Given the pressing need to develop treatments and interventions for psychiatric and neurological diseases, the development and use of new tools for neuroscience research has garnered major attention both by scientists and funders of science alike. This paper reviews the evidence from studies in monkeys that one such tool that allows for the manipulation of cells and circuits using light—called optogenetics—in order to evaluate the claim that optogenetics is ready to be deployed in such treatments and interventions.
Keywords: open science, translational psychiatry, translational neuroscience, nonhuman primates, optogenetics
Abstract
Translational neuroscience is committed to generating discoveries in the laboratory that ultimately can improve human lives. Optogenetics has received considerable attention because of its demonstrated promise in rodent brains to manipulate cells and circuits. In a recent report, Tremblay et al. [28] introduce an open resource detailing optogenetic studies of the nonhuman primate (NHP) brain and make robust claims about the translatability of the technology. We propose that their quantitative (e.g. a 91% success rate) and theoretical claims are questionable because the data were analyzed at a level relevant to the rodent but not NHP brain. Injections were clustered within a few monkeys in a few studies in a few brain regions, and their definitions of success were not clearly relevant to human neuropsychiatric disease. A reanalysis of the data with a modified definition of success that included a behavioral and biological effect revealed a 62.5% success rate that was lower when considering only strong outcomes (53.1%). This calls into question the current efficacy of optogenetic techniques in the NHP brain and suggests that we are a long way from being able to leverage them in ‘the service of patients with neurological or psychiatric conditions’ as the Tremblay report claims.
INTRODUCTION
The goal of translational neuroscience is to generate discoveries at the bench that can be implemented at the bedside, ultimately improving people’s lives. Yet, traversing the landscape between bench and bedside to forge a translational bridge is so challenging that it is often referred to as the ‘valley of death’ [13]. Robust, repeatable, valid methods and experiments carried out in cell lines and some animal models often fail to produce tractable observations, treatments and interventions for humans. These failures occur for many reasons, but one of the most prominent and important reasons is that the most frequently used models, be they cell lines or animal models, do not recapitulate the human brain’s structure and function with high enough fidelity to allow for translation. Dominant animal models, like mice, do not share key neuroanatomical, developmental or behavioral homologies with humans [18, 24, 31, 32]. Yet, many nonhuman primates (NHPs), particularly cercopithecine monkeys such as macaques, do share such homologies and thus can be a translational bridge between bench and bedside (e.g. [25, 26]).
Nowhere is there more demand to traverse the translational neuroscience ‘valley of death’ than in the case of new technology that allows for precise modulation of cellular level activity within the brain. Tools like chemogenetics, in which a viral vector is used to transfect neurons to express a designer receptor that responds to a specific chemical compound, and optogenetics, in which a viral vector is used to implant light-sensitive ion channels in cell membranes that thenare activated/deactivated with light, have been suggested to have the capacity to radically change the health landscape for humans by generating new treatments and interventions to modulate cells, circuits and ultimately human behavior and experience. Evaluating the efficacy of these newer technologies is critical because the trade-off between technologies is a zero-sum game in the research funding landscape—once new tools are thought to be effective, grant applications using better-established technologies tend to be criticized as lacking innovation. Moreover, the rush to use new technology for technology’s sake may deplete scientific resources when the failure rate of new techniques is high.
Originally developed in mice, optogenetics is being increasingly used in NHPs over the past few years, promising the use of circuit-specific modulation via light in the primate brain [10]. The goal of this commentary is to explore the idea that NHP optogenetics is a functioning technology for basic neuroscience, one that is ready for translational neuroscience with clinical applications as has been recently claimed. We focus on a recent review of published and unpublished optogenetics studies in NHPs, recently compiled into an open resource [28].
HAS OPTOGENETICS REACHED THE POINT OF TRANSLATION FOR PSYCHIATRIC DISEASE?
From a translational science perspective, a neurobiological technology is only truly useful if it can be readily deployed in humans and meets expectations in terms of addressing health concerns when it is deployed. What this would mean for optogenetics is that human neurons could be transduced, a light could be placed near the neurons, ideally without causing significant damage to other brain areas, and then activation or deactivation of those neurons via light would alter neural activity and behavior or phenomenological experience.
The goal of translating optogenetics to humans has generated increased interest in carrying out optogenetic studies in NHPs who are thought to be a translational bridge for the valley of death. Enough studies using optogenetics in NHPs have been carried out and/or published to now allow for the concatenation of an open resource to share NHP optogenetics data (https://osf.io/mknfu/). A companion manuscript was published by Tremblay et al. [28] detailing those data and making, in our opinion, bold claims about what the implications of their collected data for the readiness of optogenetics to remediate neuropsychiatric illness. For example, these authors state ‘we hope this resource will be used not only by basic scientists trying to uncover the workings of the primate brain using optogenetics but also by translational scientists hoping to bring this powerful technology to the service of patients with neurological or psychiatric conditions’ (p. 1086). The authors go on to conclude ‘The way is paved for development of clinical technologies relying on optogenetics to control neural populations and pathways with unprecedented precision’ (pp. 1086–1087). Such claims suggest that the open resource has direct translatability and the technology is ready to be deployed for the treatment and intervention of neuropsychiatric diseases.
Optogenetics is highly effective in modifying neurophysiology and behavior in mice [6, 11], especially in combination with genetic engineering to facilitate targeted, cell-type specific expression of opsins. But, differences in mouse and human behavior and biology call into question the efficacy of this technology for human application. First, mouse brains are small and lissencephalic, which facilitates access to specific brain regions for viral transfection as well as the delivery of light needed to modify neural activity. Indeed, Tremblay et al. [28] noted the technical challenges associated with gene expression and light delivery in larger NHP brains. Second, although some behavioral assays in mice may exhibit face validity with specific features of behaviors associated with human neuropsychiatric diseases, the extent to which they capture the complexity of the behaviors associated with human neuropsychiatric diseases and other forms of validity (e.g. construct, predictive) is questionable. Aberrations in complex behaviors leading to depression, anxiety, compulsions and psychosis are likely emergent and rely on degenerate circuitry (for a discussion of degeneracy, see [8]). Thus, explicit validation of the effectiveness of optogenetic manipulation of brain circuits relevant to psychiatry in larger NHP brains is doubly critical, in terms of the physical properties of the brain as well as the modulation of more complex behavioral processes.
The open resource [28] reflects contributions from 45 research laboratories, with 1042 individual data points included (552 previously unpublished) representing experiments in 198 monkeys and thus aggregates an enormous amount of technical expertise on neurosurgical placement of viral vectors in the NHP brain, a substantial contribution to the field. Moreover, meta- and open science efforts in NHP neuroscience are critical and until very recently, very rare. By metascience, we refer to a collection of approaches that include meta-analyses (analyses of existing analyses), pooling of data across laboratories, splitting data collection efforts strategically across laboratories to guard against protocols being laboratory specific and gathering published and unpublished data in shared resources, among others (e.g. the ‘many labs’ projects; e.g. [7, 15, 16]). These efforts are important for NHP neuroscience because our field typically uses small sample sizes and has had a historical bias against replicating experiments, constraints that are both ethical and practical. By open science, we mean the sharing of scientific resources including primary materials and data (for an NHP specific example, see [20, 21]). Combining meta- and open data practices, as the optogenetics database does, sets the stage for scientists to be able to evaluate the effectiveness of methods or experiments and to determine where efforts are needed to move science forward and to determine the rigor and reproducibility of the established methods [3]. This particular effort deserves significant recognition because of the large number of unpublished data sets and the number of ‘unsuccessful’ studies included in it. These two types of data are critical for determining if an approach is truly effective. Without inclusion of those types of data, particularly in the context of a publication culture that focuses heavily on null hypothesis testing and rewards significant effects to a greater degree than those that are not significant (with regard to ease and visibility of publications), the success of an approach is likely to be overestimated. In that vein, efforts that gather data that is both published and unpublished, as well as successful and unsuccessful, on the efficacy of new tech is both important and laudable.
The large database of optogenetics studies in NHPs positions us well to evaluate whether optogenetic technology in NHPs is ready for deployment in a translational context—either to ask questions about the etiology of neuropsychiatric diseases or to actually be deployed to treat such diseases. In reading Tremblay et al. [28], we drew different conclusions from the authors about the usefulness of optogenetics in NHPs and in turn the impact of this method in the service of understanding and developing treatment interventions for psychiatric and neurologic disease. Two issues are notable. First, the definition of experimental success in Tremblay et al. [28] was overly broad on two dimensions. Studies were considered successful if they generated histological, or neurophysiological, or behavioral effects, and what was counted as a successful behavioral effect included subjective evaluations of narrative accounts of animals’ behaviors as well as effects qualitatively judged to be ‘weak’. At a minimum, the successful use of optogenetics in behavioral or systems neuroscience (separate from its translational relevance) would require some evidence that neurons have been modulated either via histology of viral expression or altered neuronal activity measured in addition to an optogenetic mediated change in behavioral outcomes. This necessitates looking at conjunctions of success—cases in which there are both behavioral and histological effects or cases in which there are both behavioral and neurophysiological effects.
A second issue is that the unit of analysis for descriptive statistics in the original report is the single injection. It is perhaps not surprising that this database is organized injection-by-injection, given the success of single injections in rodent experiments to generate functional outcomes [5]. But, it is unclear whether single injections would have measurable outcomes in primates, particularly meaningful behavioral outcomes that are clearly grounded in neurophysiology and/or histology. Given that multiple injections of a viral vector into brain region are often required to achieve adequate coverage in larger NHP brains, a more appropriate basis to judge the relative success of experiments would include an analysis at the level of brain region and monkey and taking into consideration multiple markers of success simultaneously.
The summary of the resources in the paper by Tremblay states that optogenetics in NHPs has a 91% success rate (Figure 4D of [28])—based histological, neurophysiological, or behavioral outcomes. This metric included injections where effects were weak or mixed. When only ‘strong’ outcomes were considered, only 76% of injections were considered successful. In addition to being predicated on a broad definition of success, those rates are based on individual injections that may or may not have behavioral or physiological effects in NHP. Most importantly, the data points are clustered within animals, and it is animals, not neurons, which produce behaviors [27].
Given the issues detailed above, we carried out a re-analysis of the database from Tremblay et al. [28] with a specific focus on definitions of success that, in our opinion, are relevant to translational neuroscience for neuropsychiatric diseases—namely having both a neural AND behavioral outcome. We also used the experiment rather than the injection as the level of analysis. Further, we consider where the injection sites were in the brain and whether success (using our definition or the more liberal definition from [28]) speaks to the readiness to use optogenetics productively in NHPs to research or treat neuropsychiatric disease.
METHODS
We examined the publicly available database described in the Neuron paper and then requested the unredacted spreadsheet from the first author so that we could unambiguously determine which experiments were performed in a single animal, something that was not possible using only the publicly posted database (https://osf.io/mknfu/). This allowed us to evaluate the number of successful experiments at the level of brain regions and animals, rather than single injections of viral vectors. In this way, we considered a single ‘experiment’ to be one or more injections of the same viral vector into one brain area in one animal. Injections into two or more brain regions in the same monkey would be considered as two separate experiments in this analysis, even though they may be biologically constrained by being in the same animal. The choice of level of analysis in clustered data has a significant impact on outcomes, given that adjacent injections into the same area in the same animal could reasonably be considered to not be independent experiments (see also [1]). It is also this level of analysis that is most pertinent to behavioral neuroscience investigations of how neural circuits control behavior.
The database we received included 1085 injections. This was increased from the 1042 injections reported in the original publication, reflecting the utility of the database as an open resource (although it has not been updated since that time). We read this database into R and assigned experiment identifiers based on unique conjunctions of laboratory, animal identifier and brain region, resulting in 383 unique experiments. We then read this database back out and carried out analyses to determine success rates at the experiment level. We categorized each injection as missing, failed, weak success or strong success for histology, physiology and behavior, attempting to follow the descriptions in [28]. Any experiment that had at least one injection classified as ‘strong success’ was categorized as a strong success for that domain; experiments that had no strong success but at least one ‘weak success’ were categorized as weak success and so forth. Thus, this categorization may still overestimate the true success rate in cases where many injections were placed in a brain area but only a subset had successful effects. The redacted database with experiment identifiers and associated R analysis script are available at https://osf.io/j7rm2/.
RESULTS AND DISCUSSION
When we analyzed success rates considering injections as nested within monkey and brain region—what we refer to as individual ‘experiments’—rather than injection as the unit of analysis, a somewhat different picture from that painted by Tremblay et al. [28] emerged. First, very few experiments assessed all three outcomes (histology, neurophysiology, and behavior). Only 39 out of the 383 cases that we identified in the database (10.2%) evaluated the efficacy of an optogenetic manipulation in terms of histological, physiological, and behavioral effects. In this small subset of experiments, there was a 59% (23/39) success rate on all three outcomes. The majority of these successes were in premotor cortex (7/9) and visual cortex (7/12). Put another way, only 6% (23/383) of the experiments attempted and reported were successful in using optogenetics to bridge levels of analysis from neurons to behavior.
When we examined each outcome domain separately, we found high success rates, as reported in the original paper, for histological (226/261, 86.6%) and neurophysiological (168/197, 85.3%) verifications of optogenetics. Success rates using behavioral outcomes (47/74, 63.5%) to validate optogenetic manipulations were lower. These success rates are lower than those reported and highlighted in the original paper, especially when we only considered strong effects: histology (72.4%), neurophysiology (75.1%) and behavior (52.7%). Moreover, these proportions consider only cases in which histology, physiology or behavior was tested (Fig. 1). The different success rates between histological and functional evaluations of optogenetics suggests the impediment in using optogenetics to study the primate brain is not merely viral transduction per se, but rather difficulties in modulating sufficient numbers of neurons in the larger primate brain [12]. Without question, the further development of NHP specific genetic tools, rather than simply porting tools developed in rodent models to NHP, is necessary to meet the goal of using NHPs as a translational bridge between behavioral and systems neuroscience and the clinic.
Speaking practically, a successful optogenetic experiment involves evaluating success in one or more of the outcome domains mentioned above. With the end goal of using optogenetics in NHPs to study the neural control of behavior at the level of neurons and circuits, it is reasonable to expect that behavioral manipulations should be validated in combination with either histology or neurophysiology. Therefore, we considered a definition of success as yielding a combination of effects, either behavior neurophysiology or behavior histology (Fig. 2). Using a broad definition that included both weak and strong effects of success, there was a 62.5% (45/72) success rate among experiments that reported optogenetics effects on behavior and neurophysiology and a 62.5% (25/40) success rate among experiments that reported optogenetic effects on both behavior and histology. When we again adopted Tremblay and colleagues’ approach in considering only strong effects, success rates dropped to 48.6% (35/72) for studies that evaluated behavior in combination with neurophysiology and to 57.5% (23/40) for studies that evaluated behavior in combination with histology. Notably, these results are for experiments pooled across brain regions and laboratories.
Perhaps more concerning for scholars interested in psychiatric and neurological diseases is that 66.3% (254/383) of the experiments evaluated optogenetics in primary sensory or motor areas. Modulation of those areas should produce fairly stereotyped, clear functional (i.e. behavioral) outcomes, yet only 29 (11.4%, 29/254) of those experiments even evaluated both behavior and physiology. Cortex—particularly primate cortex—is heterogenous in its structure and success in one region does not guarantee success across the entire brain. Further, the brain’s inherent degeneracy and organization as a complex system means that different structures can accomplish, or contribute to, the same functional outcomes [8]—as functional outcomes become more complex, so too do the possible configurations of neural circuity that can support them. This problem is compounded by the neocortical expansion of the primate brain [32]. Practically, this means that the sorts of complex behaviors of interest to many NHP systems neuroscientists working are less likely perturbed by the manipulation of single circuits.
In our view, a 62.5% (all effects) or 53.1% (strong effects only) success rate, averaged across the two conjunctions that include behavior as a functional outcome, is meaningfully different from the 91% (all effects) and 76% (strong effects only) success rate suggested by Tremblay et al. [28]. The lower success rate that we identified suggests that optogenetics is still in development as a tool in NHPs, rather than ready to be deployed to study functional outcomes in primates or to intervene in clinical contexts. This may reflect its true efficacy in mammals, previously masked by the lower costs and higher throughput associated with rodent models. Or, it may reflect issues with deploying viral techniques in primates, a perspective supported by mixed results of experiments using designer receptors exclusively activated by designer drugs to manipulate behavior in primates, despite histological validation [9, 29]. Recognizing that further research is needed to determine the origin of the low success rates, it is nevertheless the case that at the moment success in obtaining strong effects across multiple functional outcomes when pursuing optogenetics in NHPs amounts to flipping a coin. This warrants a very different conclusion from the one that a reader might reasonably draw after reading the survey by Tremblay et al. [28].
Even if we were to imagine that optogenetics was ready to be deployed in the context of neuropsychiatry, a number of other major challenges exist when translating the technology from use in rodents to use in primates. For example, for optogenetics to work, a light probe must be proximate to the structure being manipulated and this represents a significant engineering challenge in deep areas of the primate brain. That means that studies that demonstrate success in primary/secondary sensory or motor cortex, which sit at the surface of the brain are fundamentally different from those evaluating the manipulation of structures deep within the brain. Accessing deep structures in NHPs—e.g. the amygdala, hippocampus and subgenual cingulate cortex—that are part of circuitry relevant to psychiatric and neurological disorders without damaging the areas through which probes are passed is a significant engineering challenge that has not yet been sufficiently addressed. Moreover, when optogenetics was successfully used to perturb a deep structure, like the superior colliculus [4], it took advantage of the detailed functional mapping that specified how the superior colliculus controls saccadic eye movements. We are a long way from conquering the engineering problems, much less interpretive issues, that will allow for the ‘development of clinical technologies relying on optogenetics to control neural populations and pathways with unprecedented precision’ ([28], pp. 1086–1087) in the context of complex behaviors relevant to neural circuits involved in psychiatric disease. The challenges of reliably manipulating brain areas that have detailed and stereotyped maps will multiply when targeting brain areas and circuits that lack such structure. Although enough experiments may succeed to produce new information in a basic research context, success rates must be much higher for direct translational application. When optogenetics sufficiently matures to be used as a reliable tool in primate systems neuroscience, it is more likely to allow elegant circuit dissections of behavior that can inform existing treatments than to be a frontline treatment for psychiatric disorders [19].
WHAT IS THE WAY FORWARD WITH OPTOGENETICS IN NHPS?
As the drive for new technology to manipulate neural activity pushes forward, we propose that the ability to manipulate behavior, specifically, should remain the gold standard by which we evaluate success. This view echoes recent calls to reexamine the utility of behavior itself in neuroscience [17, 22]. We would suggest that an approach with a current success rate of 53.1% or 62.5% (on average) should be evaluated differently than one with a success rate of 76% or 91% in terms of choosing a method for manipulating neural circuits. Given this, we propose that more effort needs to be devoted to validating and optimizing optogenetics and related technologies specifically in NHPs. Validation and optimization is needed before such tools can be efficiently deployed in experimental contexts and certainly before claims about their translational relevance are made. Such basic science efforts may not have the glamour or appeal of new technological invention or the deployment of new technology in experimental contexts, but they are vital for moving the field forward and ultimately for generating new effective treatments and interventions of psychiatric and neurological disease.
Given the data in the open resource, we propose that the state of the technology for use in NHPs may be discussed in the literature as if it is more mature than it actually is. The drive for technological innovation from the BRAIN Initiative (https://braininitiative.nih.gov/) has created a primacy of technology such that optogenetics and related methods are often viewed by science funders as a better approach for systems neuroscience than ‘classical’ experimental methods in spite of interpretive difficulties with transient manipulations of neural activity (e.g. [23]). Believing that a technology is further along in its development than it actually is may result in abandoning tools that actually work before the new tools are actually ready. This can limit what science is done and the inferences that can be drawn from that science. Imagine, for example, using such a new technology to temporarily deactivate a hub of the primate brain thought to be involved in neuropsychiatric disease, about which there are functional hypotheses but for which the function has not been clearly established. A scientist employs the technology, measures behavior and gets null results. Without established ground truth about the brain area’s function (determined with the use of ‘old’ tools), there is no way to determine if the functional hypothesis was wrong or whether temporary deactivation leads to different behavioral consequences that were observed in permanent deactivation, stimulation or recording studies (the ‘old’ tools) or if the technology simply didn’t work. Until the function of the tool is well established, it is critical that we maintain tried and true methods [14, 30], deployed in the context of testing innovative hypotheses to both speed scientific discovery about the brain but also to help us improve the functionality of the new tools. This is the case even if the goal of developing optogenetics, and other novel neuromodulation approaches in NHPs, is for interrogation of circuit function purely in a basic research context rather than with a goal of transposing this technology into human brains.
The data provided by the open NHP optogenetics resource leads us to conclude that optogenetic manipulation is not yet at a state of development where it can be reliably applied to investigate functional effects of neural circuit manipulations in NHPs in the context of complex behaviors relevant to psychiatric disease. Major questions and challenges remain about the application of this technology, both in terms of methodological approaches (e.g. how to target deep structures without causing significant brain damage, how to achieve transfection coverage of those structures at a rate sufficient to impact behavior, etc.) and theoretical ones (e.g. what is positive evidence that the technology is ready to move into humans, let alone be deployed in greater numbers of NHPs). We hope that critical considerations of where we are as a field, like this one, might move us forward as a community to promote the development of these and related approaches in order to successfully cross the translational ‘valley of death’. We celebrate the meta- and open science nature of the database, as well as the efforts of project leader and the individual scientific teams, because they allow for a gestalt evaluation of the efficacy of optogenetics in the NHP brain, which we hope will ultimately speed science and discovery.
We suggest, based on our re-analysis of the data in the open NHP optogenetics resource, that this technology is still immature, with few successes in modulating circuits that are relevant for psychiatric neuroscience. We do not suggest, on this basis, that the enterprise of developing optogenetic approaches in NHPs should be abandoned. But, we would propose that perhaps more fundamental engineering and validation work should be a priority to increase the success rate of this approach, especially in brain structures and circuits outside of primary sensory and motor function. We would also suggest that as systems and cognitive neuroscientists, we reflect on the relationship between technological innovation and fundamental questions that are critical to our field, as well as their translation to psychiatry. The ability to execute technologically dazzling ‘proof of principle’ experiments potentially comes at a cost of allocating resources to research that uses less technologically advanced approaches but nevertheless is effective in addressing fundamentally critical questions. As we have argued here and elsewhere [2], the interpretation of the functional impact of any manipulation of neuronal activity cannot come from a single technical strategy. Especially for translational research in NHPs, innovation in relating circuits to behavior with the potential for elucidating mechanisms of psychiatric disease cannot be hobbled by reliance on as-yet-unstable technological approaches.
SUPPLEMENTARY MATERIAL
Supplementary material are available at Oxford Open Neuroscience online.
FUNDING
None.
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
None declared.
AUTHORS’ CONTRIBUTIONS
All authors contributed equally.
DATA AVAILABILITY
Supplementary Material
Contributor Information
Eliza Bliss-Moreau, Department of Psychology, University of California Davis, CA 95616, USA; California National Primate Research Center, University of California Davis, CA 95616, USA.
Vincent D Costa, Department of Behavioral Neuroscience, Oregon Health Sciences University, OR 97239, USA; Oregon National Primate Research Center, Oregon Health Sciences University, OR 97239, USA.
Mark G Baxter, Nash Family Department of Neuroscience, Icahn School of Medicine at Mount Sinai, NY 10029-5674, USA.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
The authors wish to thank Peter Rudebeck for helpful comments on a previous draft of this manuscript.
References
- 1.Aarts E, Verhage M, Veenvliet JVet al. A solution to dependency: using multilevel analysis to accommodate nested data. Nat Neurosci. 2014;17:491–6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Baxter MG, Costa VD. Clozapine is my favorite color: chemogenetic modulation of anxiety-related behavior in primates. Mol Ther. 2021;29:3322–4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Bliss-Moreau E, Amara RR, Buffalo EAet al. Improving rigor and reproducibility in nonhuman primate research. Am J Primatol. 2021;83:e23331. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Cavanaugh J, Monosov IE, McAlonan Ket al. Optogenetic inactivation modifies monkey visuomotor behavior. Neuron. 2012;76:901–7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Deisseroth K. Optogenetics and psychiatry: applications, challenges, and opportunities. Biol Psychiatry. 2012;71:1030–2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Deisseroth K. Optogenetics: 10 years of microbial opsins in neuroscience. Nat Neurosci. 2015;18:1213–25 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Ebersole CR, Atherton OE, Belanger ALet al. Many labs 3: evaluating participant pool quality across the academic semester via replication. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2016;67:68–82 [Google Scholar]
- 8.Edelman GM, Gally JA. Degeneracy and complexity in biological systems. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2001;98:13763–8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Eldridge MAG, Lerchner W, Saunders RCet al. Chemogenetic disconnection of monkey orbitofrontal and rhinal cortex reversibly disrupts reward value. Nat Neurosci. 2016;19:37–9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.El-Shamayleh Y, Horwitz GD. Primate optogenetics: progress and prognosis. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2019;116:26195–203 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Fenno L, Yizhar O, Deisseroth K. The development and application of Optogenetics. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2011;34:389–412 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Gradinaru V. Expanding the brain researcher’s toolkit. Science. 2020;369:637–7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Insel TR. A bridge to somewhere. Transl Psychiatry. 2011;1:e2–2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Katz LN, Yates JL, Pillow JWet al. Dissociated functional significance of decision-related activity in the primate dorsal stream. Nature. 2016;535:285–8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Klein RA, Ratliff KA, Vianello Met al. Investigating variation in replicability: a “many labs” replication project. Soc Psychol. 2014;45:142–52 [Google Scholar]
- 16.Klein RA, Vianello M, Hasselman Fet al. Many labs 2: investigating variation in replicability across samples and settings. Adv Methods Pract Psychol Sci. 2018;1:443–90 [Google Scholar]
- 17.Krakauer JW, Ghazanfar AA, Gomez-Marin Aet al. Neuroscience needs behavior: correcting a reductionist bias. Neuron. 2017;93:480–90 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Laubach M, Amarante LM, Swanson Ket al. What, if anything, is rodent prefrontal cortex? ENeuro. 2018;5:ENEURO.0315-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Lüscher C, Pollak P. Optogenetically inspired deep brain stimulation: linking basic with clinical research. Swiss Med Wkly. 2016;146:w14278 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Milham M, Petkov CI, Margulies DSet al. Accelerating the evolution of nonhuman primate neuroimaging. Neuron. 2020;105:600–3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Milham MP, Ai L, Koo Bet al. An open resource for non-human primate imaging. Neuron. 2018;100:61–74.e2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Niv Y. On the primacy of behavioral research for understanding the brain. Behav Neurosci. 2021;135:601–609 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Otchy TM, Wolff SBE, Rhee JYet al. Acute off-target effects of neural circuit manipulations. Nature. 2015;528:358–63 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Preuss TM, Wise SP. Evolution of prefrontal cortex. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2022;47:3–19 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Roberts AC, Clarke HF. Why we need nonhuman primates to study the role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in the regulation of threat- and reward-elicited responses. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2019;116:26297–304 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Rudebeck PH, Rich EL, Mayberg HS. From bed to bench side: reverse translation to optimize neuromodulation for mood disorders. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2019;116:26288–96 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Tinbergen N. On aims and methods of ethology. Z Tierpsychol. 1963;20:410–33 [Google Scholar]
- 28.Tremblay S, Acker L, Afraz Aet al. An open resource for non-human primate optogenetics. Neuron. 2020;108:1075–1090.e6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Upright NA, Brookshire SW, Schnebelen Wet al. Behavioral effect of chemogenetic inhibition is directly related to receptor transduction levels in rhesus monkeys. J Neurosci. 2018;38:7969–75 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Vaidya AR, Pujara MS, Petrides Met al. Lesion studies in contemporary neuroscience. Trends Cogn Sci. 2019;23:653–71 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Wallis CU, Cardinal RN, Alexander Let al. Opposing roles of primate areas 25 and 32 and their putative rodent homologs in the regulation of negative emotion. Proc Natl Acad Sci. 2017;114:E4075–84 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Wise SP. Forward frontal fields: phylogeny and fundamental function. Trends Neurosci. 2008;31:599–608 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.