Skip to main content

This is a preprint.

It has not yet been peer reviewed by a journal.

The National Library of Medicine is running a pilot to include preprints that result from research funded by NIH in PMC and PubMed.

Research Square logoLink to Research Square
[Preprint]. 2024 Mar 8:rs.3.rs-3977169. [Version 1] doi: 10.21203/rs.3.rs-3977169/v1

Improvements in well-being following naturalistic psychedelic use and underlying mechanisms of change in older adults: A prospective cohort study

Hannes Kettner 1, Leor Roseman 2, Adam Gazzaley 3, Robin Carhart-Harris 4, Lorenzo Pasquini 5
PMCID: PMC10942571  PMID: 38496492

Abstract

Affective symptoms such as anxiety, low mood, and loneliness are prevalent and highly debilitating symptoms among older adults (OA). Serotonergic psychedelics are novel experimental interventions for affective disorders, yet little is known regarding their effects in OA. Using a prospective cohort design, we identified 62 OA (age ≥ 60 years) and 62 matched younger adults (YA) who completed surveys two weeks before, and one day, two weeks, four weeks, and six months after a guided psychedelic group session in a retreat setting. Mixed linear regression analyses revealed significant well-being improvements in OA and YA, amplified in OA with a history of a psychiatric diagnosis. Compared to YA, acute subjective psychedelic effects were attenuated in OA and did not significantly predict well-being changes. However, a psychosocial measure of Communitas emerged as a predictor in OA, suggesting that the relational components in psychedelic group settings may hold particular value for OA.

Keywords: aging, psychedelics, mental health, well-being, cohort study, naturalistic

Introduction

Emotions are central to human functioning, guiding thought and action from the earliest to the latest days of life (Carstensen et al., 1999). Emotional experiences change over the adult life span, with older adults (OA) shifting their motivational goals towards optimizing emotional regulation and reporting positive emotions more often than their younger counterparts (Carstensen et al., 2003) However, affective symptoms, such as anxiety, mood instability, loneliness, and apathy, are common among OA and may herald incipient neuropsychiatric and neurological disorders, such as late-life depression and Alzheimer’s disease (Conwell et al., 2023; Donovan & Blazer, 2020; Donovan et al., 2017; Rosenberg et al., 2013; Steffens, 2012). In particular, loneliness, or the subjective feeling of being socially isolated, has been identified as a major modifiable risk factor for cognitive decline and worsening of mental well-being in OA (Donovan & Blazer, 2020; Donovan et al., 2017; Luo et al., 2012). Loneliness has been shown to spread among social networks, to predict low life satisfaction, depressive symptoms, cognitive impairments, and Alzheimer’s disease dementia (Hawkley & Cacioppo, 2010), highlighting the importance of nurturing healthy social connections in the elderly. Crucially, there is convergent evidence that conventional antidepressants – including selective serotonin reuptake inhibitors – are less effective in treating affective symptoms in OA patient populations (Banerjee et al., 2011; Rosenberg et al., 2010). On the contrary, their use has been associated with an increased incidence of adverse respiratory and gastrointestinal events, as well as emotional blunting when compared to placebo (Banerjee et al., 2011; Carhart-Harris et al., 2021; Rosenberg et al., 2010), highlighting the need for improved treatments for affective symptoms in OA.

Serotonergic psychedelics, such as psilocybin (contained in “magic mushrooms and truffles”), lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD), N, N-Dimethyltryptamine (DMT, the main ingredients of the Amazonian brew ayahuasca), and mescaline, have recently garnered increasing research interest, following several clinical trials suggesting the therapeutic potential for these substances in the treatment of affective symptoms across various neuropsychiatric disorders (Andersen et al., 2021). Acute psychedelic effects are induced though the strong affinity of these substances for the serotonin 2A receptor (Vollenweider & Smallridge, 2022; Vollenweider et al., 1998); they include an altered state of consciousness characterised by intensified affect, vivified imagination and imagery, multisensory changes in perception, distorted sense of time, perceived spiritual and mystical experiences, and facilitated psychological insight. These latter effects, especially, have been indicated as mediators of therapeutic responses to psychedelics (Haijen et al., 2018; Kangaslampi, 2020; Roseman et al., 2019; Roseman et al., 2018).

Importantly, a recent review found that among 1400 participants enrolled in 36 psychedelic trials since 1967, only 19 participants (1.4%) were 65 years or older (Bouchet et al., 2024). The safety and efficacy of psychedelic treatments in older populations thus remains largely unknown (Johnston et al., 2023), although several authors have argued for the potential of psychedelics to loosen cognitive habits in old age generally (Pollan, 2019) as well as more formally as treatments for Alzheimer’s disease (Forester et al., 2022; Sarangi & Akinkunmi, 2023; Vann Jones & O’Kelly, 2020; Winkelman et al., 2023), including mild cognitive impairment (Steinberg et al., 2023) and even healthy cognitive decline and age-related affective changes (Aday, Bloesch, et al., 2020; Beaussant & Nigam, 2023). Initial clinical trials are currently underway investigating the effects of psychedelics on affective symptoms in patients with Alzheimer’s disease (NCT04123314) and Parkinson’s disease (NCT04932434).

Outside of the highly-controlled environment of clinical trials, psychedelic substances are commonly consumed recreationally, and increasingly also for self-medicative purposes (Bornemann et al., 2021; Mason & Kuypers, 2018; Matzopoulos et al., 2022). This includes guided individual and group settings, sometimes referred to as “ceremonies”, facilitated both underground and, in some countries, legally, e.g. at psychedelic retreats (Rucker & Young, 2021), where emphasis is placed on curating social contexts that foster interpersonal trust, openness, and expression of vulnerability (Kettner et al., 2021). This is typically achieved through encouraging the structured sharing of personal and subjective experiences in group circles before and after psychedelic sessions and by exercising compassionate non-judgemental listening, reflecting some of the principles also employed in group psychotherapy (Callon et al., 2021; Gearin, 2015; Tramacchi, 2000). As such, previous work has shown that psychedelic group settings can enhance psychological well-being and social connectedness by generating a sense of togetherness and self-disclosure within and outside of the acute substance sessions (Kettner et al., 2021).

Prospective observational studies of group ceremonies and other naturalistic psychedelic use can therefore be used to monitor psychological outcomes among psychedelic users in a more ecologically valid fashion and in more diverse populations. For example, a recent study following this approach has shown improved well-being among young adult (YA) and adolescent psychedelic users, as well as age-dependent differences in salutogenic mechanisms (Izmi et al., in prep). Yet, little is known regarding the potential of psychedelic experiences for improving mental health among OA. To investigate this question, we leveraged self-report data from an observational prospective cohort study of participants attending guided psychedelic group sessions (reported on also in Kettner et al., 2021).

Materials and Methods

Study design and participants.

The present study employed a prospective cohort design utilizing an online convenience sample of individuals planning to attend an organised psychedelic retreat or group-based guided psychedelic ceremony session, on their own accord. Recruitment took place via two routes: firstly, through online advertisements on psychedelic-related social media channels (Facebook groups, Twitter), email newsletters, and online forums (e.g., Reddit), and secondly, through retreat facilitators who advertised the study to their prospective clients. Participants were able to review study information online, provided informed consent, and subsequently completed surveys through multiple e-mails sent before and after their planned experience: 1) Within two weeks prior to the session, assessing demographics and baseline scores of mental health related outcome variables; 2) 24 h after the session, assessing acute subjective effects; 3) one day after leaving the ceremony or retreat location, including variables related to the overall experience; 4) two weeks, four weeks, and six months after the experience, measuring changes in the outcome variables. Eligibility criteria included being ≥18 years old, a good comprehension of the English language, and intentions to attend a psychedelic ceremony (i.e., involving use of psilocybin/magic mushrooms/truffles, LSD, ayahuasca, DMT/5-MeO-DMT, mescaline, or iboga/ibogaine). The study was approved by the Imperial College London’s Research Ethics Committee (ICREC) and the Joint Research Compliance Office (JRCO). For a full overview of the study design, see Kettner (et al., 2021).

Participants were included if they had completed at least the baseline and the 24h post-session survey. OA were identified based on a reported age of ≥60 and a set of matched YA was selected using nearest neighbour matching via the MatchIt package implemented in R (https://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/MatchIt/vignettes/MatchIt.html), matched for gender, education, psychiatric history, previous psychedelic use, baseline well-being, and drug dose.

Measures

Baseline predictors.

At baseline, age, gender, education, extent of prior experience with psychedelic substances, expectations regarding potential beneficial effects of the experience (0–100 visual analogue scale), and self-reported history of psychiatric diagnoses were assessed, as well as basic information regarding the planned experience, such as substance type and location.

Outcome measures.

The Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Wellbeing Scale (WEMWBS) (Tennant et al., 2007)wasassessed at baseline and at the three endpoints two weeks, four weeks, and six months following the session to measures changes in mental well-being.

Acute psychedelic effects.

One day after the psychedelic session, measures of acute psychedelic effects were assessed. These included: (1) the Ego-Dissolution Inventory (EDI) (Nour et al., 2016), a measure reflecting the loss of a subjective experience of the self, which is typically induced by psychedelics, that has participants rate 10 items on a 0–100 scale; (2) the Mystical Experience Questionnaire (MEQ) (Barrett et al., 2015), a 30-item 6-point Likert scale measuring facets of mystical-type and peak experiences; (3) the Challenging Experience Questionnaire (CEQ) (Barrett et al., 2016), a 26-item 6-point Likert scale assessing difficult responses to the drug, such as fear, paranoia, and physiological alterations; (4) the Emotional Breakthrough Inventory (EBI) (Roseman et al., 2019), a six-item scale assessing emotional release and resolution of past trauma; and (5) the Communitas Scale (COMS) a 8-item questionnaire assessing acute relational experiences of togetherness and collective joy during psychedelic group sessions (Kettner et al., 2021).

Post-acute mediators.

One day post-retreat (on the day after leaving the ceremony location), participants completed: (1) the Psychological Insight Scale (PIS), assessing the degree to which the psychedelic experience was perceived as psychologically insightful via six 0–100 visual analogue scale items and (2) a version of the communitas scale (COMSPR), modified to assess relational experiences during the overall retreat, as opposed to the substance session only.

Statistical analyses

Three mixed linear effects models, each including a random intercept, were used to assess changes in WEMWBS scores from baseline to two weeks, four weeks, and six months after the psychedelic session: a first model in OA, including only time point as a fixed effect, a second model in OA to which baseline demographic characteristics and their interaction with time points were added, and a third model comparing longitudinal changes in WEMWBS across OA and matched YA by including the interaction of time points and age group in the fixed portion of the model. Two-tailed paired t-tests were used to further assess significant WEMWEBS changes from baseline to each of the later time points in the whole OA sample and in a subsample of OA with a psychiatric diagnosis.

Next, we used MANCOVA and ANOVA models to compare the intensity of subjective acute psychedelic effects across the OA and YA groups. Additionally, a fourth mixed linear model with OA and matched YA was used to assess a three-way interaction between subjective acute psychedelic effects, age group, and changes in WEMWBS, aiming to expose age-related differences in salutogenic mechanisms. Pseudo-standardised regression coefficients (β) were calculated for this model to facilitate interpretability of the findings. Additional pairwise Pearson’s correlations between WEMWBS change scores from baseline to the four-week endpoint and subjective acute and post-acute psychedelic effects scores were calculated separately for OA and YA to illustrate the three-way interaction results in a simplified manner. A significance level of p<.05 was applied to all statistical tests. Where applicable in case of multiple comparisons, both uncorrected and Bonferroni-corrected levels of significance are reported.

Results

Demographic information

A total of 882 participants that signed up to the study, out of whom 819 provided baseline information. Among 106 participants that reported ≥ 60 years of age, 62 had completed the baseline and also the 24h post-session survey, yielding the final OA sample analysed in our study. Among these 62 OA, 53 completed the post-retreat questionnaire, evaluating experiences across the entire retreat period; 44 completed the two-week, 61 completed the four-week, and 23 completed the six-months endpoints. From a total of 430 adults with age < 60 years who completed at least the baseline and the 24h post-session survey, a set of 62 matched YA were selected using nearest neighbour matching (Table 1). For an overview of demographic information in the full YA sample, see Supplementary Table S1.

Table 1.

Demographic characteristics of the study samples. OA: older adults; SD: Standard Deviation; WEMWBS: Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Wellbeing Scale; YA: younger adults

OA (N = 62) YA matched (N = 62)
Age in years
Mean (SD) 65.1 (4.02) 46.5 (10.0)
Median [Min, Max] 64.0 [60.0, 75.0] 49.0 [24.0, 59.0]
Gender
Male 31 (50.0%) 31 (50.0%)
Female 31 (50.0%) 31 (50.0%)
Other - -
Education/degrees
None 1 (1.6%) 0 (0%)
High school 3 (4.8%) 2 (3.2%)
Technical degree 4 (6.5%) 5 (8.1%)
College diploma 11 (17.7%) 18 (29.0%)
Master 19 (30.6%) 30 (48.4%)
PhD/MD/Law degree 24 (38.7%) 7 (11.3%)
Psychiatric diagnoses
Yes 16 (25.8%) 15 (24.2%)
No 46 (74.2%) 47 (75.8%)
Psychedelic use #
Never 35 (56.5%) 28 (45.2%)
1–5 times 15 (24.2%) 10 (16.1%)
> 5 times 12 (29.3%) 24 (38.7%)
WEMWBS baseline
Mean (SD) 48.5 (7.79) 48.8 (9.79)
Median [Min, Max] 50.0 [31.0, 63.0] 50.5 [20.0, 70.0]
Psychedelic dose
Mean (SD) 2.10 (0.646) 2.13 (0.586)
Median [Min, Max] 2.00 [1.00, 3.00] 2.00 [1.00, 3.00]

The mean age in the identified 62 OA was 65.1 years (SD = 4.02; range = 60–75) and exactly half (31/62, 50.0%) were male. A majority (43/62, 68.5%) of OA had a Master’s degree or higher, no history of diagnosed mental illness (46/62, 74.2%), and no prior experiences with psychedelics (35/62, 56.5%). Among OA who indicated psychiatric diagnoses, the most common were major depressive disorder (10/16) and anxiety disorder (9/16); alcohol dependence and ADHD were indicated by two individuals, respectively; personality, bipolar, and eating disorders by one person each. 59 OA attended psilocybin mushroom or truffle sessions (57 of which took place at retreat centres in the Netherlands or Jamaica), while three individuals indicated ayahuasca as the used psychedelic. Further, a sample of 62 YA (mean age in years (SD) = 46.5(10); range = 24–59) was selected via nearest neighbour matching for comparison purposes.

Post-psychedelic mental health improvements

A mixed effects linear regression model revealed WEMWBS increases in OA following the psychedelic session (Fig. 1A). An average increase of four points on the WEMWBS was found at the two-week endpoint (B = 4.09, 95% CI [1.87, 6.31], p < .001); this remained a three-point increase at the four-week endpoint (B = 3.05, 95% CI [1.04, 5.06], p = .004), indicating meaningful improvements in well-being. At six months post dosing, well-being scores were still nominally elevated by 1.7 units, which did not, however, reach significance (B = 1.72, 95% CI [−1.00, 4.44], p = .22). Paired t-tests comparing endpoint to baseline scores confirmed this pattern, with significant well-being increases at two weeks (p = .006, Cohen’s d = 0.48, t(34)= −2.87) and four weeks (p = .004, Cohen’s d = 0.44 t(45)= −3.01) but not at six months (p = .63, Cohen’s d = 0.10, t(20)=−0.48).

Figure 1. Mental well-being increases in OA following a psychedelic group session.

Figure 1

Violinplots showing the distribution of WEMWBS scores in all OA (A) and OA with a lifetime psychiatric diagnosis only (B)at each time point, with lines reflecting individual trajectories. OA: older adults; WEMWBS: Warwick-Edinburgh Mental Wellbeing Scale; **: p<.01, ***: p<.001

Having established that well-being improves in OA following psychedelic group sessions, we next investigated whether any individual demographic characteristics predicted post-psychedelic changes in mental well-being. We conducted a mixed effects linear regression model including age, gender, education, extent of prior experience with psychedelic substances, expectations on the beneficial effects of psychedelics, and history of mental illness as predictors. This model revealed only an interaction of history of mental illness with time (B = 6.62, 95% CI [1.53, 11.71], p = .019 at two weeks; B = 5.69, 95% CI [1.14, 9.94], p = .016 at four weeks), indicating that well-being increased more drastically in OA reporting a lifetime psychiatric diagnosis (Table 2). Paired t-tests within the subsample of OA with a psychiatric diagnosis (Fig. 1B) revealed significantly increased WEMWBS scores at two weeks (p = .007, Cohen’s d = 1.02, t(10)=−3.42), with an average increase of 9.4 points at four weeks (p = .004, Cohen’s d = 0.89 t(14)=−3.44) but not at six months (p = .48, Cohen’s d = 0.10, t(7)=−0.74).

Table 2.

Main effects and two-way interactions of baseline and demographic variables on wellbeing across time in OA. f: female; SE: Standard Error.

Term B SE t-value p
Intercept 19.28 17.99 1.071 .28
2-week endpoint 9.77 18.42 0.530 .60
4-week endpoint 15.74 16.39 0.960 .34
Age 0.39 0.26 1.502 .14
Gender (f) −1.44 2.12 −0.681 .50
Psychedelic use 0.61 0.67 0.914 .36
Psychiatric history −3.00 2.45 1.207 .23
Expectations .01 .06 0.258 .80
Age:2-week endpoint 0.00 0.27 −0.016 .99
Age:4-week endpoint −0.17 0.23 −0.734 .47
Gender (f):2-week endpoint −0.82 2.21 −0.373 .71
Gender (f):4-week endpoint 2.46 1.96 1.259 .21
Psychedelic use:2-week endpoint −0.70 0.65 −1.083 .28
Psychedelic use:4-week endpoint 0.06 0.57 0.097 .92
Psychiatric history:2-week endpoint −6.62 2.77 −2.388 .02
Psychiatric history:4-week endpoint −5.69 2.32 −2.457 .02
Expectations:2-week endpoint 0.02 0.06 0.403 .69
Expectations:4-week endpoint 0.02 0.05 0.4000 .69

We lastly aimed to assess whether changes in OA were comparable to those observed in YA. We conducted a mixed effects linear regression model with the OA and matched YA samples, which revealed no significant interactions between age group and time, indicating that well-being improvements in OA and YA were statistically indistinguishable in this sample (B=−2.36, 95% CI [−5.48, 0.76], p = 0.14 and B=−2.55, 95% CI [−5.53, 0.42], p = .097, for two weeks and four weeks post-dosing, respectively; Supplementary Figure S1).

Comparing subjective psychedelic effects in OA and YA

A MANOVA comparing ratings of acute subjective effects (EBI, MEQ, EDI, CEQ, COMS) between OA and YA revealed significant differences between the groups (Pillais’ Trace = .14, F(1,111) = 3.72, p = .004). Follow-up ANOVAs were then conducted (Fig. 2), revealing significantly lower intensity scores for OA on all tests included subjective effects measures, except for the CEQ (F(1,111) = 0.61, d = 0.06, p = .43) and only at trend level for the COMS (F(1,111) = 2.91, d = 0.30, p = .09), suggesting that OA experienced overall less intense subjective psychedelic effects compared to YA. The differences in mean scores between OA and YA were greatest for the EDI (48.6%, M = 25.4 vs 41.7, F(1,111) = 13.75, d = 0.62, p < .001) and the MEQ (39.1%, M = 60.1 vs M = 89.3, F(1,111) = 17.65, d = 0.74, p < .001), followed by the EBI (31.0%, M = 40.9 vs M = 55.9, F(1,111) = 5.44, d = 0.45, p = .02). Differences on the MEQ and EDI were significant also after Bonferroni-correction.

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Boxplots showing intensity ratings of subjective psychedelic effects. Acute and post-acute effects were found to be attenuated in OA (> 60 years) compared to YA. OA: Older adults; PR: post-retreat; YA: Younger adults. +:p<.01, *:p<.05; **: p<.01; ***: p<.001

In a second MANOVA, post-acute effects, including insights (PIS) and Communitas experienced across the retreat as a whole (COMS-PR) were also found to be different by age group (Pillais’ Trace = .07, F(1,86) = 3.11, p = .05). Follow-up ANOVAs revealed this difference to be based on COMS-PR scores, which were significantly lower in OA (F(1,86) = 6.30, d = 0.55, p = .01), while PIS scores were not significantly different (F(1,86) = 0.27, d = 0.24, p = .60).

Differential mechanisms predicting well-being changes in OA and YA

We subsequently explored whether age group-dependent acute and post-acute subjective psychedelic effects predicted long-term well-being outcomes via a mixed effects linear regression model including three-way interaction terms between age group, time, and each acute predictor variable. To prevent multicollinearity issues, variance inflation factors (VIF) for each included predictor variable were calculated, resulting in the removal of the MEQ from the model (VIF = 3.7) based on the generally accepted VIF cut-off of 2.5 (Johnston et al., 2018). After excluding MEQ, the highest VIF was found for EBI but beneath the established cut-off (VIF = 2.1). The resulting model revealed a significant negative three-way interaction between EBI, older age group, and post-psychedelic end-points at two- (β=−0.78, 95% CI [−1.43, −0.13], p = .02) and four-weeks (β =−0.80, 95% CI [−1.46, −0.15], p = .02), indicating that emotional breakthrough experiences contributed less to improved well-being in OA compared to YA. Furthermore, a marginally significant positive three-way interaction was detected for post-retreat COMS scores, OA, and the four-week end-point, suggesting that relational experiences of sharing and togetherness across the retreat played a larger role for predicting improved well-being in OA when compared to YA (β = 2.04, 95% CI [−0.21, 4.28], p = .08).

Correlation analyses between predictors and well-being change scores (Fig. 3) further illustrate these relationships: EBI was positively associated with well-being change scores at four-weeks in YA (r(35) = .43, p = .008), but not in OA (r(46)=−.04, p = .79), while post-retreat COMS-PR was positively associated with well-being change scores at four-weeks in OA (r(27) = .37, p = .03) but not in YA (r(34) = .01, p = .94). Interestingly, none of the other acute and post-acute variables that significantly correlated with well-being changes in YA were shown to significantly correlate with well-being changes in OA. Comprehensively, these findings suggest that the psychedelic experience fundamentally differs between OA and YA indicating a unique role for psychosocial experiences in the older group.

Figure 3.

Figure 3

Correlation heatmap showing associations between subjective psychedelic effects and changes in well-being from baseline to two weeks (2W) and four weeks (4W) after a psychedelic group session. CEQ: Challenging Experience Questionnaire; COMS: Communitas Scale; EBI: Emotional Breakthrough Inventory; EDI: Ego-Dissolution Inventory; MEQ: Mystical Experience Questionnaire; OA: Older adults; PR: Post-retreat; YA: Younger adults. *:p<.05; **: p<.01

Discussion

In this prospective study, we investigated the effects of naturalistic guided psychedelic group sessions on OA’s well-being by leveraging an opportunity sample of 62 participants aged 60 years or older attending self-initiated psychedelic ceremonies or retreats. Analyses revealed clinically meaningful improvements in well-being in OA at two and four weeks following a psychedelic group session, in line with prior naturalistic studies in YA (Kettner et al., 2021; Mason et al., 2019; Ruffell et al., 2021; Uthaug et al., 2021). Interestingly, this was the case despite lower acute subjective effects scores in the OA sample, indicating that differential salutogenic mechanisms may be at play in this age group. This exploratory hypothesis was partially confirmed through regression and correlational analyses showing a primacy of relational mechanisms, as opposed to classic intrasubjective psychedelic effects in OA attending psychedelic group sessions.

Among baseline and demographic variables predicting well-being increases in OA, only the presence of a psychiatric diagnosis showed significant effects. This finding was stable also when controlling for expectation effects, a hypothesized confounder in psychedelic trials (Muthukumaraswamy et al., 2021) and is in line with the transdiagnostic utility of psychedelic treatments for a number of mental health disorders (Brennan & Belser, 2022; Kelly et al., 2021; Kočárová et al., 2021), including major depression, alcohol-use disorder, and anorexia nervosa. Indeed, resilience to expectancy is consistent with recent research that failed to support its influence in driving therapeutic response to psilocybin therapy for depression (Szigeti et al., 2024), implying a substantive direct therapeutic action. Outside regulated clinical trial settings, the structured, user-reviewed services offered by retreat centres might have particular appeal to OA when compared to individual use (e.g., at-home). OA may have less access to or tend to avoid the acquisition of scheduled substances over the black market, may have greater psychological needs for safety, structure and social contact (Albert & Duffy, 2012), and the economical means to afford the often high financial cost of psychedelic retreats or ceremonies.

Crucially, clinically relevant improvements in mental health in OA were indistinguishable from those found in a sample of YA, matched to account for several demographic factors including higher OA well-being at baseline, a common finding in the literature (Carstensen et al., 2003). For example, elevated baseline well-being levels in OA are in accordance with representative population level studies showing that in wealthy English-speaking countries, happiness and hedonic experiences are lowest around ages 45–54 and tend to increase with age, following an inverted U-shape (Steptoe et al., 2015, Blanchflower, 2021). The observed return of well-being levels to baseline at the six-months follow-up time point in OA is in contrast with prior studies showing long-term mental health improvements following psychedelic-assisted psychotherapy (reviewed in Aday, Mitzkovitz, et al., 2020). Two prior naturalistic studies in YA have also found sustained two-year increases in protective psychological traits such as resilience and mindfulness (Mans et al., 2021), or nature relatedness (Kettner et al., 2019), although, similar to the present study, affective measures of well-being have thus far been shown to remain increased only at nominal, non-significant levels (Mans et al., 2021). The conditions under which psychedelic-induced salutogenesis remains stable therefore remains a critical unanswered question, considering that in clinical studies, improvements appear to remain significant for months to years following treatment (Agin-Liebes et al., 2020; Carhart-Harris et al., 2018; Gukasyan & Nayak, 2021; Johnson et al., 2017).

Our study revealed differential acute psychedelic effects and salutogenic mechanisms in OA when compared to YA. This is of clinical importance, since current models of psychedelic function propose that the acute psychedelic effects are key mediators of mental health improvements (Griffiths et al., 2008; Griffiths et al., 2006; Ko et al., 2022; Majić et al., 2015; Roseman et al., 2018). In contrast to prior controlled research reporting challenging experiences to be negatively correlated with age (Ko et al., 2023; Kopra et al., 2022; Studerus et al., 2012), the OA group in the present sample showed lower acute effects scores on all metrics except for challenging experiences. One potential reason for this apparent discrepancy may be the overall younger age (means ranging from 27–36) and lack of participants aged 60 or above in the abovementioned controlled studies. It is thus possible that the intensity of challenging experiences under psychedelics peaks among the younger distribution of YA and remains stable after a certain age, pointing to population diversity as a key strength of naturalistic studies such as this one.

Nonetheless, the absence of any significant correlations between acute psychedelic effects and long-term changes in OA contradicts previous work showing that the quality of the acute experience constitutes a key predictor of psychedelic-induced changes in well-being (Haijen et al., 2018; Kangaslampi, 2020, 2023; Kettner et al., 2021; Peill et al., 2022; Roseman et al., 2019; Roseman et al., 2018). In contrast, only the experience of Communitas rated in reference to the entire retreat – not just the psychedelic session – was associated with well-being changes in OA. This finding suggests that OA might benefit from psychedelics for different reasons than YA, greater relevance being given to the experience of togetherness and social bonds that can occur in group settings than to the individual, intrapersonal experience. The witnessing of other participants’ vulnerability and the resulting emotional intimacy generated through sharing rounds before and after dosing sessions might be particularly impactful to OA, for whom social contact, especially with nonfamily members, is known to decrease (Sander et al., 2017). Indeed, from the present data it is unclear to what extent the consumption of the psychedelic substance itself would have even been necessary for OA to experience the detected psychological benefits. Future research should thus further explore the details of psychotherapeutic and group activities taking place at psychedelic retreats, and their psychological benefits for participants, as well as the validity of instruments assessing the overall experience in OA. Conceivably, the psychedelic session itself could be seen as a non-essential part, primarily providing the context for an intimate and intergenerational group-based intervention with the potential to tackle the negative emotional and cognitive health consequences of social isolation in the elderly (Krzeczkowska et al., 2021; Murayama et al., 2015).

Further, the present findings of reduced acute psychedelic effects and increased importance of social connections may relate to the consolidation of “emotional landscapes” in OA (Carstensen et al., 2003). Our findings are in line with Carstensen’s (et al., 1999) Socioemotional Selectivity Theory posing that OA optimize emotional experiences to prioritize meaningful social connections and foster positive experiences and emotional satisfaction. Intriguingly, reduced acute psychedelic effects in OA may mechanistically also relate to age-dependent reductions in cortical serotonin receptor density, which is most pronounced for the 2A receptor (Karrer et al., 2019), the primary target of psychedelics (Madsen et al., 2019; Vollenweider et al., 1998).

Several limitations need to be considered when interpreting our findings. Most importantly, the context of psychedelic use in the present sample was limited to ceremony and retreat settings, raising the question whether well-being improvements, attenuated acute psychedelic effects, and greater importance of psychosocial mechanisms detected in the current sample would also occur in other naturalistic or controlled psychotherapeutic settings. Replications in larger and more representative samples will therefore be crucial to further explore the effects of psychedelic on the elderly outside psychedelic ceremony and retreat settings, and in samples less biased towards white, highly educated participants. Controlled laboratory studies administering psychedelics to OA will potentially be able to clarify the role of acute psychedelic effects in environments that do not provide the psychosocial group benefits present at psychedelic retreats. Additional limitations include the inaccurate qualitative assessment of psychedelic dose, as well as possible co-use of other substances - common in naturalistic samples (Kuc et al., 2021; Zeifman et al., 2023) - which was not controlled for in the present study. Furthermore, the potential of systematic biases through attrition effects constitutes another limitation related to the remote nature of this survey study, although prior research has shown that attrition in prospective psychedelic surveys follows similar patterns as in other fields (Hübner et al., 2021).

Conclusions and future directions

Echoing previous observational studies in YA and clinical trials, our findings suggest that psychedelic group sessions may induce rapid and sustained benefits on OA’s well-being, especially in those with a history of a psychiatric illness. While further replication and controlled studies are necessary, the present findings further suggest that OA might experience an overall attenuated intensity of acute psychedelic effects, and that acute effects might be less predictive of prospective changes in mental well-being. Instead, the greater importance of psychosocial experiences indicate that the group setting may be particularly valuable for OA. Our findings represent a first inquiry into the factors that contribute to the enduring nature of salutogenic psychedelic effects in OA and call for longer-term observational studies and clinical trials in OA.

Table 3.

Three-way interactions showing differential salutogenic mechanisms for OA and YA. For a full tabulation of main and two-way interaction effects, see Supplementary Table S2. CEQ: Challenging Experience Questionnaire; COMS: Communitas Scale; EBI: Emotional Breakthrough Inventory; EDI: Ego-Dissolution Inventory; PR: Post-retreat; PIS: Psychological Insight Scale; SE: Standard Error.

Term β SE t-value p
EDI:Age 60+:2-week endpoint 0.03 0.12 0.25 .80
EDI:Age 60+:4-week endpoint 0.02 0.11 0.217 .83
EBI:Age 60+:2-week endpoint −0.22 0.10 −2.354 .02
EBI:Age 60+:4-week endpoint −0.21 0.09 −2.416 .02
CEQ:Age 60+:2-week endpoint 0.08 0.11 0.788 .43
CEQ:Age 60+:4-week endpoint 0.09 0.10 0.855 .39
COMS:Age 60+:2-week endpoint 0.18 0.25 0.727 .47
COMS:Age 60+:4-week endpoint −0.06 0.24 −0.24 .81
COMSPR:Age 60+:2-week endpoint −0.02 0.38 −0.04 .97
COMSPR:Age 60+:4-week endpoint 0.68 0.38 1.793 .08
PIS:Age 60+:2-week endpoint 0.00 0.02 −0.16 .87
PIS:Age 60+:4-week endpoint 0.00 0.02 −0.234 .82

Acknowledgements:

We thank the participant of the study for their invaluable contribution to research. This work was supported by the following agencies: L.P.: K99-AG065457 (NIA), R00AG065457 (NIA), and philanthropic support from David Dolby and the Dolby family.

Footnotes

Declarations

Competing interests: HK is a scientific advisor for Maya Health. RCH is a scientific advisor for Mydecine, Maya Health, Osmind, Synthesis Institute, Tryp Therapeutics, Journey Collab, Journey Space, Beckley Psytech and Usona Institute.

Previous presentations: AAGP New Orleans 2023; MAPS Psychedelic Science Denver 2023

Contributor Information

Hannes Kettner, Imperial College London.

Leor Roseman, University of Exeter.

Adam Gazzaley, UCSF.

Robin Carhart-Harris, University of California San Francisco.

Lorenzo Pasquini, University of California San Francisco.

Data Sharing Statement:

Raw data can be shared from the corresponding author following reasonable request.

References

  • 1.Aday J. S., Bloesch E. K., & Davoli C. C. (2020). Can psychedelic drugs attenuate age-related changes in cognition and affect? Journal of Cognitive Enhancement, 4, 219–227. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Aday J. S., Mitzkovitz C. M., Bloesch E. K., Davoli C. C., & Davis A. K. (2020). Long-term effects of psychedelic drugs: A systematic review. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 113, 179–189. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Agin-Liebes G. I., Malone T., Yalch M. M., Mennenga S. E., Ponté K. L., Guss J., Bossis A. P., Grigsby J., Fischer S., & Ross S. (2020). Long-term follow-up of psilocybin-assisted psychotherapy for psychiatric and existential distress in patients with life-threatening cancer. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 34(2), 155–166. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Albert S. M., & Duffy J. (2012). Differences in risk aversion between young and older adults. Neuroscience and neuroeconomics, 2012(1). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Andersen K. A., Carhart-Harris R., Nutt D. J., & Erritzoe D. (2021). Therapeutic effects of classic serotonergic psychedelics: A systematic review of modern-era clinical studies. Acta Psychiatrica Scandinavica, 143(2), 101–118. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Banerjee S., Hellier J., Dewey M., Romeo R., Ballard C., Baldwin R., Bentham P., Fox C., Holmes C., & Katona C. (2011). Sertraline or mirtazapine for depression in dementia (HTA-SADD): a randomised, multicentre, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. The Lancet, 378(9789), 403–411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Barrett F. S., Bradstreet M. P., Leoutsakos J.-M. S., Johnson M. W., & Griffiths R. R. (2016). The Challenging Experience Questionnaire: Characterization of challenging experiences with psilocybin mushrooms. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 30(12), 1279–1295. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Barrett F. S., Johnson M. W., & Griffiths R. R. (2015). Validation of the revised Mystical Experience Questionnaire in experimental sessions with psilocybin. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 29(11), 1182–1190. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Beaussant Y., & Nigam K. (2023). Expending Perspectives on the Potential for Psychedelic-Assisted Therapies to Improve the Experience of Aging. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 31(1), 54–57. 10.1016/j.jagp.2022.09.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Bornemann J., Close J. B., Spriggs M. J., Carhart-Harris R., & Roseman L. (2021). Self-medication for chronic pain using classic psychedelics: a qualitative investigation to inform future research. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Bouchet L., Sager Z., Yrondi A., Nigam K. B., Anderson B. T., Ross S., Petridis P. D., & Beaussant Y. (2024). Older adults in psychedelic-assisted therapy trials: A systematic review. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 02698811231215420. 10.1177/02698811231215420 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Brennan W., & Belser A. B. (2022). Models of psychedelic-assisted psychotherapy: A contemporary assessment and an introduction to EMBARK, a transdiagnostic, trans-drug model. Frontiers in psychology, 1879. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Callon C., Williams M., & Lafrance A. (2021). “Meeting the Medicine Halfway”: Ayahuasca Ceremony Leaders’ Perspectives on Preparation and Integration Practices for Participants. Journal of Humanistic Psychology, 00221678211043300. [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Carhart-Harris R., Giribaldi B., Watts R., Baker-Jones M., Murphy-Beiner A., Murphy R., Martell J., Blemings A., Erritzoe D., & Nutt D. J. (2021). Trial of psilocybin versus escitalopram for depression. New England Journal of Medicine, 384(15), 1402–1411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Carhart-Harris R. L., Bolstridge M., Day C., Rucker J., Watts R., Erritzoe D., Kaelen M., Giribaldi B., Bloomfield M., & Pilling S. (2018). Psilocybin with psychological support for treatment-resistant depression: six-month follow-up. Psychopharmacology, 235(2), 399–408. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Carstensen L. L., Fung H. H., & Charles S. T. (2003). Socioemotional selectivity theory and the regulation of emotion in the second half of life. Motivation and emotion, 27, 103–123. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Carstensen L. L., Isaacowitz D. M., & Charles S. T. (1999). Taking time seriously: A theory of socioemotional selectivity. American psychologist, 54(3), 165. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Conwell Y., Lin F. V., & Wang K. H. (2023). Can Emotional Well-Being Maintain Health and Prevent Suicide in Later Life? A National Priority for Research. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry, 31(6), 425–427. 10.1016/j.jagp.2023.01.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Donovan N. J., & Blazer D. (2020). Social Isolation and Loneliness in Older Adults: Review and Commentary of a National Academies Report. Am J Geriatr Psychiatry, 28(12), 1233–1244. 10.1016/j.jagp.2020.08.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Donovan N. J., Wu Q., Rentz D. M., Sperling R. A., Marshall G. A., & Glymour M. M. (2017). Loneliness, depression and cognitive function in older U.S. adults. Int J Geriatr Psychiatry, 32(5), 564–573. 10.1002/gps.4495 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Forester B., Lanctôt K., Mintzer J., & Rosenberg P. (2022). Cannabinoids and Psychedelics for Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Alzheimer’s: Addressing Disparities Through Clinical Trials. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 30(4), S7. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Gearin A. K. (2015). An Amazonian shamanic brew in Australia: Ayahuasca healing and individualism. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Griffiths R. R., Richards W. A., Johnson M. W., McCann U. D., & Jesse R. (2008). Mystical-type experiences occasioned by psilocybin mediate the attribution of personal meaning and spiritual significance 14 months later. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 22(6), 621–632. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Griffiths R. R., Richards W. A., McCann U., & Jesse R. (2006). Psilocybin can occasion mystical-type experiences having substantial and sustained personal meaning and spiritual significance. Psychopharmacology, 187(3), 268–283. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Gukasyan N., & Nayak S. M. (2021). Psychedelics, placebo effects, and set and setting: Insights from common factors theory of psychotherapy. Transcultural Psychiatry, 1363461520983684. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Haijen E. C., Kaelen M., Roseman L., Timmermann C., Kettner H., Russ S., Nutt D., Daws R. E., Hampshire A. D., & Lorenz R. (2018). Predicting responses to psychedelics: a prospective study. Frontiers in Pharmacology, 9, 897. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Hawkley L. C., & Cacioppo J. T. (2010). Loneliness matters: A theoretical and empirical review of consequences and mechanisms. Annals of behavioral medicine, 40(2), 218–227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hübner S., Haijen E., Kaelen M., Carhart-Harris R. L., & Kettner H. (2021). Turn on, tune in, and drop out: predictors of attrition in a prospective observational cohort study on psychedelic use. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 23(7), e25973. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Johnson M. W., Garcia-Romeu A., & Griffiths R. R. (2017). Long-term follow-up of psilocybin-facilitated smoking cessation. The American journal of drug and alcohol abuse, 43(1), 55–60. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Johnston C. B., Mangini M., Grob C., & Anderson B. (2023). The safety and efficacy of psychedelic-assisted therapies for older adults: knowns and unknowns. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 31(1), 44–53. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Johnston R., Jones K., & Manley D. (2018). Confounding and collinearity in regression analysis: a cautionary tale and an alternative procedure, illustrated by studies of British voting behaviour. Quality & quantity, 52, 1957–1976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Kangaslampi S. (2020). Uncovering psychological mechanisms mediating the effects of drugs: some issues and comments using the example of psychedelic drugs. Psychopharmacology, 237(12), 3799–3802. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kangaslampi S. (2023). Association between mystical-type experiences under psychedelics and improvements in well-being or mental health – A comprehensive review of the evidence. Journal of Psychedelic Studies. 10.1556/2054.2023.00243 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Karrer T. M., McLaughlin C. L., Guaglianone C. P., & Samanez-Larkin G. R. (2019). Reduced serotonin receptors and transporters in normal aging adults: a meta-analysis of PET and SPECT imaging studies. Neurobiology of aging, 80, 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Kelly J. R., Gillan C. M., Prenderville J., Kelly C., Harkin A., Clarke G., & O’Keane V. (2021). Psychedelic therapy’s transdiagnostic effects: a research domain criteria (RDoC) perspective. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 800072. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Kettner H., Gandy S., Haijen E. C., & Carhart-Harris R. L. (2019). From egoism to ecoism: Psychedelics increase nature relatedness in a state-mediated and context-dependent manner. International journal of environmental research and public health, 16(24), 5147. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Kettner H., Rosas F., Timmermann C., Kärtner L., Carhart-Harris R. L., & Roseman L. (2021). Psychedelic communitas: intersubjective experience during psychedelic group sessions predicts enduring changes in psychological wellbeing and social connectedness. Frontiers in Pharmacology, 12, 234. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Ko K., Carter B., Cleare A. J., & Rucker J. J. (2023). Predicting the Intensity of Psychedelic-Induced Mystical and Challenging Experience in a Healthy Population: An Exploratory Post-Hoc Analysis. Neuropsychiatric Disease and Treatment, 2105–2113. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ko K., Knight G., Rucker J. J., & Cleare A. J. (2022). Psychedelics, mystical experience, and therapeutic efficacy: a systematic review. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 13, 917199. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kočárová R., Horáček J., & Carhart-Harris R. (2021). Does psychedelic therapy have a transdiagnostic action and prophylactic potential? Frontiers in Psychiatry, 1068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Kopra E. I., Ferris J. A., Winstock A. R., Young A. H., & Rucker J. J. (2022). Adverse experiences resulting in emergency medical treatment seeking following the use of magic mushrooms. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 36(8), 965–973. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Krzeczkowska A., Spalding D. M., McGeown W. J., Gow A. J., Carlson M. C., & Nicholls L. A. B. (2021). A systematic review of the impacts of intergenerational engagement on older adults’ cognitive, social, and health outcomes. Ageing Research Reviews, 71, 101400. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Kuc J., Kettner H., Rosas F., Erritzoe D., Haijen E., Kaelen M., Nutt D., & Carhart-Harris R. L. (2021). Psychedelic experience dose-dependently modulated by cannabis: results of a prospective online survey. Psychopharmacology, 1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Luo Y., Hawkley L. C., Waite L. J., & Cacioppo J. T. (2012). Loneliness, health, and mortality in old age: A national longitudinal study. Social science & medicine, 74(6), 907–914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Madsen M. K., Fisher P. M., Burmester D., Dyssegaard A., Stenbæk D. S., Kristiansen S., Johansen S. S., Lehel S., Linnet K., & Svarer C. (2019). Psychedelic effects of psilocybin correlate with serotonin 2A receptor occupancy and plasma psilocin levels. Neuropsychopharmacology, 44(7), 1328–1334. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Majić T., Schmidt T. T., & Gallinat J. (2015). Peak experiences and the afterglow phenomenon: when and how do therapeutic effects of hallucinogens depend on psychedelic experiences? Journal of Psychopharmacology, 29(3), 241–253. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Mans K., Kettner H., Erritzoe D., Haijen E. C., Kaelen M., & Carhart-Harris R. L. (2021). Sustained, multifaceted improvements in mental well-being following psychedelic experiences in a prospective opportunity sample. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 1038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Mason N. L., & Kuypers K. P. (2018). Mental health of a self-selected sample of psychedelic users and self-medication practices with psychedelics. Journal of Psychedelic Studies, 2(1), 45–52. [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Mason N. L., Mischler E., Uthaug M. V., & Kuypers K. P. (2019). Sub-acute effects of psilocybin on empathy, creative thinking, and subjective well-being. Journal of psychoactive drugs, 51(2), 123–134. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Matzopoulos R., Morlock R., Morlock A., Lerer B., & Lerer L. (2022). Psychedelic Mushrooms in the USA: Knowledge, Patterns of Use, and Association With Health Outcomes [Brief Research Report]. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12. 10.3389/fpsyt.2021.780696 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Murayama Y., Ohba H., Yasunaga M., Nonaka K., Takeuchi R., Nishi M., Sakuma N., Uchida H., Shinkai S., & Fujiwara Y. (2015). The effect of intergenerational programs on the mental health of elderly adults. Aging & mental health, 19(4), 306–314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Muthukumaraswamy S. D., Forsyth A., & Lumley T. (2021). Blinding and expectancy confounds in psychedelic randomized controlled trials. Expert review of clinical pharmacology, 14(9), 1133–1152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Nour M. M., Evans L., Nutt D., & Carhart-Harris R. L. (2016). Ego-dissolution and psychedelics: validation of the ego-dissolution inventory (EDI). Frontiers in human neuroscience, 10, 269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Peill J. M., Trinci K. E., Kettner H., Mertens L. J., Roseman L., Timmermann C., Rosas F. E., Lyons T., & Carhart-Harris R. L. (2022). Validation of the Psychological Insight Scale: A new scale to assess psychological insight following a psychedelic experience. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 02698811211066709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Pollan M. (2019). How to change your mind: What the new science of psychedelics teaches us about consciousness, dying, addiction, depression, and transcendence. Penguin. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Roseman L., Haijen E., Idialu-Ikato K., Kaelen M., Watts R., & Carhart-Harris R. (2019). Emotional breakthrough and psychedelics: validation of the emotional breakthrough inventory. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 33(9), 1076–1087. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Roseman L., Nutt D. J., & Carhart-Harris R. L. (2018). Quality of acute psychedelic experience predicts therapeutic efficacy of psilocybin for treatment-resistant depression. Frontiers in Pharmacology, 8, 974. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Rosenberg P. B., Martin B. K., Frangakis C., Mintzer J. E., Weintraub D., Porsteinsson A. P., Schneider L. S., Rabins P. V., Munro C. A., & Meinert C. L. (2010). Sertraline for the treatment of depression in Alzheimer disease. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18(2), 136–145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Rosenberg P. B., Mielke M. M., Appleby B. S., Oh E. S., Geda Y. E., & Lyketsos C. G. (2013). The association of neuropsychiatric symptoms in MCI with incident dementia and Alzheimer disease. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 21(7), 685–695. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Rucker J. J., & Young A. H. (2021). Psilocybin: from serendipity to credibility? Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 659044. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Ruffell S. G., Netzband N., Tsang W., Davies M., Butler M., Rucker J. J., Tófoli L. F., Dempster E. L., Young A. H., & Morgan C. J. (2021). Ceremonial Ayahuasca in Amazonian Retreats—Mental Health and Epigenetic Outcomes From a Six-Month Naturalistic Study. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 12, 898. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Sander J., Schupp J., & Richter D. (2017). Getting together: Social contact frequency across the life span. Developmental Psychology, 53(8), 1571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Sarangi A., & Akinkunmi O. (2023). Psychedelics for the Neuropsychiatric Symptoms of Alzheimer’s disease. The American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 31(3), S44–S45. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Steffens D. C. (2012). Depressive symptoms and mild cognitive impairment in the elderly: an ominous combination. Biological psychiatry, 71(9), 762–764. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Steinberg M., Rosenberg P., & Garcia-Romeu A. (2023). The Potential for Psychedelics to Improve Coping Skills and Resilience in Persons With Newly Diagnosed Mild Cognitive Impairment or Early Alzheimer’s Disease. Current Geriatrics Reports, 12(3), 149–155. [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Studerus E., Gamma A., Kometer M., & Vollenweider F. X. (2012). Prediction of psilocybin response in healthy volunteers. PloS one, 7(2), e30800. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Szigeti B., Weiss B., Rosas F. E., Erritzoe D., Nutt D., & Carhart-Harris R. (2024). Assessing expectancy and suggestibility in a trial of escitalopram v. psilocybin for depression. Psychological medicine, 1–8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Tennant R., Hiller L., Fishwick R., Platt S., Joseph S., Weich S., Parkinson J., Secker J., & Stewart-Brown S. (2007). The Warwick-Edinburgh mental well-being scale (WEMWBS): development and UK validation. Health and Quality of life Outcomes, 5(1), 1–13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Tramacchi D. (2000). Field tripping: psychedelic communitas and ritual in the Australian bush. Journal of Contemporary Religion, 15(2), 201–213. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Uthaug M., Mason N., Toennes S., Reckweg J., de Sousa Fernandes Perna E., Kuypers K., van Oorsouw K., Riba J., & Ramaekers J. (2021). A placebo-controlled study of the effects of ayahuasca, set and setting on mental health of participants in ayahuasca group retreats. Psychopharmacology, 238, 1899–1910. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Vann Jones S. A., & O’Kelly A. (2020). Psychedelics as a treatment for Alzheimer’s disease dementia. Frontiers in synaptic neuroscience, 12, 34. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Vollenweider F. X., & Smallridge J. W. (2022). Classic psychedelic drugs: update on biological mechanisms. Pharmacopsychiatry. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Vollenweider F. X., Vollenweider-Scherpenhuyzen M. F., Bäbler A., Vogel H., & Hell D. (1998). Psilocybin induces schizophrenia-like psychosis in humans via a serotonin-2 agonist action. Neuroreport, 9(17), 3897–3902. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Winkelman M. J., Szabo A., & Frecska E. (2023). The potential of psychedelics for the treatment of Alzheimer’s disease and related dementias. European Neuropsychopharmacology, 76, 3–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Zeifman R. J., Kettner H., Pagni B. A., Mallard A., Roberts D. E., Erritzoe D., Ross S., & Carhart-Harris R. L. (2023). Co-use of MDMA with psilocybin/LSD may buffer against challenging experiences and enhance positive experiences. Scientific reports, 13(1), 13645. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Raw data can be shared from the corresponding author following reasonable request.


Articles from Research Square are provided here courtesy of American Journal Experts

RESOURCES