Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2024 Mar 8.
Published in final edited form as: J Marriage Fam. 2015 Mar 4;77(2):351–354. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12165

Looking Forward in Candidate Gene Research: Concerns and Suggestions

Gabriel L Schlomer *, H H Cleveland *, David J Vandenbergh **, Gregory M Fosco ***, Mark E Feinberg ****
PMCID: PMC10923596  NIHMSID: NIHMS917973  PMID: 38463629

Abstract

Candidate Gene × Environment (cGxE) interaction research holds promise for helping us understand for whom and why environments matter for families and development. In their commentary on our target article (G. L. Schlomer, G. M. Fosco, H. H. Cleveland, D. J. Vandenbergh, & M. E. Feinberg, 2015), J. E. Salvatore and D. M. Dick (2015) present their view of the current state and future of cGxE research and frame the debate regarding its merits for advancing knowledge of gene–environment interplay. In this reply, we discuss points of agreement and departure and provide a list of 5 domains by which the quality of cGxE research should be evaluated. Our hope is that researchers will use this list as a guide for their own work.

Keywords: candidate gene, GxE, sociobiology, susceptibility


We are grateful to the Journal of Marriage and Family for this forum in which to discuss important issues in candidate Gene × Environment (cGxE) family research and to Drs. Salvatore and Dick (2015) for sharing their expertise and their thoughtful and balanced commentary on our article (Schlomer, Fosco, Cleveland, Vandenbergh, & Feinberg, 2015). We found many points of agreement in their commentary and believe that differences in our views are a matter of degree and emphasis rather than contrast.

We agree with Salvatore and Dick that a major challenge in cGxE research is “which gene.” We differ, however, with the suggestion that considering the “usual suspects” is necessarily problematic. Although this strategy should not be exclusively pursued, there is now a decades-old literature on these usual-suspect markers (e.g., SLC6A4, DRD4, MAOA, CHRNA5), including genomic function, neurological/hormonal correlates, endophenotypes, psychological/behavioral outcomes, and environmental moderation. It is because these markers have been well characterized, at multiple analytic levels, that they have been incorporated into family/developmental research. Given adequate sophistication and care (see Cleveland et al., in press), research with these genes can provide valuable insights into gene–environment interplay.

Salvatore and Dick note that genome-wide association studies (GWAS) have shown inconsistent associations with psychiatric phenotypes and suggest we should be no better at “guessing” sensitivity genes. Our own skepticism (Schlomer et al., 2014) has been somewhat abated, however, by the growing number of studies that have found cGxE patterns consistent with plasticity theories (i.e., differential susceptibility theory, diathesis–stress, vantage sensitivity), including compelling evidence from experimental studies (e.g.,van IJzendoorn & Bakermans-Kranenburg, in press) and prevention/intervention studies that use random assignment (e.g., Brody Beach, Philibert, Chen, & Murry, 2009). Two additional experimental studies of DRD4 and alcohol use (Creswell et al., 2012; Larsen et al., 2010) have made a particularly strong case that some genes deserve the status of a usual suspect.

GWAS and cGxE approaches have unique strengths, and neither should be dismissed on the basis of their limitations. GWAS analyses are greatly important for discovering gene–phenotype links. However, the inconsistency noted by Salvatore and Dick may be the result of the search for population-level main effects. Because they require large samples, GWAS findings likely reflect a gene’s average effect over the wide range of environments from which the samples were drawn. However, if a genetic effect is larger in one environment and smaller in others, the overall main effect may be small and thus difficult to detect statistically. The solution to this problem has been to increase power through sample size. Interaction-based work underscores a reality that in many cases (based on the shape of the interaction and the distribution of the environments sampled) a null main effect may hide an interaction, as exemplified in our findings (Schlomer et al., 2015). Thus, in conventional GWAS a null main effect overlooks the possibility that an allelic association may be environmentally contingent. The remedy for inconsistent GWAS findings may include more than increasing sample size, such as considering conditional associations (i.e., moderation).

Moving Forward in cGxE Research

Our view of the future of cGxE research is optimistic, tempered by the need for careful and critical evaluation of this work. To assist in this process, we emphasize five domains in which cGxE research should be evaluated: Design, Measurement, Theory, Biological Role, and Population Structure.

Design

There are substantial benefits of applying randomized designs to cGxE research. In epidemiological studies, causality is difficult to determine because experience–outcome associations may equally reflect causal environmental influences or self-selection into those environmental experiences. These niche-picking processes reduce both the ability to infer cause (see G. J. Duncan, Magnuson, & Ludwig, 2004) and the statistical power to identify interactions. Prevention/intervention trials, owing to randomization, eliminate nonrandom selection to the environment (i.e., intervention vs. control) and create a unique opportunity to examine cGxE interactions without confounds due to gene–environment correlation (rGE). Moreover, randomized intervention designs offer substantially more power to detect interactions in cGxE than other designs (see Bakermans-Kranenberg & van IJzendoorn, 2015; McClelland & Judd, 1993).

Measurement

Researchers should be wary of categorically dismissive conclusions about cGxE research. For example, some of the strongest critiques of cGxE research have emerged from research reviews (e.g., L. E. Duncan & Keller, 2011; Munafo, Durrant, Lewis, & Flint, 2009; Risch et al., 2009) that include studies that vary in measurement quality. We, along with others (e.g., Karg, Burmeister, Shedden, & Sen, 2011; Rutter,Thapar, &Pickles,2009; Uher&McGuffin, 2008), disagree with conclusions drawn by these reviews, including the notion that all cGxE research should be considered suspect (see Gildersleeve, Haselton, & Fales, 2014). Family and developmental researchers are in a unique position to capitalize on high-quality measurement, which, among other strengths, can increase statistical power to a greater extent than increasing sample size (Manchia et al., 2013).

Theory

We agree with Salvatore and Dick that using theory to shape cGxE research questions is critical. Not only can theory direct researchers toward the most relevant constructs to consider, but it also can play an important role in helping researchers avoid searching for findings and leveraging chance results.

Biological Role

Equally important to theory is considering the underlying biological processes for genetic associations or environmental interactions. We suggest that the “which gene” decision be made on a range of levels, from broad to narrow, that demonstrate a cogent role for the marker vis-à-vis both the phenotype and environment. For instance, one might start by examining literature that demonstrates a gene is associated with behavioral outcomes, proceeding to more narrow associations (e.g., perceptual, neurocognitive) and, finally, to the physiological and molecular level (e.g., cell activity and gene expression). It is additionally important to be cognizant of allele coding schemes across analyses (e.g., dominant, additive). For an application of these criteria see Cleveland et al. (in press).

Population Structure

Because allele frequencies can vary across populations, inattention to population structure (e.g., genetic ancestry) can lead to spurious results (Knowler, Williams, Pettitt, & Steinberg, 1988). The best strategies to address population structure confounds will vary by sample size, sample diversity, genes examined, outcomes considered, and the combination of these that pose threats to internal validity. Helpful readings on this topic area include Ziv and Gonzalez-Burchard (2003) and Keller (2014).

Conclusion

By considering these five areas necessary for comprehensive cGxE research, a more nuanced—and, we hope, explanatory—set of results will lead the field forward. Although it may be difficult for any single study to maximize quality in each domain, how these domains are addressed as a whole strongly influences validity and generalizability of results. We agree with Salvatore and Dick that research into family processes is likely to be a rich and productive avenue and reveal much about gene–environment interplay.

References

  1. Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ & van IJzendoorn MH (2015). The hidden efficacy of interventions: Gene × Environment experiments from a differential susceptibility perspective. Annual Review of Psychology 66, 381–409. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Brody GH, Beach SR, Philibert RA, Chen YF, & Murry VM (2009). Prevention effects moderate the association of 5-HTTLPR and youth risk behavior initiation: Gene × Environment hypotheses tested via a randomized prevention design. Child Development, 80, 645–661. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2009.01288.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Cleveland HH, Schlomer GL, Vandenbergh DJ, Feinberg ME, Greenberg M, . . . Hair KL. (in press; ). The conditioning of intervention effects on early adolescent alcohol use by maternal involvement and DRD4 and 5-HTTLPR candidate genes. Development and Psychopathology. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Creswell KG, Sayette MA, Manuck SB, Ferrell RE, Hill SY, & Dimoff JD (2012). DRD4 polymorphism moderates the effect of alcohol consumption on social bonding. PLoS ONE, 7, e28914. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0028914 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Duncan GJ, Magnuson KA, & Ludwig J (2004). The endogeneity problem in developmental studies. Research in Human Development, 1, 59–80. [Google Scholar]
  6. Duncan LE, & Keller MC (2011). A critical review of the first 10 years of candidate gene-by-environment interaction research in psychiatry. American Journal of Psychiatry, 168, 1041–1049. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.2011.11020191 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Gildersleeve K, Haselton MG, & Fales MR (2014). Meta-analysis and p-curves support robust cycle shifts in women’s mate preferences: Reply to Wood and Carden (2014) and Harris, Pashler, and Mickes (2014). Psychological Bulletin, 140, 1272–1280. doi: 10.1037/a0037714 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Karg K, Burmeister M, Shedden K, & Sen S (2011). The serotonin transporter promoter variant (5-HTTLPR), stress, and depression meta-analysis revisited: Evidence of genetic moderation. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 444–454. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2010.189 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Keller MC (2014). Gene × Environment interaction studies have not properly controlled for potential confounders: The problem and the (simple) solution. Biological Psychiatry, 75, 18–24. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2013.09.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Knowler WC, Williams RC, Pettitt DJ, & Steinberg AG (1988). Gm3;5,13,14 and type 2 diabetes mellitus: An association in American Indians with genetic admixture. American Journal of Human Genetics, 43, 520–526. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Larsen H, van der Zwaluw CS, Overbeek G Granic I., Franke B., & Engles RC. (2010). A variable-number-of-tandem-repeats polymorphism in the dopamine D4 receptor gene affects social adaptation of alcohol use: Investigation of a gene–environment interaction. Psychological Science, 21, 1064–1068. doi: 10.1177/0956797610376654 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Manchia M, Cullis J, Truecki G, Rouleau GA, Uher R, & Alda M (2013). The impact of phenotypic and genetic heterogeneity on results of genome wide association studies of complex diseases. PLoS ONE, 10, e76295. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0076295 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. McClelland GH, & Judd CM (1993). Statistical difficulties of detecting interactions and moderator effects. Psychological Bulletin, 114, 376–390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Munafò MR, Durrant C, Lewis G, & Flint J (2009). Gene × Environment interactions at the serotonin transporter locus. Biological Psychiatry, 65, 211–219. doi: 10.1016/j.biopsych.2008.06.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Risch N, Herrell R, Lehner T, Liang K, Eaves L, Hoh J, . . . Merikangas KR (2009). Interaction between the serotonin transporter gene (5-HTTLPR), stressful life events, and risk of depression: A meta-analysis. Journal of the American Medical Association, 301, 2462–2471. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Rutter M, Thapar A, & Pickles A (2009). Gene–environment interactions: Biologically valid pathway or artifact? Archives of General Psychiatry, 66, 1287–1289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Salvatore JE, & Dick DM (2015). Gene–environment interplay: Where we are, where we are going. Journal of Marriage and Family, 77, 344–350. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12164 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Schlomer GL, Cleveland HH, Vandenbergh DJ, Feinberg ME, Neiderhiser JM, . . . Redmond C. (2014). Developmental differences in early adolescent aggression: A Gene × Environment × Intervention analysis. Journal of Youth and Adolescence. Advance online publication. doi: 10.1007/s10964-014-0198-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Schlomer GL, Fosco GM, Cleveland HH, Vandenbergh DJ, & Feinberg ME (2015). Interparental relationship sensitivity leads to adolescent internalizing problems: Different genotypes, different pathways. Journal of Marriage and Family, 77, 329–343. doi: 10.1111/jomf.12168 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Uher R, & McGuffin P (2008). The moderation by the serotonin transporter gene of environmental adversity in the aetiology of mental illness: Review and methodological analysis. Molecular Psychiatry, 12, 131–146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. van IJzendoorn MH, & Bakermans-Kranenburg MJ (in press; ). Genetic differential susceptibility on trial: Meta-analytic support from randomized controlled experiments. Development and Psychopathology. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Ziv E, & Gonzalez-Burchard E (2003). Human population structure and genetic association studies. Pharmacogenomics, 4, 431–441. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES