Highlights
Corvids and parrots with brains of 5–20 g show cognitive properties similar to those of great apes like chimpanzees with brains of about 400 g.
An isocortex is ideal to represent and process sensory information but is not a necessity for complex cognition.
We identified four convergently evolved neural features of smart birds and mammals: many associative pallial neurons, a pallial area that overtakes functions of the mammalian prefrontal cortex, dense dopaminergic innervation of associative pallial areas, and flexible neural fundaments of working memory.
As in mammals, avian cognition emerges from transiently activated forebrain networks.
Keywords: pallium, evolution, neuron number, prefrontal cortex, mammals, cognition
Abstract
Many cognitive neuroscientists believe that both a large brain and an isocortex are crucial for complex cognition. Yet corvids and parrots possess non-cortical brains of just 1–25 g, and these birds exhibit cognitive abilities comparable with those of great apes such as chimpanzees, which have brains of about 400 g. This opinion explores how this cognitive equivalence is possible. We propose four features that may be required for complex cognition: a large number of associative pallial neurons, a prefrontal cortex (PFC)-like area, a dense dopaminergic innervation of association areas, and dynamic neurophysiological fundaments for working memory. These four neural features have convergently evolved and may therefore represent ‘hard to replace’ mechanisms enabling complex cognition.
Many cognitive neuroscientists believe that both a large brain and an isocortex are crucial for complex cognition. Yet corvids and parrots possess non-cortical brains of just 1–25 g, and these birds exhibit cognitive abilities comparable with those of great apes such as chimpanzees, which have brains of about 400 g. This opinion explores how this cognitive equivalence is possible. We propose four features that may be required for complex cognition: a large number of associative pallial neurons, a prefrontal cortex (PFC)-like area, a dense dopaminergic innervation of association areas, and dynamic neurophysiological fundaments for working memory. These four neural features have convergently evolved and may therefore represent ‘hard to replace’ mechanisms enabling complex cognition.
Tales of cortical superiority
Many comparative cognitive neuroscientists subscribe to the idea that complex cognitive abilities like those found in primates require at least two key neural features. The first is a large brain. A wealth of data show that, within taxa, large-brained animals display more cognitive skills than those with smaller brains [1]. This is visible in primates [2], Cetacea [3], and birds [4,5], but has also been shown in insects [6]. Although details about relative or absolute brain size and similar variables are debated [5], the idea that larger brains can in principle provide more neurons and thus more computing power is generally accepted [5,7]. The second common prerequisite for complex cognition is the mammalian cerebral cortex (Box 1). The isocortical component of the cerebral cortex is considered particularly important given its unique topographical organization. In the isocortex (see Glossary), vertical input and output systems closely interact with long-distance horizontal fibers such that neurons across cortical areas can be associated to represent and memorize multiple objects’ features through Hebbian learning [8]. This design allows the various attributes of a currently perceived object to be linked with each other to form a single percept that can be associated with an internal representation [9].
Box 1. The many names of the cerebral cortex.
The word ‘cortex’ refers to several laminated brain structures like the (mostly) six-layered cerebral cortex, the three-layered cerebellar cortex, or the three-layered reptilian cortex of the dorsal pallium. To be more specific, it is important to use the term ‘cerebral cortex’ that in humans constitutes 82% of the brain mass but contains only 19% of all neurons of the human brain [108]. In primates, about 90% of the cerebral cortex comprises the six-layered isocortex. The Ancient Greek term ‘isos’ means ‘equal’ or ‘same’. All areas of the isocortex have a similar appearance, although detailed analyses uncover regional differences that are used to subdivide the isocortex into different areas. The isocortex is sometimes also called the ‘neocortex’ because it is an evolutionary novelty of mammals. The remaining 10% of the cerebral cortex comprises the ‘allocortex’, with the Ancient Greek term ‘allos’ meaning ‘all others’ or ‘rest’. These others are traditionally subdivided into the ‘archicortex’, with the Ancient Greek term ‘arche’ meaning ‘origin’, and the ‘paleocortex’, from the Ancient Greek ‘palaios’ meaning ‘old’. The archicortex contains, among others, the hippocampal formation, the entorhinal cortex, and the retrosplenial cortex. The paleocortex contains many structures, some of which have six layers while others are hardly layered at all. To list a few, the paleocortex contains the olfactory bulb and tubercle, the cortical nucleus of the amygdala, the septum, and the prepiriform cortex [109]. Previously, it was assumed that paleo-, archi-, and isocortex constitute a phylogenetic sequence that moves from old to novel. This view is no longer accepted.
Alt-text: Box 1
When compared with large mammals, birds have very small brains that comprise seemingly homogeneous nuclear clusters. These glaring anatomical differences should cast a dim prospect on avian cognition. However, across an array of cognitive tasks, several avian taxa perform similarly to great apes and appear to rely on similar cognitive algorithms. How is that possible? Our inability to answer this question shows that we are still far from a generic understanding of the link between brain structure and cognition.
This opinion attempts to partially solve this riddle by identifying features that characterize the brains of both ‘smart’ mammals and ‘smart’ birds. Such an approach might show which neural mechanisms are necessary for complex cognition. To this end, we start by first characterizing avian cognition by providing several examples. Then, we question the assumption that big brains are a useful measure of processing capacity, proceed to provide a novel view on the organization of the bird forebrain, and review the first studies on large-scale neural networks that could constitute the substrate for avian cognition. In the end, we argue that four features are currently excellent candidates for complex cognition in corvids, parrots, and great apes. These are: (i) large quantities of associative pallial neurons; (ii) a PFC-like area; (iii) dense dopaminergic innervation of association areas; and (iv) dynamic neurophysiological properties of neuron groups that hold information in working memory. There are strong arguments that these have evolved independently in birds and mammals and thus could constitute ‘hard to replace’ mechanisms for brains that perform complex cognition.
Smart birds
When Irene Pepperberg started her studies on cognitive feats in African gray parrots (Figure 1A) in the late 1970s, the concept of small, non-cortical bird brains was so dominant that she sometimes received unusual reviewer comments like ‘What are you smoking?’ ([10], p. 72). This has changed. Especially in the past few years, studies have shown that some birds, particularly corvids and parrots, have cognitive abilities that rival those of great apes. For example, ravens plan for different kinds of future events. They will refuse an immediately available small reward and instead select a tool or a token. The next day they either use the tool or barter the token to obtain a larger reward [11]. When New Caledonian crows use tools (Figure 1B), they can mentally represent each step, thereby minding the identity and location of an apparatus and out-of-sight tools while planning sequences of behaviors [12]. In Australia, sulfur-crested cockatoos learned to open household waste bins with individual style and site-specific differences (Figure 1C). This culture rapidly spread via social learning to 44 Sydney suburbs, thereby forcing people to develop a counterculture of technical measures to prevent pilfering [13]. However, could it be that birds excel in only a few cognitive domains, while primates demonstrate sophisticated domain-general cognition? A detailed comparison of studies in eight cognitive domains concluded that corvids, parrots, and non-human primates possess similar domain-general cognitive capacities. [14]. A subsequent review of the results of 28 different cognitive domains tested on corvids, parrots, and primates came to a similar conclusion [15]. Using the Primate Cognition Test Battery to study cognitive development, it could be demonstrated that ravens perform on par with orangutans and chimps, except for spatial skills [16].
Thus, corvids, parrots, and primates reach similarly high levels of performance. However, do they rely on the same algorithms (i.e., the selection and sequence of necessary mental processes) to solve a task? Here, it seems that all avian species tested to date use mental algorithms similar to mammals, whether corvid, parrot, chicken, or pigeon. For example, pigeons not only discriminate between stimuli differing in numerosity but, like monkeys, display distance effects and are constrained by Weber’s law [17]. Corvids go through cognitive developmental steps of object permanence identical to those of children and make the same errors when tested before they have reached a certain stage [18., 19., 20.]. Similarly, the development of sensorimotor skills and motor self-regulation in corvids follows the mammalian sequence [21]. Domestic chicks [22] prefer a left-to-right orientation of ascending numerosities – the mental number line – like adult human subjects [23] and newborn babies [24]. Pigeons learn the orthography of a limited number of four-letter English words (Figure 1D) and thereby utilize the same cognitive strategies as primary-school children [25]. Keas exhibit complex statistical inference by integrating relative quantities as well as physical and social information, thereby utilizing domain-general thinking similar to that of humans and great apes [26].
These studies show that birds and mammals operate with highly similar mental processes when facing a cognitive task, but this does not imply that all mammal species or all bird species are identical with respect to the same levels of cognitive performance. Although both pigeons [27] and chickens [28,29] can master unexpectedly complex tasks, comparisons of pigeons, corvids, and parrots tested with similar procedures reveal important differences [30]. Pigeons acquire these tasks much more slowly [17], require more exemplars to learn an abstract rule [31,32], discount more steeply in delay studies [33], and have problems in transferring their knowledge to other kinds of stimuli [34]. A detailed analysis shows that pigeons simply need more trials to learn an association and generalize less successfully to similar features [30].
Taking these findings together, birds and mammals utilize similar mental algorithms when working on cognitive tasks. Within the avian clade, corvids and parrots reach levels of cognitive performance throughout all domains that match those of great apes. These involve abilities like prospection of future encounters, reasoning about the causality of observations, imagining the perspectives of others, flexibility to transfer learned rules to other tasks, and several more. This collection can be seen as a toolkit for ‘complex cognition’ [14., 15., 16.,35]. Since corvids, parrots, and great apes show equal abilities for complex cognition, the idea that large and isocortical brains are a prerequisite for complex cognition is challenged. In the following sections, we present scientific results that show how this riddle could be solved. More importantly, these insights could pave the way to understanding which neural features really matter for complex cognition.
Not size but content matters
The size of mammalian brains varies enormously, from 0.074 g in the lesser bamboo bat (Tylonycteris pachypus) to about 9 kg in sperm whales (Physeter macrocephalus). Bird brains overlap with those of mammals in the lower range of this distribution. The brain:body ratios of mammals and birds are also comparable [1]. The main difference is that mammalian brains can become very large when their body size increases, while bird brains remain small, even when their body size enlarges. In mammals, complex cognitive abilities were reported in animals with large brains of 400–1600 g, like dolphins, pinnipeds, and Hominidae (humans and great apes) [36,37]. This is in stark contrast to birds. Ostriches are the largest living birds and surpass human body weights but have brains of about 40 g [38]. Around 50 000 years ago, 600-kg dromornithids were the largest birds that ever roamed the Earth but had brains of only 123 g, about one-third of a chimpanzee brain [39]. Corvids and parrots have brains of just 1–25 g [4]. How could these birds become so smart with walnut-sized brains?
A landmark study showed that bird brains contain twice as many neurons per unit brain volume as primates and up to four times more than rodents [40]. Since neurons are the computational processing unit of brains, more neurons per gram brain should produce more processing capacity. Pallial neurons are especially relevant for flexible cognition since they can orchestrate brain processes along a common goal. While the percentage of cortical neurons is 19% in primates, the corresponding numbers are 62% and 78% in parrots and songbirds, respectively [38,40]. Consequently, rooks and marmosets have identical brain weights but rooks have three times more pallial neurons. Within 111 bird species, pallial neuron numbers were determined to be the best predictors of complex cognition [5]. So, do large pallial neuron numbers explain the similar cognitive capacities of corvids, parrots, and great apes? Not quite. While parrots and corvids are cognitively on par with apes [14., 15., 16.], their pallial neuron numbers still differ widely (kea: 1.28 billion neurons; raven: 1.2 billion neurons; chimpanzee: 7.4 billion neurons) [40,41]. Thus, high neuron densities and a higher proportion of pallial neurons reduce the quantitative gap between birds and primates without closing it.
However, birds have one more card up their sleeves. Experience-dependent flexible cognition may be especially related to the number of associative neurons that are situated between sensory and motor systems [42]. It was demonstrated that crow species have a selectively greater number of neurons in their associative pallial areas than chickens, pigeons, and ostriches [43]. This was especially visible when corvids were compared with ostriches. Here, crows had twice the number of associative neurons although their brains were 2.5 times and their bodies more than 200 times smaller than those of ostriches. In addition, the number of corvid associative neurons was roughly on par with prefrontal neuron numbers in chimpanzees [41,43]. This comparison is not entirely fair since mammalian associative neurons also reside beyond the PFC, but it highlights the unexpected quantity of associative neurons in crow brains of just 8–10 g. How do birds cope with the huge energy demands of so many neurons? Recently, it was discovered that the energy budget of pigeon neurons is three times lower than that of mammals [44]. So, birds can afford so many pallial neurons because they are fuel efficient. However, as we see later, avian cognition depends not only on the quantities of neurons, but also on their organization.
A new view on the avian forebrain
The last common ancestor of today’s birds and mammals lived about 324 million years ago [45] and over time the two taxa developed vastly different forebrains. In the mammalian lineage, the dorsal pallium developed into the cerebral cortex, the majority of which is isocortical. By contrast, the homologous avian dorsal pallium became the hyperpallium, which has a nuclear appearance. Most of the remaining avian pallial nuclei are located under the lateral ventricle and are jointly called the dorsal ventricular ridge (DVR). This area has no direct counterpart in mammals and its putative homologies to various components of the mammalian pallium are debated (Box 2).
Box 2. Tracing the genetic makeup of the avian pallium.
Across vertebrates, the early embryonic pallium is patterned by the conserved expression of a small number of master developmental regulatory genes [110]. According to the field homology model [111], this patterning subdivides the amniote pallium into four pallial domains: medial (hippocampal formation), dorsal (isocortex), lateral (claustrum, insula), and ventral (olfactory cortex, pallial amygdala) (see Figure 2B in the main text). These partitions are seen as highly conserved such that homologies between structures in, say, mammals and birds can be determined by their location in the same embryonal pallial field [112]. The alternative model is the cell type/circuit homology model, which assumes that homology can be traced back to specific neurons and neuronal circuits of an amniote ancestor and can be identified in extant species by their cellular identities [113]. As a result, structures as diverse as laminated isocortex and nuclear DVR can harbor homologous neuronal systems by virtue of cellular homologies [47,114]. It is easy to see that these models are quite often at odds with each other: While the field homology model sees, for example, the NCL as part of the avian amygdala since it is part of the ventral pallium, the cell type/circuit homology model could easily see it as cortical [47,115]. Colquitt et al. [116] offered a solution by studying neurons in two nuclei of the song system that are both located in the DVR. According to the field homology model, they should be part of the ventral pallium and therefore cannot be cortical. When studying the expression of genes that encode transcription factors of these songbird glutamatergic neurons, the authors discovered strong similarities to the mammalian ventral pallium. If, however, activity-related effector genes that are more closely related to cellular functions were studied, strong parallels to cortical neurons became visible. This finding shows that pallial domains do not severely restrict the developmental trajectory of cells. Thus, cortex-equivalent systems can emerge from outside the dorsal pallium [117]. An extreme example is the finding of Stacho et al. [56], who discovered a fully laminated avian sensory isocortex with iteratively repeated canonical circuits in the avian lateral and ventral pallium. This finding implies that the embryonic location of neurons in a certain pallial domain must neither predict the morphology nor the function of a neuronal system and thus cannot be used to determine the adult form.
Alt-text: Box 2
These major differences between mammals and birds are reflected by the overall pallial pattern. While the topographic arrangement of functional cortical areas in mammals can essentially be projected on an ‘unfolded’ 2D cortical surface, this is not possible with the 3D architecture of the avian pallium. In addition, the locations of the avian functional pallial areas differ both topographically and topologically from those of mammals [46,47]. Despite such major differences, the pigeon pallial connectome with its small-world network, its hierarchical arrangement of specialized modules, and its densely interconnected core of central hub nodes strongly resembles its mammalian counterparts [48,49]. Thus, the dynamics of information flow within macroscale circuitries are remarkably similar, although the pallial architectures differ importantly. However, many species, regardless of cognitive aptitude, have a universal set of features like specialized modules, high topological integration, and short pathways that are driven by the evolutionary pressure for efficient systems communication [50,51]. Thus, similarities between avian and mammalian pallial macroscale connectomes may be insufficient to explain cognitive parallels. What is needed are analyses in which the pallial connectomes of species from the same vertebrate class with high and low cognitive prowess are contrasted.
The absence of cortical architecture was always seen as a major primitive hallmark of the avian pallium. However, local tracing studies in the past years showed layer- and column-like patterns in several sensory pallial areas and started to cast doubt on the idea that the bird pallium is completely devoid of isocortex-like areas [52., 53., 54., 55.]. A combination of 3D polarized light imaging and various tracing techniques showed that the entire avian sensory pallium is characterized by layers and columns, which are embedded into an orthogonal pattern of horizontal and vertical fibers [56]. This sensory ‘isocortex’ of birds constitutes more than half of the pallial volume and encompasses all sensory areas of the hyperpallium and DVR. Detailed reconstructions of connectivity patterns within these areas revealed iteratively repeated columnar canonical circuits that are highly similar across all sensory domains and nearly identical in barn owls and pigeons, two phylogenetically distant species [57]. In addition, this avian ‘isocortex’ harbors long-range axonal systems that horizontally link columns and connect them to hippocampal, ‘prefrontal’, and (pre)motor areas (Figure 2A). Thus, mesoscale circuitries in the avian ‘isocortex’ also resemble the mammalian isocortex. It is, however, important to note that these avian ‘isocortical’ mesoscale circuitries are visible only in the sensory pallial areas and do not comprise associative and motor pallial structures, which all display a nuclear arrangement.
Our knowledge of microscale circuitries is far less detailed. The connections within avian ‘isocortical’ canonical circuits resemble those of mammals but display two important differences (Figure 2, C1–C3). First, while the main thalamocortical input layer IV in mammals receives only limited feedback from other cortical layers [9], the avian thalamopallial input lamina receives massive feedback [56]. This could potentially change the computational dynamics in the avian canonical circuit. Second, the avian ‘isocortex’ in the DVR has no layer that provides descending output systems like cortical layers V/VI, but funnels its descending output to the arcopallium, a nuclear structure, from where subpallial projections emerge [56]. Finally, we know little about the microcircuitries of the nuclear pallial areas of the DVR. As we see later, they play the most important role in cognition.
An avian ‘prefrontal area’
What kind of role do these nucleus-shaped pallial areas play in cognition? Surprisingly, the most posterior part of these non-isocortical pallial areas harbors the nidopallium caudolaterale (NCL), which constitutes the functional equivalent of the mammalian PFC. Both the NCL and the PFC are involved in virtually all aspects of cognition (Figure 3, Key figure), show the highest density of dopaminergic innervation in the pallium, and connect with all associative and premotor structures of the pallium [14,58]. The representation of information in the PFC can best be understood as a flexible code for relevant information [59,60]. The same holds for the NCL, where sensory information is represented in a categorical manner [61]; for instance ‘long’ versus ‘short’ [62] or numerical categories [63,64], even including the numerosity zero [65]. Another typical prefrontal function is the representation of abstract rules [66] regardless of whether they are presented as stimuli [67,68] or implicit and derived from feedback on errors [69]. Unsurprisingly for a structure at the apex between perception and sensation, prefrontal and NCL neurons can present stimuli in a retrospective (see later) or a prospective [70,71] manner depending on task demands and which representational format is more efficient [72]. Last, elusive signals indicative of executive control [73] or even sensory consciousness have been found to be coded in the NCL [74,75].
A key prefrontal function is working memory. It lies at the core of cognition since it maintains information for active processing [76,77]. In other words, we hold in working memory what we are thinking about. This memory system relies on neural activity, which allows direct investigations of working memories and their representations using, for instance, neurophysiological recordings [78]. Several lines of evidence demonstrate that the NCL is involved in working memory, including lesion, pharmacological, and neurophysiological experiments [79]. For instance, single-cell recordings in the PFC and NCL during working memory paradigms reveal ‘delay activity’, increased neural activity related to a memorized stimulus [80., 81., 82.]. Working memory capacity is often seen as a proxy for cognitive capacity. Working memory capacity is comparable between birds and mammals [83., 84., 85.] and the underlying pattern of information loss is very similar between the PFC and NCL and in crows [86]. Recent advances in recording and analysis techniques changed the models of delay activity in the primate PFC [87]. We now know that it shows dynamic temporal patterns within a trial – only when averaging multiple trials does it appear continuously sustained. This pattern is found not only in single neurons but also in local field potentials (LFPs); working memory-related increases in LFP power occur in short bursts and are not continuous across the memory delay [88]. A more detailed analysis of these bursts further revealed that gamma bursts are associated with bottom-up processes such as the encoding of novel stimuli into working memory; beta bursts, by contrast, are more involved in top-down processes and inhibit gamma [87]. We recently found strong evidence for these dynamic processes also in the NCL of crows, supporting the notion that, even in the absence of cortical layering, the microcircuits of avian cognition produce comparable dynamics of neural activity along with similar neurophysiological fingerprints, not just in single neurons but also in the local sum of electrical activity – the LFP [89]. How do these dynamics unfold over the whole brain? Pigeon dreams permit us to see that.
When pigeons dream
Cognition in mammals emerges from a cortical network that is activated during tasks and connects nodes with various subfunctions that continuously share information [90,91]. Thus, cognition is not supported by highly specialized neurons in one specific brain region but by functional networks that are transiently and context-dependently activated [92]. To understand the neural fundaments of avian cognition, we therefore must switch from studying singular areas to studying pallial networks.
To this end, an functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI)-based operant Go/NoGo paradigm for pigeons was run under ultrahigh magnetic field conditions with close to zero motion artifacts over hundreds of trials [93]. During a color discrimination task, they could disambiguate neural networks that were active during the different trial components, such as when seeing the Go stimulus or when inhibiting their own response during NoGo trials. This novel method can uncover avian neural networks during all critical task components. Recently, the same approach was used to identify brain activations in naturally sleeping pigeons during rapid eye movement (REM) and non-REM sleep [94]. In humans, REM sleep is associated with widespread neural activity patterns of internally generated sensory activity that is often associated with vivid, bizarre, and emotional dreams [95]. As in humans, blood-oxygen level-dependent (BOLD) activity during REM sleep in pigeons was found in limbic and premotor areas as well as in visual and multimodal pallial regions. A detailed analysis of both brainstem and telencephalic BOLD signals evinced activations in areas that process optic flow during self-motion with additional local motion signals of moving objects [96]. Furthermore, cerebellar areas that process tactile input from the wings and the overall body as well as pallial areas for frontal and lateral vision were active [97., 98., 99.]. Obviously, the interpretation of these and further BOLD signals remains currently speculative. However, it is tempting to venture the idea that the pigeons might have dreamed about flying while avoiding accidents with other objects.
These studies also show that, in birds, cognitive processes go along with transient activations of widespread neural networks. Although these activated networks resemble those in mammals, some differences become visible. Most importantly and different from mammals, both during REM and non-REM sleep pigeons evinced no hippocampal activation [94]. In mammals, the interplay of the hippocampus and cortex during sleep consolidates memories that are at least partly stored at the cortical level, with the different sleep cycles having diverse contributions [100]. Key elements of this process like hippocampal sharp-wave ripples during non-REM sleep, thalamocortical spindles, and a hippocampal theta rhythm during REM sleep are absent in birds [101., 102., 103.]. In addition, the hippocampus–prefrontal connection that enables memory consolidation and context-dependent memory retrieval does not exist in birds [48]. Thus, in sharp contrast to the discovered similarities between birds and mammals, it is conceivable that birds found a different way to consolidate and retrieve memories. This alternative pathway seems to perform exceptionally well. Ravens plan for the future, and food-caching birds hold memory records among non-human animals, are naturally selected for their memory performance, and depend on their hippocampus to remember their caches [11,104,105].
Concluding remarks
We began with a simple question: what constitutes a smart brain? The first answer was size, and it proved to be wrong [38,40]. A more likely answer is large numbers of associative pallial neurons that constitute computational units akin to hidden layers in deep neural networks (Figure 3).
The second answer was the isocortex and this was also wrong, at least partly. The mammalian isocortex derives from the dorsal pallium and contains all sensory, associative, and motor areas (Box 1). Birds also have an ‘isocortex’, but it contains only the sensory and not the associative and motor areas [56]. In addition, the DVR, which derives from the lateral and ventral pallia, also constitutes parts of the avian ‘isocortex’. These DVR components of the avian ‘isocortex’ (lateral and ventral pallium) can therefore not be homologous to the mammalian isocortex (dorsal pallium) but must have evolved convergently to process sensory input, thereby possibly representing a hard-to-replace mechanism for this kind of neural computation (Figure 2A,B). Possibly, an isocortex is an optimal neural architecture for the representation of the sensory maps from the retina, cochlea, and body surface. However, since critical parts of avian cognitive functions are processed in the nucleus-shaped NCL, an isocortex seems to be dispensable for complex cognition.
Both mammals and birds have an associative prefrontal area that is densely innervated by dopaminergic fibers to provide feedback signals for successful actions and for flexible selection of relevant information. Since the NCL is part of the DVR, the NCL and PFC cannot be homologous but result from convergent evolution. Since crocodiles also have a tiny NCL with dense dopaminergic innervation [106,107], the NCL and PFC seem to have evolved in parallel in mammals and archosaurs. It might also exist with similar key properties in other kinds of species that underwent evolutionary selection for complex cognition.
Finally, working memory as a representation of flexible cognitive capacity shows overlapping neuronal temporal patterns. This makes it likely that similar meso- and microcircuits with the same dynamic neurophysiological fingerprints have convergently evolved in birds and mammals. All of these components possibly also represent hard-to-replace mechanisms.
Obviously, this is a speculative list of neural features that characterize convergently evolved neural systems for complex cognition in birds and mammals. This collection and the concepts from which they originate need thorough empirical testing in a large variety of species. We are confident that our collection of critical features (Figure 3) is not exhaustive and further components will be added over time (Box 3). To study these and other endeavors is not easy, but its reward could be the discovery of universal neural mechanisms for cognitive prowess. If indeed nature faces limited degrees of freedom to evolve smart brains, we should be able to identify such common neural patterns across a wide variety of species (see Outstanding questions).
Box 3. Putative further candidates for complex cognition.
A potential future candidate for complex cognition is the cerebellum, which is tightly connected to cognitive processes in humans [118]. Parrots not only have excellent cognition but also a drastically enlarged avian-specific midbrain structure that serves as a link between the arcopallium, a premotor structure, and the cerebellum [119]. This corresponds to the enlargement of the mammalian pontine nuclei, which are especially large in great apes. That said, parrots and corvids do not have a larger cerebellum than other birds [120], making it possible that the avian cerebellar contribution to cognition is differently organized than in primates.
A further candidate is the high body temperature of 40–42°C of birds [121,122]. This is considerably warmer than the 36–37°C of eutherian mammals like rats and humans [123]. This temperature difference could potentially be another factor that makes birds smart. Temperature has multiple effects on neural function. It acts on biophysical properties of neurons such as membrane potentials, synaptic transmission, and channel kinetics and has a positive impact on conduction velocity, energy consumption, and refraction [124,125]. These effects are large enough to produce significant benefits to neural processing. For example, warming the eyes of swordfish results in higher temporal resolution of vision [126] and brain temperature is a controlling factor in the speed of birdsong [127]. Furthermore, higher temperatures reduce the energetic costs of action potentials [128] and, along with the small size of neurons, this might explain the overall low energy consumption of the avian brain. This low energy consumption in turn might explain why birds can afford high neuron numbers and generate the fast action potentials and the high firing rates that are especially beneficial to small neurons [44,129].
Alt-text: Box 3
Outstanding questions.
What are the neural networks of avian cognition? To reveal these, we need techniques like imaging with which transient activity patterns of the brain can be tracked while birds are actively pursuing a task.
How do birds make memories? Crucial components of the neural systems with which mammals store, consolidate, and retrieve information seem to differ in birds, while their memory capacity is comparable with that of mammals.
What are the cellular and computational dynamics within the avian ‘isocortical’ canonical circuits? Research on this point is currently in its infancy but will be crucial to understand the neural fundaments of sensory processing and beyond.
How are the neuronal networks in the nuclear ‘prefrontal’ NCL and motor arcopallium organized? Although a wealth of studies has revealed single-unit properties of NCL neurons during various tasks, the network structure of these nuclear areas is unknown in both birds and reptiles.
How do cortical/pallial connectomes of comparable species with high and low cognitive prowess differ? Such a research avenue could provide important insights into the network structure of cognition.
We hypothesize that large numbers of associative pallial neurons, a prefrontal-like pallial structure, dense dopaminergic innervation of associative pallial areas, and a highly dynamic neuronal fundament of working memory are essential neural components of complex cognition, while an isocortex is not. Can this conclusion be generalized, possibly even outside the realm of vertebrates?
Alt-text: Outstanding questions
Acknowledgments
Acknowledgments
Supported by the project AVIAN MIND, ERC-2020-ADG, LS5, GA No. 101021354 (O.G.) and funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation) Project ID 316803389 – SFB 1280 Project A01 (O.G. and R.P.) and Project A 19 (J.R.). We are grateful to Katrin Amunts, Mary Flaim, and Kaya von Eugen for all their smart ideas that enriched the manuscript during the process of writing.
Declaration of interests
No interests are declared.
Glossary
- Connectome
a network of connections between synapses, neurons, brain areas, or axonal fiber systems.
- Convergent evolution
describes a case in which two species share a feature of similar appearance or function that has evolved independently due to similar selection pressures. It is important to note that this feature was not present in the last common ancestor of these two species.
- Homology
defines a case in which a shared attribute of two species can be traced back to a common ancestor without interruption.
- Hub node
a densely interconnected node that has an above average amount of connections to other nodes.
- Isocortex
the six-layered sheet of gray matter with mostly similar cytoarchitecture that creates the outermost part of the cerebrum of the mammalian brain and constitutes in primates more than 90% of the cerebral cortex.
- Local field potential (LFP)
an electrical potential that is recorded with electrodes within the brain and that shows the transient summed cellular activity of nearby neuronal populations.
- Modules
segregated network communities that promote specialized and local information processing.
- Pallium
the upper part of the cerebrum that incorporates the cortex or cortex-homolog structures as well as the hippocampus, pallial amygdala, olfactory bulb, and claustrum.
- Small-world network
a network that combines the ability of random networks to have short connections to some distant nodes with the high local clustering of nearby elements that is typical of regular networks. Thus, a small-world network is both globally and locally efficient.
- Topological integration
a network that enables short pathways between distant network components by a densely connected core of hub nodes.
References
- 1.Dicke U., Roth G. Neuronal factors determining high intelligence. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 2016;371 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Deaner R.O., et al. Overall brain size, and not encephalization quotient, best predicts cognitive ability across non-human primates. Brain Behav. Evol. 2007;70:115–124. doi: 10.1159/000102973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Marino L., et al. Cetaceans have complex brains for complex cognition. PLoS Biol. 2009;5 doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0050139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Cnotka J., et al. Extraordinary large brains in tool-using Caledonian crows (Corvus moneduloides) Neurosci. Lett. 2008;433:241–245. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2008.01.026. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Sol D., et al. Neuron numbers link innovativeness with both absolute and relative brain size in birds. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 2022;6:1381–1389. doi: 10.1038/s41559-022-01815-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Collado M.Á., et al. Brain size predicts learning abilities in bees. R. Soc. Open Sci. 2021;8 doi: 10.1098/rsos.201940. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Herculano-Houzel S. Numbers of neurons as biological correlates of cognitive capability. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2017;16:1–7. [Google Scholar]
- 8.Luhmann H.J. Dynamics of neocortical networks: connectivity beyond the canonical microcircuit. Pflugers Arch. 2023;475:1027–1033. doi: 10.1007/s00424-023-02830-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Larkum M. A cellular mechanism for cortical associations: an organizing principle for the cerebral cortex. Trends Neurosci. 2013;36:141–151. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2012.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Pepperberg I. Harper; 2009. Alex & Me: How a Scientist and a Parrot Discovered a Hidden World of Animal Intelligence – and Formed a Deep Bond in the Process. [Google Scholar]
- 11.Kabadayi C., Osvath M. Ravens parallel great apes in flexible planning for tool-use and bartering. Science. 2017;357:202–204. doi: 10.1126/science.aam8138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Gruber R., et al. New Caledonian crows use mental representations to solve metatool problems. Curr. Biol. 2019;29:686–692. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.01.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Klump B.C., et al. Innovation and geographic spread of a complex foraging culture in an urban parrot. Science. 2021;373:456–460. doi: 10.1126/science.abe7808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Güntürkün O., Bugnyar T. Cognition without cortex. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016;20:291–303. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.02.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Lambert M.L., et al. Birds of a feather? Parrot and corvid cognition compared. Behaviour. 2019;156:505–594. [Google Scholar]
- 16.Pika S., et al. Ravens parallel great apes in physical and social cognitive skills. Sci. Rep. 2020;10:20617. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-77060-8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Scarf D., et al. Pigeons on par with primates in numerical competence. Science. 2011;334:1664. doi: 10.1126/science.1213357. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Pollok B., et al. Development of object-permanence in the food-storing magpie (Pica pica) J. Comp. Psychol. 2000;114:148–157. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.114.2.148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Ujfalussy D.J., et al. Ontogeny of object permanence in a non-storing corvid species, the jackdaw (Corvus monedula) Anim. Cogn. 2013;16:405–416. doi: 10.1007/s10071-012-0581-z. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Jacobs I., et al. The development of sensorimotor cognition in common ravens (Corvus corax) and its comparative evolution. Anim. Behav. Cogn. 2019;6:194–212. [Google Scholar]
- 21.Kabadayi C., et al. The development of motor self-regulation in ravens. Front. Psychol. 2017;8:2100. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.02100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Rugani R., et al. Animal cognition. Number-space mapping in the newborn chick resembles humans’ mental number line. Science. 2015;347:534–536. doi: 10.1126/science.aaa1379. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Dehaene S., et al. The mental representation of parity and number magnitude. J. Exp. Psychol. 1993;122:371–396. [Google Scholar]
- 24.de Hevia M.D., et al. At birth, humans associate "few" with left and "many" with right. Curr. Biol. 2017;27:3879–3884.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2017.11.024. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Scarf D., et al. Orthographic processing in pigeons (Columba livia) Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2016;113:11272–11276. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1607870113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Bastos A.P.M., Taylor A.H. Kea show three signatures of domain-general statistical inference. Nat. Commun. 2020;11:828. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-14695-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Wasserman E.A., et al. Resolving the associative learning paradox by category learning in pigeons. Curr. Biol. 2023;33:1112–1116.e2. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2023.01.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Daisley J.N., et al. Low-rank Gallus gallus domesticus chicks are better at transitive inference reasoning. Commun. Biol. 2021;4:1344. doi: 10.1038/s42003-021-02855-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Hillemacher S., et al. Roosters do not warn the bird in the mirror: the cognitive ecology of mirror self-recognition. PLoS One. 2023;18 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0291416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Güntürkün O., et al. Apes, feathered apes, and pigeons: differences and similarities. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2017;16:35–40. [Google Scholar]
- 31.Wright A.A. Functional relationships for investigating cognitive processes. Behav. Process. 2013;93:4–24. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2012.11.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Wright A.A., Katz J.S. Mechanisms of same/different concept learning in primates and avians. Behav. Process. 2006;72:234–254. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2006.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Vanderveldt A., et al. Delay discounting: pigeon, rat, human – does it matter? J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Learn. Cogn. 2016;42:141–162. doi: 10.1037/xan0000097. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Elmore L.C., et al. Change detection by rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta) and pigeons (Columba livia) J. Comp. Psychol. 2012;126:203–212. doi: 10.1037/a0026356. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Emery N.J., Clayton N.S. The mentality of crows: convergent evolution of intelligence in corvids and apes. Science. 2004;306:1903–1907. doi: 10.1126/science.1098410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Manger P.R., et al. The evolutions of large brain size in mammals: the ‘over-700-gram’ club quartet. Brain Behav. Evol. 2013;82:68–78. doi: 10.1159/000352056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Güntürkün O. Is dolphin cognition special? Brain Behav. Evol. 2014;83:177–180. doi: 10.1159/000357551. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kverková K., et al. The evolution of brain neuron numbers in amniotes. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2022;119 doi: 10.1073/pnas.2121624119. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Handley W.D., Worthy T.H. Endocranial anatomy of the giant extinct Australian mihirung birds (Aves, Dromornithidae) Diversity. 2021;13:124. [Google Scholar]
- 40.Olkowicz S., et al. Birds have primate-like numbers of neurons in the forebrain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2016;113:7255–7260. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1517131113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Collins C.E., et al. Cortical cell and neuron density estimates in one chimpanzee hemisphere. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2016;1137:740–745. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1524208113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Mehlhorn J., et al. Tool making New Caledonian crows have larger associative brain areas. Brain Behav. Evol. 2010;75:63–70. doi: 10.1159/000295151. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Ströckens F., et al. High associative neuron numbers could drive cognitive performance in corvid species. J. Comp. Neurol. 2022;530:1588–1605. doi: 10.1002/cne.25298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.von Eugen K., et al. Avian neurons consume three times less glucose compared to mammals. Curr. Biol. 2022;23:4306–4313. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2022.07.070. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.dos Reis M., et al. Uncertainty in the timing of origin of animals and the limits of precision in molecular timescales. Curr. Biol. 2015;25:2939–2950. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.09.066. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Mouritsen H., et al. The neural basis of long-distance navigation in birds. Annu. Rev. Physiol. 2016;78:133–154. doi: 10.1146/annurev-physiol-021115-105054. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Jarvis E.D., et al. Global view of the functional molecular organization of the avian cerebrum: mirror images and functional columns. J. Comp. Neurol. 2013;521 doi: 10.1002/cne.23404. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Shanahan M., et al. Large-scale network organization in the avian forebrain: a connectivity matrix and theoretical analysis. Front. Comput. Neurosci. 2013;7:89. doi: 10.3389/fncom.2013.00089. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Goulas A., et al. A blueprint of mammalian cortical connectomes. PLoS Biol. 2019;17 doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.2005346. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.van den Heuvel M.P., et al. Comparative connectomics. Trends Cogn. Sci. 2016;20:345–361. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2016.03.001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Suárez R., Halley A.C. Evolution of developmental timing as a driving force of brain diversity. Brain Behav. Evol. 2022;97:3–7. doi: 10.1159/000524334. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Wang Y., et al. Laminar and columnar auditory cortex in avian brain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2010;107:12676. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1006645107. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Ahumada-Galleguillos P., et al. Anatomical organization of the visual dorsal ventricular ridge in the chick (Gallus gallus): layers and columns in the avian pallium. J. Comp. Neurol. 2015;523:2618–2636. doi: 10.1002/cne.23808. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Fernández M., et al. Parallel organization of the avian sensorimotor arcopallium: tectofugal visual pathway in the pigeon (Columba livia) J. Comp. Neurol. 2020;528:597–623. doi: 10.1002/cne.24775. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Fernández M., et al. A canonical interlaminar circuit in the sensory dorsal ventricular ridge of birds: the anatomical organization of the trigeminal pallium. J. Comp. Neurol. 2021;529:3410–3428. doi: 10.1002/cne.25201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Stacho M., et al. A cortex-like canonical circuit in the avian forebrain. Science. 2020;369 doi: 10.1126/science.abc5534. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Jarvis E.D., et al. Whole-genome analyses resolve early branches in the tree of life of modern birds. Science. 2014;346:1320–1331. doi: 10.1126/science.1253451. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Güntürkün O., et al. Avian pallial circuits and cognition: a comparison to mammals. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2021;71:29–36. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2021.08.007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Fusi S., et al. Why neurons mix: high dimensionality for higher cognition. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2016;37:66–74. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2016.01.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Miller E.K., Cohen J.D. An integrative theory of prefrontal cortex function. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2001;24:167–202. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.167. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Güntürkün O., et al. The neuroscience of perceptual categorization in pigeons: a mechanistic hypothesis. Learn. Behav. 2018;46:229–241. doi: 10.3758/s13420-018-0321-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Wagener L., Nieder A. Categorical representation of abstract spatial magnitudes in the executive telencephalon of crows. Curr. Biol. 2023;33:2151–2162.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2023.04.013. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Ditz H.M., Nieder A. Neurons selective to the number of visual items in the corvid songbird endbrain. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2015;112:7827–7832. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1504245112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Ditz H.M., Nieder A. Numerosity representations in crows obey the Weber–Fechner law. Proc. Biol. Sci. 2016;283 doi: 10.1098/rspb.2016.0083. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Kirschhock M.E., et al. Behavioral and neuronal representation of numerosity zero in the crow. J. Neurosci. 2021;41:4889–4896. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0090-21.2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Wallis J.D., et al. Single neurons in prefrontal cortex encode abstract rules. Nature. 2001;411:953–956. doi: 10.1038/35082081. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Moll F.W., Nieder A. The long and the short of it: rule-based relative length discrimination in carrion crows, Corvus corone. Behav. Process. 2014;107:142–149. doi: 10.1016/j.beproc.2014.08.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Veit L., Nieder A. Abstract rule neurons in the endbrain support intelligent behaviour in corvid songbirds. Nat. Commun. 2013;4:2878. doi: 10.1038/ncomms3878. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Hahn L.A., Rose J. Executive control of sequence behavior in pigeons involves two distinct brain regions. eNeuro. 2023;10 doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0296-22.2023. ENEURO.0296-22.2023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Packheiser J., et al. Trial-by-trial dynamics of reward prediction error-associated signals during extinction learning and renewal. Prog. Neurobiol. 2021;197 doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2020.101901. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Veit L., et al. Associative learning rapidly establishes neuronal representations of upcoming behavioral choices in crows. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2015;112:15208–15213. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1509760112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Colombo M., et al. Prospective processing: behavioural and neural evidence. Jpn J. Animal Psychol. 2017;67:47–61. [Google Scholar]
- 73.Rose J., Colombo M. Neural correlates of executive control in the avian brain. PLoS Biol. 2005;3 doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Nieder A. Consciousness without cortex. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2021;71:69–76. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2021.09.010. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Nieder A., et al. A neural correlate of sensory consciousness in a corvid bird. Science. 2020;369:1626–1629. doi: 10.1126/science.abb1447. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Adam K.C.S., Serences J.T. Working memory: flexible but finite. Neuron. 2019;103:184–185. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.06.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Baddeley A. Working memory: looking back and looking forward. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2003;4:829–839. doi: 10.1038/nrn1201. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Nieder A. Inside the corvid brain – probing the physiology of cognition in crows. Curr. Opin. Behav. Sci. 2017;16:8–14. [Google Scholar]
- 79.Güntürkün O. Evolution of cognitive neural structures. Psychol. Res. 2012;76:212–219. doi: 10.1007/s00426-011-0377-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Diekamp B., et al. Working memory neurons in pigeons. J. Neurosci. 2002;22:RC210. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-04-j0002.2002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81.Fuster J.M., Alexander G.E. Neuron activity related to short-term memory. Science. 1971;173:652–654. doi: 10.1126/science.173.3997.652. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Veit L., et al. Neuronal correlates of visual working memory in the corvid endbrain. J. Neurosci. 2014;34:7778–7786. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0612-14.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Balakhonov D., Rose J. Crows rival monkeys in cognitive capacity. Sci. Rep. 2017;7:8809. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-09400-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Hahn L.A., Rose J. Working memory as an indicator for comparative cognition – detecting qualitative and quantitative differences. Front. Psychol. 2020;11:1954. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01954. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Johnson M.K., et al. The relationship between working memory capacity and broad measures of cognitive ability in healthy adults and people with schizophrenia. Neuropsychology. 2013;27:220–229. doi: 10.1037/a0032060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Hahn L.A., et al. Working memory capacity of crows and monkeys arises from similar neuronal computations. Elife. 2021;10 doi: 10.7554/eLife.72783. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Miller E.K., et al. Working memory 2.0. Neuron. 2018;100:463–475. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2018.09.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Lundqvist M., et al. Gamma and beta bursts underlie working memory. Neuron. 2016;90:152–164. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.02.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Hahn L.A., et al. Oscillations without cortex: working memory modulates brainwaves in the endbrain of crows. Prog. Neurobiol. 2022;219 doi: 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2022.102372. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.Sporns O., Betzel R.F. Modular brain networks. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2016;67:613–640. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-122414-033634. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Bazinet V., et al. Multiscale communication in cortico-cortical networks. Neuroimage. 2021;243 doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2021.118546. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92.Herbet G., Duffau H. Revisiting the functional anatomy of the human brain: toward a meta-networking theory of cerebral functions. Physiol. Rev. 2020;100:1181–1228. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00033.2019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93.Behroozi M., et al. Event-related functional MRI of awake behaving pigeons at 7T. Nat. Commun. 2020;11:4715. doi: 10.1038/s41467-020-18437-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94.Ungurean G., et al. Wide-spread brain activation and reduced CSF flow during avian REM sleep. Nat. Commun. 2023;14:3259. doi: 10.1038/s41467-023-38669-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95.Chow H.M., et al. Rhythmic alternating patterns of brain activity distinguish rapid eye movement sleep from other states of consciousness. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U. S. A. 2013;110:10300–10305. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1217691110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 96.Wylie D.R., et al. Visual–cerebellar pathways and their roles in the control of avian flight. Front. Neurosci. 2018;12:223. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2018.00223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97.Remy M., Güntürkün O. Retinal afferents of the tectum opticum and the nucleus opticus principalis thalami in the pigeon. J. Comp. Neurol. 1991;305:57–70. doi: 10.1002/cne.903050107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98.Güntürkün O., Hahmann U. Functional subdivisions of the ascending visual pathways in the pigeon. Behav. Brain Res. 1999;98:193–201. doi: 10.1016/s0166-4328(98)00084-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99.Gutiérrez-Ibáñez C., et al. Pretecto- and ponto-cerebellar pathways to the pigeon oculomotor cerebellum follow a zonal organization. J. Comp. Neurol. 2022;530:817–833. doi: 10.1002/cne.25247. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100.Klinzing J.G., et al. Mechanisms of systems memory consolidation during sleep. Nat. Neurosci. 2019;22:1598–1610. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0467-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101.van der Meij J., et al. Intra-“cortical” activity during avian non-REM and REM sleep: variant and invariant traits between birds and mammals. Sleep. 2019;42 doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsy230. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 102.van der Meij J., et al. Divergent neuronal activity patterns in the avian hippocampus and nidopallium. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2020;52:3124–3139. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14675. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103.Rattenborg N.C., et al. Hippocampal memory consolidation during sleep: a comparison of mammals and birds. Biol. Rev. Camb. Philos. Soc. 2011;86:658–691. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.2010.00165.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104.Sherry D.F. Neuroecology. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2006;57:167–197. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105.Sonnenberg B.R., et al. Natural selection and spatial cognition in wild food-caching mountain chickadees. Curr. Biol. 2019;29:670–676. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2019.01.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106.Wynne B., Güntürkün O. The dopaminergic innervation of the forebrain of the pigeon (Columba livia): a study with antibodies against tyrosine hydroxylase and dopamine. J. Comp. Neurol. 1995;357:446–464. doi: 10.1002/cne.903570309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107.Billings B.K., et al. Nuclear organization and morphology of catecholaminergic neurons and certain pallial terminal networks in the brain of the Nile crocodile, Crocodylus niloticus. J. Chem. Neuroanat. 2020;109 doi: 10.1016/j.jchemneu.2020.101851. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108.Herculano-Houzel S. The human brain in numbers: a linearly scaled-up primate brain. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 2009;3:31. doi: 10.3389/neuro.09.031.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109.Zilles K., et al. In: Brain Mapping: an Encyclopedic Reference. Toga A.W., editor. Elsevier; 2015. Cytoarchitecture and maps of the human cerebral cortex; pp. 115–135. [Google Scholar]
- 110.Sugahara F., et al. Reconstructing the ancestral vertebrate brain. Develop. Growth Differ. 2017;59:163–174. doi: 10.1111/dgd.12347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111.Puelles L., et al. In: Evolution of Nervous Systems. 2nd edn. Kaas J., editor. Academic Press; 2017. The pallium in reptiles and birds in the light of the updated tetrapartite pallium model; pp. 519–555. [Google Scholar]
- 112.Puelles L. Current status of the hypothesis of a claustro-insular homolog in sauropsids. Brain Behav. Evol. 2022;96:212–241. doi: 10.1159/000520742. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113.Briscoe S.D., Ragsdale C.W. Homology, neocortex, and the evolution of developmental mechanisms. Science. 2018;362:190–193. doi: 10.1126/science.aau3711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 114.Karten H.J. Vertebrate brains and evolutionary connectomics: on the origins of the mammalian ‘neocortex’. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 2015;370 doi: 10.1098/rstb.2015.0060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115.Medina L., et al. Evolution and development of amygdala subdivisions: pallial, subpallial, and beyond. Brain Behav. Evol. 2023;98:1–21. doi: 10.1159/000527512. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 116.Colquitt B.M., et al. Cellular transcriptomics reveals evolutionary identities of songbird vocal circuits. Science. 2021;371:6530. doi: 10.1126/science.abd9704. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 117.Colquitt B.M. Organizational conservation and flexibility in the evolution of birdsong and avian motor control. Brain Behav. Evol. 2022;96:255–264. doi: 10.1159/000525019. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 118.Schmahmann J.D., et al. The theory and neuroscience of cerebellar cognition. Annu. Rev. Neurosci. 2019;42:337–364. doi: 10.1146/annurev-neuro-070918-050258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 119.Gutiérrez-Ibáñez C., et al. Parrots have evolved a primate-like telencephalic-midbrain-cerebellar circuit. Sci. Rep. 2018;8:9960. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-28301-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 120.Cunha F., et al. A quantitative analysis of cerebellar anatomy in birds. Brain Struct. Funct. 2021;226:2561–2583. doi: 10.1007/s00429-021-02352-2. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 121.Kilgore D.L., et al. Brain temperatures in birds. J. Comp. Physiol. B. 1976;110:209–215. [Google Scholar]
- 122.McNab B.K. An analysis of the body temperatures of birds. Condor. 1966;68:47–55. [Google Scholar]
- 123.Andersen P., Moser E.I. Brain temperature and hippocampal function. Hippocampus. 1995;5:491–498. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450050602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124.Laughlin S.B., Sejnowski T.J. Communication in neuronal networks. Science. 2003;301:1870–1874. doi: 10.1126/science.1089662. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125.Laughlin S.B. Energy as a constraint on the coding and processing of sensory information. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2001;11:475–480. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(00)00237-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126.Fritsches K.A., et al. Warm eyes provide superior vision in swordfishes. Curr. Biol. 2005;15:55–58. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2004.12.064. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 127.Aronov D., Fee M.S. Natural changes in brain temperature underlie variations in song tempo during a mating behavior. PLoS One. 2012;7 doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0047856. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 128.Yu Y., et al. Warm body temperature facilitates energy efficient cortical action potentials. PLoS Comput. Biol. 2012;8 doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002456. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 129.Sengupta B., et al. The effect of cell size and channel density on neuronal information encoding and energy efficiency. J. Cereb. Blood Flow Metab. 2013;33:1465–1473. doi: 10.1038/jcbfm.2013.103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]