Skip to main content
Brain Communications logoLink to Brain Communications
. 2024 Feb 26;6(2):fcae043. doi: 10.1093/braincomms/fcae043

Predicting functional decline in aging and Alzheimer’s disease with PET-based Braak staging

Arthur C Macedo 1,2,3, Joseph Therriault 4,5,6, Cécile Tissot 7,8,9, Jaime Fernandez-Arias 10,11,12, Pamela C L Ferreira 13, Paolo Vitali 14,15,16, Stijn Servaes 17,18,19, Nesrine Rahmouni 20,21,22, Marie Vermeiren 23, Gleb Bezgin 24,25,26, Firoza Z Lussier 27,28, Jenna Stevenson 29,30,31, Yi-Ting Wang 32,33,34, Kely Quispialaya Socualaya 35,36,37, Peter Kunach 38,39,40, Tahnia Nazneen 41,42,43, Seyyed Ali Hosseini 44,45,46, Vanessa Pallen 47, Alyssa Stevenson 48, João Pedro Ferrari-Souza 49, Bruna Bellaver 50, Douglas Teixeira Leffa 51, Kok Pin Ng 52, Eduardo R Zimmer 53,54,55, Tharick A Pascoal 56,57, Serge Gauthier 58,59, Pedro Rosa-Neto 60,61,62,
PMCID: PMC10935644  PMID: 38482373

Abstract

The progression of PET-based Braak stages correlates with cognitive deterioration in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Here, we investigate the association between PET-based Braak stages and functional impairment and assess whether PET-based Braak staging predicts a longitudinal decline in the performance of activities of daily living. In this cohort study, we evaluated cognitively unimpaired individuals and individuals with mild cognitive impairment or Alzheimer’s disease dementia. Participants underwent [18F]MK6240 tau-PET, were assigned a PET-based Braak stage at baseline and were followed for a mean (SD) of 1.97 (0.66) years. Functional performance was evaluated with the Functional Activities Questionnaire, Everyday Cognition and functional Clinical Dementia Rating sum of boxes. Multiple linear regressions assessed the association of PET-based Braak stages with baseline functionality and with the longitudinal rate of change in functional scores, adjusting for age, sex and amyloid-β load. We employed voxel-based regression models to investigate the association between functionality and tau-PET signal and assessed the voxel overlap with Braak regions of interest. We included 291 individuals (181 cognitively unimpaired, 56 amyloid-β+ mild cognitive impairment and 54 amyloid-β+ Alzheimer’s disease) aged 70.60 (7.48) years. At baseline, PET-based Braak stages III–IV (β = 0.43, P = 0.03) and V–VI (β = 1.20, P < 0.0001) showed associations with poorer Functional Activities Questionnaire scores. Similarly, stages III–IV (β = 0.43, P = 0.02) and V–VI (β = 1.15, P < 0.0001) were associated with worse Everyday Cognition scores. Only stages V–VI were associated with higher functional Clinical Dementia Rating sum of boxes (β = 1.17, P < 0.0001) scores. Increased tau-PET signals in all Braak regions of interest were linked to worse performance in all tools. The voxelwise analysis showed widespread cortical associations between functional impairment and tau-PET and high voxel overlap with Braak regions of interest. Baseline PET-based Braak stages V–VI predicted significant longitudinal functional decline as assessed by the Functional Activities Questionnaire (β = 1.69, P < 0.0001), the Everyday Cognition (β = 1.05, P = 0.001) and the functional Clinical Dementia Rating sum of boxes (β = 1.29, P < 0.0001). Our results suggest that functional impairment increases with the severity of tau accumulation. These findings also indicate that PET-based Braak staging is a good predictor of functional impairment in the Alzheimer’s disease continuum. Finally, our study provides evidence for the clinical significance of the PET-based Braak staging framework.

Keywords: Braak stages, Alzheimer’s disease, PET, neurofibrillary tangles, activities of daily living


Macedo et al. report that difficulties in the performance of activities of daily living increase with the severity of tau accumulation. Their findings also suggest that Braak staging assigned with PET may be useful to predict functional impairment and to select participants for clinical trials targeting Alzheimer’s disease.

Graphical Abstract

Graphical Abstract.

Graphical Abstract

Introduction

Alzheimer’s disease is characterized by abnormal accumulation of amyloid-β (Aβ) and neurofibrillary tangles (NFTs) in the brain, which begin decades before symptom onset.1 Based on the NFT accumulation pattern, Braak and Braak2 proposed a histopathological classification for Alzheimer’s disease comprising six successive stages ranging from transentorhinal cortex involvement (stage I) to degeneration of primary sensory cortices (stage VI). The Braak staging scheme accurately indicates the severity of NFT spreading in the brain, having been included in the neuropathological diagnostic criteria for Alzheimer’s disease.3 However, employing the Braak staging scheme in living humans is impossible because it relies on post-mortem observations.

PET imaging of tau NFT is one of the biomarkers representing the ‘T’ category of the AT(N) system proposed by the National Institute on Aging and Alzheimer’s Association (NIA-AA) research criteria for Alzheimer’s disease.4 Given that tau-PET measures the regional NFT deposition in the brain, it allows for the stratification of individuals in the Alzheimer’s disease continuum based on in vivo tau Braak staging.5-10 While the progression of PET-based Braak stages was shown to correlate with clinical deterioration in the Alzheimer’s disease continuum, most studies have focused on cognitive and global disease severity measures and not on the level of independence in activities of daily living (ADL).7-11

An impaired performance on ADL is one of the required NIA-AA clinical diagnostic criteria for probable Alzheimer’s disease dementia.12 Even though mildly impaired complex instrumental ADL (IADL) may also be observed in mild cognitive impairment (MCI) patients,13 functional decline in ADL is a critical clinical feature to stratify individuals in the Alzheimer’s disease continuum and assess disease severity. Based on their complexity, ADL may be divided into three categories: (i) basic ADL (BADL; self-care tasks such as bathing and feeding); (ii) IADL (activities to maintain an independent household such as doing housework, taking medications and financial activities); and (iii) advanced ADL (AADL; related to the performance of societal, community and family roles).14 Impairments in more complex tasks (IADL and AADL) tend to precede those in BADL with the progression of cognitive deterioration.15-17

The relationship between functional autonomy in ADL and PET-based Braak staging is yet to be clarified. In the present study, we aim to investigate the association between PET-based Braak stages and functional impairment as assessed by tools used in clinical practice to diagnose dementia. We also assessed whether PET-based Braak staging predicts a longitudinal decline in the performance of ADL. Based on the assumption that functional decline is due to cognitive impairment, as stated in the dementia diagnostic criteria,12 we hypothesize that functional impairment will be associated with middle to late PET-based Braak stages (III–VI). As a secondary aim, we investigated the impact of these associations to the population recruitment of randomized controlled trials (RCTs) using measures of functionality as outcomes.

Materials and methods

Participants

In this prospective longitudinal study, we evaluated cognitively unimpaired (CU) individuals and individuals diagnosed with either MCI or Alzheimer’s disease dementia from the Translational Biomarkers in Aging and Dementia (TRIAD) cohort.18 CU individuals were recruited from the community, while individuals with MCI or Alzheimer’s disease dementia were recruited from the community or the McGill Research Centre of Studies on Aging outpatient memory clinic, in Montreal, Quebec, Canada. Recruitment took place from July 2016 to December 2021 through referrals, advertisements and word of mouth. CU participants were those with no objective cognitive impairment. Following the assessment of a multidisciplinary team composed of neurologists, neuropsychologists and nurses, participants with MCI and participants with Alzheimer’s disease dementia met, respectively, the NIA-AA criteria for MCI due to Alzheimer’s disease19 and for probable Alzheimer’s disease dementia.12 Of note, the Alzheimer’s disease dementia group included individuals with both the amnestic and non-amnestic variants—behavioural/dysexecutive Alzheimer’s disease,20 logopenic primary progressive aphasia21 and posterior cortical atrophy.22 The Aβ status of participants was assessed at baseline using [18F]AZD4694 Aβ PET, and, similar to previous research,23 only Aβ+ MCI and Aβ+ Alzheimer’s disease individuals were included since our aim was to assess functional performance in the aging and Alzheimer’s disease continuum. Participants also underwent [18F]MK6240 tau-PET and brain MRI at baseline. Potential participants were excluded if they presented visual and auditory impairments that hampered neuropsychological evaluation, inability to speak French or English, recent traumatic brain injury or major surgery, MRI/PET contraindications or inadequately treated neurological, psychiatric or systemic disorders. In case a reliable informant (e.g. a family member or a close friend) was unavailable, potential participants were also excluded. A subset of participants returned for a follow-up visit, in which another clinical assessment was performed. This study received approval from the Montreal Neurological Institute PET working committee and the Douglas Mental Health University Institute Research Ethics Board (IUSMD 16-60). Participants signed informed consent forms after being explained about all research procedures.

Neuroimaging acquisition and processing

A 3 T Siemens Magnetom scanner using a standard head coil was used to acquire structural brain MRI, while a brain-dedicated Siemens high-resolution research tomograph was employed to acquire [18F]AZD4694 and [18F]MK6240 PET scans. The injected radiation activity per participant per PET scan ranged from 5 to 7 mCi. Tau-PET images were obtained after 90–110 min of the [18F]MK6240 intravenous bolus injection and reconstructed employing a sequential subset expectation–maximization algorithm on a 4D volume with four frames (4 × 300 s), as previously reported.24 The acquisition of Aβ PET images was done after 40–70 min of the [18F]AZD4694 intravenous injection; reconstruction was performed with a sequential subset expectation–maximization algorithm on a 4D volume with three frames (3 × 600 s).25 In order to correct the attenuation, a 6-min transmission scan with a rotating 137Cs point source was performed following each PET acquisition. Corrections for motion, dead time, decay and random and scattered coincidences were performed. Subsequently, PET images were linearly registered to T1-weighted MRI image space and then linearly and nonlinearly registered to the Montreal Neurological Institute reference space. Furthermore, PET images were smoothed spatially to provide a full width of 8 mm at a half-maximum resolution. Meninges were further stripped in native space from [18F]MK6240 PET images before transformations and blurring to prevent meningeal spill-over into adjacent brain areas.7 The standardized uptake value ratio (SUVR) in [18F]MK6240 PET was determined using the cerebellum crus I grey matter as the reference region.9,26 To calculate [18F]AZD4694 SUVRs, the whole cerebellum grey matter was used as the reference region, and the following regions were included in the global [18F]AZD4694 SUVR composite: precuneus, prefrontal, orbitofrontal, parietal, temporal and cingulate cortices.25 The threshold to consider [18F]AZD4694 SUVR as positive was established as an SUVR > 1.55, as previously described.25

PET-based Braak staging methods

PET-based Braak stages were defined as reported elsewhere.7 The Braak regions of interest (ROIs) were defined using the Desikan–Killiany–Tourville atlas.27 The transentorhinal cortex underwent segmentation as previously described.6,7,9

Each individual was assigned a PET-based Braak stage based on the latest stage in which tau-PET was found to be abnormal. The thresholds for this abnormality in Braak ROIs were calculated as 2.5 SD higher than the mean SUVR of CU aged <26 years, as reported elsewhere.6,7 Aiming to increase the sample size in each Braak staging group, participants were grouped according to the simplified Braak staging system into four different PET-based Braak staging groups: 0, I–II, III–IV and V–VI.2

Clinical and neuropsychological assessments

All participants underwent a detailed clinical and neuropsychological evaluation. Functional impairment was evaluated with the Functional Activities Questionnaire (FAQ), the Everyday Cognition (ECog) and the Clinical Dementia Rating sum of boxes (CDR-SB) functional domains.

The FAQ is a 10-item scale in which an informant rates the participant’s ability on 10 IADLs.28 Each item is scored from 0 (normal) to 3 (dependent), with the total score ranging from 0 to 30 and higher scores reflecting poorer functioning.28

The ECog measures subtle and mild functional changes in older adults that are relevant to certain cognitive domains: memory, language, visual–spatial and perceptual abilities, and executive functioning (planning, organization and divided attention).29 In our study, this questionnaire was answered by an informant who rated the current ability of the participant to accomplish specific tasks as compared to their performance 10 years ago. The responses to the 39 questions range from 1 (better or no change) to 4 (consistently much worse), with the final score being the average of all answers. A higher average score means greater impairment.29

The CDR-SB assesses the impact of cognitive deficits on the performance of everyday activities and was developed as a tool to stage dementia severity.30 It consists of a semi-structured interview with the participant and an informant, who is asked to describe the participant’s degree of impairment. The scale includes three cognitive (‘memory’, ‘orientation’, and ‘judgment and problem solving’) and three functional domains (‘community affairs’, ‘home and hobbies’, and ‘personal care’), whose scores vary from 0 (healthy) to 3 (severe dementia).30 Combined, the scores of the functional domains result in the ‘functional CDR-SB’ (CDR-SB-F), with scores ranging from 0 to 9.

We selected these tools, which are commonly used in research and clinical practice, to compare their relationship with PET-based Braak staging. Considering that the FAQ and the ECog encompass questions regarding more complex ADL, we expected associations with earlier PET-based Braak stages than the CDR-SB-F, which also evaluates the performance in BADL.

Statistical analysis

Statistical analyses were conducted on R software version 3.5.3. Demographic data were compared using the Kruskal–Wallis for continuous variables [age, years of education, Mini-Mental State Examination (MMSE) and SUVRs] and χ2 test for categorical variables (sex and APOE ε4 status). In addition, we compared the functional scores of participants at different PET-based Braak stages using one-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) and performed post hoc analyses with Tukey’s honest significance test. Multiple linear regression (MLR) was used to assess the association of PET-based Braak stages and Braak ROI SUVR with functional measures at baseline. We also employed voxel-based regression models to investigate the association between functional scores and tau-PET signal at baseline, adjusting for age, sex and total Aꞵ PET load. The voxel-based analyses were conducted using VoxelStats,31 a MATLAB package, and corrected for multiple comparisons with the random field theory method employing a threshold of P < 0.001. Subsequently, we calculated the voxel overlap between the t-maps generated from the voxelwise analysis and the masks for each Braak ROI. Furthermore, we employed MLR to investigate whether baseline PET-based Braak stages predicted the annual change in functional scores, as most participants had a single follow-up timepoint. Briefly, the annual change was calculated as the difference between the follow-up and baseline scores divided by the time (in years) between the visits. For the annual change, we considered data points below 2.5 SD than the mean of the whole sample to be outliers. These outliers were excluded from any analyses due to the lack of biological and clinical plausibility. All MLR analyses were adjusted for age, sex and neocortical [18F]AZD4694 Aꞵ PET SUVR. We also ran exploratory models adjusted for clinical diagnosis to understand its contribution to the relationship between Braak staging and ADL performance. All continuous variables added to the MLR models were Z-scored based on the whole sample. Statistical significance was considered if P < 0.05. Finally, we calculated sample size required in a hypothetical RCT to observe 25% reduction in functional decline in the treatment group, with a power of 80%, using a well-validated formula.32

Results

A PET-based Braak stage was assigned to 291 individuals (112 at stage 0, 78 at I/II, 35 at III/IV and 66 at V/VI), who had a mean age of 70.60 (7.48) and were 62.8% (n = 183) females (Table 1). In total, 181 participants were classified as CU, 56 as Aβ+ MCI and 54 as Aβ+ Alzheimer’s disease. Significant differences between groups were observed for age, clinical diagnosis, MMSE scores, APOE ε4 status and [18F]AZD4694 PET SUVR.

Table 1.

Demographic, clinical and biomarker characteristics of the study cohort at baseline

Braak 0 (N = 73) Braak I-II (N = 58) Braak III-IV (N = 21) Braak V-VI (N = 36) P-value
Age (years), mean (SD) 69.93 (7.6) 72.23 (6.8) 72.54 (5.0) 68.77 (8.6) 0.01
Female, n (%) 68 (60.7%) 55 (70.5%) 22 (62.9%) 38 (57.6%) 0.40
Years of education, mean (SD) 15.64 (3.7) 15.22 (3.8) 15.94 (3.5) 14.65 (3.9) 0.37
Clinical diagnosis, n (%) 108 CU (96.4%); 4 MCI (3.6%) 56 CU (71.8%); 19 MCI (24.4%); 3 Alzheimer’s disease (3.8%) 17 CU (48.6%); 11 MCI (31.4%); 7 Alzheimer’s disease (20.0%) 22 MCI (33.3%); 44 Alzheimer’s disease (66.7%) <0.001
MMSE, mean (SD)a 29.07 (1.1) 28.76 (1.7) 28.03 (3.1) 22.00 (6.2) <0.001
APOE ε4 carriers, n (%)b 24 (21.4%) 29 (37.2%) 18 (51.4%) 35 (53.0%) <0.001
Neocortical [18F]AZD4694 SUVR, mean (SD)c 1.38 (0.3) 1.75 (0.5) 2.01 (0.6) 2.64 (0.5) <0.001

P-values were calculated using the Kruskal–Wallis test for age, years of education, MMSE and SUVRs. χ2 test was performed for sex proportion and APOE ε4 status comparison. Statistical significance was considered if P < 0.05 (results in bold). CU, cognitively unimpaired; MCI, mild cognitive impairment; MMSE, Mini-Mental State Examination; ROI, region of interest; SD, standard deviation; SUVR, standardized uptake value ratio. a Two missing data. b Twenty missing data. c Three missing data.

A total of 188 participants returned for a follow-up visit after a mean (SD) of 1.97 (0.66) years. The minimum and maximum follow-up times were 0.8 and 3.6 years, respectively. No significant difference was observed in the groups’ follow-up time. A complete description of their demographic, clinical and biomarkers characteristics is displayed in Supplementary Table 1.

For all tools, ANOVA analyses indicated significant differences in the scores of participants at different Braak stages, both regarding baseline measures and rates of change (P < 0.0001 for all comparisons). Significantly lower baseline functional scores, as compared to stage 0, started to be observed at stages III–IV for the FAQ and the ECog and at stages V–VI for the CDR-SB-F (Fig. 1A–C). In turn, a significantly higher annual decline in functionality was seen at stages V–VI for all tools (Fig. 2A–C). Importantly, three outliers for the annual rate of change variables were identified and excluded.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Progression of functional decline across PET-based Braak stages. The graphs depict the median baseline scores obtained by participants at different PET-based Braak stages in the FAQ (A), ECog (B), and CDR-SB-F (C). Comparisons were performed with one-way ANOVA and post hoc analyses with Tukey’s honest significance test. CDR-SB-F, functional Clinical Dementia Rating sum of boxes; ECog: Everyday Cognition; FAQ: Functional Activities Questionnaire. *P < 0.05. **P < 0.01. ***P < 0.001.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Annual change in the functional scores according to baseline PET-based Braak stage. The graphs show the median annual change in the functional scores (A, FAQ; B, ECog; and C, CDR-SB-F) per PET-based Braak stage. Comparisons were performed with one-way ANOVA and post hoc analyses with Tukey’s honest significance test. CDR-SB-F, functional Clinical Dementia Rating sum of boxes; ECog, Everyday Cognition; FAQ: Functional Activities Questionnaire. ***<0.001.

In the cross-sectional analysis (Table 2), PET-based Braak stages III–IV [β = 0.43, 95% confidence interval (CI) 0.05–0.81, P = 0.03] and V–VI (β = 1.20, 95% CI 0.75–1.65, P < 0.0001) were associated with worse FAQ scores. Similarly, a significant association was also found between stages III–IV (β = 0.43, 95% CI 0.07–0.79, P = 0.02) and V–VI (β = 1.15, 95% CI 0.73–1.58, P < 0.0001) and a poorer performance in the ECog. Only stages V–VI, however, were associated with higher scores in the CDR-SB-F (β = 1.17, 95% CI 0.80–1.54, P < 0.0001).

Table 2.

Regression coefficients of the cross-sectional association between PET-based Braak stages and functional scores at baseline

FAQ ECog CDR-SB-F
Beta (95% CI) T-value P-value Beta (95% CI) T-value P-value Beta (95% CI) T-value P-value
PET-based Braak stage I–II 0.02 (−0.27 to 0.31) 0.15 0.88 0.08 (−0.19 to 0.36) 0.60 0.55 0.05 (−0.20 to 0.30) 0.39 0.70
PET-based Braak stage III–IV 0.43 (0.05–0.81) 2.24 0.03 0.43 (0.07–0.79) 2.37 0.02 0.15 (−0.18 to 0.49) 0.89 0.38
PET-based Braak stage V–VI 1.20 (0.75–1.65) 5.24 <0.0001 1.15 (0.73–1.58) 5.36 <0.0001 1.17 (0.80–1.54) 6.18 <0.0001
Age (years) −0.12 (−0.25 to 0.003) −1.93 0.06 −0.13 (−0.25 to −0.01) −2.19 0.03 −0.09 (−0.19 to 0.008) −1.80 0.07
Sex (male) 0.08 (−0.14 to 0.31) 0.73 0.47 0.13 (−0.09 to 0.34) 1.18 0.24 0.07 (−0.13 to 0.27) 0.71 0.48
Neocortical [18F]AZD4694 SUVR 0.18 (0.007–0.36) 2.05 0.04 0.28 (0.12–0.45) 3.36 <0.001 0.16 (0.02–0.30) 2.26 0.02

Adjusted R2: 0.33, F-stat: 19.39 (FAQ); adjusted R2: 0.41, F-stat: 26.01 (ECog); adjusted R2: 0.35, F-stat: 26.83 (CDR-SB-F). Statistical significance was considered if P < 0.05 (results in bold). CDR-SB-F, functional Clinical Dementia Rating sum of boxes; ECog, Everyday Cognition; FAQ, Functional Activities Questionnaire; SUVR, standardized uptake value ratio.

Increased SUVR in Braak I–II (β = 0.27, 95% CI 0.09–0.45, P = 0.004), III–IV (β = 0.62, 95% CI 0.43–0.80, P < 0.0001) and V–VI (β = 0.54, 95% CI 0.38–0.71, P < 0.0001) ROIs was significantly associated with worse performance in the FAQ (Fig. 3A). Significant associations were also observed between higher SUVR in Braak I–II (β = 0.33, 95% CI 0.80–1.54, P < 0.001), III–IV (β = 0.61, 95% CI 0.44–0.78, P < 0.0001) and V–VI (β = 0.51, 95% CI 0.36–0.67, P < 0.0001) and poorer ECog scores (Fig. 3B). Higher tau-PET SUVR in all Braak ROIs (I–II: β = 0.41, 95% CI 0.26–0.57, P < 0.0001; III–IV: β = 0.73, 95% CI 0.62–0.85, P < 0.0001; and V–VI: β = 0.69, 95% CI 0.58–0.80, P < 0.0001) was also associated with an increased impairment as assessed by the CDR-SB-F (Fig. 3C).

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Association between the SUVR in Braak ROIs and functional scores at baseline. The graphs depict the association between tau-PET SUVR in Braak regions and functional performance as assessed by the FAQ (A), the ECog (B), and the CDR-SB-F (C). Depicted β-coefficients and P-values were obtained in linear regression models adjusted for age, sex and neocortical [18F]AZD4694 Aꞵ PET SUVR. CDR-SB-F, functional Clinical Dementia Rating sum of boxes; ECog, Everyday Cognition; FAQ, Functional Activities Questionnaire; ROI, region of interest; SUVR, standardized uptake value ratio.

The voxelwise analysis showed the most notable associations between FAQ scores and tau-PET SUVR in the left precentral and lateral occipital gyri, right isthmus cingulate and bilaterally in the supramarginal, superior temporal, superior parietal, inferior parietal, lingual and fusiform gyri, as well as in the precuneus, cuneus and hippocampi (Fig. 4A). In turn, the highest T-values for the ECog were seen in the cuneus, precuneus, inferior parietal and isthmus of the cingulate, in the right hemisphere; in the precentral, superior parietal and lateral occipital gyri, in the left hemisphere; and in the entorhinal, hippocampal, parahippocampal, fusiform, lingual, superior temporal and inferior temporal cortices, bilaterally (Fig. 4C). For the CDR-SB-F, widespread strong clusters were observed across the whole cerebral cortex (Fig. 4C).

Figure 4.

Figure 4

Tau-PET correlates of functional impairment and their overlap with Braak ROIs. The images show the T-values resulting from voxel-based linear regressions assessing the associations between [18F]MK6240 SUVR and performance at the FAQ (A), ECog (B), and CDR-SB-F (C). Adjustment was performed for age, sex and total [18F]AZD4694 Aꞵ PET burden. The pie graphs represent the proportion of overlap between the generated t-maps and the masks representing each Braak ROI. CDR-SB, Clinical Dementia Rating sum of boxes; ECog, Everyday Cognition; FAQ, Functional Activities Questionnaire; ROI, region of interest; SUVR, standardized uptake value ratio.

For all functional outcomes, the voxelwise analysis showed a high topographic overlap between the significant voxels and the masks for Braak ROIs (Fig. 4A–C). The FAQ t-map had an overlap with the Braak I–II, III–IV and V–VI masks of 95%, 98.8% and 93.2%, respectively. The overlap of the ECog t-map was of 98.3% with Braak I–II, 98.3% with III–IV and 93.4% with V–VI. Finally, the CDR-SB-F t-map displayed the highest overlaps: 100% with Braak I–II and III–IV, and 99.9% with Braak V–VI ROIs.

In the longitudinal analysis (Table 3), baseline PET-based Braak stages V–VI predicted a significantly higher rate of increase in the FAQ (β = 1.69, 95% CI 1.05–2.32, P < 0.0001), the ECog (β = 1.05, 95% CI 0.42–1.69, P = 0.001) and the CDR-SB-F (β = 1.29, 95% CI 0.79–1.78, P < 0.0001). The same significant associations were found previously to the elimination of the three outliers (Supplementary Table 2).

Table 3.

Regression coefficients of the association between baseline PET-based Braak stages and the annual change in the functional scores

FAQ ECog CDR-SB-F
Beta (95% CI) T-value P-value Beta (95% CI) T-value P-value Beta (95% CI) T-value P-value
PET-based Braak stage I–II 0.01 (−0.34 to 0.36) 0.06 0.95 0.003 (−0.39 to 0.39) 0.01 0.99 −0.04 (−0.34 to 0.26) −0.26 0.79
PET-based Braak stage III–IV 0.02 (−0.47 to 0.51) 0.08 0.93 0.02 (−0.52 to 0.57) 0.08 0.93 −0.03 (−0.45 to 0.40) −0.12 0.91
PET-based Braak stage V–VI 1.69 (1.05–2.32) 5.29 <0.0001 1.05 (0.42–1.69) 3.29 0.001 1.29 (0.79–1.78) 5.17 <0.0001
Age (years) −0.05 (−0.25 to 0.15) −0.53 0.59 0.07 (−0.15 to 0.28) 0.59 0.55 −0.14 (−0.29 to 0.01) −1.80 0.07
Sex (male) 0.02 (−0.27 to 0.32) 0.17 0.87 0.15 (−0.18 to 0.49) 0.91 0.36 0.02 (−0.23 to 0.26) 0.12 0.90
Neocortical [18F]AZD4694 SUVR 0.21 (−0.04 to 0.45) 1.70 0.09 0.25 (0.006–0.50) 2.03 0.04 0.15 (−0.05 to 0.34) 1.50 0.14

Adjusted R2: 0.50, F-stat: 17.61 (FAQ); adjusted R2: 0.26, F-stat: 8.36 (ECog); adjusted R2: 0.36, F-stat: 18.15 (CDR-SB-F). Statistical significance was considered if P < 0.05 (results in bold). CDR-SB-F, functional Clinical Dementia Rating sum of boxes; ECog, Everyday Cognition; FAQ, Functional Activities Questionnaire; SUVR, standardized uptake value ratio.

In a hypothetical RCT, for a 25% reduction in functional decline to be observed, the required sample size per study arm would range from 78 to 288 individuals at Braak stages V–VI, depending on the questionnaire used (Table 4). In case individuals at earlier stages were to be recruited, for a similar performance of the drug, the sample size would have to be 2.13–50.65 times bigger.

Table 4.

The minimum sample size required per RCT arm to observe a considerable reduction in functional decline

FAQ ECog CDR-SB-F
Braak I–II 3951 6925 4020
Braak III–IV 1170 565 8204
Braak V–VI 78 265 288

The table contains the calculation of the minimum sample required for a 25% slowing in functional decline to be observed, considering the baseline PET-based Braak stage of participants. CDR-SB-F, functional Clinical Dementia Rating sum of boxes; ECog, Everyday Cognition; FAQ, Functional Activities Questionnaire; RCT, randomized controlled trial.

Exploratory analysis

We conducted exploratory analyses with models adjusting for clinical diagnosis. Statistical significance was lost for the associations between baseline functional scores and baseline PET-based Braak stages (Supplementary Table 3). On the other hand, statistical significance remained for the relationship between Braak III–IV SUVR and ECog scores (β = 0.18, 95% CI 0.01–0.35, P = 0.04) as well as between CDR-SB-F scores and SUVR in Braak I–II (β = 0.24, 95% CI 0.11–0.37, P < 0.001), III–IV (β = 0.45, 95% CI 0.32–0.58, P < 0.0001) and V–VI (β = 0.43, 95% CI 0.31–0.55, P < 0.0001). Baseline Braak V–VI remained associated with the annual rate of change in the FAQ (β = 1.28, 95% CI 0.63–1.93, P < 0.001) and in the CDR-SB-F (β = 0.65, 95% CI 0.13–1.17, P = 0.01) but not in the ECog (Supplementary Table 4).

Discussion

In the present study, we applied in vivo PET-based Braak staging to evaluate the association between cerebral NFT distribution and functional impairment. Our findings demonstrate that functional decline, a core criterion of dementia diagnosis,12 is associated with middle to late PET-based Braak stages. Additionally, we show that PET-based Braak staging can predict a longitudinal decline in the performance of ADL. Taken together, these results are in agreement with previous data suggesting that the progression of PET-based Braak stages has a good correspondence with clinical deterioration in the Alzheimer’s disease continuum.7-11 These findings also provide in vivo evidence that functional impairment increases with the severity of NFT accumulation.

Results of the FAQ are consistent with our hypothesis and with findings showing that IADL deficits are observed either in prodromal (MCI) or, most commonly, early dementia because they repose upon more complex cognitive functions, which are affected earlier in the disease.14 Similarly, the association of PET-based Braak stages III–IV and V–VI with functional impairment in the ECog might be explained by the focus of this scale on subtle cognitive changes, especially memory, which have been known to occur earlier in Alzheimer’s disease pathogenesis.33 Even though the CDR-SB-F includes domains evaluating tasks of higher complexity (i.e. ‘community affairs’ and ‘home and hobbies’), no association with middle PET-based Braak stages was observed. This is possibly explained by the fact that the CDR-SB-F includes a domain assessing BADL (i.e. ‘personal care’). Nonetheless, we found that decreased functionality, regardless of the tool used, is associated with higher tau-PET signal in all Braak ROIs. These findings were corroborated by voxelwise analyses showing several significant clusters in Braak-related areas.

Moreover, participants at Braak V/VI, but not at lower stages, had significantly higher rates of functional decline than controls. This indicates not only a potential prognostic value of PET-based Braak staging but also its possible utility when recruiting patients for clinical trials. Indeed, tau-PET staging has been employed to select patients for RCTs of disease-modifying therapies for Alzheimer’s disease. For instance, the phase II and III donanemab RCTs showed higher efficacy among Aβ+ individuals with lower tau burden as assessed by tau-PET.34,35 Here, we indicate that functionality might be a useful outcome in trials targeting participants at advanced but not early stages of tau accumulation. The inclusion of participants at lower Braak stages requires much larger sample sizes for considerable slowing in functional decline to be observed, leading to increased costs and logistic challenges.

The literature regarding the relationship between functional impairment and PET-based Braak stages is still scarce. A study using tau-PET [18F]AV1451 found an association between poorer CDR-SB scores and higher SUVR in ROIs corresponding to Braak stages I-IV in the MCI group.36 Among Alzheimer’s disease individuals, impairment on the CDR-SB correlated with higher SUVR in regions belonging to Braak stages III-VI.36 Nonetheless, no specific analyses were performed with the functional domains of the CDR-SB.36 These results, however, reinforce the correspondence between CDR-SB scores and PET-based Braak stages in the Alzheimer’s disease continuum. Our results are also in line with neuropathological findings suggesting that higher Braak stages correlate with worse antemortem performances in two functional measures: the FAQ37 and the Functional Assessment Staging.38 Furthermore, in a small study including 52 participants, Qiu et al.39 investigated the association of Braak staging with antemortem ECog average score and subscores. Diverging from our findings, this study found no significant correlation, which may be explained by the underpowered sample.39

Previous studies investigated tau-PET correlates of functional impairment and returned conflicting results. In individuals across the Alzheimer’s disease continuum, higher FAQ scores showed correlations with increased signal in the precuneus and in a global tau ROI, but only the latter survived correction for Aβ load.40 Higher FAQ scores also correlated with tau accumulation in the entorhinal and inferior temporal cortices in another sample of CU and CI participants, but no adjustment for Aβ load was performed.41 Moreover, informant-rated but not self-reported poorer performance in ADL was linked to signal in the entorhinal and inferior temporal cortices among CU participants, even though statistical significance was lost after correction for Aβ-PET burden.42 In a sample of CI individuals, functional impairment demonstrated associations with greater signal in several ROIs (anterior cingulate, bilateral dorsolateral prefrontal, entorhinal cortex, inferior temporal, lateral parietal, medial orbitofrontal and medial occipital regions), which did not survive adjustment for age, sex and global cognition.43

Discordance is also seen in longitudinal data. In 74 Aβ+ CI participants at early symptomatic stages, no associations were observed between [18F]PI2620 signal and baseline or longitudinal FAQ scores, in models with stepwise adjustment for age, sex and Aβ-PET.44 Meanwhile, baseline [18F]AV1451 signal in different ROIs (bilateral entorhinal cortex, inferior temporal, precuneus, posterior cingulate, supramarginal and the dorsolateral prefrontal) predicted increase in FAQ scores in a sample of 334 CU and 247 CI participants, even with adjustments for age, gender, the interaction of baseline age with time, cognitive measures and Aβ-PET.45 Here, we add to these findings by showing that a tau-PET staging approach might be useful to predict future functional decline in the Alzheimer’s disease continuum.

Unlike functional assessments, multiple studies investigated the relationship between cognitive performance and PET-based Braak stages.10,11 Notably, studies using [18F]MK6240 have shown positive correlations between PET-based Braak stages and cognitive impairment in the Alzheimer’s disease continuum. In a cross-sectional study using [18F]MK6240 PET, Pascoal et al.7 demonstrated that the ligand signal in Braak regions was negatively associated with MMSE scores, especially in later stages (V–VI). In an ordinal logistic regression analysis, [18F]MK6240 six-stage Braak model was highly correlated with CDR and MMSE scores.7 Similarly, another cross-sectional study using [18F]MK6240 PET found that higher ligand binding in all Braak stages correlated with poorer MMSE scores.8 In a study including 324 participants, Therriault et al.9 also suggested that [18F]MK6240 PET-based Braak stage is associated with cognitive impairment and dementia severity. Specifically, early PET-based Braak stages correlated with isolated memory deficits, while late stages correlated with dementia severity as measured by the CDR.9 Overall, cognitive symptoms appeared around PET-based Braak stages II–IV and had their progression associated with the advance of the stages.9 Our results regarding functional decline complement these findings and provide further evidence of the good clinical correspondence of the PET-based Braak staging framework with the Alzheimer’s disease continuum.

Interestingly, our findings demonstrated large variability in the functional scores of participants in the same PET-based Braak stage. Although cognitive decline is related to ADL impairment46 and associated with the progression of PET-based Braak stages,7-11 cognitive reserve may explain the variability of functional scores at the same Braak stage.47 Furthermore, even though individuals with ADL impairment present worse overall cognition,48 the ADL functional status in individuals with dementia varies significantly relative to the MMSE score.49 It should be noted that, besides cognitive impairment, other factors may also lead to functional decline in older adults, such as neuropsychiatric symptoms50 and other comorbidities.51

Some limitations of our study must be pointed out. Firstly, our population did not include patients with a CDR > 2 or living in nursing homes, whose functional impairment is expected to be greater, adding a selection bias to be considered. Moreover, our sample is mostly composed of White participants, limiting the generalization of our findings to other populations. Another external validity issue is the instruments we used to assess functionality were designed to target symptomatic populations rather than patients at preclinical stages. The absence of neuropathological confirmation at autopsy, the gold standard method to stage individuals according to the Braak framework, is another caveat. Even though the correspondence between [18F]MK6240 PET-based and autopsy-assigned Braak staging was shown by a recent study,6 there is still the need for studies including larger and more diverse samples to strengthen our confidence in these findings. In addition, case-to-autopsy studies must be performed to confirm that [18F]MK6240 PET can detect NFT in early Braak regions. Nonetheless, [18F]MK6240 uptake pattern was shown to reliably recapitulate late Braak stage deposition.7 PET imaging is also known for its limited spatial resolution, which makes it challenging to detect tau pathology in small areas of the medial temporal lobe. The use of functionality measures based on information obtained from a third party should also be highlighted, since it may lead to informant bias, which we tried to reduce by choosing reliable informants. Nonetheless, using informant-based questionnaires in cognitively impaired individuals is important as they may lack awareness regarding their condition.

In conclusion, our study demonstrates that functional impairment is associated with middle and late PET-based Braak stages and increases with the severity of NFT pathological changes. Moreover, we provide evidence that the PET-based Braak staging may be a good prognostic tool in patients in the Alzheimer’s disease continuum regarding their independence to perform ADL. Our results suggest a good clinical correspondence between the Alzheimer’s disease continuum and the PET-based Braak staging framework, which could be useful to define recruitment strategies for trials of Alzheimer’s disease disease-modifying treatments. Future research should assess the discriminatory and psychometric properties of each scale regarding PET-based Braak stages and compare the progression of other functional outcomes across PET-based Braak stages, including performance-based and self-reported measures.

Supplementary Material

fcae043_Supplementary_Data

Acknowledgements

We thank the study participants and the McGill Center for Studies in Aging staff. We also acknowledge the contributions of Alexey Kostikov, Dean Jolly, Karen Ross, Mehdi Boudjemeline, Monica Samoila-Lactatus and Sandy Li for the radiochemistry production and of Arturo Aliaga Carley Mayhew, Edith Strauss, Guylaine Gagne, Hung-Hsin Hsiao, Karen Wan, Meong Jin Joung, Miloudza Olmand, Reda Bouhachi, Richard Strauss, Rim Nazar, Sarah Sbeiti and Tasha Vinet-Celluci for data acquisition.

Contributor Information

Arthur C Macedo, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1A1, Canada; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada.

Joseph Therriault, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1A1, Canada; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada.

Cécile Tissot, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1A1, Canada; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada.

Jaime Fernandez-Arias, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1A1, Canada; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada.

Pamela C L Ferreira, Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, 15213, USA.

Paolo Vitali, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1A1, Canada; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada.

Stijn Servaes, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1A1, Canada; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada.

Nesrine Rahmouni, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1A1, Canada; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada.

Marie Vermeiren, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada.

Gleb Bezgin, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1A1, Canada; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada.

Firoza Z Lussier, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, 15213, USA.

Jenna Stevenson, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1A1, Canada; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada.

Yi-Ting Wang, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1A1, Canada; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada.

Kely Quispialaya Socualaya, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1A1, Canada; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada.

Peter Kunach, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1A1, Canada; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada.

Tahnia Nazneen, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1A1, Canada; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada.

Seyyed Ali Hosseini, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1A1, Canada; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada.

Vanessa Pallen, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada.

Alyssa Stevenson, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada.

João Pedro Ferrari-Souza, Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, 15213, USA.

Bruna Bellaver, Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, 15213, USA.

Douglas Teixeira Leffa, Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, 15213, USA.

Kok Pin Ng, Department of Neurology, National Neuroscience Institute, Singapore, 308433, Singapore.

Eduardo R Zimmer, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Pharmacology, Graduate Program in Biological Sciences: Pharmacology and Therapeutics, and Biochemistry, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, 90.035-003, Brazil; Brain Institute of Rio Grande do Sul, PUCRS, Porto Alegre, 90610-000, Brazil.

Tharick A Pascoal, Department of Psychiatry, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, 15213, USA; Department of Neurology, School of Medicine, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, PA, 15213, USA.

Serge Gauthier, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1A1, Canada.

Pedro Rosa-Neto, Translational Neuroimaging Laboratory, The McGill University Research Centre for Studies in Aging, Douglas Mental Health Institute, Montreal, Quebec, H4H 1R3, Canada; Department of Neurology and Neurosurgery, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 1A1, Canada; Montreal Neurological Institute, McGill University, Montreal, Quebec, H3A 2B4, Canada.

Supplementary material

Supplementary material is available at Brain Communications online.

Funding

This research is supported by the Weston Brain Institute, Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR; MOP-11-51-31; RFN 152985, 159815, 162303), Consortium canadien en neurodégénérescence associée au vieillissement/Canadian Consortium of Neurodegeneration and Aging (CCNV/CCNA; MOP-11-51-31 -team 1), the Alzheimer's Association [NIRG-12-92090, NIRP-12-259245], Brain Canada Foundation (CFI Project 34874; 33397), and the Fonds de Recherche du Québec - Santé (FRQS; Chercheur Boursier, 2020-VICO-279314). A.C.M. is supported by Mitacs (IT27627), the Faculty of Medicine, McGill University (F225864C02), McGill University (M159875C51), and the Canada First Research Excellence Fund and Fonds de recherche du Québec (I262757C0G). T.A.P., S.G., and P.R.-N. are members of the CIHR-CCNA Canadian Consortium of Neurodegeneration in Aging. Colin J. Adair Charitable Foundation. The funding sources had no participation in the design of the study, in the collection, analysis, and interpretation of data or in the manuscript writing.

Competing interests

S.G. has served as a scientific advisor to Cerveau Therapeutics. E.R.Z. serves on the scientific advisory board of Next Innovative Therapeutics. The other authors declare that they have no competing interests. Outside the work presented in this paper, P.R.N. provides consultancy services for Roche, Cerveau Radiopharmaceuticals, Lilly, Eisai, Pfizer, and Novo Nordisk. P.R.N. also serves as a clinical trials investigator for Biogen, Novo Nordisk, and Biogen.

Data availability

All requests for raw and analysed data and materials will be promptly reviewed by McGill University to verify if the request is subject to any intellectual property or confidentiality obligations. Anonymized data will be shared upon request to the study’s senior author from a qualified academic investigator for the sole purpose of replicating the procedures and results presented in this article. Any data and materials that can be shared will be released via a material transfer agreement. Data are not publicly available due to information that could compromise the privacy of research participants. Related documents, including study protocol and informed consent forms, can similarly be made available upon request.

References

  • 1. Jack  CR  Jr, Knopman  DS, Jagust  WJ, et al.  Tracking pathophysiological processes in Alzheimer’s disease: An updated hypothetical model of dynamic biomarkers. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12:207–216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Braak  H, Braak  E. Neuropathological stageing of Alzheimer-related changes. Acta Neuropathol. 1991;82(4):239–259. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Hyman  BT, Phelps  CH, Beach  TG, et al.  National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association guidelines for the neuropathologic assessment of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2012;8:1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Jack  CR  Jr, Bennett  DA, Blennow  K, et al.  NIA-AA Research Framework: Toward a biological definition of Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2018;14(4):535–562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Schöll  M, Lockhart  SN, Schonhaut  DR, et al.  PET imaging of tau deposition in the aging human brain. Neuron. 2016;89(5):971–982. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Pascoal  TA, Benedet  AL, Tudorascu  DL, et al.  Longitudinal 18F-MK-6240 tau tangles accumulation follows Braak stages. Brain. 2021;144(11):3517–3528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Pascoal  TA, Therriault  J, Benedet  AL, et al.  18F-MK-6240 PET for early and late detection of neurofibrillary tangles. Brain. 2020;143(9):2818–2830. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Kreisl  WC, Lao  PJ, Johnson  A, et al.  Patterns of tau pathology identified with 18F-MK-6240 PET imaging. Alzheimers Dement. 2021;18(2):272–282. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Therriault  J, Pascoal  TA, Lussier  FZ, et al.  Biomarker modeling of Alzheimer’s disease using PET-based Braak staging. Nat Aging. 2022;2:526–535. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Macedo  AC, Tissot  C, Therriault  J, et al.  The use of tau PET to stage Alzheimer disease according to the Braak staging framework. J Nucl Med. 2023;64(8):1171–1178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Macedo  AC, Durço  DFPA, Tissot  C, et al.  Clinical correlates of the PET-based Braak staging framework in Alzheimer’s disease. J Prev Alzheimers Dis. 2024;11:414–421. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. McKhann  GM, Knopman  DS, Chertkow  H, et al.  The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):263–569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Brown  PJ, Devanand  DP, Liu  X, Caccappolo  E. Functional impairment in elderly patients with mild cognitive impairment and mild Alzheimer disease. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2011;68(6):617–626. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Reuben  DB, Solomon  DH. Assessment in geriatrics. Of caveats and names. J Am Geriatr Soc. 1989;37(6):570–572. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Farias  ST, Park  LQ, Harvey  DJ, et al.  Everyday cognition in older adults: Associations with neuropsychological performance and structural brain imaging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2013;19(4):430–441. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Takechi  H, Kokuryu  A, Kubota  T, Yamada  H. Relative preservation of advanced activities in daily living among patients with mild-to-moderate dementia in the community and overview of support provided by family caregivers. Int J Alzheimers Dis. 2012;2012:418289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Cahn-Weiner  DA, Farias  ST, Julian  L, et al.  Cognitive and neuroimaging predictors of instrumental activities of daily living. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2007;13(5):747–757. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Therriault  J, Benedet  AL, Pascoal  TA, et al.  Association of apolipoprotein E ε4 with medial temporal tau independent of amyloid-β. JAMA Neurol. 2020; 77(4):470–479. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Albert  MS, DeKosky  ST, Dickson  D, et al.  The diagnosis of mild cognitive impairment due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(3):270–279. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Ossenkoppele  R, Singleton  EH, Groot  C, et al.  Research criteria for the behavioral variant of Alzheimer disease: A systematic review and meta-analysis. JAMA Neurol. 2022;79(1):48–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Gorno-Tempini  ML, Hillis  AE, Weintraub  S, et al.  Classification of primary progressive aphasia and its variants. Neurology. 2011;76:1006–1014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Crutch  SJ, Schott  JM, Rabinovici  GD, et al.  Consensus classification of posterior cortical atrophy. Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13:870–884. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Jack  CR  Jr, Wiste  HJ, Schwarz  CG, et al.  Longitudinal tau PET in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2018;141(5):1517–1528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Pascoal  TA, Shin  M, Kang  MS, et al.  In vivo quantification of neurofibrillary tangles with [18F]MK-6240. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2018;10(1):74. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Therriault  J, Benedet  AL, Pascoal  TA, et al.  Determining amyloid-B positivity using 18F-AZD4694 PET imaging. J Nucl Med. 2021;62:247–252. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Jack  CR  Jr, Wiste  HJ, Weigand  SD, et al.  Defining imaging biomarker cut points for brain aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 2017;13(3):205–216. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Desikan  RS, Ségonne  F, Fischl  B, et al.  An automated labeling system for subdividing the human cerebral cortex on MRI scans into gyral based regions of interest. Neuroimage. 2006;31(3):968–980. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Pfeffer  RI, Kurosaki  TT, Harrah  CH  Jr, Chance  JM, Filos  S. Measurement of functional activities in older adults in the community. J Gerontol. 1982;37(3):323–329. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Farias  ST, Mungas  D, Reed  BR, et al.  The measurement of everyday cognition (ECog): Scale development and psychometric properties. Neuropsychology. 2008;22(4):531–544. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Morris  JC. The Clinical Dementia Rating (CDR): Current version and scoring rules. Neurology. 1993;43(11):2412–2414. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Mathotaarachchi  S, Wang  S, Shin  M, et al.  VoxelStats: A MATLAB package for multi-modal voxel-wise brain image analysis. Front Neuroinform. 2016;10:20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Jack  CR  Jr, Wiste  HJ, Schwarz  CG, et al.  Longitudinal tau PET in ageing and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 2018;141:1517–1528. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Farias  ST, Mungas  D, Harvey  DJ, Simmons  A, Reed  BR, Decarli  C. The measurement of everyday cognition: Development and validation of a short form of the Everyday Cognition scales. Alzheimers Dement. 2011;7(6):593–601. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Sims  JR, Zimmer  JA, Evans  CD, et al.  Donanemab in early symptomatic Alzheimer disease: The TRAILBLAZER-ALZ 2 randomized clinical trial. JAMA. 2023;330(6):512–527. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Mintun  MA, Lo  AC, Duggan Evans  C, et al.  Donanemab in early Alzheimer’s disease. N Engl J Med. 2021;384(18):1691–1704. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. King-Robson  J, Wilson  H, Politis  M. Alzheimer’s disease neuroimaging initiative. Associations between amyloid and tau pathology, and connectome alterations, in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. J Alzheimers Dis. 2021;82(2):541–560. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Malpas  CB, Sharmin  S, Kalincik  T. The histopathological staging of tau, but not amyloid, corresponds to antemortem cognitive status, dementia stage, functional abilities and neuropsychiatric symptoms. Int J Neurosci. 2021;131(8):800–809. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Sabbagh  MN, Cooper  K, DeLange  J, et al.  Functional, global and cognitive decline correlates to accumulation of Alzheimer’s pathology in MCI and AD. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2010;7(4):280–286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Qiu  W-Y, Yang  Q, Zhang  W, et al.  The correlations between postmortem brain pathologies and cognitive dysfunction in aging and Alzheimer’s disease. Curr Alzheimer Res. 2018;15(5):462–473. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. Devous  MD  Sr, Fleisher  AS, Pontecorvo  MJ, et al.  Relationships between cognition and neuropathological tau in Alzheimer’s disease assessed by 18F flortaucipir PET. J Alzheimers Dis. 2021;80(3):1091–1104. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Halawa  OA, Gatchel  JR, Amariglio  RE, et al.  Inferior and medial temporal tau and cortical amyloid are associated with daily functional impairment in Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2019;11(1):14. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Dubbelman  MA, Sanchez  J, Schultz  AP, et al.  Everyday functioning and entorhinal and inferior temporal tau burden in cognitively normal older adults. J Prev Alzheimers Dis. 2022;9(4):801–808. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Marshall  GA, Gatchel  JR, Donovan  NJ, et al.  Regional tau correlates of instrumental activities of daily living and apathy in mild cognitive impairment and Alzheimer’s disease dementia. J Alzheimers Dis. 2019;67(2):757–768. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Bullich  S, Mueller  A, De Santi  S, et al.  Evaluation of tau deposition using 18F-PI-2620 PET in MCI and early AD subjects—A MissionAD tau sub-study. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2022;14(1):105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Dubbelman  MA, Mimmack  KJ, Sprague  EH, Amariglio  RE, Vannini  P, Marshall  GA. Regional cerebral tau predicts decline in everyday functioning across the Alzheimer’s disease spectrum. Alzheimers Res Ther. 2023;15(1):120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Pereira  FS, Yassuda  MS, Oliveira  AM, Forlenza  OV. Executive dysfunction correlates with impaired functional status in older adults with varying degrees of cognitive impairment. Int Psychogeriatr. 2008;20(6):1104–1115. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Ossenkoppele  R, Lyoo  CH, Jester-Broms  J, et al.  Assessment of demographic, genetic, and imaging variables associated with brain resilience and cognitive resilience to pathological tau in patients with Alzheimer disease. JAMA Neurol. 2020;77:632–642. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Perry  RJ, Hodges  JR. Relationship between functional and neuropsychological performance in early Alzheimer disease. Alzheimer Dis Assoc Disord. 2000;14(1):1–10. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Juva  K, Sulkava  R, Erkinjuntti  T, Ylikoski  R, Valvanne  J, Tilvis  R. Staging the severity of dementia: Comparison of clinical (CDR, DSM-III-R), functional (ADL, IADL) and cognitive (MMSE) scales. Acta Neurol Scand. 1994;90(4):293–298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Starkstein  SE, Jorge  R, Mizrahi  R, Robinson  RG. A prospective longitudinal study of apathy in Alzheimer’s disease. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 2006;77(1):8–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Fried  LP, Tangen  CM, Walston  J, et al.  Frailty in older adults: Evidence for a phenotype. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci. 2001;56(3):M146–M156. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

fcae043_Supplementary_Data

Data Availability Statement

All requests for raw and analysed data and materials will be promptly reviewed by McGill University to verify if the request is subject to any intellectual property or confidentiality obligations. Anonymized data will be shared upon request to the study’s senior author from a qualified academic investigator for the sole purpose of replicating the procedures and results presented in this article. Any data and materials that can be shared will be released via a material transfer agreement. Data are not publicly available due to information that could compromise the privacy of research participants. Related documents, including study protocol and informed consent forms, can similarly be made available upon request.


Articles from Brain Communications are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press

RESOURCES