Skip to main content
NPJ Mental Health Research logoLink to NPJ Mental Health Research
. 2024 Mar 18;3:14. doi: 10.1038/s44184-023-00048-5

The impact of mindfulness apps on psychological processes of change: a systematic review

Natalia Macrynikola 1,, Zareen Mir 2, Tishmattie Gopal 3, Erica Rodriguez 4, Sunnie Li 4, Milann Cox 5, Gloria Yeh 1, John Torous 1
PMCID: PMC10955957  PMID: 38609511

Abstract

Mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs) have demonstrated therapeutic efficacy for various psychological conditions, and smartphone apps that facilitate mindfulness practice can enhance the reach and impact of MBIs. The goal of this review was to summarize the published evidence on the impact of mindfulness apps on the psychological processes known to mediate transdiagnostic symptom reduction after mindfulness practice. A literature search from January 1, 1993, to August 7, 2023 was conducted on three databases, and 28 randomized controlled trials involving 5963 adults were included. Across these 28 studies, 67 outcome comparisons were made between a mindfulness app group and a control group. Between-group effects tended to favor the mindfulness app group over the control group in three psychological process domains: repetitive negative thinking, attention regulation, and decentering/defusion. Findings were mixed in other domains (i.e., awareness, nonreactivity, non-judgment, positive affect, and acceptance). The range of populations examined, methodological concerns across studies, and problems with sustained app engagement likely contributed to mixed findings. However, effect sizes tended to be moderate to large when effects were found, and gains tended to persist at follow-up assessments two to six months later. More research is needed to better understand the impact of these apps on psychological processes of change. Clinicians interested in integrating apps into care should consider app-related factors beyond evidence of a clinical foundation and use app databases to identify suitable apps for their patients, as highlighted at the end of this review.

Subject terms: Psychology, Health care

Introduction

Mindfulness-based interventions (MBIs) have demonstrated efficacy in improving a range of clinical outcomes, such as depression and anxiety1. In a rigorous randomized controlled trial, mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) was even found to be non-inferior to antidepressant medication2. However, MBI delivery and impact remain limited by various factors, two important ones being barriers to access and difficulties with sustained engagement. That is, for many individuals, MBIs remain inaccessible for the same reasons that mental health treatment remains inaccessible, including cost, stigma, a shortage of clinicians, and various logistical barriers (e.g., lack of transportation, lack of childcare)3,4. In addition, MBIs necessitate practice outside of session, which contributes to outcomes5; however, many struggle to sustain a consistent mindfulness practice on their own outside of in-person sessions.

Technology can bridge the gap in both of these situations. Mindfulness apps can provide an alternative when in-person MBIs are inaccessible, and integrating mindfulness apps into in-person treatment can facilitate practice and increase intervention impact6,7. Yet most commercially available mindfulness apps have not been scientifically evaluated8, and most mental health apps struggle to keep users engaged9. Related, uptake of mindfulness apps is low in treatment, despite interest from clinicians10 and their patients11,12. One commonly cited barrier is a lack of knowledge about which apps are credible and effective13. To address these barriers and stimulate more research into building the mindfulness app evidence base, we conducted a systematic review to assess these apps’ effectiveness in shifting psychological processes of change related to mindfulness.

Recent reviews suggest that mindfulness app effects on clinical outcomes are often inconsistent. For example, one review found generally small app effects on depression and contradictory results for anxiety14. However, the common approach of evaluating app effects on such distal psychological outcomes as psychiatric disorders is problematic because app intervention periods tend to be too brief for these types of outcomes to demonstrate significant and consistent change. A recent meta-analysis of 23 mindfulness app evaluations found that only nine studies used intervention periods that adhered to the recommended eight weeks of such MBIs as MBSR and MBCT15. Therefore, a more suitable approach to reviewing mindfulness app efficacy may be to focus on the more proximal processes of change, or mechanisms, that have been empirically demonstrated to explain the effects of mindfulness practice on more distal psychological outcomes. Temporally, mechanisms shift first16; thus, focusing on these intermediary outcomes may provide a clearer picture of the efficacy of mindfulness apps.

Adopting a mechanisms-as-outcomes approach has three additional benefits. First, the knowledge gained from such an approach can lead to more targeted apps, which may enhance their efficacy. Second, current evidence suggests that mHealth app engagement in the general public falls to near zero after two weeks9. Given this reality, it is key to understand whether the brief periods in which apps tend to be evaluated have any impact on mechanistic targets. If they do not, it will be important to focus efforts on sustaining engagement for longer in the hopes of seeing a substantial impact on these important targets. Third, this approach provides valuable insights for clinicians specializing in evidence-based treatments as many of the mechanisms of mindfulness practice (e.g., emotion regulation) are also the transdiagnostic mechanisms targeted in such therapies17,18. Therefore, knowledge gained from this approach can aid clinicians in evaluating such apps as potential complements to ongoing treatment goals.

To date, no mindfulness app review of which we are aware has focused on the mechanisms of mindfulness training as outcomes. Thus, a systematic review is warranted to investigate the evidence of mindfulness app effects on the mechanistic processes through which mindfulness training has been demonstrated to influence transdiagnostic symptom change19.

Methods

This systematic review was conducted according to PRISMA guidelines20 and registered on the International Platform of Registered Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis Protocols (#202350017). To identify mechanisms of mindfulness practice, we first searched for papers that proposed likely mechanisms based on a thorough rationale. We searched for these papers in Pubmed (using the keywords “mindful*” in the Title field, and “mechanism” or “mediat*” in the Text field). This method yielded four theory papers2124, from which we extracted the proposed mechanisms. For each proposed mechanism, we then searched the literature for empirical support (obtained through mediation analysis). Our list of theoretically and empirically supported mechanisms of mindfulness practice appears in Table 1. (For an overview of corresponding theories, see eTable 1).

Table 1.

List of outcomes of interest.

Mindfulness mechanism Theoretical paper proposed in Empirically supported?
Shapiro Holzel MAT MMT
Decentering/defusion x x yes
Self-regulation x yes
Values clarification x yes
Acceptance/psychological flexibility x x yes
Awareness xa xa yes
Nonreactivity xa xb yes
Attention regulation x x yes
Emotion regulation x yes
- Reappraisal xb x yes
- Extinction xb yes
- Suppression xb yes
- Worry xc yes
- Rumination xc yes
Non-judgment x x yes
Positive affect x yes

adescribed as part of exposure.

bdescribed as part of emotion regulation.

cnot explicitly described as part of emotion regulation but added here, given that they are recognized emotion regulation strategies.

To be included in this review, a study had to (a) be a randomized controlled trial design, (b) evaluate a mindfulness-based mobile app, (c) assess change in one or more of our identified mechanisms using a validated, reliable measure, (d) focus on adults (≥18 years), and be (e) peer-reviewed and (f) written in English. A mindfulness-based app was defined as any app that was designed for the sole purpose of facilitating mindfulness practice. We excluded studies on Web-only or text-based interventions, as we were most interested in apps for their accessibility and scalability. To avoid sample biases, we also excluded studies of non-smartphone technology (e.g., VR, wearables, tablet apps), which are not yet widely adopted. We also excluded studies on adolescents because many mindfulness apps limit use to adults in their terms and conditions, and because some recent evidence suggests that mindfulness practice may affect adolescents differently than it does adults25. Finally, regarding validated measures, we made an exception for ecological momentary assessment (EMA) studies, which tend to use few items to reduce participant burden.

An electronic literature search was performed by the first author on October 26, 2022, on Pubmed, APA PsycINFO, and Web of Science. The search was updated on August 7, 2023. (For search strategy, see eTable 2). Studies identified were divided among four pairs of reviewers (NM & ZM, NM & TG, NM & ER, NM & SL). Reviewers independently assessed studies based on title and abstract and gave inclusion/exclusion recommendations, which were subsequently compared; any disagreements were resolved through discussion in each pair, consulting JT if consensus could not be reached. The same process was followed for full-text review, data extraction, and quality assessment (QA). The Quality Assessment Tool for Quantitative Studies, which has evidence of validity and reliability26, guided the quality assessment process. The tool outlines assessment criteria for eight domains of bias. Overall QA ratings and domain-specific section ratings for each study appear in eTable 3 and eTable 4, respectively.

The range of clinical and methodological characteristics in the studies included in this review prevented a meta-analysis, and we employed a narrative synthesis of the data. We first grouped studies by thematic similarity. Within each group, we assessed studies by findings, searching for similarities and differences. When findings were contradictory within a group of studies, we examined potential contributors (e.g., differences across studies in sample and study characteristics, such as control group strength, type of app evaluated, and measurement instruments). The results of this process are described in the subsequent sections.

Results

A PRISMA flow diagram summarizing the results of our study selection process appears in eFigure 1. In total, data was collected from 5963 adults across 28 studies that varied widely in terms of location. The mean age across 23 studies that reported it was ~33 (SD = 8.98). Only 17 studies described the racial/ethnic composition of the sample; samples were predominantly White, and none were nationally representative. Approximately 79% identified as female (across the 24 studies that reported on female gender) and 19% as male (across the 17 studies that reported on male gender). Only one study reported on sexual orientation. See Table 2 for detailed sample characteristics.

Table 2.

Sample characteristics.

First author & year (study location) Sample size MF app group (n) Ctrl group (n) Sample description Age (M) Age (SD) Gender Race/ethnicity Sexual orientation
Abbott 2023 (USA) 106 50 56 Adults with elevated anxiety or worry 24 9

80% female

18% male

2% other

62% White, 14% Biracial or Multiracial, 10% Hispanic or Latinx, 8% Asian, 5% Black or AfAm, 2% NatAm 77% straight, 14% bisexual, 4% pansexual, 3% gay/lesbian, 3% asexual
Ainsworth 2022 (UK) 144 93 51 Adults with asthma 51.11 14.65 NR MF App Group: 97% White, 3% Indian. Control Group: 93% White, 2% Chinese/SE Asian, 2% Indian, 2% NR NR
Axelsen 2022 (Denmark) 459 167 292 Healthy adults in small- to medium-sized Danish companies 38.83 9.68

53% male

47% female

NR NR
Bjorkstrand 2019 (Sweden) 26 11 15 Healthy university employees with high educational attainment (>12 yrs) 35.10 6.2

80% female

Other genders NR

NR NR
Gao 2022 (USA) 71 36 35 Adults withs sleep-interfering worry 41.41 17.57

80% female

17% male

3% prefer not to answer

62% White, 15% Asian, 14% Biracial or Multiracial, 7% Hispanic, 1% Black NR
Goldberg 2020 (USA) 343 228 115 University of Wisconsin-Madison faculty, staff, and students 41.74 12.52

85% female

15% male

1% nonbinary

1% prefer not to say

82% White, 10% Multiracial, 5% Asian, 2% Black, 1% Latinx, 1% Prefer not to say NR
Haliwa 2021 (USA) 139 69 70 College students with psychology major 19.43 1.26

81% female

19% male

1% Other

74% White, 8% Black, 6% Hispanic, 6% Asian, 1% NatAm, 5% Other NR
Hirshberg 2022 (USA) 662 344 318 Wisconsin school system employees

<20yo: 0.2%

20–30yo: 15%

30–40yo: 29%

40–50yo: 30%

50–60yo: 21%

>60yo: 4%

NR

88% female

12% male

0.1% non-binary

91% White, 5% Latinx, 4% Black/AfAm, 2% Asian/PI, 1% AI/AN NR
Howells 2016 (11 countries) 121 57 64 “Happiness seekers” (members of self-improvement newsletters & social media groups) 40.70 10.6

87% female

Other genders NR

90% White, 2% Asian/PI, 2% Hispanic, 5% Other/Multiracial, 2% Prefer not to say NR
Huberty 2019 (USA) 72 33 39 Adults with high stress (≥14 on Perceived Stress Scale) 21.18 4.9

90% female

10% male

55% White, 17% Asian, 11% Biracial/Multiracial, 6% Black, 6% Other, 6% Prefer not to say NR
Kubo 2019 (USA) - caregiver sample 26 13 13 Caregivers of patient sample from Kubo 2019 study 63 (median age) NR

58% female

Other genders NR

77% White, 13% Other, 6% Asian, 3% AfAm NR
Kubo 2019 (USA) - patient sample 72 40 32 Patients with cancer currently/recently treated 59 (median age) NR

69% female

Other genders NR

65% White, 18% Other, 7% Asian, 6%AfAm, 4% Unknown NR
Levin 2022 (USA) 16 10 13 Students on college counseling center waitlist 20.43 2.46 100% female 87% White non-Hispanic, 9% White Hispanic; 4% AI & White NR
Low 2020 (Australia) 23 12 11 Adults with subclinical and clinical insomnia 36.39 11.74 13% male NR NR
Ly 2014 (Sweden) 81 41 40 Adults diagnosed with MDD 36.10 10.8

70% female

30% male

NR NR
Orosa-Duarte 2021 (Spain) 84 31 53 Students of health sciences (medicine, psychology, nursing, or nutrition) 23 4.16

85% female

NR for other genders

NR NR
Rich 2021 (UK) 100 45 56 University employees NR NR

70% female

30% male

NR NR
Roy 2021 (USA) 61 28 33 Adults with at least moderate worry (≥10 on GAD-7) 41.95 15.43

90% female

8% male

2% Other

87% White, 3% Black, 2% Asian, 8% Other NR
Sala 2021 (USA) 228 93 135 Adults who smoke 5+ cigarettes a day & had some motivation to quit 41.48 12.48

75% female

Other genders NR

81% White, 10% AfAm, 4% Hispanic/Latinx, 2% Multiracial, 1% Asian, 1% NatAm, & 1% Unknown NR
Schulte-Frankenfeld 2021 (Germany) 64 30 34 College students who work part time 24.75 5.42

64% female

36% male

NR NR
Sun 2021 (China) 168 84 84 Depressed pregnant women (>9 on EPDS or >4 on PHQ-9) 29.91 4.01 100% female 100% Asian (99% Han, 1% Hui) NR
Taylor 2022 (UK) 2182 1095 1087 Adult health care workers in England 40.53 10.97

83% female

16% male

1% transgender male, other, or prefer not to say

93% White, 4% Asian, 2% Mixed or Multiracial, 1% Black NR
van Emmerik 2018 (Netherlands) 377 191 186 Adults with an interest in mindfulness and spirituality 44.72 9.83

96% female

4% male

NR NR
Versluis 2018 (Netherlands) 128 46 90 Adults with work stress 43.23 11.39

74% female

Others NR

NR (95% Dutch) NR
Versluis 2020 (Netherlands) 22 9 13 High-worrying young adults (45+ on PSWQ) 25.36 5.22

68% female

Other genders NR

NR NR
Walsh 2019 (Canada) 86 45 41 College students 20.02 2.53

84% female

NR for the rest

NR NR
Yang 2018 (USA) 88 45 43 All students from a Southeastern US medical school 25.11 NR

64% female

36% male

47% White, 25% Asian/PI, 10% Biracial/Multiracial, 7% Black, 6% Latinx, 6% Other NR
Ziegler 2019 (USA) 40 22 18 Healthy young adults 18–35 (MNR) NR NR NR NR

AfAm African American, AI/AN American Indian Alaskan Native, NatAm Native American, PI Pacific Islander, SE Southeast, NR Not Reported.

Study characteristics

Studies assessed Headspace (n = 12), VGZ Mindfulness Coach (n = 3), Unwinding Anxiety (n = 2), Healthy Minds Program (n = 2), Calm (n = 1), Stop, Breathe & Think (n = 1), Craving to Quit (n = 1), MediTrain (n = 1), Balloon App (n = 1), REM Volver a Casa (n = 1), Spirits Healing (n = 1), Wildflowers (n = 1), and Mindfulness (n = 1). These apps are available on both Apple and Android phones, except two: one offered on iPhones only (Mindfulness app19) and one that was commercially available at the time of investigation but now appears to be defunct (Wildflowers app27). (For more details on these apps, see eTable 7).

Most studies prescribed a specific dose, or amount, of app-delivered mindfulness practice (n = 20), ranging from 10 minutes a day (n = 9), several exercises a day (n = 5), daily (n = 3) or weekly (n = 1), or beginning at 10–20 minutes daily and gradually increasing use (n = 2). (For more details on app features designed to facilitate mindfulness practice, see eTable 7).

All 28 studies had at least one control group. Active control groups tended to be digital in nature, with most involving non-mindfulness apps (n = 10), one offering a WeChat-based health consultation, one a multimedia stress-related psychoeducation website, and one in-person MBSR. Non-mindfulness apps used to control for cognitive expectancies and attention included emotion self-monitoring apps (n = 3), cognitive training apps such as the 2048 app and the Peak app (n = 2), apps delivering other psychological interventions such as behavioral activation and progressive muscle relaxation (n = 2), a list-making app (n = 1), a music app (n = 1), and directions to split time equally among three apps (i.e., Duolingo, Tai Chi app, or logic games) identified in a prior study as matched in cognitive outcome expectancy (n = 1). Passive control group participants were either waitlisted (n = 15), offered treatment as usual (n = 2), or provided with no intervention (n = 1). See Table 3.

Table 3.

Study characteristics.

First author & year (study location) Mindfulness (MF) app tested MF app platforma Active control group Passive control group Type of active control group Type of passive control groupb Supportc Incentivesd Dropout rate Intervention period (weeks) Follow-up time point (weeks)
Abbott 2023 (USA) Headspace Both - Y - WL 0 1 35% 4 -
Ainsworth 2022 (UK) Headspace Both - Y - WL 0 0 30% 6 12
Axelsen 2022 (Denmark) Headspace Both Y Y Music app No intervention 1 0 26% 4 -
Bjorkstrand 2019 (Sweden) Headspace Both - Y - WL NR 2 0% 4 -
Gao 2022 (USA) Unwinding Anxiety Both - Y - TAU 1 2 11% 8 16
Goldberg 2020 (USA) Healthy Minds Program Both - Y - WL 0 2 46% 8 -
Haliwa 2021 (USA) Headspace Both Y - Peak app (cognitive training) - 1 2 10% 1.43 -
Hirshberg 2022 (USA) Healthy Minds Program Both - Y - WL 1 2 13% 4 12
Howells 2016 (11 countries) Headspace Both Y Catch Notes (list-making app) - 1 NR 38% 1.43 -
Huberty 2019 (USA) Calm Both - Y - WL 1 2 19% 8 12
Kubo 2019 (USA) - cg Headspace Both - Y - WL 3 3 16% 8 -
Kubo 2019 (USA) - pt Headspace Both - Y - WL 3 3 26% 8 -
Levin 2022 (USA) Stop, Breathe, and Think Both - Y - WL 0 0 30% 4 -
Low 2020 (Australia) Headspace Both Y - Headspace PMR app - NR 5 0% 6.85 -
Ly 2014 (Sweden) Mindfulness Apple Y - Beh. activ. (BA) app by researchers - 2 NR 14% 8 24
Orosa-Duarte 2021 (Spain) REM Volver a casa Both Y Y Weekly in-person MBSR WL NR 4 45% 8 -
Rich 2021 (UK) Headspace Both - Y - WL 1 NR 19% 8 -
Roy 2021 (USA) Unwinding Anxiety Both - Y - TAU 3 2 1% 4 8
Sala 2021 (USA) Craving to Quit Both Y - App w/ same look as MF app but only exp. sampling - 1 3 27% 3.14 -
Schulte-Frankenfeld 2021 (Germany) Balloon App Both - Y - WL NR 0 35% 8 -
Sun 2021 (China) Spirits Healing Both Y - WeChat text-based consultation - 1 1 31% 8 18
Taylor 2022 (UK) Headspace Both Y - Moodzone (psychoed site) - 1 0 35% 4 18
van Emmerik 2018 (The Netherlands) VGZ Mindfulness Coach Both - Y - WL 1 0 41% 8 20
Versluis 2018 (Netherlands) VGZ Mindfulness Coach Both Y Y Emotion self-monitoring WL 2 2 13% 4 -
Versluis 2020 (Netherlands) VGZ Mindfulness Coach Both Y - Emotion self-monitoring - 1 2 15% 4 -
Walsh 2019 (Canada) Wildflowers app Both Y - 2048 app (cognitive training) - 1 1 20% 3 -
Yang 2018 (USA) Headspace Both - Y - WL 0 0 24% 4 8
Ziegler 2019 (USA) MediTrain Both Y -

Duolingo

Tai Chi app

logic games app

- 3 1 25% 6 -

aApple, Android, or both; bWL waitlist, TAU treatment as usual; cSupport: 0 = none offered, 1 = automated reminders to use app,

2 = human support provided, 3 = both 1 & 2, NR not reported; d0 = none offered, 1 = financial incentives to use the app, 2 = financial incentives for self-report completion only, 3 = both 1 & 2, 4 = other incentive, NR not reported. cg caregiver sample. pt patient sample.

The average intervention phase lasted ~5.46 weeks (SD = 2.23). In all studies, participants were asked to train with the mindfulness app on their own (rather than in a controlled lab environment). Outcomes were measured with pre- and post-intervention self-report questionnaires in all studies but three. These three studies used objective behavioral tasks to measure outcomes, with one administering a gamified app remotely28 and two administering cognitive tasks in a lab environment27,29. Only 10 studies included follow-up assessments (i.e., assessments taking place at least one month after the end of the intervention period) to examine whether changes in the outcomes of interest to this review were sustained in the long term. (See Table 3).

App engagement metrics reported varied widely. Some reported engagement in terms of average number of minutes of app use (total or per day or week), average days practiced, and average number of app sessions/exercises completed (total or per day). As such, it was difficult to determine patterns of engagement across studies. To identify patterns, we grouped studies with similar metrics by intervention length and computed ratios based on the two metrics most often reported. Results indicated that engagement was generally low (see eTable 5).

Methodological quality

Overall, study quality was rated as moderate to weak, with all studies having some concerns (see eTable 3). Most studies minimized measurement, allocation, and detection bias, as they assessed outcomes with valid and reliable measures or tasks, used appropriate allocation methods, and ensured research staff were blinded to condition. Bias tended to arise in terms of selection, attrition, and lack of attention on minimizing potential confounders. Most studies used self-referred convenience samples from one setting, and attrition rates ranged from moderate (i.e., 21%–40%) to high (i.e., >40%), with an average of 23% (SD = 13%) across studies. Most studies did not adjust for important confounders (see eTable 4 note). In addition, 12 studies were underpowered. Implementation bias was difficult to detect, as most studies did not report the percentage of participants who received the allocated intervention as it was intended (i.e., recommended dose of app use).

Outcomes and findings

Across 28 studies, 67 outcome comparisons were made between the intervention and control group. Of these 67 comparisons, 35 (53%) revealed a between-group difference favoring the intervention group. Of the 35 between-group effects favoring the intervention group, most were found when the mindfulness app was evaluated against a passive (n = 28; 65%) versus an active (n = 7; 30%) control group. (Note: Passive, or inactive, control groups involved either waitlisting participants, or offering them treatment as usual or no intervention. Active control groups offered participants a comparable task to engage in, such as a non-mindfulness app.) Effect sizes tended to be moderate to large across domains, and gains from using mindfulness apps were generally sustained at follow-up. (See Table 4). Results by outcome domain appear in Table 4 and Fig. 1.

Table 4.

Study findings by outcome category.

T1 effecta Study MF appb Results (at post-intervention) Effect sustained at follow-up?
Awareness (n = 15)
1 Levin 2022 SBT Medium between-group effect favoring the MF app for acting with awareness, Hedge’s g = 0.68 (CI −0.17, 1.58) N/A
1 Hirshberg 2022 HMP Small between-group effect favoring MF app for mindful action, d = 0.21 (CI 0.06, 0.36), p < 0.01 No. Trend toward significant effect at 12-week follow-up, d = 0.14 (CI −0.01, 0.29), p = 0.07
1 Rich 2021 Headspace Medium between-group effect favoring MF app group for acting with awareness, F(1,122) = 8.05, p < 0.01, d = 0.51 N/A
1 Roy 2021 Unwinding Anxiety Large between-group effect favoring MF app group for interoceptive awareness, median increase of 22 (IQR 30, p < 0. 01, r = 0.72) in MF app group & no change in controls Yes, effect persisted at 8-week follow-up, with median increase of 26 (IQR 28.5, p < 0.01, r = 0.85) in MF app group and no sig. change in control group
1 Orosa 2021(p) REM Between-group effect favoring MF app group; larger changes in MF app group than controls, change = 3.6 (CI 0.1, 7.1) N/A
1 van Emmerik 2018 VGZ Mindfulness Coach Medium between-group effect favoring MF app group, b = 2.95, SE = 0.59, p < 0.01, d = 0.49 Yes, gains maintained at 20-week follow-up, b = 2.56, SE = 0.70, p < 0.01, d = 0.57
1 Huberty 2019 Calm Between-group effect favoring MF app group; greater significant improvement in MF app (vs. control) group (change = 4.74, p < 0.01, effect size 0.83) Yes, changes sustained at 12-week follow-up
2 Orosa 2021 (a) REM No between-group differences; both app and active control group improved N/A
2 Yang 2018 Headspace NR (only changes in primary outcomes were reported from pre- to post-intervention) No between-group differences; both MF app and controls improved from baseline to follow-up 8 weeks later, F(2,138) = 4.29, p < 0.05
2 Kubo 2019 (pt) Headspace No between-group differences but trend toward significant between-group effect favoring MF app group, F = 3.74, p = 0.06, d = 0.43; MF app group had significant within-group increase from baseline (M 17.2, SD 3.8) to post-intervention (M 18.5, SD 3.5), p < 0.05 N/A
2 Ainsworth 2022 Headspace No between-group differences but significant medium-sized increase in mindful awareness at 6 weeks in MF app group, mean diff −2.20 (CI −3.92, −0.48), d = 0.32 Yes, improvement of large effect size sustained at 12-week follow-up in MF app group, mean diff = −4.65 (CI −6.19, −3.10), d = 0.74
2 Sala 2021 Craving to Quit No between-group differences; awareness increased in both groups, b = 0.01, SE = 0.01 (CI 0.00, 0.02), p < 0.05 N/A
3 Walsh 2021 Wildflowers No between- or within-group differences N/A
3 Haliwa 2021 Headspace No between- or within-group differences N/A
3 Kubo 2019 (cg) Headspace No between- or within-group differences N/A
Non-reactivity (n = 12)
1 Gao 2022 Unwinding Anxiety Significant between-group effect favoring MF app group, β = 3.8, SE = 0.78, p < 0.01; MF app group (control group) had a 27% (4%) average increase in non-reactivity Yes, gains in MF group maintained at 16-week follow-up, p < 0.01
1 Rich 2021 Headspace Medium between-group effect favoring MF app group for non-reactivity, F(1, 122) = 4.78, p < 0.05, d = 0.39 N/A
1 Roy 2021 Unwinding Anxiety Large between-group effect favoring MF app group for non-reactivity, with median increase of 5 (IQR 6.3, p < 0.01, r = 0.95) in MF app group and no change in controls Yes, effect persisted at 8-week follow-up, with median increase of 7.5 (IQR 6, p < 0.01, r = 0.95) in MF app group and no change in controls
1 van Emmerik 2018 VGZ Medium between-group effect favoring MF app group for non-reactivity, b = 2.16, SE = 0.49, p < 0.01, d = 0.43 Yes, gains maintained at 20-week follow-up, b = 3.03, SE = 0.60, p < 0.01, d = 0.77
1 Huberty 2019 Calm Between-group effect favoring MF app group; greater improvement in MF app (vs. control) group for non-reactivity (change = 3.78, p < 0.01, effect size 0.92) Yes, changes sustained at 12-week follow-up
1 Orosa 2021(p) REM Between-group effect favoring MF app group; larger changes in MF app group than controls, change = 4.4 (CI 1.6, 7.1). N/A
2 Orosa 2021(a) REM No between-group differences; both app and active control group improved N/A
2 Yang 2018 Headspace NR No between-group differences; both MF app and active control group improved on non-reactivity from T1 (baseline) to T3 (follow-up 8 weeks later), F(2,138) = 11.45, p < 0.01
2 Kubo 2019 (pt) Headspace No between-group differences in non-reactivity but trend toward effect favoring the MF app group, F = 2.94, p = 0.09, d = 0.45; MF app group had a significant within-group increase from baseline (M 14.9, SD 3.7) to post-intervention (M 16.6, SD 3.3), p < .05. N/A
3 Kubo 2019 (cg) Headspace No between-group differences in non-judgment; controls had within-group improvement from baseline (M 17.1, SD 4.2) to post-intervention (M 19.2, SD 5.1), p < 0.05
3 Haliwa 2021 Headspace No between- or within-group differences in non-reactivity N/A
0 Levin 2022 SBT Small between-group effect favoring the control group for non-reactivity, Hedge’s g = −0.31 (CI −1.17, 0.54) N/A
Non-judgment (n = 10)
1 Levin 2022 SBT Medium between-group effect favoring MF app group for non-judgment, Hedge’s g = 0.56 (CI −0.28, 1.46) N/A
1 Orosa 2021(p) REM Between-group effect favoring MF app group; larger changes in MF app (vs. control) group, change = 5.7 (CI 2.2, 9.2) N/A
1 van Emmerik 2018 VGZ Small-to-medium between-group effect favoring MF app group, b = 2.19, SE = 0.71, p < 0.01, d = 0.34 Yes, gains maintained at 20-week follow-up, b = 2.68, SE = 0.76, p < 0.01, d = 0.47
1 Huberty 2019 Calm Between-group effect favoring MF app group; greater improvement in MF app (vs. control) group for non-judgment, change = 4.94, p < 0.01, effect size 0.76 Yes, changes sustained at 12-week follow-up
2 Orosa 2021(a) REM No between-group differences; both groups improved N/A
2 Kubo 2019 (pt’s) Headspace No between-group differences in non-judgment; MF app group had a significant within-group increase from baseline (M 17.3, SD 4.9) to post-intervention (M 18.4, SD 4.2), p < 0.05, but no change in controls N/A
2 Haliwa 2021 Headspace No between-group differences; significant increase in both groups, F(1,137) = 8.57, p < 0.01, np2 = 0.06 N/A
3 Rich 2021 Headspace No between-group differences; trend toward significant btn-group effect favoring MF app group, F(1,122) = 3.32, p = 0.07, d = 0.33. (Note: Significant btn-grp effect for MF app group for completers of Headspace foundation course) N/A
3 Yang 2018 Headspace NR No between-group differences; trend toward significant increase in both MF app and controls from baseline to follow-up 8 weeks later, F(2,140) = 2.83, p = 0.06
3 Kubo 2019 (cg) Headspace No between- or within-group differences N/A
Positive affect (n = 5)
1 Sun 2021 Spirits Healing Medium between-group effect favoring the MF app group for positive affect, as indicated by significant group by time interaction, x2_4 = 8.4, p < 0.05. N/A
1 Howells 2016 Headspace Medium between-group effect favoring the MF app group for positive affect, F = 9.13, p < 0.01, np2 = 0.07 N/A
2 Haliwa 2021 Headspace No between-group differences in positive affect; significant increase in both MF app group, F(9,129) = 4.65, p < 0.01, np2 = 0.33, and control group, F(9,129) = 3.60, p < 0.01, np2 = 0.20 N/A
2 Low 2020 Headspace No between-group differences; both groups improved on daytime positive affect, F(1,21) = 5.84, p < 0.05
3 Versluis 2020 VGZ No between- or within-group differences N/A
Repetitive negative thinking: worry (n = 7)
1 Taylor 2022 Headspace Small between-group effect favoring MF app group in terms of worry reduction, b = −0.30, SE = 0.11 (CI −0.51, −0.09), p < 0.01 Between-group differences were significant at 4.5 months
1 Gao 2022 Unwinding Anxiety Significant between-group effect favoring MF app group for worry, β = -6.4, SE = 1.89, p < 0.01; MF app (control) group had an average worry reduction of 12% (0.3%) Yes, gains in MF group were maintained at 16-week follow-up, p < 0.01
1 Roy 2021 Unwinding Anxiety Medium-to-large between-group effect favoring the MF app group for worry, with median reduction of 7.5 (IQR 8.5, p < 0.01, r = 0.67) in MF app group but of 3 (IQR 4, p = 0.01, r = 0.44) in control grp. Mediation analysis revealed that worry reduction partially mediated the relationship between mindfulness training and anxiety reduction at 2 months, indirect effect = −0.19 (CI 0.40, −0.02), p < 0.05 Yes, effect persisted at 8-week follow-up, with median reduction of 15 (IQR 14.3, p < 0.01, r = 0.88) in MF app group and of 3 (IQR 6, p < 0.01, r = 0.61) in control group
2 Versluis 2018 (a) VGZ No between-group differences in trait worry, which decreased over time for all participants, B = −1.18, p < 0.05 N/A
2 Versluis 2018 (p) VGZ No between-group differences in trait worry, which decreased over time for all participants, B = −1.18, p < 0.05 N/A
3 Versluis 2020 VGZ No between- or within-group differences N/A
3 Abbott 2023 Headspace No between- or within-group differences at 4 weeks N/A
Repetitive negative thinking: perseverative thinking (n = 2)
1 Hirshberg 2022 HMP Small-to-medium between-group effect favoring MF app, d = −0.35 (CI −0.51, −0.20), p < 0.01 Yes, persisted at 3-month follow-up, d = −0.22 (CI −0.37, −0.07), p < 0.05
1 Goldberg 2020 HMP MF app group (vs controls) showed greater improvements in perseverative thinking, ddiff = −0.18, p = 0.01 N/A
Repetitive negative thinking: rumination (n = 1)
4 Taylor 2022 Headspace No between-group differences in rumination but trend toward significant effect favoring the MF app group, b = −0.06, SE = 0.03 (CI −0.12 to 0), p = 0.06 No
Attention regulation (n = 4)
1 Walsh 2019 Wildflowers Small-to-medium between-group effect favoring the MF app group for the conflict monitoring component of attentional control, estimate = −0.47 (0.21), t(84) = −2.29, p < 0.05, effect size = −0.24; no between- or within-group changes in alerting or orienting for either group N/A
1 Axelsen 2022 (a) Headspace Large between-group effect favoring MF app group for sustained attention, F(2, 459) = 17.97, p < 0.01; greater significant changes in MF app group, paired t(166) = −10.37, p < 0.01, d = −0.80, than in active control group, paired t(151) = −3.62, p < 0.01, d = −0.30 N/A
1 Axelsen 2022 (p) Headspace Large between-group effect favoring MF app for sustained attention, F(2,459) = 17.97, p < 0.01; greater significant changes in MF app group, paired t(166) = −10.37, p < 0.01, d = −0.80, but none in passive control group N/A
1 Ziegler 2019 MediTrain Medium-to-large between-group effect favoring MF app group in sustained attention, F(1,37) = 6.4 (CI −17.8, −2.0), p < 0.05, d = −0.66 N/A
Decentering/defusion (n = 3)
1 Hirshberg 2022 HMP Medium between-group effect favoring MF app, d = 0.40 (CI 0.25, 0.56), p < 0.01 Yes, persisted at 3-month follow-up, d = 0.35 (CI 0.20, 0.50), p < 0.01
1 Haliwa 2021 Headspace Large effect favoring the MF app group for decentering, F(9,129) = 7.99, p < 0.01, np2 = 0.36 N/A
1 Goldberg 2020 HMP MF app (versus control) group showed greater increases in defusion, ddiff = 0.41, p < 0.01 N/A
Acceptance/psychological flexibility (n = 3)
2 Ly 2014 MF app dev. by res group No significant between-group differences; medium-to-large within-group differences for both MF app group, d = 1.06 (CI −2.33 to 4.44), p < 0.05, and controls, d = 0.80 (CI −1.61, 3.21), p < 0.01 Only MF app group sustained improvement at 6-month follow-up, d = 1.68 (CI −1.42, 4.78), p < 0.01
3 Ainsworth 2022 Headspace No between- or within-group differences No between- or within-group differences in mindful acceptance at 12-week follow-up
3 Versluis 2020 VGZ No between- or within-group differences N/A
Reappraisal, suppression, self-regulation, values, & extinction (n = 5)
1 Schulte 2021 Balloon App Large between-group effect favoring MF app group for reappraisal, F = 9.72, p < 0.01, np2 = 0.14 N/A
1 Schulte 2021 Balloon App Large between-group effect favoring MF app group for self-regulation, F = 15.05, p < 0.01, np2 = 0.20 N/A
1 Levin 2022 SBT Large between-group effect favoring MF app for values progress, Hedge’s g = 0.85 (CI −0.06, 1.83) N/A
1 Bjorkstrand 2019 Headspace MF app group had greater significant retention of extinction learning compared to control group, as indicated by less spontaneous recovery of conditioned threat responses in the 24 h after extinction training, t = 2.47, p < 0.05, d = 0.98 N/A
2 Schulte 2021 Balloon App No between-group differences in suppression; both groups improved, F = 5.71, p < 0.05, np2 = 0.08 N/A

Note. a. T1 Effect = Effect at post-intervention. MF app group = Mindfulness app group. 0 = between-group effect favoring the control group, 1 = between-group effect favoring the MF app group, 2 = no between-group differences as both groups improved or there was a within-group difference favoring MF app group, 3 = no between-group differences as neither group improved or there was a within-group difference favoring control group, 4 = no between-group differences and unclear whether both or neither improved. b. SBT = Stop, Breathe, & Think, HMP = Healthy Minds Program, REM = REM Volver a casa, VGZ = VGZ Mindfulness Coach. *Waitlist control participants received the intervention at the end of the 4-week intervention period. (a) = comparison with active control group. (p) = comparison with passive control group. (pt) patient sample. (cg) caregiver sample.

Fig. 1. Summary of results.

Fig. 1

Dark green represents a between-group effect favoring the mindfulness app group; orange represents a between-group effect favoring the control group. Light green denotes studies that found no between-group effect (i.e., both groups improved or within-group effect favoring the mindfulness app group present); gray denotes studies that found no between-group effect (i.e., neither group improved or within-group effect favoring the control group). Light blue represents no between-group effect but unclear whether both or neither group improved. Rep. Neg. Thkg = Repetitive Negative Thinking.

Awareness. The most frequently examined outcome was awareness, assessed in 15 comparisons and measured with the Acting With Awareness subscale of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ30)3136 or of its short-form version (FFMQ-SF37)3840, a one-item measure based on the FFMQ Acting With Awareness subscale in an experience sampling study41, the Acceptance subscale of the Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale (PHLMS42)43, the Multidimensional Assessment of Interoceptive Awareness (MAIA44)45, or the Interoceptive Respiration Task27. Findings were mixed, with about half the studies (n = 7) finding an effect favoring the intervention group (small to large effect sizes), five finding that both groups improved, and three that neither improved. Studies that found an effect favoring the intervention (versus those that did not) used passive control groups and tended to have samples with a greater female composition (see eTable 6). The four studies that used active control groups found that either both groups improved39,41 or neither did27,36.

Nonreactivity was assessed in 12 comparisons and measured with the nonreactivity subscale of the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ31) in all but two studies that instead used the nonreactivity subscale from its 24-item short-form version (FFMQ-SF37)38,40. Findings were mixed, with six comparisons yielding an effect favoring the mindfulness app (medium to large effect sizes)34,35,38,39,45,46, three showing that both groups improved33,39,40, two that neither did36,40, and one yielding an effect favoring the control group30. All six comparisons that yielded an effect favoring the mindfulness app were made with passive control groups and tended to have samples with a greater female composition. Two studies that used active control groups found that either both groups improved39 or neither did36. The study finding an effect favoring the control group had a very small sample size and was underpowered30. No consistent associations between intervention length and outcomes were apparent across studies.

Non-judgment was assessed in 10 comparisons, using the non-judging of inner experience subscale from either the Five Facet Mindfulness Questionnaire (FFMQ31) or its short-form version (FFMQ-SF37). Findings were mixed, with four finding an effect favoring the mindfulness app30,34,35,39, three that both groups improved36,39,40, and three that neither improved33,40,47. Only two studies used active control groups, both finding that both groups improved36,39.

Positive affect was examined in five studies and measured with the Positive and Negative Affect Scale48,49 or one-item measures in EMA studies36,50,51. Findings were mixed, with two finding an effect favoring the intervention group48,49, two that both groups improved36,51, and one that neither group improved.52 All five studies used an active control group, although in two, control groups were non-equivalent48,52. Two of the three that found no between-group differences were underpowered51,52, and in one, the intervention app dose varied across participants, with some receiving it for 40 days and some for 6051. The two studies that found a between-group difference had samples with a greater female composition.

Repetitive negative thinking. Ten comparisons assessed repetitive negative thinking styles, including worry (n = 7), perseverative thinking (n = 2), and rumination (n = 1). Worry was assessed with the Penn State Worry Questionnaire. Three studies found an effect favoring the intervention group, with small to large effect sizes45,46,53, and one of these had an active control group53. Two studies that found that neither group improved were underpowered52,54. Studies that found a between-group difference (versus none) had samples with a greater female composition.

Two studies examined perseverative thinking32,55, assessing it with the Perseverative Thinking Questionnaire (PTQ56), a measure of both worry and rumination, and using a waitlist control group. Both studies found an effect favoring the mindfulness app. Only one study examined rumination directly53, measuring it with the brooding subscale of the Ruminative Response Scale (RRS57); no significant between-group differences were found.

Attention regulation was evaluated in only three studies (that yielded four group comparisons) and measured with behavioral tasks, including the Centre for Research on Safe Driving-Attention Network Test (CRSD-ANT58)27, which is a validated briefer version of the Attention Network Test (ANT59); the validated sustained attention task Test of Variables of Attention (TOVA60)29; and a gamified sustained attention task (“Go Sushi Go”)28 based on the validated Sustained Attention to Response Task (SART61). All four yielded an effect favoring the intervention group, with effect sizes ranging from small to large. All studies used an active control group.

Decentering/defusion was examined in three studies. Two32,55 used the Drexel Defusion Scale62 and one36 the decentering subscale of the Toronto Mindfulness Scale63. All three found a between-group difference favoring the intervention group; one had an active control group36.

Acceptance/psychological flexibility was examined in three studies and measured with the acceptance subscale of the Philadelphia Mindfulness Scale (PHLMS64)43, or with the English65 or Dutch52 version of the Acceptance and Action Questionnaire—II (AAQ-II66). No between-group differences were found; one study that used an active control group of a behavioral activation app found that both groups improved65. Two other studies found that neither group improved43,52, although one was underpowered52.

Finally, only one study each examined self-regulation, reappraisal, suppression, values, and extinction, with one study examining the first three against a waitlist control group67 using the Self-Regulation Scale68 and the German version of the Emotion Regulation Questionnaire69. This study found a between-group effect favoring the app group for self-regulation and reappraisal, but not suppression. One study assessed behavioral enactment of values30 with the Valuing Questionnaire70 and used a waitlist control group; results favored the intervention over the control group. The study that examined extinction71 used a two-day lab-based aversive Pavlovian conditioning and extinction procedure and a waitlist control group. Results showed that after using the mindfulness app for 4 weeks, the intervention (versus waitlist control) group had greater retention of extinction learning, as demonstrated by less spontaneous recovery of conditioned threat responses one day after extinction training.

Mediation analysis

Only two studies conducted mediation analysis with a psychological disorder as an outcome. One study found that worry partially mediated the relationship between mindfulness practice and anxiety45 and the other that worry fully mediated the association between mindfulness training and worry-related sleep disturbance46.

Heterogeneity & certainty of evidence

The range of populations in which apps were evaluated and inconsistent app engagement likely contributed to heterogeneity in findings. Methodological quality was also a likely contributor to inconsistent findings, as quality was moderate to low across studies. In the awareness domain, for example, of studies that found no between-group differences, one was underpowered,27one used a single-item measure that did not correlate highly with the full measure41, another had a 45% dropout rate39, and in another, data came from only 4% of eligible patients who enrolled43. Such methodological weaknesses, found across domains, likely increased the heterogeneity of findings and lower confidence that the lack of effects was due to a lack of app efficacy.

Methodological weaknesses also lower the certainty of evidence in domains with more consistent findings. In most domains, when effects favoring the mindfulness apps were found, most or all were from studies with passive, rather than active, control groups. In only two domains did all studies use active control groups: positive affect and attention regulation. However, in the positive affect domain, studies finding an effect favoring the mindfulness app group had relatively high attrition rates (38% and 35%), lowering confidence in findings. (For context, the average attrition rate in a recent meta-analysis of mHealth studies was 24%;72 objectively, attrition rates of up to 20% are considered ideal, and those nearing 40% are deemed to be high as they risk introducing bias26).

The domain of attention regulation was the strongest set of studies. All studies in this domain employed not just an active digital control group but also objective task measures to assess outcomes, increasing the certainty of evidence, although more studies are needed in this domain.

Discussion

This systematic review identified 28 RCTs that evaluated a mindfulness app and examined as an outcome at least one theoretically and empirically supported mechanism of mindfulness practice. By focusing on mechanisms, this review aimed to provide a more nuanced understanding of the psychological impact of mindfulness apps. Overall, more research is needed in most outcome domains assessed in this review. Effects tended to favor the mindfulness app (versus control) group in the domains of attention regulation, repetitive negative thinking, and decentering/defusion, and findings were mixed in the domains of awareness, nonreactivity, non-judgment, positive affect, and acceptance/psychological flexibility. Various methodological issues, population characteristics, and app engagement problems likely contributed to the heterogeneity of findings.

The attention regulation domain was the strongest set of research studies. Results favoring the mindfulness app group in this domain are promising and consistent with other findings suggesting that in-person MBIs have positive effects on executive function73,74. They are also consistent with other study findings suggesting that those with (versus without) meditation experience exhibit greater cognitive flexibility75.

A trend that became apparent across most sets of studies is that studies with more female participants tended to more consistently find effects favoring the mindfulness app group. This trend is in line with other recent findings suggesting that females (versus males) may benefit more from mindfulness-based interventions14,7678. Some have suggested that this difference may be due to the fact that mindfulness targets rumination, a problematic emotion regulation strategy more often used by females than males; in contrast, men tend to more often use distraction, and the focus on the present-moment experience that mindfulness training requires may initially increase negative affect for men76. Based on this finding, more research into these potential gender differences is warranted. If this finding is indeed replicated, gender-specific modifications in app delivery for males (e.g., emphasis on non-judgmental observation of experience) may be beneficial.

Another likely moderator of mixed findings was app engagement. Engagement metrics reported across studies varied widely, and it was difficult to assess overall engagement across the majority of studies. From the available metrics, however, engagement appeared to be generally low. The lack of consensus on engagement metrics is a recognized challenge in the mHealth space79,80, as is the difficulty sustaining engagement over time81. Notably, some studies that found no between-group differences found a mindfulness app effect at higher engagement rates38,40. Such findings are in line with evidence of a dose-response relationship between home practice and outcomes in in-person MBIs, which also demonstrate problems with adherence to at-home mindfulness practice, as data suggests that MBI participants complete, on average, only about 64% of the assigned amount of home practice5. This nevertheless amounts to a much higher rate of daily practice than seen in the studies of mindfulness apps in this review, underscoring the importance of incorporating strategies to increase app engagement so that the efficacy of these apps can be better evaluated.

It is also worth noting two other potential contributors to heterogeneity that relate to broader issues in the field. There is a lack of consensus on the definition of mindfulness, and the resulting diverse mindfulness conceptualizations82 may lead to different teams emphasizing different aspects of mindfulness practice during intervention implementation—differences that may have contributed to heterogeneity in outcomes. In addition, despite more mechanism-driven research into in-person MBIs over the past decade, these mechanisms are not yet well understood83, with some leading mindfulness mechanism theories at times yielding mixed support84. A better understanding of the transdiagnostic factors through which in-person MBIs impact change in mental health outcomes will lead not just to more refined mHealth interventions but also stronger evidence for the theories informing these interventions.

Limitations of body of evidence and future directions

To advance this literature, we propose several future directions and research recommendations. First, future studies replicating these findings should employ strategies that foster app engagement. Sustained app engagement is key to obtaining accurate estimates of apps’ impact on various outcomes. In addition, although the use of incentives is acceptable in (and in line with the goals of) earlier stages of research, it is not a scalable strategy for real-world dissemination. Selecting theory-based strategies (e.g., goal-setting features, support) and building them into an app’s design, even in earlier stages of research, paves the way toward creating efficacious apps that have a greater likelihood of successful dissemination.

Related, more fine-grained details on app engagement would likely aid in resolving some of the inconsistent findings. Even mindfulness apps have a variety of features, some of which do not necessarily strengthen practice (e.g., soothing sounds or music that several apps offered, as seen in eTable 7). Better understanding how participants were using apps could help clarify why app use, in some cases, was less impactful. In addition, some people stop engaging with apps as they achieve their mental health goals, a phenomenon referred to in the literature as “e-attainment.”85 Thus, in some cases, discontinuation could be associated with positive outcomes, as some may have stopped using the app because mindfulness practice became a part of their routines. Thus, assessing reasons for app discontinuation can also help clarify inconsistent outcomes.

Second, future studies should better control for digital placebo effects. Many of the studies that found app effects used passive control groups, which provides encouraging evidence but does not rule out the possibility that improvements were due to simply using an app rather than to the mindfulness-specific aspects of the app. At the same time, active control groups should be chosen with careful consideration. For example, one study used a progressive muscle relaxation app as an active control and found no between-group differences in positive affect51. This finding may be expected, however, as relaxation has also been found to increase positive affect86.

Third, future studies should carefully consider the measurement of mindfulness-related constructs. There is growing concern that the conceptualization of mindfulness—and thus its measurement—is culturally biased, with some evidence suggesting that such widely used measures as the FFMQ may not actually perform well in non-Western populations87. Without this awareness, researchers risk continuing to build a body of evidence based on mindfulness definitions that are not necessarily universally accessible. Fortunately, alternative, more culturally relevant measures are starting to be developed88. In addition, although objective outcome measures are often not widely available, when they are, they should be used in future studies. Some examples of objective outcome measures include app-based cognitive games that are gamified versions of validated neuropsychological paradigms28, implicit tasks (e.g., the IPANAT for positive affect89), wearables to measure physiological reactivity (which, when combined with self-reported arousal, can be a measure of experiential avoidance90), or rumination induction tasks91 to assess whether participants who have been practicing mindfulness more are better able to exit such repetitive negative thinking states. Confidence in findings from self-report measures can be strengthened by the addition of objective measures.

With respect to study population, future studies should evaluate apps in nationally representative samples to increase the generalizability of findings. However, studies should also continue to evaluate apps in specific populations but test population-specific, theory-driven hypotheses about specific mechanisms most pertinent to that population. Doing so can help inform ways to tailor app delivery to each population to better target mechanisms. Related, greater empirical focus is needed on evaluating mindfulness apps in minoritized populations, who continue to be underrepresented in mHealth research92—a trend that also became apparent in the studies included in this review. Some evidence suggests that being African American is associated with lower odds of accessing and continuing to use a leading commercially available mindfulness app93, and lower educational attainment is also associated with lower odds of app access93. It is critical that future research studies focus on minoritized populations to avoid perpetuating disparities and introducing new ones in the form of digital inequities.

In addition, most studies did not report on implementation details, including details on how mindfulness was explained to participants. Yet how an intervention is introduced affects engagement and outcomes94,95, and calls have been made for mindfulness intervention studies to report on the explicit instruction given to participants regarding mindfulness82. This is especially important, given evidence that core aspects of mindfulness practice are often misunderstood by the general public96 and given the different conceptualizations of mindfulness82 that may lead to differences in intervention design and implementation. Better reporting on instruction details may elucidate some heterogeneity in findings. Researchers can also focus on other aspects of delivery beyond instructions, such as tailoring recommendations regarding timing and practice. For example, in samples of socioeconomically disadvantaged individuals facing multiple daily stressors, special attention could be placed on creating a tailored practice schedule. This discussion would help integrate mindfulness practice into their daily routine and better relate the practice to their specific challenges (e.g., constant worry regarding financial strain). This strategy may increase app relevance to each population’s contextual factors and heighten the app’s impact on hypothesized mechanisms.

Finally, moderators should be conceptualized and measured. While heterogeneity is often viewed as a signal of low efficacy, it is, in fact, normal and expected97. Aside from main and mediating effects, it is also important to consider when and for whom app effects are strengthened or weakened. Population-specific moderator hypotheses can relate to technology (e.g., app features), the individual (e.g., beliefs about technology), and their context (e.g., app integration into lifestyle). Special consideration should be paid to gender differences to increase our understanding of how gender influences mindfulness app outcomes. Overall, there has been little empirical focus on individual differences in the broader MBI literature too98, a gap that needs to be addressed in both of these areas of research.

Guidance for clinicians: integrating apps into care

Although this review focuses on mindfulness apps’ clinical foundation, it is important to note that evidence of efficacy is just one of the five factors clinicians need to consider when selecting apps to recommend to patients. The other four factors are described in the APA app evaluation model99, a framework for helping clinicians choose suitable apps: accessibility (e.g., app cost, offline features), privacy and safety (i.e., data protection), app usability, and data integration toward the therapeutic goal (e.g., can app data be easily shared with the provider?)99. To ease the process of evaluating these factors, clinicians can use an app database, such as mindapps.org, a constantly updated database designed to make the APA framework easily actionable for public use. Using such tools can leave clinicians empowered to integrate mindfulness apps that may improve outcomes into care.

Limitations

Several limitations of this review are worth noting. First, we did not extend the search into gray literature, which may bias results to only published evidence. Second, despite efforts to be inclusive of mindfulness mechanisms, we neglected to include self-compassion, one mechanism that has also been theoretically and empirically supported100. Future research should extend the focus on this important potential intermediary outcome of mindfulness app use. Third, our review did not focus on SMS-based interventions, which are also promising digital mental health tools that can enhance the impact of MBIs101 and thus warrant future empirical attention. Finally, given that our research question focused on discrete mechanistic targets that theories suggest would change after the onset of mindfulness practice, we excluded studies that only reported on composite measures of mindfulness (e.g., FFMQ, MAAS). Given that these scales measured several of our constructs of interest together, they were deemed out of the scope of this review. Although this limitation was partially addressed by a recent meta-analysis on composite measures of mindfulness as an outcome of mindfulness app interventions14, whether included studies examined mindfulness as a mechanism was not reported. Thus, a future review on this topic may be potentially fruitful.

Conclusion

Mindfulness-based mobile apps can not only enhance mental health treatment but also offer scalable solutions to address barriers to in-person MBI access. The literature on the psychological impact of mindfulness apps is still nascent and suggests that mindfulness-based apps are promising, especially for regulating attention, reducing repetitive negative thinking, and promoting decentering/defusion. Continuing to elucidate mindfulness apps’ impact on processes of change that account for transdiagnostic symptom reduction is crucial in optimizing app design to enhance app efficacy and truly realize the potential of these apps as viable complements to routine care.

Supplementary Information

eSUPPLEMENT (560.5KB, pdf)

Author contributions

N.M. conceptualized the review, led each part of the process of the review (i.e., screening, full-text review, data extraction, QA), and wrote and edited substantial portions of the manuscript. Z.M., T.G., E.R., S.L., and M.C. were involved in screening, full-text review, data extraction, QA, writing, and editing. G.Y. provided consultation and editing to the manuscript. J.T. contributed to the review conceptualization, consulted on all parts of the review process, and edited the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final version of the manuscript. This research did not receive any specific grant from funding agencies in the public, commercial, or not-for-profit sectors.

Data availability

While most of the data generated during this study are included in this published article, additional data on outcome measures and app engagement metrics extracted from each study is available from the corresponding author upon request.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Supplementary information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s44184-023-00048-5.

References

  • 1.Creswell, J. D. Mindfulness interventions. Annu. Rev. Psychol.68, 491–516 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hoge, E. A. et al. Mindfulness-based stress reduction vs escitalopram for the treatment of adults with anxiety disorders: a randomized clinical trial. JAMA Psychiatry80, 13–21 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Wilhelm, S. et al. Efficacy of app-based cognitive behavioral therapy for body dysmorphic disorder with coach support: initial randomized controlled clinical trial. Psychother. Psychosom.91, 277–285 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Harvey, A. G. & Gumport, N. B. Evidence-based psychological treatments for mental disorders: modifiable barriers to access and possible solutions. Behav. Res. Ther.68, 1–12 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Parsons, C. E., Crane, C., Parsons, L. J., Fjorback, L. O. & Kuyken, W. Home practice in mindfulness-based cognitive therapy and mindfulness-based stress reduction: a systematic review and meta-analysis of participants’ mindfulness practice and its association with outcomes. Behav. Res. Ther.95, 29–41 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Ben-Zeev, D., Buck, B., Meller, S., Hudenko, W. J. & Hallgren, K. A. Augmenting evidence-based care with a texting mobile interventionist: pilot randomized controlled trial. Psychiatr. Serv.71, 1218–1224 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Austin, S. F., Jansen, J. E., Petersen, C. J., Jensen, R. & Simonsen, E. Mobile app integration into dialectical behavior therapy for persons with borderline personality disorder: qualitative and quantitative study. JMIR Ment. Health7, e14913 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Schultchen, D. et al. Stay present with your phone: a systematic review and standardized rating of mindfulness apps in european app stores. Int.J. Behav. Med.28, 552–560 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Baumel, A., Muench, F., Edan, S. & Kane, J. M. Objective user engagement with mental health apps: systematic search and panel-based usage analysis. J. Med. Internet Res.21, e14567 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Schueller, S. M., Washburn, J. J. & Price, M. Exploring mental health providers’ interest in using web and mobile-based tools in their practices. Internet Interv.4, 145–151 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Noel, V. A., Acquilano, S. C., Carpenter-Song, E. & Drake, R. E. Use of mobile and computer devices to support recovery in people with serious mental illness: survey study. JMIR Ment. Health6, e12255 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Torous, J., Friedman, R. & Keshavan, M. Smartphone ownership and interest in mobile applications to monitor symptoms of mental health conditions. JMIR mHealth and uHealth2, e2994 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Morton, E., Torous, J., Murray, G. & Michalak, E. E. Using apps for bipolar disorder—an online survey of healthcare provider perspectives and practices. J. Psychiatr. Res.137, 22–28 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Tan, Z. Y. A., Wong, S. H., Cheng, L. J. & Lau, S. T. Effectiveness of mobile-based mindfulness interventions in improving mindfulness skills and psychological outcomes for adults: a systematic review and meta-regression. Mindfulness13, 2379–2395 (2022). [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Gal, E., Stefan, S. & Cristea, I. The efficacy of mindfulness meditation apps in enhancing users’ well-being and mental health related outcomes: a meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. J. Affect. Disord.279, 131–142 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Kazdin, A. E. Mediators and mechanisms of change in psychotherapy research. Annu. Rev. Clin. Psychol.3, 1–27 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Linehan, M. M. Cognitive-behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. xvii, 558 (Guilford Press, 1993).
  • 18.Farchione, T. J. et al. Unified protocol for transdiagnostic treatment of emotional disorders: a randomized controlled trial. Behav. Ther.43, 666–678 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Gu, J., Strauss, C., Bond, R. & Cavanagh, K. How do mindfulness-based cognitive therapy and mindfulness-based stress reduction improve mental health and wellbeing? A systematic review and meta-analysis of mediation studies. Clin. Psychol. Rev.37, 1–12 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Liberati, A. et al. The PRISMA statement for reporting systematic reviews and meta-analyses of studies that evaluate health care interventions: explanation and elaboration. Ann. Intern. Med.151, W–65 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Shapiro, S. L., Carlson, L. E., Astin, J. A. & Freedman, B. Mechanisms of mindfulness. J. Clin. Psychol.62, 373–386 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hölzel, B. K. et al. How does mindfulness meditation work? proposing mechanisms of action from a conceptual and neural perspective. Perspect. Psychol. Sci.6, 537–559 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Garland, E. L., Farb, N. A., R. Goldin, P. & Fredrickson, B. L. Mindfulness broadens awareness and builds eudaimonic meaning: a process model of mindful positive emotion regulation. Psychol. Inq.26, 293–314 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Lindsay, E. K. & Creswell, J. D. Mechanisms of mindfulness training: Monitor and Acceptance Theory (MAT). Clin. Psychol. Rev.51, 48–59 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Yuan, J. P. et al. Gray matter changes in adolescents participating in a meditation training. Front. Hum. Neurosci.14, 319 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Thomas, B. H., Ciliska, D., Dobbins, M. & Micucci, S. A process for systematically reviewing the literature: providing the research evidence for public health nursing interventions. Worldviews Evid. Based Nurs.1, 176–184 (2004). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Walsh, K. M., Saab, B. J. & Farb, N. A. Effects of a mindfulness meditation app on subjective well-being: active randomized controlled trial and experience sampling study. JMIR Ment. Health6, e10844 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Axelsen, J. L., Meline, J. S. J., Staiano, W. & Kirk, U. Mindfulness and music interventions in the workplace: assessment of sustained attention and working memory using a crowdsourcing approach. BMC Psychol.10, 108 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Ziegler, D. A. et al. Closed-loop digital meditation improves sustained attention in young adults. Nat. Hum. Behav.3, 746–757 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Baer, R. A., Smith, G. T., Hopkins, J., Krietemeyer, J. & Toney, L. Using self-report assessment methods to explore facets of mindfulness. Assessment13, 27–45 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Levin, M. E., Hicks, E. T. & Krafft, J. Pilot evaluation of the stop, breathe & think mindfulness app for student clients on a college counseling center waitlist. J. Am. Coll. Health70, 165–173 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Hirshberg, M. J. et al. A randomized controlled trial of a smartphone-based well-being training in public school system employees during the COVID-19 pandemic. J. Educ. Psychol.114, 1895–1911 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 33.Yang, E., Schamber, E., Meyer, R. M. L. & Gold, J. I. Happier healers: randomized controlled trial of mobile mindfulness for stress management. J. Altern. Complement. Med.24, 505–513 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.van Emmerik, A. A. P., Berings, F. & Lancee, J. Efficacy of a mindfulness-based mobile application: a randomized waiting-list controlled trial. Mindfulness9, 187–198 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Huberty, J. et al. Efficacy of the mindfulness meditation mobile app “calm” to reduce stress among college students: randomized controlled trial. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth7, e14273 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Haliwa, I., Ford, C. G., Wilson, J. M. & Shook, N. J. A mixed-method assessment of a 10-day mobile mindfulness intervention. Front. Psychol.12, 722995 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Bohlmeijer, E., ten Klooster, P. M., Fledderus, M., Veehof, M. & Baer, R. Psychometric properties of the five facet mindfulness questionnaire in depressed adults and development of a short form. Assessment18, 308–320 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Rich, R. M., Ogden, J. & Morison, L. A randomized controlled trial of an app-delivered mindfulness program among university employees: effects on stress and work-related outcomes. IJWHM14, 201–216 (2021). [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Orosa-Duarte, Á. et al. Mindfulness-based mobile app reduces anxiety and increases self-compassion in healthcare students: a randomised controlled trial. Med. Teach.43, 686–693 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kubo, A. et al. A randomized controlled trial of mhealth mindfulness intervention for cancer patients and informal cancer caregivers: a feasibility study within an integrated health care delivery system. Integr. Cancer Ther.18, 153473541985063 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Sala, M., Roos, C. R., Brewer, J. A. & Garrison, K. A. Awareness, affect, and craving during smoking cessation: an experience sampling study. Health Psychol.40, 578–586 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Bishop, S. R. et al. Mindfulness: a proposed operational definition. Clin. Psychol. Sci. Pract.11, 230–241 (2004). [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ainsworth, B. et al. A feasibility trial of a digital mindfulness-based intervention to improve asthma-related quality of life for primary care patients with asthma. J. Behav. Med.45, 133–147 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Mehling, W. E. et al. The multidimensional assessment of interoceptive awareness (MAIA). PLoS One7, e48230 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Roy, A. et al. Clinical efficacy and psychological mechanisms of an app-based digital therapeutic for generalized anxiety disorder: randomized controlled trial. J. Med. Internet Res.23, e26987 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Gao, M. et al. Targeting anxiety to improve sleep disturbance: a randomized clinical trial of app-based mindfulness training. Psychosom. Med.84, 632–642 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Rich, A. et al. Evaluation of a novel intervention to reduce burnout in doctors-in-training using self-care and digital wellbeing strategies: a mixed-methods pilot. BMC Med. Educ.20, 294 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Sun, Y. et al. Effectiveness of smartphone-based mindfulness training on maternal perinatal depression: randomized controlled trial. J. Med. Internet Res.23, e23410 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Howells, A., Ivtzan, I. & Eiroa-Orosa, F. J. Putting the ‘app’ in happiness: a randomised controlled trial of a smartphone-based mindfulness intervention to enhance wellbeing. J. Happiness Stud.17, 163–185 (2016). [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Versluis, A., Verkuil, B., Spinhoven, P., Brosschot, F. & Effectiveness, J. of a smartphone-based worry-reduction training for stress reduction: a randomized-controlled trial. Psychol. Health33, 1079–1099 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Low, T., Conduit, R., Varma, P., Meaklim, H. & Jackson, M. L. Treating subclinical and clinical symptoms of insomnia with a mindfulness-based smartphone application: a pilot study. Internet Interv.21, 100335 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Versluis, A., Verkuil, B., Spinhoven, P. & Brosschot, J. F. Feasibility and effectiveness of a worry-reduction training using the smartphone: a pilot randomised controlled trial. Br. J. Guid. Counc.48, 227–239 (2020). [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Taylor, H., Cavanagh, K., Field, A. P. & Strauss, C. Health care workers’ need for headspace: findings from a multisite definitive randomized controlled trial of an unguided digital mindfulness-based self-help app to reduce healthcare worker stress. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth10, e31744 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Abbott, D., Lack, C. W. & Anderson, P. Does using a mindfulness app reduce anxiety and worry? a randomized-controlled trial. J. Cogn. Psychother.37, 26–42 (2023). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Goldberg, S. B. et al. Testing the efficacy of a multicomponent, self-guided, smartphone-based meditation app: three-armed randomized controlled trial. JMIR Ment. Health7, e23825 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Ehring, T. et al. The perseverative thinking questionnaire (PTQ): validation of a content-independent measure of repetitive negative thinking. J. Behav. Ther. Exp. Psychiatry42, 225–232 (2011). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Nolen-Hoeksema, S. & Morrow, J. A prospective study of depression and posttraumatic stress symptoms after a natural disaster: the 1989 Loma Prieta earthquake. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.61, 115–121 (1991). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Weaver, B., Bédard, M. & McAuliffe, J. Evaluation of a 10-minute version of the attention network test. Clin. Neuropsychol.27, 1281–1299 (2013). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Fan, J., McCandliss, B. D., Sommer, T., Raz, A. & Posner, M. I. Testing the efficiency and independence of attentional networks. J. Cogn. Neurosci.14, 340–347 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Greenberg, L. M. TOVA continuous performance test manual (The TOVA Company, 1996).
  • 61.Robertson, I. H., Manly, T., Andrade, J., Baddeley, B. T. & Yiend, J. ‘Oops!’: performance correlates of everyday attentional failures in traumatic brain injured and normal subjects. Neuropsychologia35, 747–758 (1997). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Forman, E. M. et al. The Drexel defusion scale: a new measure of experiential distancing. J. Context. Behav. Sci.1, 55–65 (2012). [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Lau, M. A. et al. The toronto mindfulness scale: development and validation. J. Clin. Psychol.62, 1445–1467 (2006). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Cardaciotto, L., Herbert, J. D., Forman, E. M., Moitra, E. & Farrow, V. The assessment of present-moment awareness and acceptance: the philadelphia mindfulness scale. Assessment15, 204–223 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Ly, K. H. et al. Behavioural activation versus mindfulness-based guided self-help treatment administered through a smartphone application: a randomised controlled trial. BMJ Open4, e003440 (2014). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Bond, F. W. et al. Preliminary psychometric properties of the acceptance and action questionnaire–ii: a revised measure of psychological inflexibility and experiential avoidance. Behav. Ther.42, 676–688 (2011). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Schulte‐Frankenfeld, P. M. & Trautwein, F. App‐based mindfulness meditation reduces perceived stress and improves self‐regulation in working university students: a randomised controlled trial. Appl. Psychol. Health Well Being14, 1151–1171 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Diehl, M., Semegon, A. B. & Schwarzer, R. Assessing attention control in goal pursuit: a component of dispositional self-regulation. J. Pers. Assess.86, 306–317 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Abler, B. & Kessler, H. ERQ - emotion regulation questionnaire - deutsche Fassung. 10.23668/psycharchives.402 (2011).
  • 70.Smout, M., Davies, M., Burns, N. & Christie, A. Development of the valuing questionnaire (VQ). J. Context. Behav. Sci.3, 164–172 (2014). [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Bjorkstrand, J. et al. The effect of mindfulness training on extinction retention. Sci. Rep.9, 19896 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 72.Linardon, J. & Fuller-Tyszkiewicz, M. Attrition and adherence in smartphone-delivered interventions for mental health problems: a systematic and meta-analytic review. J. Consult. Clin. Psychol.88, 1–13 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Tang, Y.-Y. et al. Short-term meditation training improves attention and self-regulation. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci.104, 17152–17156 (2007). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Zeidan, F., Johnson, S. K., Diamond, B. J., David, Z. & Goolkasian, P. Mindfulness meditation improves cognition: evidence of brief mental training. Conscious. Cogn.19, 597–605 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Hodgins, H. S. & Adair, K. C. Attentional processes and meditation. Conscious. Cogn.19, 872–878 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Rojiani, R., Santoyo, J. F., Rahrig, H., Roth, H. D. & Britton, W. B. Women benefit more than men in response to college-based meditation training. Front. Psychol.20, 551 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 77.de Vibe, M. et al. Mindfulness training for stress management: a randomised controlled study of medical and psychology students. BMC Med. Educ.13, 107 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Roos, C. R., Stein, E., Bowen, S. & Witkiewitz, K. Individual gender and group gender composition as predictors of differential benefit from mindfulness-based relapse prevention for substance use disorders. Mindfulness10, 1560–1567 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Lipschitz, J. M. et al. Digital mental health interventions for depression: scoping review of user engagement. J. Med. Internet Res.24, e39204 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Ng, M. M., Firth, J., Minen, M. & Torous, J. User engagement in mental health apps: a review of measurement, reporting, and validity. Psychiatr. Serv70, 538–544 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Amagai, S., Pila, S., Kaat, A. J., Nowinski, C. J. & Gershon, R. C. Challenges in participant engagement and retention using mobile health apps: literature review. J. Med. Internet Res.24, e35120 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Van Dam, N. T. et al. Mind the hype: a critical evaluation and prescriptive agenda for research on mindfulness and meditation. Perspect. Psychol. Sci.13, 36–61 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Alsubaie, M. et al. Mechanisms of action in mindfulness-based cognitive therapy (MBCT) and mindfulness-based stress reduction (MBSR) in people with physical and/or psychological conditions: a systematic review. Clin. Psychol. Rev.55, 74–91 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Simione, L., Raffone, A. & Mirolli, M. Acceptance, and not its interaction with attention monitoring, increases psychological well-being: Testing the monitor and acceptance theory of mindfulness. Mindfulness12, 1398–1411 (2021). [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Sanatkar, S. et al. Using cluster analysis to explore engagement and e-attainment as emergent behavior in electronic mental health. J. Med. Internet Res.21, e14728 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Unger, C. A., Busse, D. & Yim, I. S. The effect of guided relaxation on cortisol and affect: stress reactivity as a moderator. J. Health Psychol.22, 29–38 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Karl, J. A. et al. The cross-cultural validity of the five-facet mindfulness questionnaire across 16 countries. Mindfulness11, 1226–1237 (2020). [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Ng, S. & Wang, Q. Measuring mindfulness grounded in the original Buddha’s discourses on meditation practice. In: Assessing Spirituality in a Diverse World (eds. Ai, A. L., Wink, P., Paloutzian, R. F. & Harris, K. A.) 355–381 (Springer International Publishing, 2021). 10.1007/978-3-030-52140-0_15.
  • 89.Quirin, M., Kazén, M. & Kuhl, J. When nonsense sounds happy or helpless: the implicit positive and negative affect test (IPANAT). J. Pers. Soc. Psychol.97, 500–516 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Leonidou, C. & Panayiotou, G. Can we predict experiential avoidance by measuring subjective and physiological emotional arousal? Curr. Psychol.41, 1–13 (2021). [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Hilt, L. M. & Pollak, S. D. Getting out of rumination: comparison of three brief interventions in a sample of youth. J. Abnorm. Child Psychol.40, 1157–1165 (2012). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Adu-Brimpong, J., Pugh, J., Darko, D. A. & Shieh, L. Examining diversity in digital therapeutics clinical trials: descriptive analysis. J. Med. Internet Res.25, e37447 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Jiwani, Z. et al. Examining equity in access and utilization of a freely available meditation app. npj Ment. Health Res.2, 10 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Breitenstein, S. M. et al. Implementation fidelity in community-based interventions. Res. Nurs. Health33, 164–173 (2010). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Borghouts, J. et al. Barriers to and facilitators of user engagement with digital mental health interventions: systematic review. J. Med. Internet Res.23, e24387 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Choi, E., Farb, N., Pogrebtsova, E., Gruman, J. & Grossmann, I. What do people mean when they talk about mindfulness? Clin. Psychol. Rev.89, 102085 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Bryan, C. J., Tipton, E. & Yeager, D. S. Behavioural science is unlikely to change the world without a heterogeneity revolution. Nat. Hum. Behav.5, 980–989 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Farias, M., Wikholm, C. & Delmonte, R. What is mindfulness-based therapy good for?. Lancet Psychiatry3, 1012–1013 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Lagan, S. et al. Actionable health app evaluation: translating expert frameworks into objective metrics. npj Digit. Med.3, 1–8 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Baer, R. A. Self-compassion as a mechanism of change in mindfulness- and acceptance-based treatments. in Assessing mindfulness and acceptance processes in clients: Illuminating the theory and practice of change 135–153 (Context Press/New Harbinger Publications, 2010).
  • 101.Watson, T., Simpson, S. & Hughes, C. Text messaging interventions for individuals with mental health disorders including substance use: a systematic review. Psychiatry Res.243, 255–262 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

eSUPPLEMENT (560.5KB, pdf)

Data Availability Statement

While most of the data generated during this study are included in this published article, additional data on outcome measures and app engagement metrics extracted from each study is available from the corresponding author upon request.


Articles from NPJ Mental Health Research are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES