Abstract
Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) has increasingly been used to characterize structure–function relationships during white matter neuroplasticity. Biological sex differences may be an important factor that affects patterns of neuroplasticity, and therefore impacts learning and rehabilitation. The current study examined a participant cohort before and after visuo-motor training to characterize sex differences in microstructural measures. The participants (N = 27) completed a 10-session (4 week) complex visuo-motor training task with their non-dominant hand. All participants significantly improved movement speed and their movement speed variability over the training period. White matter neuroplasticity in females and males was examined using fractional anisotropy (FA) and myelin water fraction (MWF) along the cortico-spinal tract (CST) and the corpus callosum (CC). FA values showed significant differences in the middle portion of the CST tract (nodes 38–51) across the training period. MWF showed a similar cluster in the inferior portion of the tract (nodes 18–29) but did not reach significance. Additionally, at baseline, males showed significantly higher levels of MWF measures in the middle body of the CC. Combining data from females and males would have resulted in reduced sensitivity, making it harder to detect differences in neuroplasticity. These findings offer initial insights into possible female versus male differences in white matter neuroplasticity during motor learning. This warrants investigations into specific patterns of white matter neuroplasticity for females versus males across the lifespan. Understanding biological sex-specific differences in white matter neuroplasticity may have significant implications for the interpretation of change associated with learning or rehabilitation.
Subject terms: Myelin biology and repair, Learning and memory, Motor control, Imaging techniques, Neurology
Introduction
Neuroplasticity is a lifelong process where the brain reorganizes neural networks based on aging, life experiences, and directed practice and learning1–4. Motor learning is the process by which new motor skills are obtained and refined5,6. While the concept of neuroplasticity and the fact that the brain has a lifelong capacity to change is well accepted, the underlying mechanisms involved in this process are not yet fully understood. Multiple imaging methods appear to be sensitive to the underlying processes activated during neuroplasticity7.
Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) can be used to non-invasively characterize change in both structural and functional neuroanatomy8–15. MRI-derived diffusion tensor imaging (DTI) investigations of experience-dependent changes have provided in-vivo evidence for microstructural changes, specifically in white matter (WM)1,9,10,12,13,16–19. MRI-derived myelin water imaging (MWI) demonstrates histological accuracy as an in-vivo myelin measurement technique20,21, exhibiting sensitivity across diverse groups22–24. Moreover, it complements diffusion tensor imaging (DTI)25 as another MRI-derived modality to detect changes in myelin levels10.
The cortico-spinal tract (CST) is the primary descending pathway for voluntary motor control in the human central nervous system. Approximately 90% of the CST crosses the midline in the brainstem to provide control for contralateral portions of the body26–29. Reid et al. trained participants in a motor task with their left hands and examined MRI changes in the contralateral right hemisphere. The authors reported increases in fractional anisotropy (FA) along the right contralateral CST, measured with DTI12. Our group found similar results after training participants to perform a motor maze tracking task with both their left and right hands8–10. Participants showed significantly improved speed and accuracy with their non-dominant left hands, but not their right. We also noted structural and functional changes in the contralateral hemisphere, but not in the ipsilateral hemisphere8–10. Other recent MRI studies have shown contralateral neuroplasticity changes associated with motor learning (i.e., brain changes in the hemisphere contralateral to the trained limb)8–12,30. Yet no work to date has considered whether neuroplastic change associated with motor learning differs between biologically female and male individuals.
Biological sex differences may influence the mechanisms that underly neuroplastic change31,32. Both estrogen and testosterone affect neuroplasticity33–36. Catenaccio et al. completed a systematic review of neuroimaging literature and discovered physiological variation of brain macro- and microstructure in females caused by menstrual cycle phase, hormonal contraceptives, and menopause. Although generalizing findings across studies is difficult due to sample and methodology heterogeneity, the Catenaccio review concluded that ovarian hormones drive neuroplasticity33. Similarly, studies in mice have found that genetic background can have a large effect on learning, neuroplasticity, and behavior37,38. Additionally, genetic variation (specifically in regard to brain-derived neurotrophic factor) affects stroke rehabilitation39,40. Therefore, if subtle genetic differences can affect neuroplasticity, then they may also impact motor skill learning.
The use of non-invasive neuroimaging to investigate differences in female and male brains has increased over the past three decades. The majority of early studies researching human sex based differences in the brain have focused on the size of major brain structures, or the size of the brain as a whole41–44. Varied conclusions regarding sex differences in the corpus callosum (CC) have been drawn from DTI data45. FA values46 and relative anisotropy47 have been reported to be higher in male CCs. Other work showed higher FA in female CC splenium48 and different FA levels based on area of CC49,50, or no difference at all51–53. According to Toschi et al., WM degeneration associated with healthy aging in several regions, including the CC, begins earlier in males (> 10 years) than females, with males also showing higher FA values than females45. Interestingly, Schmithorst et al. used FA to show females (ages 5–18 years) displayed a trend of increasing organization with age (reflected in increasing FA values) only in the right hemisphere, while age matched males only showed this trend in the left hemisphere48. Yet it is not clear how to interpret higher FA in this context, as FA values in males did not correlate with improved cognitive function, while it did with females54.
The body of the CC has been associated with motor and visuomotor task activation55–57 and FA measures in this region positively correlate with performance during a motor learning session58. While past work has shown relationships between FA in the CST and CC, with measures of motor learning8–12,30,59–63 very little work has considered how biological sex impacts this relationship. This gap in our knowledge fundamentally limits our ability to tailor sex specific motor learning interventions. Even from a methodological perspective, it is important to better understand whether sex-differences represent a factor in WM neuroplasticity (and neuroplasticity in general).
Thus, the current study aimed to characterize differences in neuroplastic change in female and male CST and CC regions associated with learning a complex semi-immersive, visuo-motor task. Given the prior WM microstructural MRI evidence, we focused on FA and myelin water fraction (MWF) differences. We predicted that underlying FA and MWF differences between females and males would be significantly detectable in the CST and CC, and specifically for the CST these differences would be evident in the contralateral hemisphere following motor learning with the non-dominant hand (Study Summary - Fig. 1).
Results
Behaviour
Across 10,000 trained arm movements, participants showed significant decreases in mean movement speed and movement speed variability (decrease in movement speed standard deviation) from the first 200 movements to the last 200 movements (Fig. 2). At baseline, female and male mean movement speed and mean movement variation had equal variances [based on Levene’s: Speed: F(1,25) = 0.970, p = 0.33. Variance: F(1,25) = 0.945, p = 0.34]. Therefore, a two-tailed independent samples t-test determined baseline mean movement speed and baseline mean movement variation were not significantly different between sexes [speed: t(25) = 1.684, p = 0.105, g = 0.651; and variation: t(25) = 0.859, p = 0.399, g = 0.332]. Significant main effects of time were observed for movement speed [F(1,25) = 195.41, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.887] and movement speed variability [F(1,25) = 24.838, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.498]. However, the sex × time interaction was not significant for either dependent measure [Speed: F(1,25) = 2.952, p = 0.098, ηp2 = 0.106 & Variability: F(1,25) = 1.027, p = 0.321, ηp2 = 0.039]. Additionally, female and male ages had equal variances [based on Levene’s: F(1,25) = 0.069, p = 0.796]. A two-tailed independent samples t-test determined that female and male participant groups did not show a significant age difference [t(25) = 1.170, p = 0.253, g = 0.452].
Neuroimaging
Cortico-spinal tract
FA and MWF tract profiles of CST contralateral to the trained non-dominant arm showed sex-specific changes from pre- to post-behaviour training along the tract. Cluster-based permutation testing of the FA tract profile showed significant change in both female and male separated profiles in the middle portion of the CST tract (14 nodes38–51, cluster-based approach p < 0.005) (Fig. 1). A mixed repeated measures analysis of variance (RM-ANOVA) did not show a significant main effect of time [F(1,25) = 0.348, p = 0.561, ηp2 = 0.014] or sex [F(1,25) = 0.364, p = 0.552, ηp2 = 0.014], but showed a significant sex × time interaction in this area for FA [F(1,25) = 17.109, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.406] (Fig. 3). Additionally, post-hoc testing showed significant change in this cluster for both females and males (females: p = 0.007, males: p = 0.006, Bonferroni corrected). The MWF tract profile only displayed a non-significant cluster of 12 nodes in the inferior portion of the tract (nodes 18–29) for females. Examination of the results showed increased MWF variability, particularly in the male CST.
Corpus callosum
While no main effect of time or sex × time interactions were observed in the CC for FA or MWF, there was a significant MWF difference at baseline between females and males. Males had greater MWF in baseline tracts through the body of the CC compared to females, specifically in the middle portion of the tract (26 nodes [nodes42–67, permutation test peak p < 0.025) and RM-ANOVA post-hoc testing showed a significant male greater than female difference at baseline in this area of the MWF tract [t(25) = 2.526, p < 0.025, g = 0.976, Bonferroni corrected] (Fig. 4).
Discussion
The current study characterized changes in female and male brains associated with visuo-motor learning. We used a similar tract-based analysis to Kirby et al.10, which stemmed from original tract profile publications like Yeatman et al.64 and Dayan et al.65, to analyze data before and after complex visuo-motor training and directly evaluate the CST and the CC. The results of our analyses suggest structural differences between female and male patterns of neuroplastic change. Secondarily, the study demonstrated baseline CC tract (male greater than female MWF) differences that were not evident following motor learning.
To our knowledge, this is the first investigation to evaluate female and male neuroplastic change in WM using multimodal MRI. At minimum, the current study provided three noteworthy insights: (1) microstructural MRI measures of neuroplasticity in WM appear to be consistent with emerging evidence from the prior literature8–12,30, (2) the findings stressed the importance of disaggregating female and male data, and (3) sex related differences in WM were noted despite similar change in motor behaviour for females and males. Consistent with our past studies on WM neuroplasticity9,10, FA and MWF showed sensitivity to neuroplastic change in the CST contralateral to the trained limb, following behavioural change. Contrary to our previous findings, MWF did not reach significance in the current study. This may be due the older age in the current study, as MRI-based myelin levels have been previously associated with age66,67, or due to task related differences. As this complex task may have activated more networks, future studies can dig deeper into this potential more widespread change. Still, our findings highlight the importance of analyzing data by biological sex separately, as combining female and male data sets could result in a loss of significant WM findings, as shown by the absence of a significant time main effect in the RM-ANOVA. It is interesting to note that differences in structural WM were noted between females and males even though the pattern of behavioural change did not differ between the groups. The structural MRI evidence is consistent with fMRI studies of female versus male fMRI activation differences68–73. Gorbet et al.72 determined that performance between females and males in an increasingly complex visuo-motor task did not differ, but showed differences in fMRI activity between females and males. The current findings further underscore that differences may exist in one measurement modality and not the other. Future investigations are needed to characterize the underlying biological significance of these data.
The current study supports the importance of including data that are disaggregated by biological sex in analyses. Structurally, males have shown greater cortical surface areas in regions across the entire cortex than females. Importantly, males have greater variability in cortical surface area measures, as well as subcortical volume and cortical thickness, across the brain74. Furthermore, in studies with large numbers of participants, regional differences between females and males have shown that males have greater cortical surface area, while females show a greater cortical thickness for many regions across the cortex, including motor and somatosensory regions74,75. Less variability and increased thickness may lead to more focal neural processing. Functionally, this has been demonstrated by Andrushko et al.73 where greater functional volumes and larger inter-subject variability of fMRI activation was observed in males compared to females across 12 different voluntary motor tasks. Thus, a more focal and consistent neural signaling was noted in females during voluntary motor movements73. While not fully understood, these data hint at the possibility that different neural processes underpin brain activity in females versus males. It is possible, that in the current work more focused and efficient neural processing by females during motor learning led to neuroplasticity differences in WM. Speculatively, a more focal and efficient neural processing by females would not need an increased speed of signal transmission from a large cortical surface area, but instead would be focused on more direct WM structures for efficient signal transmission, such as the CST. This pattern of activity could be a potential explanation for the significant FA increase in the CST in females and not males; future work that contains both functional and structural data will have to test this hypothesis.
It is curious that at baseline MWF tract profiles of the CC body showed that males had more myelin as compared to females. At endpoint this difference no longer stayed significant. Others have reported similar data. Liu et al.49, Björnholm et al.50, and Shin et al.46 showed that males had higher FA as compared to females in multiple body portions of the CC. Altered myelination in the CC between females and males has previously been accredited to histological studies showing the total number and density of fibers to be decreased rather than increased in male CC regions76. As a result, Westerhausen et al. and Schmithorst et al. hypothesized that higher microstructural properties in male CC regions are due to fewer but thicker myelinated fibers47,48, further explaining higher MWF levels in males. It is known that thicker myelinated fibers increase the speed of signal transmission77. Thus, it is possible that greater myelination across the corpus callosum may be used to recruit a higher surface area of cortex to carry out voluntary motor tasks in males. However, at this time these ideas are speculative. As this field of research is still in its early stages, the underlying causes of microstructural and neuroplasticity differences between female and male brains are not yet fully understood.
Several issues may limit the generalizability of the data reported here. It should be noted that the female (n = 17) and male (n = 10) subsamples were not closely matched by number and they are relatively small. Accordingly, the current findings must be replicated with larger, matched sample sizes. Indeed, with higher sample sizes it may be possible to detect more subtle but significant changes in the corpus callosum between females and males. Furthermore, as sex hormones change across the lifespan, future work should include data from a large age range and incorporate reproductive factors that affect hormone exposure. An increased age range can also offer greater generalizability and potentially reveal different mechanisms of neuroplasticity across the lifespan between biological sex, as this biological sex and age connection has been explored in neuroplasticity and exercise efficacy78. The current findings were obtained from a specific task in targeted CST and CC WM structures. Complexity, task-specific goals, and baseline motor skills represent additional factors affecting WM neuroplasticity, as these have previously been shown to impact motor skill learning in general79,80. As such it is not clear whether the differences shown in the current data set will generalize to other motor learning tasks. Further, FA has also demonstrated low sensitivity to a single neurobiological process81 making it difficult to characterize the exact mechanism behind neuroplasticity changes, specifically the decrease in measures noted in males. Prior research investigating neuroplasticity has observed reductions in FA associated with learning. This has been interpreted as improved performance and learning-related FA or diffusivity decline according to previous studies61, while another study attributed decrease in μFA (a different form of FA) to synaptic pruning82. Future studies should integrate other non-MRI measures, such as magnetoencephalography, to further investigate female and male neural processing differences and the neuroplasticity that underlies this. Lastly, differences in patterns of change in females vs males should be investigated in patient populations, including stroke, to better understand neuroplastic mechanisms of recovery and to enhance the effectiveness of neurorehabilitation interventions.
Conclusion
While not definitive, due to the limitations outlined above, our findings suggest that female and male differences in WM neuroplasticity may be detectable using MRI FA measures. MWF changes followed a similar trend but were insignificant. Importantly, we discovered differences in WM for females versus males despite both groups showing significant behavioural changes associated with motor learning. These findings suggest that there are distinct patterns of change that take place in female versus male brains to support motor learning. Critically, had the two groups been combined these WM different patterns would not have been detected. Despite increasing recognition in the field of neurosciences and health related research that sex should be considered in neuroscience study design and analysis83,84, most studies fail to do so85,86. Our behavioral and imaging results are in line with the demand of all funding agencies to include sex as a biological factor86–88 and provide further support for the importance of including sex-based analyses, as females and males may exhibit differences in neural mechanisms45,89,90. Our data suggest that future work should consider how differences in biological sex affect patterns of both structural and functional change associated with motor learning.
Methods
Experimental paradigm
The current study analyzed a healthy normative data set from a larger study that focused on stroke rehabilitation. Twenty-seven (N = 27) neurologically intact, older adult participants (mean age ± standard deviation: 64.2 ± 8.5 years, 2 left-hand dominant, 17 females, female mean age ± standard deviation: 62.8 ± 8.0 years, male mean age ± standard deviation: 66.7 ± 8.3 years) completed a 10-session (4 week) complex visuo-motor training task. Task specifics are provided in Kraeutner et al.91. Briefly, participants engaged in a semi-immersive virtual reality-based intercept and release task (TRack And Intercept Task; TRAIT) that was presented on a 46-inch monitor, viewed at 72 inches away (screen refresh rate 59 Hz). Movement time in seconds was recorded for all intercept and release movements during the visuo-motor training sessions using a Microsoft Kinect (model no. 1517, Kinect for Window; Microsoft, Redmond, WA) camera. The task was calibrated using a four-point grid to personalize the workspace. The task was set in “outer space”, where the spaceship, asteroid, and sun corresponded to the position of the hand, object, and target, respectively. The task was designed to create a semi-immersive engaging and motivating environment to promote a large dose (10,000 repetitions) of skilled movements92.
Participants trained with non-dominant arms by intercepting a virtual moving object and then accurately throwing the object at the target. Auditory feedback, visual feedback and knowledge of results were provided and used to maintain engagement. Each of the 10 sessions involved 5 blocks of the task, with each block containing 200 movements (100 object intercepts and 100 object releases). An 80% success rate for two consecutive blocks advanced the participant to the next level of difficulty. Increasing object speed, decreasing object size, and decreasing target size were used to manipulate task difficulty. Informed consent from each participant was obtained according to the Declaration of Helsinki. The Research Ethics Board at the University of British Columbia approved all study procedures.
Behavioural statistical analyses
To investigate sex differences associated with motor learning, a mixed between (sex: female, male) and within (time: baseline and endpoint) RM-ANOVA was run for both mean movement time and mean movement time variability (standard deviation) for the first 200 and last 200 movements of the learning task (i.e., the first and last block of training). All RM-ANOVA tests were run through SPSS (IBM Corp., Armonk, NY)93. Additionally, the Levene test94,95 was performed on the two sex groups’ mean movement time and mean movement time variability to determine equality of variance. To ensure there were no differences at baseline between the two groups, two-tailed t-test was completed on mean movement time and mean movement time variability between the two biological sex groups. Lastly, the Levene test94,95 was performed on the two sex groups’ ages to determine equality of variance. To ensure there were no differences between the two groups, two-tailed t-test was completed on age between the two biological sex groups.
Magnetic resonance imaging acquisition
MRI data were acquired at baseline and endpoint within 24 h before motor training and after completing four weeks of motor practice with a 3 Tesla Philips Achieva (Best, The Netherlands) MRI scanner using an eight-channel sensitivity encoding head coil and parallel imaging. MWI data were collected using a 32-echo gradient and spin echo (GRASE) MWI sequence T2 scan96. Parameters were as follows: TR = 1000 ms, TE = 10, 20, …, 310, 320 ms, scan duration = 836 s, 20 slices acquired at 5 mm slice thickness, 40 slices reconstructed at 2.5 mm slice thickness, resolution = 232 × 225, final dimensions: 240 × 240 × 40. DWI-based high angular resolution diffusion imaging (HARDI) data were acquired using a single-shot echo-planar imaging sequence with 60 diffusion directions, b-value of 700 s/mm2, and one b-value of 0 s/mm2 as the final volume of the scan. Acquisition parameters included: TR = 7088 ms, TE = 60 ms, scan duration = 447 s and voxel dimensions of 2 × 2 × 2.2 mm (70 slices), for tractography purposes voxels were reconstructed to isotropic size of 2 × 2 × 2 mm (77 slices) using FSL’s FLIRT97,98.
Magnetic resonance imaging processing
MWF maps were created using DECAES99 with T2 relaxation distributions spaced from 10 to 2000 ms and a T2 distribution of 10 ms < T2 < 25 ms for MWF. Similar to Birkl et al.100 and Kirby et al.10, this range was determined by analyzing T2 distribution peaks in the data. MWF maps were then transformed to the individual’s native DWI (diffusion weighted imaging) space by registering the first echo of the GRASE MWI data to the non-diffusion weighted scan from the DWI data using FSL’s FLIRT97,98. The resulting transform was then applied to the individual’s MWF map.
DWI data were motion and eddy corrected using FSL101. DIPY102 workflows were used for tractography and tract profile processing steps103 that included FA map creation and whole brain deterministic tracking using EuDX tracking with constant solid angle peaks104,105. CSTs were extracted using RecoBundles workflow106 and a model atlas107. BUndle ANalytics (BUAN) similarity scores compared each participant’s left and right CST at endpoint to their baseline tract. Briefly, this determined difference in shape, length, and size of streamline tracts using a metric called bundle adjacency (0 = no similarity, 1 = perfect similarity)103. Any participants with a low BUAN score (< 0.7) for either their left or right CST comparison were removed to run statistical tests with and without these data. A similar pattern of results remained; therefore, the two participants’ results were kept. Similar to the CST analysis, the CC analysis used the CCMid tract from the Yeh atlas107 and Chandio et al.103. This tract looked at all tracts traversing from the left medial cortex to the right medial cortex through the CC body. There was no BUAN score < 0.7 for the CCMid analysis. DIPY was used to employ a similar technique to automated fiber quantification64 to create tract profiles of microstructural values at 100 equidistant nodes.
Magnetic resonance imaging analysis
Changes in FA and MWF of females and males were evaluated across the right and left CST between baseline and endpoint scans using a cluster-based permutation method derived from108,109 to minimize the False Discovery Rate in mass univariate testing. Briefly, a mixed between (sex: female, male), and within (time: baseline and endpoint), RM-ANOVA was evaluated at each of the 100 nodes along the CST, specifically focused on sex × time interactions. Next, only resulting F-values below a threshold (F-value corresponding to p < 0.005) are used to generate clusters by identifying connected regions of significant data points. The result is a list of clusters and corresponding summations of F-values of each of these clusters, providing information about potentially significant regions in the data. Next, the data is shuffled randomly (sex and timepoint are shuffled between subjects) and rerun through this process. The maximum cluster sum of F-values along the 100 points is then stored for each iteration. This occurs for 5000 iterations to create the null distribution. The sum of F-values resulting from the clusters from the original RM-ANOVA are then compared to the array of maximum cluster summations of F-values, and only original clusters greater than the 97.5th percentile (two-tailed test) of the maximum cluster summations of F-values are considered to still be statistically significant. Results from F-max cluster-based testing were used as a searchlight for CST sections to run through a mixed between (sex: female, male), and within (number of nodes: FA = 14, MWF = 12; and time: baseline and endpoint), RM-ANOVA to evaluate both time and sex effects separately and as an interaction. Pairwise post-hoc comparisons done in the RM-ANOVA were corrected for multiple comparisons with Bonferroni correction. Non-parametric permutation testing was used to identify portions that were significantly different between female and male CCMid tracts at baseline. This portion was then run through a mixed between (sex: female, male), and within (number of nodes: MWF = 26; and time: baseline and endpoint), RM-ANOVA for consistency between tract analyses.
Ethics statement
Informed consent from each participant was obtained according to the Declaration of Helsinki. The Research Ethics Board at the University of British Columbia approved all study procedures.
Acknowledgements
A special acknowledgement to Erin Mazerolle, who stressed the importance of examining female and male differences from the earliest days of white matter functional MRI investigations.
Author contributions
EK contributed as primary author with data analysis and manuscript drafting. JA assisted with data analysis and manuscript drafting. SR contributed with data organization and manuscript editing. RD’A and LB contributed to study design and manuscript drafting. LB also acted as lab PI.
Funding
This work has been supported in part by funding from the Natural Sciences and Engineering Research Council of Canada (Discovery Grant, PI R.C.N. D’Arcy; Discovery Grant, PI L.A. Boyd; CGS M, E.D. Kirby), Canadian Institutes of Health Research (Operating Grant, PI L.A. Boyd). JA is supported by Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) and Michael Smith Foundation for Health Research (MSFHR) postdoctoral fellowships. S.R. is supported by awards from the University of British Columbia.
Data availability
The raw data used in the current study was collected with informed consent of the participants, who agreed to data use outlined by the research ethics boards of the University of British Columbia. Sharing of this data, supporting the conclusions of this article, will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher. However, it is not currently available in any publicly accessible source. Any interest in receiving the data or post-processing code should be expressed to the corresponding author.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher's note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Contributor Information
Ryan C. N. D’Arcy, Email: ryan_darcy@sfu.ca
Lara A. Boyd, Email: lara.boyd@ubc.ca
References
- 1.Sampaio-Baptista C, Johansen-Berg H. White matter plasticity in the adult brain. Neuron. 2017;96(6):1239–1251. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.11.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Guglielman E. The ageing brain: Neuroplasticity and lifelong learning. ELearn. Pap. 2012;29:1–7. [Google Scholar]
- 3.Kesselring J. Neuroplasticity: Basis for lifelong learning. Eur. Neurol. Rev. 2015;9:143. doi: 10.17925/ENR.2014.09.02.143. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Fields RD. Change in the brain’s white matter. Science. 2010;330(6005):768–769. doi: 10.1126/science.1199139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Schmidt, R. A., Lee, T. D., Winstein, C., Wulf, G. & Zelaznik, H. N. Motor Control and Learning: A Behavioral Emphasis. (Human Kinetics, 2018).
- 6.Nieuwboer A, Rochester L, Muncks L, Swinnen SP. Motor learning in Parkinson’s disease: Limitations and potential for rehabilitation. Parkinson. Relat. Disord. 2009;15(3):S53–S58. doi: 10.1016/S1353-8020(09)70781-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Christiansen L, Siebner HR. Tools to explore neuroplasticity in humans: Combining interventional neurophysiology with functional and structural magnetic resonance imaging and spectroscopy. In: Quartarone A, Ghilardi MF, Boller F, editors. Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Elsevier; 2022. pp. 105–119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Frizzell TO, Grajauskas LA, Liu CC, GhoshHajra S, Song X, D’Arcy RCN. White matter neuroplasticity: Motor learning activates the internal capsule and reduces hemodynamic response variability. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2020;14:509258. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.509258. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Frizzell TO, et al. Imaging functional neuroplasticity in human white matter tracts. Brain Struct. Funct. 2021 doi: 10.1007/s00429-021-02407-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Kirby ED, et al. Increased myelination plays a central role in white matter neuroplasticity. NeuroImage. 2022;263:119644. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119644. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Sale MV, Reid LB, Cocchi L, Pagnozzi AM, Rose SE, Mattingley JB. Brain changes following four weeks of unimanual motor training: Evidence from behavior, neural stimulation, cortical thickness, and functional MRI. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2017;38(9):4773–4787. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23710. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Reid LB, Sale MV, Cunnington R, Mattingley JB, Rose SE. Brain changes following four weeks of unimanual motor training: Evidence from fMRI-guided diffusion MRI tractography. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2017;38(9):4302–4312. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Hamaide J, De Groof G, Van der Linden A. Neuroplasticity and MRI: A perfect match. NeuroImage. 2016;131:13–28. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Tardif CL, et al. Advanced MRI techniques to improve our understanding of experience-induced neuroplasticity. NeuroImage. 2016;131:55–72. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.047. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Keller TA, Just MA. Structural and functional neuroplasticity in human learning of spatial routes. NeuroImage. 2016;125:256–266. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Scholz J, Klein MC, Behrens TEJ, Johansen-Berg H. Training induces changes in white matter architecture. Nat. Neurosci. 2009;12(11):1370–1371. doi: 10.1038/nn.2412. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Deng F, et al. Plasticity in deep and superficial white matter: A DTI study in world class gymnasts. Brain Struct. Funct. 2018;223(4):1849–1862. doi: 10.1007/s00429-017-1594-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Islam MR, et al. Diffusion tensor-MRI detects exercise-induced neuroplasticity in the hippocampal microstructure in mice. Brain Plast. 2020;5(2):147–159. doi: 10.3233/BPL-190090. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Izadi-Najafabadi S, Gill KK, Zwicker JG. Training-induced neuroplasticity in children with developmental coordination disorder. Curr. Dev. Disord. Rep. 2020;7(2):48–58. doi: 10.1007/s40474-020-00191-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Laule C, et al. Myelin water imaging in multiple sclerosis: Quantitative correlations with histopathology. Mult. Scler. 2006;12(6):747–753. doi: 10.1177/1352458506070928. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Laule C, Kozlowski P, Leung E, Li DKB, MacKay AL, Moore GRW. Myelin water imaging of multiple sclerosis at 7 T: Correlations with histopathology. NeuroImage. 2008;40(4):1575–1580. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.12.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bouhrara M, et al. Quantitative age-dependent differences in human brainstem myelination assessed using high-resolution magnetic resonance mapping. NeuroImage. 2020;206:116307. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2019.116307. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Edwards EM, Wu W, Fritz NE. Using myelin water imaging to link underlying pathology to clinical function in multiple sclerosis: A scoping review. Mult. Scler. Relat. Disord. 2022;59:103646. doi: 10.1016/j.msard.2022.103646. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Kolind S, et al. Brain and cord myelin water imaging: A progressive multiple sclerosis biomarker. NeuroImage Clin. 2015;9:574–580. doi: 10.1016/j.nicl.2015.10.002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kolind SH, et al. Complementary information from multi-exponential T2 relaxation and diffusion tensor imaging reveals differences between multiple sclerosis lesions. NeuroImage. 2008;40(1):77–85. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.11.033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Canedo A. Primary motor cortex influences on the descending and ascending systems. Progress Neurobiol. 1997;51(3):287–335. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(96)00058-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Robert MD, Davidoff A. The pyramidal tract. Neurology. 1990;40(2):332–332. doi: 10.1212/WNL.40.2.332. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.York DH. Review of descending motor pathways involved with transcranial stimulation. Neurosurgery. 1987;20(1):70–73. doi: 10.1097/00006123-198701000-00021. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Whitehead L, Banihani S. The evolution of contralateral control of the body by the brain: Is it a protective mechanism? Laterality. 2014;19(3):325–339. doi: 10.1080/1357650X.2013.824461. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Lakhani B, et al. Motor skill acquisition promotes human brain myelin plasticity. Neural Plast. 2016;2016:e7526135. doi: 10.1155/2016/7526135. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Marrocco J, McEwen BS. Sex in the brain: Hormones and sex differences. Dialog. Clin. Neurosci. 2016;18(4):373–383. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2016.18.4/jmarrocco. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.McEwen BS, Milner TA. Understanding the broad influence of sex hormones and sex differences in the brain. J. Neurosci. Res. 2017;95(1–2):24–39. doi: 10.1002/jnr.23809. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Catenaccio E, Mu W, Lipton ML. Estrogen- and progesterone-mediated structural neuroplasticity in women: Evidence from neuroimaging. Brain Struct. Funct. 2016;221(8):3845–3867. doi: 10.1007/s00429-016-1197-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Pawluski JL, Lambert KG, Kinsley CH. Neuroplasticity in the maternal hippocampus: Relation to cognition and effects of repeated stress. Horm. Behav. 2016;77:86–97. doi: 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2015.06.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Barha CK, Galea LAM. Influence of different estrogens on neuroplasticity and cognition in the hippocampus. Biochim. Biophys. Acta BBA Gen. Subj. 2010;1800(10):1056–1067. doi: 10.1016/j.bbagen.2010.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Saleki K, Banazadeh M, Saghazadeh A, Rezaei N. Aging, testosterone, and neuroplasticity: Friend or foe? Rev. Neurosci. 2022 doi: 10.1515/revneuro-2022-0033. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Ranson A, Sengpiel F, Fox K. The role of GluA1 in ocular dominance plasticity in the mouse visual cortex. J. Neurosci. 2013;33(38):15220–15225. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2078-13.2013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Nguyen PV, Abel T, Kandel ER, Bourtchouladze R. Strain-dependent differences in LTP and hippocampus-dependent memory in inbred mice. Learn. Mem. 2000;7(3):170–179. doi: 10.1101/lm.7.3.170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Mang CS, Campbell KL, Ross CJD, Boyd LA. Promoting neuroplasticity for motor rehabilitation after stroke: Considering the effects of aerobic exercise and genetic variation on brain-derived neurotrophic factor. Phys. Ther. 2013;93(12):1707–1716. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20130053. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Stewart JC, Cramer SC. Genetic variation and neuroplasticity: Role in rehabilitation after stroke. J. Neurol. Phys. Ther. 2017;41:S17–S23. doi: 10.1097/NPT.0000000000000180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Agartz I, Sääf J, Wahlund L-O, Wetterberg L. Quantitative estimations of cerebrospinal fluid spaces and brain regions in healthy controls using computer-assisted tissue classification of magnetic resonance images: Relation to age and sex. Magn. Reson. Imaging. 1992;10(2):217–226. doi: 10.1016/0730-725X(92)90482-F. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Coffey CE, et al. Sex differences in brain aging: A quantitative magnetic resonance imaging study. Arch. Neurol. 1998;55(2):169–179. doi: 10.1001/archneur.55.2.169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Allen JS, Damasio H, Grabowski TJ. Normal neuroanatomical variation in the human brain: An MRI-volumetric study. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 2002;118(4):341–358. doi: 10.1002/ajpa.10092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Luders E, Kurth F. Structural differences between male and female brains. In: Lanzenberger R, Kranz GS, Savic I, editors. Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Elsevier; 2020. pp. 3–11. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Toschi N, Gisbert RA, Passamonti L, Canals S, De Santis S. Multishell diffusion imaging reveals sex-specific trajectories of early white matter degeneration in normal aging. Neurobiol. Aging. 2020;86:191–200. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2019.11.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Shin YW, et al. Sex differences in the human corpus callosum: diffusion tensor imaging study. NeuroReport. 2005;16(8):795. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200505310-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Westerhausen R, Walter C, Kreuder F, Wittling RA, Schweiger E, Wittling W. The influence of handedness and gender on the microstructure of the human corpus callosum: A diffusion-tensor magnetic resonance imaging study. Neurosci. Lett. 2003;351(2):99–102. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2003.07.011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Schmithorst VJ, Holland SK, Dardzinski BJ. Developmental differences in white matter architecture between boys and girls. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2008;29(6):696–710. doi: 10.1002/hbm.20431. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Liu F, Vidarsson L, Winter JD, Tran H, Kassner A. Sex differences in the human corpus callosum microstructure: A combined T2 myelin-water and diffusion tensor magnetic resonance imaging study. Brain Res. 2010;1343:37–45. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.04.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Björnholm L, et al. Structural properties of the human corpus callosum: Multimodal assessment and sex differences. NeuroImage. 2017;152:108–118. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.02.056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Dougherty RF, Ben-Shachar M, Deutsch GK, Hernandez A, Fox GR, Wandell BA. Temporal-callosal pathway diffusivity predicts phonological skills in children. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 2007;104(20):8556–8561. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0608961104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Zarei M, Johansen-Berg H, Smith S, Ciccarelli O, Thompson AJ, Matthews PM. Functional anatomy of interhemispheric cortical connections in the human brain. J. Anat. 2006;209(3):311–320. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7580.2006.00615.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Hasan KM, Ewing-Cobbs L, Kramer LA, Fletcher JM, Narayana PA. Diffusion tensor quantification of the macrostructure and microstructure of human midsagittal corpus callosum across the lifespan. NMR Biomed. 2008;21(10):1094–1101. doi: 10.1002/nbm.1286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Schmithorst VJ. Developmental sex differences in the relation of neuroanatomical connectivity to intelligence. Intelligence. 2009;37(2):164–173. doi: 10.1016/j.intell.2008.07.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Weber B, et al. Attention and interhemispheric transfer: A behavioral and fMRI study. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 2005;17(1):113–123. doi: 10.1162/0898929052880002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Tettamanti M, et al. Interhemispheric transmission of visuomotor information in humans: fMRI evidence. J. Neurophysiol. 2002;88(2):1051–1058. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.2.1051. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Fabri M, Pierpaoli C, Barbaresi P, Polonara G. Functional topography of the corpus callosum investigated by DTI and fMRI. World J. Radiol. 2014;6(12):895–906. doi: 10.4329/wjr.v6.i12.895. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Vien C, et al. Age-related white-matter correlates of motor sequence learning and consolidation. Neurobiol. Aging. 2016;48:13–22. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.08.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Azzarito M, et al. Coherent waves of myelin plasticity during motor-skill learning. BioRxiv. 2021 doi: 10.1101/2021.04.13.439402. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Moore E, Schaefer RS, Bastin ME, Roberts N, Overy K. Diffusion tensor MRI tractography reveals increased fractional anisotropy (FA) in arcuate fasciculus following music-cued motor training. Brain Cogn. 2017;116:40–46. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2017.05.001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Taubert M, et al. Dynamic properties of human brain structure: Learning-related changes in cortical areas and associated fiber connections. J. Neurosci. 2010;30(35):11670–11677. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2567-10.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Rubino C, et al. Experience-dependent learning and myelin plasticity in individuals with stroke. Neuroscience. 2022 doi: 10.1101/2022.02.17.480894. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Borich MR, Brown KE, Boyd LA. Motor skill learning is associated with diffusion characteristics of white matter in individuals with chronic stroke. J. Neurol. Phys. Ther. 2014;38(3):151–160. doi: 10.1097/NPT.0b013e3182a3d353. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Yeatman JD, Dougherty RF, Myall NJ, Wandell BA, Feldman HM. Tract profiles of white matter properties: Automating fiber-tract quantification. PLOS ONE. 2012;7(11):e49790. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0049790. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Dayan M, et al. Profilometry: A new statistical framework for the characterization of white matter pathways, with application to multiple sclerosis. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2015;37(3):989–1004. doi: 10.1002/hbm.23082. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Arshad M, Stanley JA, Raz N. Adult age differences in subcortical myelin content are consistent with protracted myelination and unrelated to diffusion tensor imaging indices. NeuroImage. 2016;143:26–39. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2016.08.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Davis SW, Dennis NA, Buchler NG, White LE, Madden DJ, Cabeza R. Assessing the effects of age on long white matter tracts using diffusion tensor tractography. NeuroImage. 2009;46(2):530–541. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.01.068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Hugdahl K, Thomsen T, Ersland L. Sex differences in visuo-spatial processing: An fMRI study of mental rotation. Neuropsychologia. 2006;44(9):1575–1583. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.01.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Goldstein JM, et al. Sex differences in prefrontal cortical brain activity during fMRI of auditory verbal working memory. Neuropsychology. 2005;19(4):509–519. doi: 10.1037/0894-4105.19.4.509. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Kuptsova SV, Ivanova MV, Petrushevsky AG, Fedina ON, Zhavoronkova LA. Sex-related differences in task switching: An fMRI study. Hum. Physiol. 2015;41(6):611–624. doi: 10.1134/S0362119715050084. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Lissek S, et al. Sex differences in cortical and subcortical recruitment during simple and complex motor control: An fMRI study. NeuroImage. 2007;37(3):912–926. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2007.05.037. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Gorbet DJ, Sergio LE. Preliminary sex differences in human cortical BOLD fMRI activity during the preparation of increasingly complex visually guided movements. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2007;25(4):1228–1239. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05358.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Andrushko JW, Rinat S, Kirby ED, Dahlby J, Ekstrand C, Boyd LA. Females exhibit smaller volumes of brain activation and lower inter-subject variability during motor tasks. Scientific Reports. 2023;13(1):17698. doi: 10.1038/s41598-023-44871-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Wierenga LM, et al. Greater male than female variability in regional brain structure across the lifespan. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2020;43(1):470–499. doi: 10.1002/hbm.25204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Ritchie SJ, et al. Sex differences in the adult human brain: Evidence from 5216 UK biobank participants. Cereb. Cortex. 2018;28(8):2959–2975. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhy109. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Highley JR, Esiri MM, McDonald B, Cortina-Borja M, Herron BM, Crow TJ. The size and fibre composition of the corpus callosum with respect to gender and schizophrenia: A post-mortem study. Brain. 1999;122(1):99–110. doi: 10.1093/brain/122.1.99. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.McDougall S, et al. Myelination of axons corresponds with faster transmission speed in the prefrontal cortex of developing male rats. ENeuro. 2018;5(4):0203. doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0203-18.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Barha CK, Liu-Ambrose T. Exercise and the aging brain: Considerations for sex differences. Brain Plast. 2018;4(1):53–63. doi: 10.3233/BPL-180067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Guadagnoli MA, Lee TD. Challenge point: A framework for conceptualizing the effects of various practice conditions in motor learning. J. Motor Behav. 2004;36(2):212–224. doi: 10.3200/JMBR.36.2.212-224. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Wulf G, Shea C, Lewthwaite R. Motor skill learning and performance: A review of influential factors. Med. Educ. 2010;44(1):75–84. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2923.2009.03421.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Jones DK, Knösche TR, Turner R. White matter integrity, fiber count, and other fallacies: The do’s and don’ts of diffusion MRI. NeuroImage. 2013;73:239–254. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Li X, et al. Microscopic fractional anisotropy detects cognitive training-induced microstructural brain changes. Tomography. 2022 doi: 10.3390/tomography8010004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Tannenbaum C, Ellis RP, Eyssel F, Zou J, Schiebinger L. Sex and gender analysis improves science and engineering. Nature. 2019;575:7781. doi: 10.1038/s41586-019-1657-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.White J, Tannenbaum C, Klinge I, Schiebinger L, Clayton J. The Integration of sex and gender considerations into biomedical research: Lessons from international funding agencies. J. Clin. Endocrinol. Metab. 2021;106(10):3034–3048. doi: 10.1210/clinem/dgab434. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Geller SE, Koch AR, Roesch P, Filut A, Hallgren E, Carnes M. The more things change, the more they stay the same: A study to evaluate compliance with inclusion and assessment of women and minorities in randomized controlled trials. Acad. Med. 2018;93(4):630–635. doi: 10.1097/ACM.0000000000002027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Rechlin RK, Splinter TFL, Hodges TE, Albert AY, Galea LAM. An analysis of neuroscience and psychiatry papers published from 2009 and 2019 outlines opportunities for increasing discovery of sex differences. Nat. Commun. 2022 doi: 10.1038/s41467-022-29903-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.C. I. of H. R. Government of Canada. Sex and Gender in Health Research: CIHR. https://cihr-irsc.gc.ca/e/50833.html. Accessed 01 Nov 2023.
- 88.Arnegard ME, Whitten LA, Hunter C, Clayton JA. Sex as a biological variable: A 5-year progress report and call to action. J. Women’s Health. 2020;29(6):858–864. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2019.8247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Kopec AM, Smith CJ, Ayre NR, Sweat SC, Bilbo SD. Microglial dopamine receptor elimination defines sex-specific nucleus accumbens development and social behavior in adolescent rats. Nat. Commun. 2018 doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-06118-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Rogojin A, Gorbet DJ, Sergio LE. Sex differences in the neural underpinnings of unimanual and bimanual control in adults. Exp. Brain Res. 2023;241(3):793–806. doi: 10.1007/s00221-023-06561-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Kraeutner SN, et al. Resting state connectivity is modulated by motor learning in individuals after stroke. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair. 2021;35(6):513–524. doi: 10.1177/15459683211006713. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Lohse KR, Boyd LA, Hodges NJ. Engaging environments enhance motor skill learning in a computer gaming task. J. Motor Behav. 2016;48(2):172–182. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2015.1068158. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.IBM SPSS Statistics for Macintosh. (IBM Corp., 2021).
- 94.Brown MB, Forsythe AB. Robust tests for the equality of variances. J. Am. Stat. Assoc. 1974;69(346):364–367. doi: 10.2307/2285659. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Olkin I. Contributions to Probability and Statistics: Essays in Honor of Harold Hotelling. Stanford University Press; 1960. [Google Scholar]
- 96.Prasloski T, et al. Rapid whole cerebrum myelin water imaging using a 3D GRASE sequence. NeuroImage. 2012;63(1):533–539. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2012.06.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Jenkinson M, Bannister P, Brady M, Smith S. Improved optimization for the robust and accurate linear registration and motion correction of brain images. Neuroimage. 2002;17(2):825–841. doi: 10.1016/s1053-8119(02)91132-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Jenkinson M, Smith S. A global optimisation method for robust affine registration of brain images. Med. Image Anal. 2001;5(2):143–156. doi: 10.1016/s1361-8415(01)00036-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Doucette J, Kames C, Rauscher A. DECAES: Decomposition and component analysis of exponential signals. Z. Med. Phys. 2020;30(4):271–278. doi: 10.1016/j.zemedi.2020.04.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Birkl C, Doucette J, Fan M, Hernández-Torres E, Rauscher A. Myelin water imaging depends on white matter fiber orientation in the human brain. Magn. Reson. Med. 2021;85(4):2221–2231. doi: 10.1002/mrm.28543. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.Andersson JLR, Sotiropoulos SN. An integrated approach to correction for off-resonance effects and subject movement in diffusion MR imaging. Neuroimage. 2016;125:1063–1078. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.10.019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.Garyfallidis E, et al. Dipy, a library for the analysis of diffusion MRI data. Front. Neuroinform. 2014;8:8. doi: 10.3389/fninf.2014.00008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Chandio BQ, et al. Bundle analytics, a computational framework for investigating the shapes and profiles of brain pathways across populations. Sci. Rep. 2020;10:17149. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-74054-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Garyfallidis, E. Towards an accurate brain tractography. PhD thesis, University of Cambridge (2012).
- 105.Aganj I, Lenglet C, Sapiro G, Yacoub E, Ugurbil K, Harel N. Reconstruction of the orientation distribution function in single and multiple shell Q-ball imaging within constant solid angle. Magn. Reson. Med. 2010;64(2):554–566. doi: 10.1002/mrm.22365. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Garyfallidis E, et al. Recognition of white matter bundles using local and global streamline-based registration and clustering. Neuroimage. 2018;170:283–295. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.07.015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Yeh F-C, et al. Population-averaged atlas of the macroscale human structural connectome and its network topology. Neuroimage. 2018;178:57–68. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.05.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Nichols TE, Holmes AP. Nonparametric permutation tests for functional neuroimaging: A primer with examples. Hum. Brain Mapp. 2002;15(1):1–25. doi: 10.1002/hbm.1058. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Fields EC, Kuperberg GR. Having your cake and eating it too: Flexibility and power with mass univariate statistics for ERP data. Psychophysiology. 2020;57(2):13468. doi: 10.1111/psyp.13468. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
The raw data used in the current study was collected with informed consent of the participants, who agreed to data use outlined by the research ethics boards of the University of British Columbia. Sharing of this data, supporting the conclusions of this article, will be made available by the authors, without undue reservation, to any qualified researcher. However, it is not currently available in any publicly accessible source. Any interest in receiving the data or post-processing code should be expressed to the corresponding author.