Abstract
Despite constituting an essential component of fitness, reproductive success can vary remarkably between individuals and the causes of such variation are not well understood across taxa. In the zebra finch—a model songbird, almost all the variation in sperm morphology and swimming speed is maintained by a large polymorphic inversion (commonly known as a supergene) on the Z chromosome. The relationship between this polymorphism and reproductive success is not fully understood, particularly for females. Here, we explore the effects of female haplotype, and the combination of male and female genotype, on several primary reproductive traits in a captive population of zebra finches. Despite the inversion polymorphism's known effects on sperm traits, we find no evidence that inversion haplotype influences egg production by females or survival of embryos through to hatching. However, our findings do reinforce existing evidence that the inversion polymorphism is maintained by a heterozygote advantage for male fitness. This work provides an important step in understanding the causes of variation in reproductive success in this model species.
Keywords: reproduction, sex chromosomes, heterozygote advantage, inversion polymorphism, Taeniopygia guttata
1. Introduction
Reproductive success is a central determinant of evolutionary fitness, so we expect traits important for reproduction to be under strong directional selection. Despite this, reproductive output often varies considerably between individuals, and the genetic basis of this variation is not well understood [1–3]. Chromosomal inversions are widespread across taxa, and inversion polymorphisms are increasingly recognized as important in the maintenance of genetic variation [4,5]. Estrildidae finches like the zebra finch (Taeniopygia guttata) are particularly prone to inversions [6,7], making them useful for studying the relationship between inversions and the maintenance of reproductive trait variation.
Like all birds, zebra finches have a ZZ/ZW sex chromosome system whereby females are the heterogametic sex (ZW). The zebra finch Z chromosome houses a polymorphic inversion consisting of (at least) three segregating haplotypes: A, B and C (figure 1a,b) [8,9]. Recombination is highly suppressed within the inverted region in heterozygous males, so mutations that arise on a given genetic background (i.e. haplotype) cannot recombine onto other haplotypes, but instead are preserved and inherited together as a single unit [10,11]. When inversions link multiple loci that act jointly to encode complex phenotypes in a balanced polymorphism, they are commonly referred to as a ‘supergene’ [8,10,12].
The zebra finch Z inversion polymorphism is large, containing over 600 genes and spanning 86% of the chromosome [6,13]. The inversion's molecular evolution is yet to be fully determined, and while A is likely to be the ancestral haplotype, it is unclear the order in which B and C arose, and whether they were both generated from A or sequentially from one another. Each haplotype has accumulated large genetic differences over time, and all three are maintained at stable frequencies in both captive and wild populations (frequencies of A, B and C respectively: 0.378, 0.408 and 0.215 in the captive population studied here; and 0.730, 0.199 and 0.122 in the wild) [8]. Importantly, the male inversion karyotype is known to be responsible for almost all the variation in sperm morphology and swimming speed in this species (figure 1c), with important consequences for male fertilization success under sperm competition [8]. Sperm morphology is extremely variable between inversion karyotypes, and heterokaryotypic males (AB or AC karyotypes) have the fastest and most successful sperm under a competitive scenario (figure 1c). This suggests that all three haplotypes, including those associated with relatively poorly performing sperm (e.g. BB, BC and CC males), are being maintained in the population at least in part by a heterozygote advantage for male reproductive success (figure 1c) [8].
Whether the observed overdominance for male reproductive success is the only force maintaining all three inversion haplotypes in this system remains unknown. Females may also influence the maintenance of the inversion polymorphism, some (non-exhaustive and non-mutually exclusive) examples include sexually antagonistic selection, variation in male-female compatibility, or through paternity bias based on male karyotype [4,10]. Females could bias paternity either before copulation via assortative mating; or post-copulation, for example via selective sperm storage, reduced egg investment, reduced incubation effort, or by modifying brood sex ratio [14–19]. Female zebra finches have been known to modify egg investment in response to partner quality [20,21], and can facultatively adjust offspring sex ratio depending on female condition [22]. In some other passerines, females have also been shown to adjust clutch sex ratio depending on mate quality [16,23]. These mechanisms of paternity bias could potentially act to maintain the inversion polymorphism by reinforcing the existing heterozygote advantage, either through propagating the benefits associated with certain male karyotypes, or by offsetting the disadvantages associated with other karyotypes, or both. However, while previous work has found evidence for strong additive effects of Z-chromosome inversion genotype on several morphological traits [6], whether females can detect and respond to these signals of male inversion karyotype is unknown.
The direct effects of the inversion polymorphism on female fitness have also not yet been fully explored. Sexually antagonistic loci–loci that contain alleles which benefit one sex at the detriment of the other, are predicted to be particularly prevalent in sex-linked regions, owing to how the unique pattern(s) of sex chromosome inheritance affect the evolutionary dynamics of sexually antagonistic selection [24–26]. Whether a Z-linked sexually antagonistic allele will reach fixation is dependent on both the sex in which it benefits, and its dominance coefficient [24–29]. For example, if recessive sexually antagonistic alleles have accumulated within the Z chromosome inversion, they could potentially act to maintain the polymorphism because haplotypes with alleles that are detrimental to males but beneficial in females can reach high frequencies (recessive alleles are always expressed in females). Conversely, the higher dose of the Z in males, coupled with the fact that dosage compensation (the equalization of gene expression between the autosomes and the sex chromosomes in the heterogametic sex) is incomplete on the avian Z, suggests that fully or partially dominant Z-linked male-beneficial alleles could be positively selected, even if sexually antagonistic [24,25,27,30–33]. To determine whether sexually antagonistic selection could be contributing to the maintenance of the Z-chromosome inversion polymorphism, it is necessary to determine whether female reproductive traits also vary based on inversion haplotype, as well as measure the survival probability of offspring with different combinations of haplotypes.
Here, we use an extensive long-term breeding database with data from 1716 genotyped captive zebra finch individuals to analyse the effects of both male and female inversion haplotype (individually and in combination) on reproductive traits including egg production (a female-specific reproductive trait) and survival rates of embryos through to hatching. This work provides an important step in improving our understanding of the consequences of inversion polymorphisms for reproduction, and more broadly the causes of variation in reproductive success in birds from both the male and female perspective.
2. Methods
(a) . Animals
All zebra finches belonged to a domesticated population maintained at The University of Sheffield between 1985 and 2016. Birds from this population were separated physically (but not visually or acoustically) by sex unless breeding, in which case a single pair was housed together without access to other individuals and with no opportunity to engage in extra pair copulations. Pairs were selected by researchers and no natural mate choice was permitted. Previously mated females were rested for at least two weeks before re-pairing with a new male to ensure sperm from the previous male was fully depleted from her sperm storage organs (the maximum duration of sperm storage is 13 days in the zebra finch [34]). Paternity was therefore known conclusively for every egg, and sperm did not have to compete for fertilization of the ova. All deceased birds were preserved in −20°C freezers and accessed later for tissue sampling and DNA extraction. All sampled birds were adults.
(b) . DNA extraction
Z chromosome inversion karyotype was already known for 197 male birds from a previous study [8]. Tissue samples (brain or toe tissue) were dissected from an additional 1785 frozen specimens (1071 females and 714 males). Tissue samples were stored at room temperature in 100% ethanol until extraction. DNA was extracted using a standard (plated) ammonium acetate method [35], and DNA samples were stored in a low TE buffer (Tris-HCL (1 M) and EDTA (0.5 M)) at −4°C and later quantified using a FLUOStar fluorometer before diluting to a concentration of 5 ng µl−1.
(c) . Single nucleotide polymorphism typing
Samples were typed for their single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP) genotypes using kompetitive allele specific polymerase chain reaction (KASP)-genotyping chemistry on an LGC SNPLine system [36] in The University of Sheffield's Molecular Ecology Laboratory. A total of nine SNPs were chosen from a zebra finch high density 600k SNP chip. These SNPs demonstrated the highest fixed allelic differences between haplotypes (see Kim et al. [8] for details). Individual assays for each SNP were designed using LGC Genomics Ltd guidelines. Genotype calling was performed using cluster analysis in the software Kraken [37]. Diagnostic SNP data obtained from Kim et al. [8] was used to call SNP genotypes for each individual [38], and genotype calling was successful for 98% of samples. Unsuccessful calling for 2% of samples was most often owing to amplification failure, indicating insufficient DNA present in the sample. A total of 1753 individuals were typed (1950 including the additional males from the Kim et al., dataset). Owing to some uncertainty in the breeding record (missing or ambiguous data), 12% of individuals (233) were removed, leaving a total of 1716 individuals, including 792 males (AA: 139; AB: 229; AC: 111; BB: 168; BC: 115; CC: 30), 924 females (A: 340; B: 376; C: 208) and 1319 unique pairings in the final datasets (see the electronic supplementary material, table S2 for sample sizes of parental genotype combinations) [38].
(d) . Measures of reproductive success
We investigated the effect of female inversion haplotype, male inversion karyotype and the combination of male and female genotype on five reproductive traits:
-
(i)
egg production: measured as the number of eggs laid per clutch (all eggs laid including infertile eggs) (n clutches: 3041; n pairs: 1319; n eggs: 12292);
-
(ii)
egg fertility and early embryo development: measured as the proportion of eggs (per pair) that were fertilized and survived past 3 days of incubation, relative to eggs that were either unfertilized or died within the first 3 days of development (n pairs: 454). Failed fertilization and death during early development were not distinguished from one another because, prior to 2008 (when microscopic techniques were developed to accurately determine the fertility status of undeveloped eggs [39]), egg fertility was primarily determined by candling (i.e. shining a light through the eggshell to visualise the contents without opening the egg). Zebra finch embryonic development is only reliably visible via candling after 3 days of incubation, therefore eggs with no visible sign of development by 3 days could have either been unfertilized, or fertilized but the embryo died very early. Eggs with visible signs of development by day 3 of incubation could be definitively classified as fertilized and had survived the very early stages of development (prior to the formation of blood vessels). Both fertilization failure and early-stage embryo mortality are more likely to be linked to genetic problems (e.g. sperm-egg incompatibility, aneuploidy), parental age effects or variation in female receptivity to sperm, compared to later stage mortality [40];
-
(iii)
hatching success of developing eggs: measured as the proportion of eggs (per clutch) that showed obvious signs of development on candling (i.e. were fertilized and developed to at least day 3 of incubation) and went on to successfully hatch, relative to those in which the embryos died at a relatively late stage of development (i.e. after day 3 of incubation), resulting in hatching failure (n pairs: 460; n clutches: 1528). Compared to early-stage embryo mortality, late-stage mortality is more likely owing to environmental conditions in the nest and/or inadequate parental care during incubation [40]. Any effect of inversion haplotype or parental haplotype combination on late-stage embryo development is therefore likely to occur through selective parental investment, rather than direct genetic effects;
-
(iv)
offspring sex ratio: measured as the deviation of offspring sex ratio from a 50 : 50 expectation (n clutches: 1769, n offspring: 880). Only offspring that survived to 100 days were included in the sex ratio data as sex was assigned based on plumage traits at sexual maturity; and
-
(v)
offspring genotype: measured as the consistency of offspring genotypes with Mendelian expectations. For example, under Mendelian expectations, an A female mated to an AB male should produce male offspring that carry either the AA or AB karyotype at a 50 : 50 ratio, and female offspring that carry the A or B haplotype at a 50 : 50 ratio (n offspring: 683).
(e) . Statistical analysis
All statistical analyses were conducted in R (v. 4.2.1) [41]. We adopted a Bayesian framework using the ‘brms’ package [42] for the first four reproductive measures described above: (i) egg production; (ii) egg fertility and early embryo development; (iii) hatching success of developing eggs; and (iv) offspring sex ratio.
For each reproductive measure, we fitted two separate multilevel models. The first model functioned to test the effects of female haplotype and parental haplotype combination on reproduction. This model included female haplotype as a fixed effect, and a second fixed effect variable termed ‘haplotypes shared’, which refers to the number of inversion haplotypes shared between the male and female, and therefore the degree of genetic similarity at the inversion. For example, a female with haplotype A shares two haplotypes with an AA male, one with AB/AC males, and none with BB/BC/CC males. Attempting to include a direct interaction between male karyotype and female haplotype would be inappropriate owing to the sheer number of possible contrasts and our limited a priori knowledge regarding how specific combinations would perform. However, if parental haplotype combination affects reproductive success, we would expect this effect to vary based on the number of haplotypes shared between parents. This model also allows us to interpret the main effect of female haplotype on reproduction, and whether any effect of parental haplotype sharing varies by female haplotype.
The model described above may miss some variation explained by specific male karyotypes. To account for this, we performed a second model including male karyotype as a separate fixed effect (with female haplotype included as a control). For every model, male and female age (scaled and mean centred) were also included as fixed effects, and male and female identity (ID) included as hierarchical groups. Whilst housing conditions were kept relatively consistent across the lifetime of the study population, the large timescales over which this population was kept meant that certain aspects of husbandry varied over time (such as food, conspecific identity and keeper identity). To ensure any effects of husbandry/environmental conditions were controlled for, we included a categorical variable of female birth year as an additional hierarchical group (17 levels, spanning 1997–2013).
Choice of family for Bayesian models was based on a combination of prior knowledge of the data and assessment of model fit using posterior predictive checks. For the ‘egg fertility and early embryo development’, ‘hatching success of developed eggs', and ‘sex ratio’ models, a binomial family was chosen. For the ‘egg production’ analysis, all variations of a Poisson distribution, negative binomial and negative binomial hurdle models fit the data very poorly with an extremely poor recovery of the tail ends of the distribution. A continuation ratio family (cRatio) (a highly flexible ordinal model) can be considered appropriate when used with data that is bounded and sequential (the attainment of one level is required for the attainment of the next). cRatio models have been proposed and used in the past for similar ordinal-like count data [43–45], and make logical sense in our case (see the electronic supplementary material, S5 for details). The use of a cRatio model also allows us to tease apart the separate probabilities associated with the attainment of each level, providing us with superior inference over a Poisson or negative binomial model. We present here the results of the cRatio model (see the electronic supplementary material, S5A-C for a summary of additional modelling attempts using more traditional approaches).
Regularizing zero-centred diffuse priors were chosen for all parameters. Models were run across four chains with 2000 warmup and 1000 post warmup iterations, except for the ‘hatching success of developed eggs' models which required 5000 warmup and 2500 post warmup iterations to reach a sufficient effective sample size. Model convergence was assessed visually using trace plots and parameter Ȓ values, none of which exceeded 1.01 indicating good convergence of between- and within-chain estimates. Posterior predictive checks were used to assess the adequacy of model fit.
We used the probability of direction (PD) to determine effect presence and direction, and the 90% highest density interval (HDI) to determine the degree of effect uncertainty. We chose a 90% rather than 95% HDI, because 90% has been suggested to be more stable [46]. The PD describes the proportion of the posterior distribution that is of the median's sign (i.e. a PD of 100% indicates all iterations were either positive or negative). The PD therefore provides the probability that a parameter is either positive or negative. As per convention, we consider PD values greater than 97.5% as substantial evidence of an effect (in combination with an assessment of the 90% HDI), because a PD of 97.5 is highly correlated with a two-sided p-value of 0.05. Effect direction is then evident by the sign of the median. All model checks and PD plots can be found in the electronic supplementary material. We performed a variance ratio analysis for each model, to calculate the variance explained by the hierarchical structure of the models (i.e. the ‘random effects’ – mother ID, father ID and mother birth year).
For the final reproductive measure, ‘offspring genotype’, we performed an exact binomial test for each combination of parental genotypes, to determine whether offspring genotype differed significantly from Mendelian expectations. Significance from binomial tests was determined by p-values of less than 0.05.
3. Results
(a) . Egg production
We found no evidence of an effect of either female inversion haplotype, haplotype sharing (the number of haplotypes shared between the male and female, i.e. their genetic similarity at the inversion) or male karyotype on egg production (figure 2; electronic supplementary material, figure S1a–h). The effect of haplotype sharing was not influenced by female haplotype (figure 2a; electronic supplementary material, figure S1c).
Across all haplotypes, the most probable clutch size was four or five eggs, after which a single-egged clutch was more probable than any other clutch size. Very few clutches produced more than seven eggs (figure 2a). The high number of single-egged clutches is probably the result of an age-related decline in clutch size: we found a strong negative effect of female (but not male) age on egg production (est. = −0.67; 90% HDI: −0.79, −0.52; PD = 100%; electronic supplementary material, figure S1e,f). As females aged, there was an increase in the probability of laying a single-egged clutch, and a strong (but smaller) increase in the probability of laying no eggs, while the probability of each step-increase in clutch size also declined with age (none of these age effects were dependant on female inversion haplotype). A summary of the variance decomposition analysis is presented in the electronic supplementary material, table S1.
(b) . Egg fertility and early embryo development
We found no evidence of an effect of female haplotype on the probability of an egg developing past day 3 of incubation (figure 3a; electronic supplementary material, figure S2a,b). Overall, we found no consistent effect of parental haplotype sharing on early embryo development. There were, however, two exceptions: firstly, a clear but small positive effect for B females that shared one haplotype with the male, relative to B females that shared no haplotypes with the male (median = 0.37; 90% HDI: 0.06, 0.66; PD = 97.9%). Specifically, B : AB and B : BC parental combinations produced eggs that were around 40% more likely to be fertilized and survive early development relative to B : AA, B : CC or B : AC parental combinations (median(odds ratio) = 1.44). Secondly, we found a clear negative effect for A females that shared two haplotypes with the male, relative to A females that shared one haplotype with the male (median = −0.45; 90% HDI: −0.81, −0.08; PD = 97.8%). Specifically, A : AA parental combinations were around half as likely to produce eggs that were fertilized and survived early development relative to A : AB and A:AC parental combinations (median(odds ratio) = 0.63) (figure 3a; electronic supplementary material, figure S2c).
We found no consistent effect of male karyotype on egg fertility and early embryo development. However, we did find a small but clear negative effect of AA males relative to AB males (median = −0.38; 90% HDI: −0.66, −0.07; PD = 97.8%) and AC males (median = −0.54; 90% HDI: −0.86, −0.17; PD = 99.6%) (figure 3b; electronic supplementary material, figure S2f,g). Specifically, eggs from females paired to AA males were around half as likely to be fertilized or survive early development, relative to eggs from females paired with AB and AC males (median(odds ratio) = 0.68 and 0.58 respectively). AB and AC carrying males did not differ substantially from any other male karyotype. There was no clear effect of male or female age on egg fertility and early embryo development (electronic supplementary material, figure S2d–e). A summary of the variance decomposition analysis is presented in the electronic supplementary material, table S1.
(c) . Hatching success of developing eggs
We found no evidence of an effect of female haplotype, haplotype sharing or male karyotype on the hatching success of developing eggs (i.e. those that showed evidence of development at day 3 of incubation) (figure 4; electronic supplementary material, figure S3a–g). The effect of haplotype sharing was also not influenced by female haplotype (figure 4a; electronic supplementary material, figure S3c). We also found no evidence of an effect of male or female age on hatching success (electronic supplementary material, figure S3d,e). A summary of the variance decomposition analysis is presented in the electronic supplementary material, table S1.
(d) . Offspring sex ratio
We found no evidence of an effect of female haplotype, the degree of haplotype sharing or male karyotype on the sex ratio of offspring (figure 5; electronic supplementary material, figure S4a–g). The effect of haplotype sharing was not influenced by female haplotype (figure 5a; electronic supplementary material, figure S4c), and there was also no evidence of an effect of male or female age (electronic supplementary material, figure S4d,e)). A summary of the variance decomposition analysis is presented in the electronic supplementary material, table S1.
(e) . Offspring genotype ratios
There was no evidence of an effect of parent genotypes on expected offspring genotype ratios, with Mendelian expectations being met for both female and male offspring, and for all combinations of parent genotype (table 1).
Table 1.
parent genotype combination (female : male) | offspring sex | expected offspring genotype | number of offspring | observed proportion of offspring genotypes | exact binomial test for Mendelian expectations |
|
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
p | 95% CI | |||||
A : AB | male | AA, AB | 29, 21 | 0.58, 0.42 | p = 0.32 | 0.43–0.72 |
female | A, B | 42, 42 | 0.5, 0.5 | p = 1 | 0.39–0.61 | |
A : AC | male | AA, AC | 21, 20 | 0.51, 0.49 | p = 1 | 0.35–0.67 |
female | A, C | 15, 28 | 0.35, 0.65 | p = 0.07 | 0.21–0.51 | |
A : BC | male | AB, AC | 13, 8 | 0.62, 0.38 | p = 0.38 | 0.38–0.82 |
female | B, C | 26, 23 | 0.41, 0.59 | p = 0.34 | 0.26–0.58 | |
B : AB | male | AB, BB | 29, 31 | 0.48, 0.52 | p = 0.90 | 0.35–0.62 |
female | A, B | 28, 38 | 0.42, 0.58 | p = 0.27 | 0.30–0.55 | |
B : AC | male | AB, BC | 16, 7 | 0.7, 0.3 | p = 0.10 | 0.47–0.87 |
female | A, C | 14, 14 | 0.5, 0.5 | p = 1 | 0.31–0.69 | |
B : BC | male | BB, BC | 24, 13 | 0.65, 0.35 | p = 0.10 | 0.47–0.80 |
female | B, C | 18, 14 | 0.56, 0.44 | p = 0.60 | 0.38–0.74 | |
C : AB | male | AC, BC | 14, 11 | 0.56, 0.44 | p = 0.69 | 0.35–0.76 |
female | A, B | 22, 28 | 0.44, 0.56 | p = 0.48 | 0.30–0.59 | |
C : AC | male | AC, CC | 6, 12 | 0.33, 0.67 | p = 0.24 | 0.13–0.59 |
female | A, C | 18, 24 | 0.43, 0.57 | p = 0.44 | 0.28–0.59 | |
C : BC | male | BC, CC | 9, 5 | 0.64, 0.36 | p = 0.42 | 0.35–0.87 |
female | B, C | 5, 5 | 0.5, 0.5 | p = 1 | 0.19–0.81 |
4. Discussion
Despite the strong effects of the Z chromosome inversion polymorphism for male fertility in zebra finches, we found no evidence that inversion haplotypes contain alleles with differential effects on egg production by females or survival of embryos through to hatching. Our results indicate it is unlikely that sexually antagonistic alleles acting upon egg production (a female-specific reproductive trait) have accumulated within the inversion. Instead, our findings suggest either: (i) the Z chromosome inversion haplotypes explain little variation in egg production, because any between-haplotype variation results in very similar mean phenotypes; (ii) prior sexual antagonism associated with egg production has already been resolved (for example through sex-biased gene expression [47]); (iii) there are relatively few genes responsible for variation in egg production located in the inversion, but instead they are located elsewhere on the Z or on the autosomes; (iv) egg production is a highly polygenic trait, and any associated loci located in the inversion exhibit a small proportion of genetic variation relative to loci elsewhere in the genome; or egg production may simply exhibit very little genetic variation.
Given the accumulated differences between inversion haplotypes, we analysed whether parental genotype combination is likely to contribute to variation in reproductive traits. We found no evidence that inversion haplotype sharing has a consistent effect on reproductive traits. We did, however, find that the probability of offspring surviving early development (relative to an egg being unfertilized or an embryo dying before 3 days of development), varied depending on the number of haplotypes shared and female haplotype. Specifically, ‘B’ females sharing one haplotype with the male (B : AB and B : BC combinations) had a higher likelihood of producing eggs that developed beyond 3 days of incubation, relative to ‘B’ females sharing no haplotypes with the male (B : AA, B : CC and B : AC combinations). Additionally, ‘A’ females sharing two haplotypes with the male (A : AA combinations) had a higher chance of eggs failing to develop, relative to ‘A’ females sharing one haplotype with the male (A : AB and A : AC combinations) (figure 3a). However, if parental haplotype sharing was consistently important, we would accordingly expect to observe a consistent effect associated with each degree of haplotypes shared. Our results do suggest that some specific combinations of parental genotypes perform better than others, but we suspect this is predominantly driven by the effect of male karyotype, because we also found that relative to homokaryotypic (AA carrying) males, heterokaryotypic (AB and AC carrying) males are more likely to produce fertilized eggs that survive beyond 3 days of development (figure 3b). This is additional to the prior evidence that AB and AC males have the fastest and most successful sperm under sperm competition [8,9]. Since multiple mating was not permitted in our population, the improved egg survival we observed must be independent of sperm competition, instead suggesting an additional non-competitive fertilization and/or developmental advantage. Our data do not allow us to discriminate between fertilization failure and embryo death as the cause of early reproductive failure in undeveloped eggs, but our findings do suggest that variation in male fertilization success may underpin the effects of haplotype sharing on egg development that we observed here. While female inversion haplotype does not appear to affect our measured reproductive traits, whether it influences other aspects of fitness remains unknown.
In their original study, despite the strong effects of the male karyotype for sperm traits, Kim et al. [8] found no systematic evidence of an effect of male inversion karyotype on hatching success, although they were unable to account for the influence of CC males. Conversely, Knief et al. [6] presented tentative evidence that mortality rates may be slightly higher in the offspring of heterokaryotypic males. Neither study accounted for the influence of female haplotype, or the interaction between male and female genotypes. Additionally, in both studies, measures of unhatched eggs represented those that failed at any stage from fertilization to late development. Our study—which does consider the influence of female haplotype, their interaction with male karyotype, and CC males, as well as partitioning hatching success into two distinct functional categories (representing separate and mechanistically divergent stages of hatching failure)—finds that aside from the positive effect of AC and AB males relative to AA males, there is no further evidence that male karyotype has a systematic influence over any other measures of reproductive success. Our finding that AB and AC males may actually carry an advantage during fertilization and/or early embryo development, lends additional support to the evidence put forward by both Kim et al. [8] and Knief et al. [9] that variation between haplotypes is maintained in the population by a heterozygote advantage for male fitness.
In some systems, females employ post-copulatory reproductive strategies to offset the costs associated with mating with less fit males [16,20,21] (e.g. those homozygous for the inversion, who have more poorly performing sperm). We explored this here by analysing whether offspring sex and genotype ratios varied by parental haplotype sharing. For example, we might expect females to favour producing a female-biased brood when mated to homozygous males that share her haplotype (i.e. in which case all her male offspring would be less fit). However, we found no evidence that offspring sex ratio was influenced by inversion haplotype, or the number of haplotypes shared between parents. Similarly, offspring genotype ratios conformed to Mendelian expectations under all parental combinations. It is therefore unlikely in this system that females employ post-reproductive differential offspring investment based on the father's inversion haplotype. Differential investment in offspring sex ratio is predicted to occur where the costs and benefits of producing offspring differ between sons and daughters [16,48,49]. In the zebra finch, females have previously been shown to employ facultative offspring sex ratio adjustments based on female condition [22], and differential offspring investment has also been observed based on partner quality [20]. It is possible that we do not observe differential investment here because conflict over optimal trait values limits the ability of females to drive such traits to fixation. Alternatively, because male karyotype has no systematic influence on female reproductive traits or on offspring survival, instead primarily influencing sperm success under sperm competition, the strength of selection for such a trait will also probably depend on the degree of extra pair paternity. Finally, if inversion haplotype is linked with genes for phenotypic or behavioural signals associated with attractiveness in males (for example male band colour [50]) females may select the more suitable males based on pre-copulation cues, providing little need to evolve a mechanism for adjusting investment post-mating. Beak length has already been found to be linked with male Z inversion karyotype in this species, providing one potential mechanism for pre-copulatory female choice [6]. However, since natural mate choice was not permitted in our study population, we were unable to test this.
Finally, although not related to the inversion polymorphism, we observed a clear decline in egg production with female age, which is consistent with general trends of reproductive senescence across many (though not all) female birds [51]. We also observed a marked increase in the number of single-egged clutches with female age (electronic supplementary material, figure S1f). It may be that for older females, reproductive fitness is maximized by concentrating effort into the formation and post-laying care of a single egg, rather than spreading limited energy over a larger clutch. Laying larger clutches may be physically very challenging for older birds that have probably experienced a reduction in their follicular reserve, a decline in normal immunological and hormonal functioning, and an increase in the energy required for somatic maintenance [52]. This effect became apparent in our population when female birds reached between 3 and 4 years of age. In the wild, mortality is high, and the median life expectancy is only approximately four months [53]. It therefore seems unlikely that this phenomenon would be observed in the wild, but whether it is a specific feature of our population, or a more general pattern observed across captive birds is also unknown.
Understanding the factors that contribute to variation in reproductive success is a key goal of evolutionary research. Our findings provide a thorough test of the effects of a sex-linked inversion polymorphism for reproductive traits. We have shown that this inversion polymorphism, or ‘supergene’, despite its important effects for male fertility, does not appear to influence egg production by females or survival of embryos through to hatching. We have also provided additional evidence that the zebra finch Z inversion is probably being maintained in a stable polymorphism owing to heterozygote advantage for male fitness, whereby heterokaryotypic males carrying one copy of the ancestral inversion not only have faster and more successful sperm under sperm competition, but may also have an increased likelihood of fertilization and/or early embryo survival relative to homokaryotypes.
Acknowledgements
We thank Rachel Tucker and Gavin Horsburgh for technical assistance. Thanks also to Phil Young, Lynsey Gregory, Gemma Newsome, Jayne Pellatt, Lola Brookes, Clair Bennison, and Tim Birkhead, for enabling the collection of the original samples and dataset. Kang-Wook Kim and Jake Pepper helped with the identification of Z-linked haplotype-defining SNPs.
Ethics
The data used for this study was collected following approval by the University of Sheffield, UK. All procedures used to collect the data conformed to the legal requirements for animal research in the UK, and were conducted under a project licence (PPL 40/3481) issued by the Home Office. All animals were humanely killed under Schedule 1 (Animals (Scientific Procedures) Act 1986).
Data accessibility
Data and code are available in the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.cz8w9gj7w [38]. Electronic supplementary material is provided online [54].
Declaration of AI use
We have not used AI-assisted technologies in creating this article.
Authors' contributions
K.A.: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, project administration, software, validation, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; O.M.: formal analysis, methodology, software, validation, visualization, writing—review and editing; J.S.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, methodology, resources, supervision, writing—review and editing; N.H.: conceptualization, funding acquisition, methodology, project administration, resources, supervision, writing—review and editing.
All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.
Conflict of interest declaration
We declare we have no competing interests.
Funding
K.A. was supported by the Natural Environment Research Council ACCE Doctoral Training Partnership (DTP) (grant no. NE/S00713X/1). N.H. was supported by a Royal Society Dorothy Hodgkin Research Fellowship (DHF160200). Genotyping of male zebra finches was performed as part of a Biotechnology and Biological Research Council (BBSRC) grant no. (BB/I02185X/1) awarded to J.S.
References
- 1.Morrow EH, Arnqvist G, Pitcher TE. 2002. The evolution of infertility: does hatching rate in birds coevolve with female polyandry? J. Evol. Biol. 15, 702-709. ( 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2002.00445.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Pei Y, Forstmeier W, Wang D, Martin K, Rutkowska J, Kempenaers B. 2020. Proximate causes of infertility and embryo mortality in captive zebra finches. Am. Nat. 196, 577-596. ( 10.1086/710956) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Clutton-Brock T. 1988. Reproductive success: studies of individual variation in contrasting breeding systems. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. [Google Scholar]
- 4.Pei Y, Forstmeier W, Knief U, Kempenaers B. 2023. Weak antagonistic fitness effects can maintain an inversion polymorphism. Mol. Ecol. 32, 3575-3585. ( 10.1111/mec.16963) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Nosil P, Soria-Carrasco V, Villoutreix R, De-la-Mora M, De Carvalho CF, Parchman T, Feder JL, Gompert Z. 2023. Complex evolutionary processes maintain an ancient chromosomal inversion. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 120, e2300673120. ( 10.1073/pnas.2300673120) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Knief U, Hemmrich-Stanisak G, Wittig M, Franke A, Griffith SC, Kempenaers B, Forstmeier W. 2016. Fitness consequences of polymorphic inversions in the zebra finch genome. Genome Biol. 17, 1-22. ( 10.1186/s13059-016-1056-3) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Christidis L. 1986. Chromosomal evolution within the family Estrildidae (Aves) I. The Poephilae. Genetica 71, 81-97. ( 10.1007/BF00058691) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kim K-W, Bennison C, Hemmings N, Brookes L, Hurley LL, Griffith SC, Burke T, Birkhead TR, Slate J. 2017. A sex-linked supergene controls sperm morphology and swimming speed in a songbird. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 1, 1168-1176. ( 10.1038/s41559-017-0235-2) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Knief U, et al. 2017. A sex-chromosome inversion causes strong overdominance for sperm traits that affect siring success. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 1, 1177-1184. ( 10.1038/s41559-017-0236-1) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Berdan EL, Flatt T, Kozak GM, Lotterhos KE, Wielstra B. 2022. Genomic architecture of supergenes: connecting form and function. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 377, 20210192. ( 10.1098/rstb.2021.0192) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Funk ER, Mason NA, Pálsson S, Albrecht T, Johnson JA, Taylor SA. 2021. A supergene underlies linked variation in color and morphology in a Holarctic songbird. Nat. Commun. 12, 6833. ( 10.1038/s41467-021-27173-z) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Thompson MJ, Jiggins CD. 2014. Supergenes and their role in evolution. Heredity 113, 1-8. ( 10.1038/hdy.2014.20) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Itoh Y, Kampf K, Balakrishnan CN, Arnold AP. 2011. Karyotypic polymorphism of the zebra finch Z chromosome. Chromosoma 120, 255-264. ( 10.1007/s00412-010-0308-3) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Jennions MD, Petrie M. 2000. Why do females mate multiply? A review of the genetic benefits. Biol. Rev. 75, 21-64. ( 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1999.tb00040.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Uller T, Eklöf J, Andersson S. 2005. Female egg investment in relation to male sexual traits and the potential for transgenerational effects in sexual selection. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 57, 584-590. ( 10.1007/s00265-004-0886-2) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Bowers EK, Munclinger P, Bureš S, Kučerová L, Nádvorník P, Krist M. 2013. Cross-fostering eggs reveals that female collared flycatchers adjust clutch sex ratios according to parental ability to invest in offspring. Mol. Ecol. 22, 215-228. ( 10.1111/mec.12106) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Wagner RH, Helfenstein F, Danchin E. 2004. Female choice of young sperm in a genetically monogamous bird. Proc. R. Soc. B 271, S134-S137. ( 10.1098/rsbl.2003.0142) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Cunningham EJA, Russell AF. 2000. Egg investment is influenced by male attractiveness in the mallard. Nature 404, 74-77. ( 10.1038/35003565) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hasegawa M, Arai E, Watanabe M, Nakamura M. 2012. High incubation investment of females paired to attractive males in barn swallows. Ornithol. Sci. 11, 1-8. ( 10.2326/osj.11.1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bolund E, Schielzeth H, Forstmeier W. 2009. Compensatory investment in zebra finches: females lay larger eggs when paired to sexually unattractive males. Proc. R. Soc. B 276, 707-715. ( 10.1098/rspb.2008.1251) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Gorman HE, Arnold KE, Nager RG. 2005. Incubation effort in relation to male attractiveness in zebra finches Taeniopygia guttata. J. Avian Biol. 36, 413-420. ( 10.1111/j.2005.0908-8857.03464.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Bradbury RB, Blakey JK. 1998. Diet, maternal condition, and offspring sex ratio in the zebra finch, Poephila guttata. Proc. R. Soc. Lond. B 265, 895-899. ( 10.1098/rspb.1998.0375) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Pike TW, Petrie M. 2003. Potential mechanisms of avian sex manipulation. Biol. Rev. 78, 553-574. ( 10.1017/S1464793103006146) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Rice W. 1984. Sex chromosomes and the evolution of sexual dimorphism. Evolution 38, 735-742. ( 10.2307/2408385) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Mank JE, Ellegren H. 2009. Sex-linkage of sexually antagonistic genes is predicted by female, but not male, effects in birds. Evolution 63, 1464-1472. ( 10.1111/j.1558-5646.2009.00618.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Ellegren H, Parsch J. 2007. The evolution of sex-biased genes and sex-biased gene expression. Nat. Rev. Genet. 8, 689-698. ( 10.1038/nrg2167) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Ellegren H. 2011. Emergence of male-biased genes on the chicken Z-chromosome: sex-chromosome contrasts between male and female heterogametic systems. Genome Res. 21, 2082-2086. ( 10.1101/gr.119065.110) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Naurin S, Hansson B, Hasselquist D, Kim Y-H, Bensch S. 2011. The sex-biased brain: sexual dimorphism in gene expression in two species of songbirds. BMC Genomics 12, 37. ( 10.1186/1471-2164-12-37) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Parsons PA, Bodmer WF. 1961. The evolution of overdominance: natural selection and heterozygote advantage. Nature 190, 7-12. ( 10.1038/190007a0) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Höök L, Leal L, Talla V, Backström N. 2019. Multilayered tuning of dosage compensation and Z-chromosome masculinization in the wood white (Leptidea sinapis) butterfly. Genome Biology and Evolution 11, 2633-2652. ( 10.1093/gbe/evz176) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Itoh Y, et al. 2007. Dosage compensation is less effective in birds than in mammals. J. Biol. 6, 2. ( 10.1186/jbiol53) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Naurin S, Hansson B, Bensch S, Hasselquist D. 2010. Why does dosage compensation differ between XY and ZW taxa? Trends Genet. 26, 15-20. ( 10.1016/j.tig.2009.11.006) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Wright AE, Moghadam HK, Mank JE. 2012. Trade-off between selection for dosage compensation and masculinization on the avian Z chromosome. Genetics 192, 1433-1445. ( 10.1534/genetics.112.145102) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Birkhead TR, Hunter FM, Pellatt JE. 1989. Sperm competition in the zebra finch, Taeniopygia guttata. Anim. Behav. 38, 935-950. ( 10.1016/S0003-3472(89)80135-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bruford MW, Hanotte O, Brookfield JFY, Burke T. 1998. Molecular genetic analysis of populations: a practical approach. Oxford, UK: IRL Press. [Google Scholar]
- 36.LGC Group. 2024. SNPline workflow. See https://www.biosearchtech.com/support/resources/snpline-instrument-genotyping-workflow.
- 37.LGC Genomics. 2011. Kraken. LGCgenomics. See https://results.lgcgenomics.com/software/kraken_manual/frames/f=advanced.htm.
- 38.Assersohn K, Morton O, Slate J, Hemmings N. 2023. Data from: A sex-linked supergene with large effects on sperm traits has little impact on reproductive traits in female zebra finches. Dryad Digital Repository. ( 10.5061/dryad.cz8w9gj7w) [DOI] [PubMed]
- 39.Birkhead TR, Hall J, Schut E, Hemmings N. 2008. Unhatched eggs: methods for discriminating between infertility and early embryo mortality. Ibis 150, 508-517. ( 10.1111/j.1474-919X.2008.00813.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Assersohn K, Marshall AF, Morland F, Brekke P, Hemmings N. 2021. Why do eggs fail? Causes of hatching failure in threatened populations and consequences for conservation. Anim. Conserv. 24, 1-12. ( 10.1111/acv.12674) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41.R Core Team. 2022. R: a language and environment for statistical computing. Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing. [Google Scholar]
- 42.Bürkner P-C. 2017. brms: an R package for Bayesian multilevel models using Stan. J. Stat. Soft. 80, 1-28. ( 10.18637/jss.v080.i01) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Berger M, Tutz G. 2021. Transition models for count data: a flexible alternative to fixed distribution models. Stat. Methods Appl. 30, 1259-1283. ( 10.1007/s10260-021-00558-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Chambers M, Drovandi C. 2023. Many-levelled continuation ratio models for frequency of alcohol and drug use data. Am. J. Drug Alcohol Abuse 49, 566-575. ( 10.1080/00952990.2023.2213868) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Valle D, Ben Toh K, Laporta GZ, Zhao Q. 2019. Ordinal regression models for zero-inflated and/or over-dispersed count data. Sci. Rep. 9, 3046. ( 10.1038/s41598-019-39377-x) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Makowski D, Ben-Shachar M, Lüdecke D. 2019. bayestestR: describing effects and their uncertainty, existence and significance within the Bayesian framework. J. Open Source Softw. 4, 1541. ( 10.21105/joss.01541) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Cox RM, Calsbeek R. 2009. Sexually antagonistic selection, sexual dimorphism, and the resolution of intralocus sexual conflict. Am. Nat. 173, 176-187. ( 10.1086/595841) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Bartlow AW, Jankowski MD, Hathcock CD, Ryti RT, Reneau SL, Fair JM. 2021. Sex ratio of western bluebirds (Sialia mexicana) is mediated by phenology and clutch size. Ibis 163, 977-989. ( 10.1111/ibi.12935) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Trivers RL, Willard DE. 1973. Natural selection of parental ability to vary the sex ratio of offspring. Science 179, 90-92. ( 10.1126/science.179.4068.90) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Burley N, Enstrom DA, Chitwood L. 2002. Extra-pair relations in zebra finches: differential male success results from female tactics. Anim. Behav. 48, 1031-1041. ( 10.1006/anbe.1994.1336) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Holmes DJ, Thomson SL, Wu J, Ottinger MA. 2003. Reproductive aging in female birds. Exp. Gerontol. 38, 751-756. ( 10.1016/S0531-5565(03)00103-7) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Assersohn K, Brekke P, Hemmings N. 2021. Physiological factors influencing female fertility in birds. R. Soc. Open Sci. 8, 202274. ( 10.1098/rsos.202274) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Williams TD, Christians JK. 2003. Experimental dissociation of the effects of diet, age and breeding experience on primary reproductive effort in zebra finches Taeniopygia guttata. J. Avian Biol. 34, 379-386. ( 10.1111/j.0908-8857.2003.03080.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Assersohn K, Morton O, Slate J, Hemmings N. 2024. A sex-linked supergene with large effects on sperm traits has little impact on reproductive traits in female zebra finches. Figshare. ( 10.6084/m9.figshare.c.7109028) [DOI] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Citations
- Assersohn K, Morton O, Slate J, Hemmings N. 2023. Data from: A sex-linked supergene with large effects on sperm traits has little impact on reproductive traits in female zebra finches. Dryad Digital Repository. ( 10.5061/dryad.cz8w9gj7w) [DOI] [PubMed]
- Assersohn K, Morton O, Slate J, Hemmings N. 2024. A sex-linked supergene with large effects on sperm traits has little impact on reproductive traits in female zebra finches. Figshare. ( 10.6084/m9.figshare.c.7109028) [DOI] [PubMed]
Data Availability Statement
Data and code are available in the Dryad Digital Repository: https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.cz8w9gj7w [38]. Electronic supplementary material is provided online [54].