partner, and this is my … partner’s partner: Constructing a polyamorous identity in a monogamous world. Journal of Constructivist Psychology. 2005;18(1):75–88. doi: 10.1080/10720530590523107. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Barta W, Kiene S. Motivations for infidelity in heterosexual dating couples: The roles of gender, personality differences, and sociosexual orientation. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2005;22(3):339–360. doi: 10.1177/0265407505052440. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Boin J, Rupar M, Graf S, Neji S, Spiegler O, Swart H. The generalization of intergroup contact effects: Emerging research, policy relevance, and future directions. Journal of Social Issues. 2021;77(1):105–131. doi: 10.1111/josi.12419. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Brooks TR, Shaw J, Reysen S, Henley TB. The vices and virtues of consensual non-monogamy: A relational dimension investigation. Psychology & Sexuality. 2022;13(3):595–609. doi: 10.1080/19419899.2021.1897034. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Burton CL, Clark KA, Pachankis JE. Risk from within: Intraminority gay community stress and sexual risk-taking among sexual minority men. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2020;54(9):703–712. doi: 10.1093/abm/kaaa014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardoso D, Rosa A, da Silva MT. (De)politicizing polyamory: Social media comments on media representations of consensual non-monogamies. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2021;50(4):1325–1340. doi: 10.1007/s10508-020-01887-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Claxton SE, van Dulmen MHM. Casual sexual relationships and experiences in emerging adulthood. Emerging Adulthood. 2013;1(2):138–150. doi: 10.1177/2167696813487181. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen MT. An exploratory study of individuals in non-traditional, alternative relationships: How “open” are we? Sexuality & Culture. 2016;20(2):295–315. doi: 10.1007/s12119-015-9324-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Conley TD, Matsick JL, Moors AC, Ziegler A. Investigation of consensually nonmonogamous relationships: Theories, methods, and new directions. Perspectives on Psychological Science. 2017;12(2):205–232. doi: 10.1177/1745691616667925. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conley TD, Moors A, Matsick J, Ziegler A. The fewer the merrier?: Assessing stigma surrounding consensually non-monogamous romantic relationships. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy. 2013;13(1):1–30. doi: 10.1111/j.1530-2415.2012.01286.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Conley TD, Moors AC, Ziegler A, Karathanasis C. Unfaithful individuals are less likely to practice safer sex than openly nonmonogamous individuals. Journal of Sexual Medicine. 2012;9(6):1559–1565. doi: 10.1111/j.1743-6109.2012.02712.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Conley TD, Piemonte JL, Gusakova S, Rubin JD. Sexual satisfaction among individuals in monogamous and consensually non-monogamous relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2018;35(4):509–531. doi: 10.1177/0265407517743078. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Conley TD, Ziegler A, Moors AC, Matsick JL, Valentine B. A critical examination of popular assumptions about the benefits and outcomes of monogamous relationships. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2012;17(2):124–141. doi: 10.1177/1088868312467087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Visser RO, McDonald D. Swings and roundabouts: Management of jealousy in heterosexual ‘swinging’ couples. British Journal of Social Psychology. 2007;46(2):459–476. doi: 10.1348/014466606X143153. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- de Visser RO, Richters J, Rissel C, Grulich A, Simpson J, Rodrigues DL, Lopes D. Romantic jealousy: A test of social cognitive and evolutionary models in a population-representative sample of adults. Journal of Sex Research. 2020;57(4):498–507. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2019.1613482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Demoulin S, Leyens J-P, Paladino M-P, Rodriguez-Torres R, Rodriguez-Perez A, Dovidio J. Dimensions of “uniquely” and “non-uniquely” human emotions. Cognition and Emotion. 2004;18(1):71–96. doi: 10.1080/02699930244000444. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Doyle DM, Molix L. Reconciling associations between minority stress and sexual minority romantic relationship functioning. Frontiers in Psychology. 2021;12:2491. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.707058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dürrbaum T, Sattler FA. Minority stress and mental health in lesbian, gay male, and bisexual youths: A meta-analysis. Journal of LGBT Youth. 2020;17(3):298–314. doi: 10.1080/19361653.2019.1586615. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Dyar C, Feinstein BA, Eaton NR, London B. The mediating roles of rejection sensitivity and proximal stress in the association between discrimination and internalizing symptoms among sexual minority women. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2018;47(1):205–218. doi: 10.1007/s10508-016-0869-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Eisenberg N, Eggum ND, Di Giunta L. Empathy-related responding: Associations with prosocial behavior, aggression, and intergroup relations. Social Issues and Policy Review. 2010;4(1):143–180. doi: 10.1111/j.1751-2409.2010.01020.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- French JE, Altgelt EE, Meltzer AL. The implications of sociosexuality for marital satisfaction and dissolution. Psychological Science. 2019;30(10):1460–1472. doi: 10.1177/0956797619868997. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Gonzalez KA, Riggle EDB, Rostosky SS. Cultivating positive feelings and attitudes: A path to prejudice reduction and ally behavior. Translational Issues in Psychological Science. 2015;1(4):372–381. doi: 10.1037/tps0000049. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Grunt-Mejer K, Campbell C. Around consensual nonmonogamies: Assessing attitudes toward nonexclusive relationships. Journal of Sex Research. 2016;53(1):45–53. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2015.1010193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Grunt-Mejer K, Chańska W. “How do they even know they love?” The image of polyamory in Polish expert discourse. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2020;49(8):2829–2847. doi: 10.1007/s10508-020-01787-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton, L. D., & Winward, S. B. (2022). Consensual non-monogamy from a developmental perspective. In D. P. VanderLaan & W. I. Wong (Eds.), Gender and sexuality development: Contemporary theory and research (pp. 613–636). Springer International Publishing. 10.1007/978-3-030-84273-4_21
- Hammack PL, Frost DM, Hughes SD. Queer intimacies: A new paradigm for the study of relationship diversity. Journal of Sex Research. 2019;56(4–5):556–592. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2018.1531281. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hangen F, Crasta D, Rogge RD. Delineating the boundaries between nonmonogamy and infidelity: Bringing consent back into definitions of consensual nonmonogamy with latent profile analysis. Journal of Sex Research. 2020;57(4):438–457. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2019.1669133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haslam N. Dehumanization: An integrative review. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2006;10(3):252–264. doi: 10.1207/s15327957pspr1003_4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haslam N, Loughnan S. Dehumanization and infrahumanization. Annual Review of Psychology. 2014;65:399–423. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010213-115045. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haupert ML, Gesselman AN, Moors AC, Fisher HE, Garcia JR. Prevalence of experiences with consensual nonmonogamous relationships: Findings from two national samples of single Americans. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy. 2017;43(5):424–440. doi: 10.1080/0092623X.2016.1178675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haupert ML, Moors AC, Gesselman AN, Garcia JR. Estimates and correlates of engagement in consensually non-monogamous relationships. Current Sexual Health Reports. 2017;9(3):155–165. doi: 10.1007/s11930-017-0121-6. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Henrich J, Boyd R, Richerson PJ. The puzzle of monogamous marriage. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 2012;367(1589):657–669. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2011.0290. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Herbitter C, Vaughan MD, Pantalone DW. Mental health provider bias and clinical competence in addressing asexuality, consensual non-monogamy, and BDSM: A narrative review. Sexual and Relationship Therapy. 2021 doi: 10.1080/14681994.2021.1969547. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Impett EA, Kim JJ, Muise A. A communal approach to sexual need responsiveness in romantic relationships. European Review of Social Psychology. 2020;31(1):287–318. doi: 10.1080/10463283.2020.1796079. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- James-Kangal N, Weitbrecht EM, Francis TE, Whitton SW. Hooking up and emerging adults’ relationship attitudes and expectations. Sexuality & Culture. 2018;22(3):706–723. doi: 10.1007/s12119-018-9495-5. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Kruger DJ, Fisher ML, Fitzgerald CJ, Garcia JR, Geher G, Guitar AE. Sexual and emotional aspects are distinct components of infidelity and unique predictors of anticipated distress. Evolutionary Psychological Science. 2015;1(1):44–51. doi: 10.1007/s40806-015-0010-z. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lecuona O, Suero M, Wingen T, de Rivas S. Does “open” rhyme with “special”? Comparing personality, sexual satisfaction, dominance and jealousy of monogamous and non-monogamous practitioners. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2021;50(4):1537–1549. doi: 10.1007/s10508-020-01865-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee BH, O’Sullivan LF. Walk the line: How successful are efforts to maintain monogamy in intimate relationships? Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2019;48(6):1735–1748. doi: 10.1007/s10508-018-1376-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lehmiller JJ. A comparison of sexual health history and practices among monogamous and consensually nonmonogamous sexual partners. Journal of Sexual Medicine. 2015;12(10):2022–2028. doi: 10.1111/jsm.12987. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lehmiller, J. J. (2018). Tell me what you want: The science of sexual desire and how it can help you improve your sex life. Da Capo Press.
- Levine, E. C., Herbenick, D., Martinez, O., Fu, T.-C., & Dodge, B. (2018). Open relationships, nonconsensual nonmonogamy, and monogamy among U.S. adults: Findings from the 2012 National Survey of Sexual Health and Behavior. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 47(5), 1439–1450. 10.1007/s10508-018-1178-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Leyens J-P, Paladino PM, Rodriguez-Torres R, Vaes J, Demoulin S, Rodriguez-Perez A, Gaunt R. The emotional side of prejudice: The attribution of secondary emotions to ingroups and outgroups. Personality and Social Psychology Review. 2000;4(2):186–197. doi: 10.1207/S15327957PSPR0402_06. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Leyens J-P, Rodriguez-Perez A, Rodriguez-Torres R, Gaunt R, Paladino M-P, Vaes J, Demoulin S. Psychological essentialism and the differential attribution of uniquely human emotions to ingroups and outgroups. European Journal of Social Psychology. 2001;31(4):395–411. doi: 10.1002/ejsp.50. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Liu Y, Zheng L. Influences of sociosexuality and commitment on online sexual activities: The mediating effect of perceptions of infidelity. Journal of Sex & Marital Therapy. 2019;45(5):395–405. doi: 10.1080/0092623X.2018.1549632. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luz R, Alvarez M-J, Godinho CA, Pereira CR. A fertile ground for ambiguities: Casual sexual relationships among Portuguese emerging adults. Frontiers in Psychology. 2022;13:823102. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.823102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacDonald G, Park Y, Hayes A, Vanasse Grosdidier I, Park SW. Quality of alternatives positively associated with interest in opening up a relationship. Personal Relationships. 2021;28(3):538–566. doi: 10.1111/pere.12377. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Mahar EA, Irving LH, Derovanesian A, Masterson A, Webster GD. Stigma toward consensual non-monogamy: Thematic analysis and minority stress. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin. 2022 doi: 10.1177/01461672221139086. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manning JC. The impact of internet pornography on marriage and the family: A review of the research. Sexual Addiction & Compulsivity. 2006;13(2–3):131–165. doi: 10.1080/10720160600870711. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Markey P, Markey C. Sociosexuality and relationship commitment among lesbian couples. Journal of Research in Personality. 2013;47(4):282–285. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2013.02.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McLean K. Negotiating (non)monogamy. Journal of Bisexuality. 2004;4(1–2):83–97. doi: 10.1300/J159v04n01_07. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McMahan KD, Olmstead SB. Are college students’ perceptions of the developmental features of emerging adulthood associated with motivations for sex? Implications for research and policy. Sexuality Research and Social Policy. 2021;18(2):450–464. doi: 10.1007/s13178-020-00457-7. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Meyer IH. Minority stress and mental health in gay men. Journal of Health and Social Behavior. 1995;36(1):38–56. doi: 10.2307/2137286. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell VE, Mogilski JK, Donaldson SH, Nicolas SCA, Welling LLM. Sexual motivation and satisfaction among consensually non-monogamous and monogamous individuals. Journal of Sexual Medicine. 2020;17(6):1072–1085. doi: 10.1016/j.jsxm.2020.02.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mogilski JK, Memering SL, Welling LLM, Shackelford TK. Monogamy versus consensual non-monogamy: Alternative approaches to pursuing a strategically pluralistic mating strategy. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2017;46(2):407–417. doi: 10.1007/s10508-015-0658-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mogilski JK, Mitchell VE, Reeve SD, Donaldson SH, Nicolas SCA, Welling LLM. Life history and multi-partner mating: A novel explanation for moral stigma against consensual non-monogamy. Frontiers in Psychology. 2020;10:3033. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.03033. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mogilski JK, Reeve SD, Nicolas SCA, Donaldson SH, Mitchell VE, Welling LL. Jealousy, consent, and compersion within monogamous and consensually non-monogamous romantic relationships. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2019;48(6):1811–1828. doi: 10.1007/s10508-018-1286-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mogilski, J. K., Rodrigues, D. L., Lehmiller, J. J., & Balzarini, R. N. (2023). Maintaining multipartner relationships: Evolution, sexual ethics, and consensual nonmonogamy. In J. K. Mogilski & T. K. Shackelford (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of evolutionary psychology and romantic relationships (pp. 461–486). Oxford University Press. 10.1093/oxfordhb/9780197524718.013.17
- Moors AC. Has the American public’s interest in information related to relationships beyond “the couple” increased over time? Journal of Sex Research. 2017;54(6):677–684. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2016.1178208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moors AC, Conley TD, Edelstein RS, Chopik WJ. Attached to monogamy? Avoidance predicts willingness to engage (but not actual engagement) in consensual non-monogamy. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2015;32(2):222–240. doi: 10.1177/0265407514529065. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moors, A. C., Gesselman, A. N., & Garcia, J. R. (2021a). Desire, familiarity, and engagement in polyamory: Results from a national sample of single adults in the United States. Frontiers in Psychology, 12, 619640. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.619640 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Moors AC, Matsick JL, Schechinger HA. Unique and shared relationship benefits of consensually non-monogamous and monogamous relationships. European Psychologist. 2017;22(1):55–71. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000278. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moors AC, Matsick J, Ziegler A, Rubin J, Conley T. Stigma toward individuals engaged in consensual nonmonogamy: Robust and worthy of additional research. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy. 2013;13(1):52–69. doi: 10.1111/asap.12020. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Moors AC, Schechinger HA, Balzarini R, Flicker S. Internalized consensual non-monogamy negativity and relationship quality among people engaged in polyamory, swinging, and open relationships. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2021;50(4):1389–1400. doi: 10.1007/s10508-020-01885-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Muise MD, Belu CF, O’Sullivan LF. Unspoken, yet understood: Exploring how couples communicate their exclusivity agreements. Canadian Journal of Human Sexuality. 2021;30(2):196–204. doi: 10.3138/cjhs.2021-0011. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Muise A, Impett EA. Applying theories of communal motivation to sexuality. Social and Personality Psychology Compass. 2016;10(8):455–467. doi: 10.1111/spc3.12261. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Muise A, Laughton AK, Moors A, Impett EA. Sexual need fulfillment and satisfaction in consensually nonmonogamous relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2019;36(7):1917–1938. doi: 10.1177/0265407518774638. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Murphy AP, Joel S, Muise A. A prospective investigation of the decision to open up a romantic relationship. Social Psychological and Personality Science. 2021;12(2):194–201. doi: 10.1177/1948550619897157. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Olmstead SB, Anders KM. Are the developmental features of emerging adulthood associated with willingness to engage in consensually nonmonogamous relationships? Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2022;51(3):1813–1822. doi: 10.1007/s10508-021-02258-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Parsons JT, Starks TJ, Gamarel KE, Grov C. Non-monogamy and sexual relationship quality among same-sex male couples. Journal of Family Psychology. 2012;26(5):669–677. doi: 10.1037/a0029561. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Philpot SP, Duncan D, Ellard J, Bavinton BR, Grierson J, Prestage G. Negotiating gay men’s relationships: How are monogamy and non-monogamy experienced and practised over time? Culture, Health & Sexuality. 2018;20(8):915–928. doi: 10.1080/13691058.2017.1392614. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pizzirani B, Karantzas GC. The association between dehumanization and intimate partner abuse. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2019;36(5):1527–1541. doi: 10.1177/0265407518811673. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Pizzirani, B., Karantzas, G. C., & Mullins, E. R. (2019). The development and validation of a dehumanization measure within romantic relationships. Frontiers in Psychology, 10, 2754. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02754 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- Previti D, Amato P. Is infidelity a cause or a consequence of poor marital quality? Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2004;21(2):217–230. doi: 10.1177/0265407504041384. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rai TS, Valdesolo P, Graham J. Dehumanization increases instrumental violence, but not moral violence. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2017;114(32):8511. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1705238114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Riggle EDB, Mohr JJ, Rostosky SS, Fingerhut AW, Balsam KF. A multifactor Lesbian, Gay, and Bisexual Positive Identity Measure (LGB-PIM) Psychology of Sexual Orientation and Gender Diversity. 2014;1(4):398–411. doi: 10.1037/sgd0000057. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ritchie A, Barker M. ‘There aren’t words for what we do or how we feel so we have to make them up’: Constructing polyamorous languages in a culture of compulsory monogamy. Sexualities. 2006;9(5):584–601. doi: 10.1177/1363460706069987. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues DL, Aybar Camposano GA, Lopes D. Stigmatization of consensual non-monogamous partners: Perceived endorsement of conservation or openness to change values vary according to personal attitudes. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2022;51(8):3931–3946. doi: 10.1007/s10508-022-02368-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues, D. L., Brooks, T. R., Moors, A. C., & Lopes, D. (2023). Examining the role of mononormative beliefs and internalized consensual non-monogamy negativity for stigma and dehumanization. Archives of Sexual Behavior. Advance online publication. 10.1007/s10508-023-02785-2 [DOI] [PubMed]
- Rodrigues DL, Fasoli F, Huic A, Lopes D. Which partners are more human? Monogamy matters more than sexual orientation for dehumanization in three European countries. Sexuality Research and Social Policy. 2018;15(4):504–515. doi: 10.1007/s13178-017-0290-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues DL, Lopes D. Sociosexuality, commitment, and sexual desire for an attractive person. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2017;46(3):775–788. doi: 10.1007/s10508-016-0814-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues DL, Lopes D, Conley TD. Non-monogamy agreements and safer sex behaviors: The role of perceived sexual self-control. Psychology & Sexuality. 2019;10(4):338–353. doi: 10.1080/19419899.2019.1649299. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues DL, Lopes D, Dawson K, de Visser R, Štulhofer A. With or without you: Associations between frequency of internet pornography use and sexual relationship outcomes for (non)consensual (non)monogamous individuals. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2021;50(4):1491–1504. doi: 10.1007/s10508-020-01782-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues DL, Lopes D, Huic A. What drives the dehumanization of consensual non-monogamous partners? Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2021;50(4):1587–1597. doi: 10.1007/s10508-020-01895-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues DL, Lopes D, Pereira M. “We agree and now everything goes my way”: Consensual sexual nonmonogamy, extradyadic sex, and relationship satisfaction. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking. 2016;19(6):373–379. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2016.0114. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues DL, Lopes D, Pereira M. Sociosexuality, commitment, sexual infidelity, and perceptions of infidelity: Data from the Second Love web site. Journal of Sex Research. 2017;54(2):241–253. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2016.1145182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues DL, Lopes D, Pereira M, de Visser R, Cabaceira I. Sociosexual attitudes and quality of life in (non)monogamous relationships: The role of attraction and constraining forces among users of the Second Love web site. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2019;48(6):1795–1809. doi: 10.1007/s10508-018-1272-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues DL, Lopes D, Smith CV. Caught in a “bad romance”? Reconsidering the negative association between sociosexuality and relationship functioning. Journal of Sex Research. 2017;54(9):1118–1127. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2016.1252308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rodrigues DL, Prada M, Lopes D. Perceived sexual self-control and condom use with primary and casual sex partners: Age and relationship agreement differences in a Portuguese sample. Psychology & Health. 2019;34(10):1231–1249. doi: 10.1080/08870446.2019.1603384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rostosky SS, Riggle ED. Same-sex relationships and minority stress. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2017;13:29–38. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2016.04.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubel A, Bogaert A. Consensual nonmonogamy: Psychological well-being and relationship quality correlates. Journal of Sex Research. 2015;52(9):961–982. doi: 10.1080/00224499.2014.942722. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rubin J, Moors A, Matsick J, Ziegler A, Conley T. On the margins: Considering diversity among consensually non-monogamous relationships. Journal für Psychologie. 2014;22(1):19–37. [Google Scholar]
- Rusbult, C., Agnew, C., & Arriaga, X. (2012). The investment model of commitment processes. In P. Van Lange, A. Kruglanski, & E. Higgins (Eds.), Handbook of theories of social psychology (Vol. 2, pp. 218–232). SAGE.
- Ryan, C., & Jetha, C. (2012). Sex at dawn: How we mate, why we stray, and what it means for modern relationships. Harper Collins.
- Schechinger HA, Sakaluk JK, Moors AC. Harmful and helpful therapy practices with consensually non-monogamous clients: Toward an inclusive framework. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 2018;86(11):879–891. doi: 10.1037/ccp0000349. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scoats R, Campbell C. What do we know about consensual non-monogamy? Current Opinion in Psychology. 2022;48:101468. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2022.101468. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Séguin LJ, Blais M, Goyer M-F, Adam BD, Lavoie F, Rodrigue C, Magontier C. Examining relationship quality across three types of relationship agreements. Sexualities. 2017;20(1–2):86–104. doi: 10.1177/1363460716649337. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Sizemore KM, Olmstead SB. Willingness of emerging adults to engage in consensual non-monogamy: A mixed-methods analysis. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2018;47(5):1423–1438. doi: 10.1007/s10508-017-1075-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stephens AK, Emmers-Sommer TM. Adults’ identities, attitudes, and orientations concerning consensual non-monogamy. Sexuality Research and Social Policy. 2020;17(3):469–485. doi: 10.1007/s13178-019-00409-w. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stults CB. Relationship quality among young gay and bisexual men in consensual nonmonogamous relationships. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2019;36(10):3037–3056. doi: 10.1177/0265407518809530. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Torres CA, Rodrigues DL. Heteronormative beliefs and internalized homonegativity in the coming out process of Portuguese and Turkish sexual minority men. Sexuality Research and Social Policy. 2022;19(2):663–677. doi: 10.1007/s13178-021-00582-x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Træen B, Thuen F. Non-consensual and consensual non-monogamy in Norway. International Journal of Sexual Health. 2022;34(1):65–80. doi: 10.1080/19317611.2021.1947931. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vaes J, Leyens J-P, Paladino MP, Miranda MP. We are human, they are not: Driving forces behind outgroup dehumanisation and the humanisation of the ingroup. European Review of Social Psychology. 2012;23(1):64–106. doi: 10.1080/10463283.2012.665250. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Valadez AM, Rohde J, Tessler J, Beals K. Perceived stigmatization and disclosure among individuals in consensually nonmonogamous relationships. Analyses of Social Issues and Public Policy. 2020;20(1):143–165. doi: 10.1111/asap.12194. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Vaughan MD, Jones P, Taylor BA, Roush J. Healthcare experiences and needs of consensually non-monogamous people: Results from a focus group study. Journal of Sexual Medicine. 2019;16(1):42–51. doi: 10.1016/j.jsxm.2018.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Visintin EP, Green EGT, Falomir-Pichastor JM, Berent J. Intergroup contact moderates the influence of social norms on prejudice. Group Processes & Intergroup Relations. 2020;23(3):418–440. doi: 10.1177/1368430219839485. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Webster G, Laurenceau J-P, Smith C, Mahaffey A, Bryan A, Brunell A. An investment model of sociosexuality, relationship satisfaction, and commitment: Evidence from dating, engaged, and newlywed couples. Journal of Research in Personality. 2015;55:112–126. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2015.02.004. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wood J, Desmarais S, Burleigh T, Milhausen R. Reasons for sex and relational outcomes in consensually nonmonogamous and monogamous relationships: A self-determination theory approach. Journal of Social and Personal Relationships. 2018;35(4):632–654. doi: 10.1177/0265407517743082. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Wood J, De Santis C, Desmarais S, Milhausen R. Motivations for engaging in consensually non-monogamous relationships. Archives of Sexual Behavior. 2021;50(4):1253–1272. doi: 10.1007/s10508-020-01873-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ziegler A, Conley T, Moors A, Matsick J, Rubin J. Monogamy. In: Richards C, Barker M, editors. The Palgrave handbook of the psychology of sexuality. Palgrave Macmillan; 2015. pp. 219–235. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Not applicable.