Summary
In monogamous species, prosocial behaviors directed towards partners are dramatically different from those directed towards unknown individuals and potential threats. Dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens has a well-established role in social reward and motivation, but how this mechanism may be engaged to drive the highly divergent social behaviors directed at a partner or unfamiliar conspecific remains unknown. Using monogamous prairie voles, we first employed receptor pharmacology in partner preference and social operant tasks to show that dopamine is critical for the appetitive drive for social interaction but not for low-effort, unconditioned consummatory behaviors. We then leveraged the sub-second temporal resolution of the fluorescent biosensor, GRABDA, to ask whether differential dopamine release might distinguish between partner and novel social access and interaction. We found that partner seeking, anticipation, and interaction resulted in more accumbal dopamine release than the same events directed towards a novel vole. Further, partner-associated dopamine release decreased after prolonged partner separation. Our results are consistent with a model in which dopamine signaling plays a prominent role in the appetitive aspects of social interactions. Within this framework, differences in partner- and novel-associated dopamine release reflect the selective nature of pair bonds and may drive the partner- and novel-directed social behaviors that reinforce and cement bonds over time. This provides a potential mechanism by which highly conserved reward systems can enable selective, species-appropriate social behaviors.
eTOC Blurb:
Pierce et al. delineate a dopaminergic mechanism underlying partner seeking and selective affiliation in monogamous relationships. They show that dopamine signaling is required for social seeking in prairie voles and that accumbal dopamine release is enhanced during partner seeking and interaction, which erodes after long-term partner separation.
Introduction
Optimally navigating social interactions is critical for survival and reproduction. Across species, dopamine plays an important role in navigating social relationships. Dopamine is released in the nucleus accumbens during social interaction, and manipulations that increase or decrease dopaminergic activity within this region promote or impair social interactions, respectively 1–3. Yet studies to date have examined real-time dopamine dynamics exclusively in laboratory species that do not form selective pair bonds 1,2,4,5. Thus, a central question remains of how differences in dopaminergic signaling directed to a pair bonded partner or novel individual may contribute to selective pair bonds and ultimately enable species appropriate behaviors.
Prairie voles are monogamous rodents that form life-long pair bonds. The formation of these bonds is facilitated by mating, an event that triggers dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens, and which results in a preference to affiliate with a specific partner, as well as aggression towards novel voles of either sex 6,7. Both of these behavioral features of pair bonds have been shown to depend on dopaminergic signaling 8–12. Blockade of dopamine D2 but not D1-class receptors during the initial mating period impedes the formation of a selective partner preference, although the same manipulation does not affect preference in established bonds 9,12. Conversely, D1-class receptors mediate agonistic behaviors; activation of these receptors increases selective aggression in bonded voles 8,13 and their activation can also impair bond formation 9. Plasticity within dopaminergic systems has also been implicated in bond formation and maintenance. Dopamine D1-class receptors are upregulated 8,13, and release dynamics are sensitized in established bonds 13. Electrical stimulation of the striatum leads to enhanced accumbal dopamine release in bonded compared to sexually naïve voles 13.
While these results indicate that dopaminergic systems play a highly conserved role in social reward, a major outstanding question is how shared neuromodulatory mechanisms can be differently engaged to create species-typical social drives. We performed a series of experiments examining the role of dopamine in partner seeking and preference expression, respectively. We found that dopamine receptor blockade did not disrupt partner preference in voles with an established bond, but that D1-class receptors instead modulate effortful seeking of social interaction. These findings are consistent with a broad role of dopamine D1 systems in appetitive aspects of motivation 14–17. Reasoning that release dynamics may provide a level of specificity masked by the receptor blockade, we tested the hypothesis that accumbal dopamine systems differentiate between interactions with a bonded partner and an unknown conspecific. We found that pair bonded partners elicit enhanced dopamine release during partner seeking and during subsequent social interactions, consistent with a reward-valuation role for dopamine in pair bonding (i.e. by assigning motivational valence) and providing a potential mechanism by which highly the conserved mesolimbic system can be engaged to elicit species-typical and selective social behaviors. Consistent with this hypothesis, we also observed an erosion of partner-enhanced dopamine release and partner-directed behaviors following bond devaluation via prolonged separation.
Results
All sample sizes and comprehensive statistical results, including effect size estimates, are reported in Supplementary Table 1.
Systemic dopamine receptor blockade does not impair expression of an existing partner preference
Prior reports indicate that D1- and D2-class signaling is not required for expression of partner preference in voles with an existing pair bond 12. Using the same antagonists in doses consistent with prior studies 12, we replicated these findings. To target D1-class receptors, we used the antagonist SCH-23390 hydrochloride, and for D2-class receptors, we used eticlopride hydrochloride 18–20. We focused on the first hour of the partner preference test, during which the animals had the highest circulating levels of antagonist and consistent with the duration of additional experiments outlined below. D1 blockade did not alter partner preference (Figure 1A,B; vehicle: one-way t-test relative to 50% (no preference) t(15) = 5.339, p = 8.27E−05; DRD1 antagonist: one-way t-test relative to 50% (no preference) t(15) = 4.936, p = 1.79E−04; vehicle vs antagonist paired t-test: t(15) = 0.099, p = 0.922). D1-class antagonist administration did not alter velocity but did increase total locomotion in the apparatus (Figure 1C-D; velocity: paired t-test: t(15) = 1.833, p = 0.087; locomotion: paired t-test: t(15) = 2.864, p = 0.012). D2-class antagonism also did not impair partner preference expression, but unlike D1 blockade, led to an increase in percent partner preference relative to vehicle treatment (Figure 1E,F; vehicle: one-way t-test relative to 50% t(15) = 3.308, p = 5.00E−03; DRD2 antagonist: one-way t-test relative to 50% t(15) = 6.86, p = 5.41E−06; vehicle vs antagonist paired t-test: t(15) = 2.653, p = 0.018). This was accompanied by a decrease in velocity and total locomotion (Figure 1G-H; velocity: paired t-test: t(15) = 2.523, p = 0.012; locomotion: paired t-test: t(15) = 2.323, p = 0.017). Although locomotor differences at these doses were not observed in previously published studies 12, our results could reflect differences in how locomotion was calculated and/or could be more evident during the first hour of the partner preference test.
Figure 1. Systemic dopamine receptor blockade does not impede partner preference in established pair bonds.
A-D) D1 antagonism (SCH-23390 0.5 mg/kg) did not disrupt partner preference (A), percent partner huddle (B), or velocity (C), but did increase distance traveled (D) during the first hour of the partner preference test. E-H) D2 antagonism (Eticlopride, 2mg/kg) did not disrupt partner preference (E) but did increase percent partner huddle (F). D2 antagonism decreased velocity (G) and distance traveled (H). n = 16. *p<0.05; ***p<0.005. See also Data S1, Figure S1.
We also assessed locomotor coordination via a rotarod apparatus (Figure S1A). We found that 1mg/kg but not 0.5mg/kg of SCH-23390 hydrochloride decreased time spent on the rod (Figure S1B, Data S1). Neither of the doses tested for eticlopride hydrochloride (1.25mg/kg and 2 mg/kg) altered locomotor effects (Figure S1C; Data S1). This indicates that locomotor coordination is at least partly dissociable from total locomotion/velocity.
Systemic D1-class but not D2-class antagonism reduces the appetitive aspects of social motivation
To systematically examine the potential role of dopamine in different facets of pair bonding, we next implemented lever pressing and barrier climbing for social access, two tasks which have been extensively used to evaluate behavioral activation and seeking behavior, key properties of motivated behavior 21–24. Female prairie voles are more adept at learning lever-pressing tasks, show more consistent behavior than do males, exhibit stronger partner- than novel-directed motivation 25–27. This, combined with prior reports showing the necessity of dopamine signaling in female voles for bond formation 11,12, led us to focus on females. We tested the functional role of D1- and D2-class receptors in pair bonded voles in two lever-pressing tasks and in a barrier climbing task. In the first version of the lever-pressing task, voles were presented with a single lever, which delivered access to a pair-bonded partner through a slotted divider for 30 seconds (Figure S1D). The second version of the task was equipped with two levers, with each lever assigned to provide direct access to the partner or a novel vole, respectively, in 5-trial blocks (Figure 2A). Pair bonded voles (Figure 2B) learned to press for social access in both paradigms. In the single lever task, the number of lever presses increased across training days (Figure S1E, Data S1), and the latency to press decreased across training days (Figure S1F, Data S1). In the dual lever task, we observed similar increases in lever pressing and decreasing latency to press as animals learned the task (Figure 2C; 2-way RM-ANOVA: Main effect of days F(9, 126) = 14.505, p = 0.002; Figure 2D; 2-way RM-ANOVA: Main effect of days F(9, 126) = 27.947, p 1.15E−04). Consistent with subsequent cohorts (Figure 4), there was no difference in the number of lever presses or the latency to press for partner or novel access (Figure 2C; 2-way RM-ANOVA: Main effect of vole (partner vs novel): F(1,14) = 0.4707, p = 0.5039; Figure 2D; 2-way RM-ANOVA: Main effect of vole (partner vs novel): F(1,14) = 0.3098, p = 0.5866). We likewise found that the latency to enter the chamber decreased across training days and did not differ between partner and novel trials (Figure 2E; Mixed Model ANOVA: Main effect of vole (partner vs novel): F(1,6) = 1.69, p = 0.241; Main effect of days F(9, 54) = 7.119, p = 0.037; Interaction (vole x days): F(9,54) = 1.271, p = 0.303). This is consistent with a prior report showing that voles will press equally for stranger and partner access when they are not forced to make a choice and if minimal effort is required for access 25. This procedure ensured we had enough trials to examine both partner- and novel-directed behaviors.
Figure 2. Systemic D1-class but not D2-class receptor signaling is required for social seeking.
A) Social operant chamber and operant trial structure. B) A partner preference test confirmed pair bond formation. C-E) The number of lever presses increased (C), and latency to lever press (D) and latency to enter the social chamber (E) decreased across training days. F-H) Compared to Vehicle, systemic D1 receptor blockade reduced number of lever presses (F) and increased the latency to press for partner and novel access (G) but did not change the latency to enter the social chamber (H). I-K) Systemic DRD2 did not reduce the number of lever presses (I), latency to lever press (J), or latency to enter chamber (K). L) Time series images of vole climbing over barrier to get access to a partner vole (under cup). M) Animals performing the barrier task had a partner preference. N) Number of attempts to climb over the barrier was reduced by systemic D1 but not D2 antagonism. n = 15, opreant task. n = 12, barrier task. *p<0.05; ***p<0.005. See also Data S1, Figure S1.
Figure 4. Partner seeking and access elicits enhanced dopamine release that erodes after partner separation.
A) Timing of a single trial. GRABDA-mediated fluorescence was recorded during interleaved blocks of partner and novel trials. B) Voles had a partner preference. C-E) Number of lever presses increased (C), and latency to press (D) and to enter the social chamber (E) decreased across training days. Pressing behavior stabilized for the last 3 days of operant access (dotted box) and did not differ between partner and novel presses for any metric. F) Representative GRABDA fluorescence in response to social operant events (days 4 – 6). G) Area under the curve (AUC) for 2 sec post-event (shaded regions in F). H) Heatmap showing Z-scored fluorescence for operant events for each vole (partner trials top, novel trials bottom). I) Graphical image of operant testing schedule prior to partner separation and a single probe operant trial after 4 weeks of partner separation. J) There are no differences in DA release for partner versus novel operant events post-separation. K) Representative traces (K) and AUC graphs (L) showing that, compared to pre-separation, dopamine release was reduced for partner-associated lever out, door opening, and chamber entry. M, N) Representative traces (M) and AUC graphs (N) showing that, compared to pre-separation, dopamine release was unchanged for novel-associated operant events. All traces from vole 4291. n = 11. *p < 0.05; ***p < 0.005. See also Data S1, Figure S2, Figure S3.
Once voles achieved consistent lever pressing, we asked whether systemic blockade of D1- or D2-class receptors altered lever pressing behaviors. In both tasks, administration of the D1-class antagonist, SCH-23390 hydrochloride, decreased lever pressing. We observed decreased lever presses and increased latency to lever press in the dual-lever task (Figure 2G, Data S1). In the single-lever task, lever pressing returned to pre-antagonist levels within 24 hours post-administration (Figure S1G; Data S1). The effects generalized to both partner and novel voles. D1-class antagonist administration reduced pressing for the partner and novel voles (Figure 2F; two-way RM-ANOVA: Main effect of partner vs novel: F(1,13) = 2.317, p = 0.1519; post-hoc Bonferroni: Partner vehicle vs partner antagonist t(13) = 10.22, p = 7.69E−05. Novel vehicle vs novel antagonist t(13) = 8.18, p = 2.984E−04; Figure 2G; two-way ANOVA: Main effect of partner vs novel: F(1,13) = 0.513, p = 0.488. post-hoc Bonferroni: Partner vehicle vs partner antagonist t(13) = 10.34, p = 4.76E−04. Novel vehicle vs novel antagonist t(13) = 8.57, p = 1.51E−03). In contrast, administration of a D2-class antagonist did not reduce motivation in either task (single lever: Figure S1H, Data S1; dual lever: Figure 2I-K; lever press: 2-way RM-ANOVA: Main effect of treatment F(1, 13) = 1.661, p = 0.222; latency to lever press: 2-way RM-ANOVA: Main effect of treatment F(1, 13) = 1.51, p = 0.243; latency to enter: Mixed Model ANOVA: Main effect of treatment F(1, 13) = 1.881, p = 0.207).
We also tested the role of D1- and D2-class signaling in a second task that leverages innate motivation to access a pair bonded partner by climbing over a mesh barrier (Figure 2L) 28,29. Systemic administration of the D1-class antagonist, but not D2-class antagonist or vehicle, reduced attempts to climb the barrier to access a pair bonded partner (Figure 2M; huddle time in partner preference test to verify pair bond: paired t-tests: t(11) = 2.402, p = 0.0351; % partner huddle one-way t-test relative to 50% (no preference) t(11) = 2.51, p = 0.0290 Figure 2N; one-way RM-ANOVA F(4, 4) = 4.158, p = 0.0061;. post-hoc Sidak: D1 antag vs veh: p = 0.021; D1 antag vs post: p = 0.0375; D2 antag vs veh: p = 0.7508). The convergence of our results across learned (lever pressing) and innate (climbing) tasks show that D1-class antagonism reduces social motivation in voles. Given that the same antagonist increased total locomotion in the partner preference test (Figure 1D), it is highly unlikely that reductions in climbing or pressing behavior are due to suppression of motor behaviors. This is further supported by the observation that D2-class antagonist administration reduced velocity and locomotion in the partner preference test (Figure 1G,H) but had no effects on lever pressing or climbing.
Dopamine dynamics reflect social operant learning
Based on the importance of D1 activity in social seeking behavior, we investigated the dynamics of dopamine release during these behaviors. We performed fiber photometry to measure GRABDA-mediated fluorescence as a proxy for dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens of voles engaged in operant responding and consumption of a social reward (Figure 3A - E) 30. Voles initially learned to associate rewards with lever pressing through food delivery (Figure S2A-D) before being presented with two new, separate levers that provided transient access to a tethered partner or novel animal, respectively (Figure 3D-F).
Figure 3. Dopamine dynamics reflect social operant learning.
A) Schematic of fiber photometry in a prairie vole. B) GRABDA and mCherry expression in the nucleus accumbens shell with ferrule track. Scale bar = 500 μm. C) Coronal atlas sections with locations of injection sites and ferrules (blue dots). D) Social operant chamber. E) Experimental timeline of operant social access. Blue outline indicates fiber photometry recording of dopamine levels. Delay (0 or 5 seconds) from lever press to chamber opening. F) Trial structure. G-I) The average number of lever presses increased (G), and latency to press (H) and latency to enter the social chamber (I) decreased across training days. J) Representative GRABDA fluorescence on the first and last day of operant social access during lever extension (lever out), lever press, social chamber opening (door open), and while crossing into the social chamber as detected by an infrared beam break (chamber entry). The onset of each event is indicated by a dashed line. K) Area under the curve (AUC) for 2 seconds post-event (shaded regions of J) comparing the first and last training days. n = 11. *p < 0.05; **p < 0.01; ***p < 0.005. See also Data S1, Figure S2.
Dopamine dynamics are typically conserved across species; thus we expected increased dopamine release for events predicting social access upon task learning 2,31,32. We compared dopamine release on the first (Day 1) and last (Day 6) days of social operant access (Figure 3). Task learning was reflected in increased lever pressing across days (Figure 3G; one-way RM-ANOVA: F(5, 10) = 6.139, p = 0.0047), a non-significant decrease in latency to press the lever (Fig 3H; mixed model ANOVA: F(5, 10) = 1.9, p = 0.178), and decreased latency to enter the social chamber after the door opened (Figure 3I; Mixed model ANOVA: F(5, 10) = 8.217, p = 0.0018). We found that dopamine release increased for operant events associated with social access on Day 6 relative to Day 1, consistent with prior reports indicating that dopamine release increases as a function of learning and subsequent reward anticipation (Figure 3J,K; paired t-tests: lever out t(10) = 2.716, p = 0.0217; lever press t(10) = 2.948, p = 0.0146; chamber open t(10) = 4.696, p = 0.0008) 2,33. We did not observe any differences in dopamine levels associated with chamber entry as a function of task learning (Figure 3J, K; paired t-test chamber entry t(10) = 1.474, p = 0.1711), consistent with chamber entry as a highly salient event associated with social reward delivery.
Partner seeking elicits enhanced dopamine release, which is eroded by long-term bond disruption
We next asked whether dopamine dynamics distinguished between partner- and novel-associated operant events during interleaved 5-trial blocks in which lever pressing resulted in partner or novel access, respectively (Figure 4A). Because partner- and novel-directed pressing behavior did not differ (Figure 4C – E, see Data S1), we asked whether dopamine dynamics could predict pressing behavior for the partner or a novel vole. We examined dopamine levels during the 1 sec tone indicating the start of a trial and immediately prior to lever extension in proficient animals (Figure 4C – E, boxed region). While tone-associated dopamine did not differ for partner and novel trials (Figure S3A, Data S1), we found tone-associated dopamine predicted whether the vole would press the lever for the partner (Figure S3B, Data S1), potentially reflecting a strong partner reward-association. This relationship between dopamine release and future lever pressing was not observed on novel trials (Figure S3C, Data S1), making it unlikely that the differences in DA release were tied to the motoric aspects of lever pressing. This was also limited to the 1 sec tone; dopamine release after lever extension did not predict pressing behavior for either partner or novel voles (see Data S1). We next asked whether dopamine release differentiated partner and novel trials after lever pressing. Across the last three days of social operant access, we observed greater dopamine release for partner lever pressing and door opening compared to the same events in novel trials (Figure 4F,G,H; paired t-tests: lever press t(10) = 2.791, p = 0.0191; door open t(10) = 2.307, p = 0.0438). Differences in dopamine release for partner and novel trials were not evident after lever extension or immediately after chamber entry (Figure 4F and 4G; paired t-tests: lever out t(10) = 1.382, p = 0.1972; chamber entry (0–2s) t(10) = 1.515, p = 0.1606).
To further explore the potentially unique features of pair bonding relative to other types of natural reward, we compared dopamine release during social operant and food operant access (Figure S2). Anticipation of food delivery (lever press and pellet dispense) and consumption of the food pellet resulted in significantly less dopamine release than lever pressing, door opening, and chamber opening, respectively, during partner trials. However, dopamine release during lever pressing for food and pellet dispensing were not significantly different than lever pressing and chamber opening for novel access (Figure S2F; Data S1). Thus, partner anticipation and partner access elicit greater dopamine than at least two other motivating events—access to a novel vole and access to food.
Finally, as prior work has shown that pair bonds erode as a function of long term separation 34–36, we asked how long-term partner separation affected partner and novel-associated dopamine dynamics. Prior to separation we performed a partner preference test to confirm that experimental voles displayed a partner preference (Figure 4B; one sample t-test relative to 50%: t(10) = 2.895, p = 0.016). We then separated pairs for four weeks – sufficient time for a vole to be able to form a new bond that supersedes the prior bond 34. We performed a single probe operant test via one day of social operant access with trial structure identical to pre-separation tests (Figure 4I). We found that although experimental voles still pressed the lever, their performance on the task was reduced. There was a significant decrease in the number of lever presses (Figure 4C; 2-way RM ANOVA; day 6 vs post separation: F(1,10) = 5.314; p = 0.044; partner vs novel: F(1,10) = 0.358; p = 0.563; timepoint x conspecific interaction: F(1,10) = 0.004; p = 0.953). While there was no change in the latency to lever press (Figure 4D; latency to press: 2-way RM ANOVA; day 6 vs post separation: F(1,8) = 1.393; p = 0.272; partner vs novel: F(1,8) = 0.143; p = 0.715; timepoint x conspecific interaction: F(1,8) = 0.134; p = 0.724), there was an increase in the latency to enter the vole’s chamber (Figure 4E; Latency to enter chamber: 2-way RM ANOVA; day 6 vs post separation: F(1,8) = 48.119; p = 1.2E−04; partner vs novel: F(1,8) = 0.024; p = 0.88; timepoint x conspecific interaction: F(1,8) = 0.341; p = 0.575). Surprisingly, we also saw a reversal in whether dopamine release predicted lever pressing, potentially reflecting a switch in reward away from the partner and towards a novel mating/bonding opportunity (Figure S3D-F, Data S1). Furthermore, after partner separation, lever pressing and chamber opening no longer elicited differences in dopamine release between partner and novel trials (Figure 4J; paired t-tests: lever out: t(10) = 0.0379, p = 0.9705; lever press: t(7) = 1.502, p = 0.1769; chamber open: t(7) = 0.639, p = 0.5432; chamber entry t(7) = 2.039, p = 0.0808). To determine the underlying changes in dopamine dynamics that led to an erasure of partner enhanced dopamine release, we compared pre- and post-separation dopamine release. We observed a consistent intra-animal decrease in operant-associated dopamine release upon lever presentation, door opening, and chamber entry for partner trials (Figure 4K,L; paired t-tests: lever out: t(10) = 2.88, p = 0.0164; lever press: t(9) = 0.04279, p = 0.9668; chamber open :t(9) = 2.403, p = 0.0397; chamber entry: t(9) = 2.705, p = 0.0242) but no change for novel trials (Figure 4M,N; paired t-tests: lever out: t(10) = 0.717, p = 0.4898; lever press: t(7) = 0.9486, p = 0.3744; chamber open: t(7) = 0.5486, p = 0.6003; chamber entry: t(7) = 1.479, p = 0.1826). As the voles still showed increases in response to the tone/lever out, lever pressing, and door opening compared to Day 1 of social operant access, this suggests that the partner-specific reduction in dopamine release was not simply a result of unlearning the task. Likewise, the lack of change in dopamine release during novel vole trials before and after separation indicates that decreased partner-associated dopamine release is not the product of technical considerations, such as a reduction in GRABDA fluorescence.
Social behavior and dopamine dynamics that differentiate partner and novel interactions are eroded by long-term separation
Social interactions between partners and with novel voles differ dramatically. We next quantified social behavior and corresponding dopamine release in the operant task after the test animal entered the social chamber. In accordance with prior reports 27, we found that pair bonded voles are more affiliative towards their partner than towards a novel vole, displaying more cumulative bouts of direct investigative contact and huddling behavior (Figure 5C, G; log-rank (Mantel-Cox) tests: direct contact investigation consisting of head, body, and anogenital sniffing: χ2 = 17.67, p = < 0.0001; huddle: χ2 = 16.25, p = < 0.0001) 7. In contrast, they show a greater number of cumulative bouts of non-contact investigation towards novel voles (Figure 5K; log-rank (Mantel-Cox) χ2 = 6.565, p = 0.0104). Physical interaction with partners – both via direct contact and huddling – produced greater dopamine release than the same behaviors directed towards a novel vole (Figure 5D, E,H,I; body sniff: paired t-test: t(5) = 8.974, p = 0.0003; huddle: unpaired t-test: t(7) = 3.268, p = 0.0137). We did not observe differences in dopamine elicited by the partner or a novel vole during non-contact investigation (Figure 5L, M; paired t-test: t(10) = 1.085, p = 0.3033).
Figure 5. Social behavior and dopamine pre- and post-separation.
A) Social interactions were scored after chamber entry during social operant tests for the last 3 days prior to separation and after 4 weeks of partner separation. B-E) Direct contact investigation: There was more direct contact displayed toward partners compared to novel voles, and after separation there was increased direct contact displayed towards partners and novels (B,C)). There was greater dopamine release during partner direct contact investigation compared to novel investigation only prior to separation (D, E). F-I) Huddling: There was no statistical difference in the duration of time spent huddling with the partner and the novel vole, and no difference in time spent huddling prior to and after separation (F). Greater partner huddling was evident in increased cumulative number of bouts before and after separation (G). There was greater dopamine release during partner huddling compared to novel huddling only prior to separation (H, I). J-M) Non-contact investigation: There was more non-contact investigation towards the novel vole while paired, and separation reduced non-contact investigation displayed towards partners and novels (J, K). There were no differences in dopamine release comparing partner and novel at either timepoint (L,M). Not all animals engaged in all behaviors, especially huddling, as reflected in the reduced number of dots and lack of connecting lines in some instances where an animal huddled only with the partner or with the novel vole. n = 11. * = p < 0.05; ** = p < 0.01; *** = p < 0.005; **** = p < 0.0005. See also Data S1.
We next asked how long-term separation affected interaction behavior and associated dopamine release (Figure 5). Changes in social interaction behavior were largely consistent with a partial erosion of the pair bond. Overall, there was a greater duration of direct contact investigation of both the partner and novel (Figure 5B; (2-way ANOVA: Main effect of separation: F(1,29) = 9.936, p = 0.004). After separation voles still displayed an increased cumulative number of bouts of direct contact toward the partner compared to the novel (Figure 5C; log-rank (Mantel-Cox) test: χ2 = 7.303, p = 0.0069. Separation did not significantly alter the duration of time spent huddling (Figure 5F; (2-way ANOVA: Main effect of separation: F(1,11) =1.838, p = 0.2), but voles still had a greater number of cumulative bouts of huddling with the partner compared to the novel (Figure 5F; log-rank (Mantel-Cox) test: χ2 = 10.66, p = 0.0011). The duration of non-contact investigation decreased after separation (Figure 5J; 2-way ANOVA: Main effect of separation: F(1,29) = 5.646, p = 0.023), and there was no difference in the number of bouts of non-contact investigation displayed toward the partner and novel vole (Figure 5K; log-rank (Mantel-Cox) χ2 = 0.1704, p =0.6797). The overall increased number of bouts of direct-contact investigation and huddling towards the partner compared to the novel vole was retained, suggesting that the test animals remembered their partner, and partner-selective behaviors were not completely erased (Figure 5B, C). None of the partner-novel differences in dopamine release detected pre-separation were evident post-separation (Figure 5D, E,H,I,L,M paired t-tests: direct contact t(6) = 0.218, p = 0.8347; huddle: t(4) = 1.198, p = 0.2971; non-contact investigation: t(7) = 0.7659, p = 0.4688). Similar to the greater dopamine release observed in partner-seeking contexts, these data support the model that dopamine release potentially signals value/motivational valence in pair-bonded social contexts, and that long-term separation reduces the value or valence of the partner.
Discussion
Dopamine modulates reward, motivation, and learning across broad contexts and has been extensively implicated in social behavior across species. Here we demonstrate that many broad dopaminergic functions are retained in monogamous prairie voles, including a role for dopamine D1-class signaling in appetitive social behaviors and conserved learning-related release dynamics. However, we also show that dopamine dynamics reflect the selective nature of pair bonds with partner-associated operant events and social interactions leading to greater accumbal dopamine release. Together, this suggests that dopamine plays a key role in mediating the appetitive aspects of pair bonding and provides a putative mechanism by which conserved neuromodulatory systems can contribute to species-appropriate and highly selective social behaviors.
We juxtaposed the well-established partner preference test with tasks that engage effort-driven seeking of social interaction. Reward acquisition in the partner preference test requires relatively little effort. In contrast, lever pressing and barrier climbing are classic tests for measuring the behavioral activation associated with the appetitive aspects of reward acquisition 37,38. Within this framework, we found that systemic dopamine signaling is not required for expression of an existing partner preference (replicating 12), but D1 receptor signaling is necessary for seeking social access, regardless of the identity of the social stimulus. Thus, our findings are consistent with prior work indicating that DA antagonism has a greater effect on appetitive/seeking behavior (i.e. “wanting”) and less effect on consummatory behavior (i.e.“liking”) 14. This also supports a broadly conserved role for D1 systems in appetitive aspects of diverse reinforcing experiences, including the necessity of D1-expressing accumbal neurons in operant self-administration for aggression in mice 39.
Inspired by prior ex vivo work showing a potentiation of evoked dopamine release in pair bonded voles 13, we asked whether differences in release dynamics differentiate partner and novel voles. Voles exhibited the same amount of lever pressing during partner and novel trials but exhibited enhanced dopamine release during partner seeking and in anticipation of access compared to the same events during novel vole trials. This finding effectively uncouples the dopamine-behavior relationship by showing that the same motoric behavior can result in differences in dopamine release when associated with access to different animals.
Unlike lever pressing, we found that social interaction behaviors were dramatically different when voles engaged with their partner or a novel vole. Voles engaged in substantially more direct contact investigation and huddling with their pair bonded partner than that with a novel vole, which was accompanied by enhanced dopamine during partner-directed behavior. In contrast, voles exhibited greater non-contact investigation of novel voles. This behavior enables assessment of a tethered animal without risking an aggressive encounter. There were no differences in dopamine when this behavior was directed towards a partner or a novel vole. Together, this suggests that dopamine release distinguishes pair bonded partners from novel voles both when seeking out potential social interaction and during the interactions themselves.
How might enhanced dopamine release activate downstream circuits to drive behavioral selectivity? Recent work in mice has shown that pharmacologically increasing dopamine in the striatum recruits more D1-type direct spiny projection neurons 40. Enhanced dopamine release may therefore recruit D1-neurons and bias voles towards partner seeking behaviors when given a choice. During social interaction, enhanced dopamine release may simply signify the relative valence and reward value of different social interactions with different voles, serving as a mechanism to reinforce partner-directed affiliation.
One of our most intriguing findings was an erosion of partner-directed behavior and partner-enhanced dopamine release after long-term separation. Consistent with a partial erosion of the bond, test animals show substantially fewer huddling bouts towards their partner and show overall increased direct contact investigation of both the partner and the stranger. They also exhibit an overall decrease in non-contact assessment behavior and no longer bias this behavior towards the stranger vole. Despite these large-scale shifts in social behavior, our test animals still exhibit more direct contact investigation toward their partner than a stranger, indicating some level of partner recognition. Given this, the lack of partner-enhanced dopamine during direct contact investigation and huddling following long-term separation is consistent with a devaluation of the bond rather than simply forgetting. The observed blunting of partner-elicited dopamine release following long term partner separation may thus enable the formation of a new bond by decreasing the selectively rewarding nature or valence of a previous partner, nullifying the exclusivity of the bond. Additional experiments are needed to control for the passage of time and delineate the timecourse of dopamine erosion.
Why do novel voles also elicit dopamine release? Potential explanations for novel-vole-elicited dopamine are likely complex. Novel-associated dopamine release may encompass reward-related signaling tied to the potential for extra-pair copulations, which can increase fitness. Another possibility is that dopamine is released in response to a novel threat and a need to defend territories from intruders. The latter is consistent with prior work suggesting that dopamine signaling may mediate aggression and novelty or threat detection in addition to social reward 3,8,33,41. Ultimately, testing these models, which posit differential roles for partner- and novelelicited dopamine release, will require functional manipulation.
While the present study provides novel insights into the real-time dopamine dynamics that contribute to bond selectivity and species-appropriate social behavior, examining aggregate changes in fluorescence as a proxy for dopamine release has some limitations. Specifically, we do not know whether there exists spatial or synapselevel specificity with respect to partner- and novel-associated dopamine release. It also remains unclear whether dopamine-mediated effects on behavior are the result of different patterns of co-release of dopamine with glutamate or GABA or combined action with oxytocin or endogenous opioids. Finally, while intriguing, additional work is needed to clarify how reductions in partner-elicited dopamine release following long-term separation may functionally contribute to loss adaptation in voles.
It also remains unknown whether differential socially-mediated dopamine release is evident in other brain regions in monogamous species. In monogamous zebra finches, which use vocal communication to elicit motivated responses, infusion of dopamine agonists into the auditory cortex enhanced preferences for less-preferred songs 42. This suggests that dopamine in sensory processing regions may shape the incentive salience of different types of social information, and enhanced partner-elicited dopamine release in various brain regions may coordinate different facets of bond-related preference behaviors.
In sum, we have shown that D1-class receptors are necessary for the appetitive aspects of social interaction and that accumbal dopamine release reflects the selective nature of pair bonds, with greater release associated with highly rewarding pair bond relationships. This work has important implications for human relationships, suggesting that dopamine may confer selectivity by predicting and reinforcing the rewarding aspects and motivational valence of partner interaction, thereby cementing relationships over time. The erosion of partner-associated dopamine release as a function of separation is consistent with a model in which pair bonds are assigned less motivational salience and/or reward following prolonged partner absence, thus providing a potential mechanism for overcoming loss. Together, this work suggests that real-time dopamine release dynamics are sensitive to experience and differentiate between relationship types, acting to shape real-time species-appropriate social decision making and behavior.
STAR Methods
RESOURCE AVAILABILITY
Lead contacts
Further information and requests for resources and reagents should be directed to and will be fulfilled by the lead contacts, Zoe R. Donaldson (Zoe.Donaldson@colorado.edu) and Anne F. Pierce (annepierce93@gmail.com).
Materials availability
Operant chamber materials and chamber designs can be accessed at https://github.com/donaldsonlab/Operant-Cage/tree/main/V2. The apparatus was controlled via custom scripts and code (https://github.com/dprotter/RPi_Operant 45; https://github.com/dprotter/RPi_Operant2 46). Partner Preference Test and Barrier Climbing chambers designs can be accessed at https://github.com/donaldsonlab/PPT-Chamber.
Data and code availability
Data used in each figure panel have been deposited in GitHub: https://github.com/donaldsonlab/Pierce2023_Currentbiology and are publicly available as of the date of publication. Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contacts upon request.
EXPERIMENTAL MODEL AND SUBJECTS DETIALS
Prairie voles were bred in house, initially imported from colonies housed at Cornell University, Emory University, and UC Davis, all of which originated from wild animals captured in Illinois. Animals were maintained at a temperature of 23–26°C on a 14:10 light:dark cycle. All procedures occurred during the light phase. Animals were given water and rabbit chow ad libitum (5326–3 by PMI Lab Diet). Rabbit chow was supplemented with sunflower seeds, dehydrated fruit bits, and alfalfa cubes. Home cages were enriched with cotton nestlets and plastic houses. At postnatal day 21, animals were weaned and placed into standard static rodent cages (17.5 l. x 9.0 w. x 6.0 h. in.) at a density of 2–4 same sex prairie voles. All females were sterilized via tubal ligation between the ages of postnatal day 72 and 96. At the onset of opposite-sex pairing, voles were placed into smaller static rodent cages (11.0 l. x 6.5 w. x 5.0 h. in.) where they remained until they were separated from their opposite-sex partner and placed in clean small rodent cages. All voles were between 94 and 118 days old at the onset of pairing. Procedures were approved by the University of Colorado Institutional Animal Care Use Committee. Samples sizes for all experiments are represented by n values in the text and figure legends.
METHOD DETAILS
Surgical Procedures
AAV infusion and ferrule implantation
Experimental animals underwent viral infusion and ferrule implantation surgery between 72 and 96 days of age. Voles were anesthetized with 1–3% isoflurane at an oxygen flow rate of 1L/min in a head-fixed stereotactic frame (David Kopf, Tujunga, CA). Body temperature was maintained at 37°C using a closed loop heating pad with a rectal thermometer (David Kopf, Tujunga, CA). Eyes were lubricated with ophthalmic ointment (Sterile Lubricant Eye Ointment). The fur was removed from the incision site using a shaver, and the wound area was disinfected with 70% isopropyl alcohol and betadine. Briefly, the scalp and any connective tissue was removed above the frontal and parietal skull plates. Two 0.5 mm guide holes were drilled—each in the parietal plates—and anchoring screws were rotated into place. The head was leveled in the anterior-posterior plane, and a 0.5 mm hole was drilled at +1.6 mm AP and +1 mm ML. A hole was drilled and a Nanoject syringe (Drummond Scientific, Broomall, PA) was lowered and 200 nL of AAV vector was injected at a rate of 1nL/sec unilaterally at −5.0, −4.9, and −4.8mm DV for a total of 600 nL. The following vectors and titers were used: AAV1-hSyn-DA4.4 M205T (GRABDA, plasmid gift from Dr. Yulong Li, packaged by Vigene) at 1.015X1013 GC/ml and AAV2-hsyn-mCherry (Addgene) at 3X1012 GC/ml. The latter AAV provided a red fluorescent signal used to normalize for motion artifacts47. The needle was left in place for 10 minutes following the last infusion. Then, a fiberoptic ferrule (0.2 mm diameter, 5 mm long; Doric Lenses Inc) was slowly lowered into position until reaching a final placement of −4.8 mm DV. The ferrule and screws were affixed to the skull with Loctite 454 cured with acrylic resin (Jet Liquid). The initial layer was covered with Loctite mixed with black carbon powder (Sigma). Extended Release Meloxicam (4 mg/kg), enrofloxacin (5 mg/kg), and saline (up to 3mL) were administered subcutaneously perioperatively for analgesia, to avoid bacterial infection at the wound site, and to prevent dehydration, respectively. Additionally, enrofloxacin (5 mg/kg) and saline (1mL) were administered subcutaneously for the three days following surgery. Animals were allowed to recover for at least 14 days prior to initiation of experiments. Ferrule placement and viral expression were confirmed posthumously (Figure 3B, C).
Tubal ligations
Females were tubally ligated to avoid confounds of pregnancy while keeping the ovaries hormonally intact. Tubal ligation was carried out during an independent surgery or under anesthesia during vector infusion and ferrule placement (described above). Briefly, hair was shaved at the incision site and the underlying skin was disinfected with betadine and 70% isopropyl alcohol A single incision was made in the midline of the back to provide access to the body cavity. The incision was pulled to one side until aligned above the ovary. A small incision was made into the body wall, the ovary was pulled through and bisected from the uterus via a cauterizer. The uterus and ovary were returned to the body cavity, the internal body wall was closed using an absorbable suture, the skin was pulled to the other side, and the procedure was repeated. The external incision was closed with staples that were removed 10–14 days later. Triple antibiotic ointment and lidocaine were placed on the closed wound.
Behavioral Methods
Voles were tubally ligated, allowed to recover, and paired at least 14 days prior to the onset of operant training, barrier climbing, or partner preference testing, sufficient time for stable pair bonding to occur25,34.
Rotarod-based assessment of locomotor coordination
We used the series 8 Rotarod apparatus from IITC Life Science Inc to evaluate whether dopamine D1- or D2-class receptor antagonist administration affected motor ability and coordination. Animals were allowed to habituate to the testing room for two hours prior to daily rotarod training/testing. Voles were trained twice a day for 3 days by placing them on top of a 3.75-inch diameter drum. On the first training day, a completed trial consisted of staying on the drums at a constant speed of 4 rpm for 60 seconds48. On all subsequent training and testing days the rotarod was set to increase continuously in speed from 4 to 6rpm over 60 seconds. Training was deemed successful if voles completed 2 trials per training day with a 20-minute break between trials. For testing, we measured the time spent on the drum before falling off or completing the trial. If animals completed the trial without falling off, they received a duration of 60 seconds for that trial; otherwise, they received the value of the duration of time they stayed on the rod before falling off. All trials on a given day were averaged for the reported duration of time spent on the rod. Voles received intraperitoneal injections of volume 0.1ml/10g body weight for vehicle and antagonist administration and were tested 3 times, 10-, 30-, and 50-minutes post-injection (see Supplementary Statistics Table for analysis by timepoint). Voles were tested in the following order with at least one day between tests: vehicle, 1mg/kg SCH23390, 0.5 mg/kg SCH23390, 1.25 mg/kg eticlopride, 2 mg/kg Eticlopride.
Operant training and timeline
Custom operant chambers were as previously described in Brusman et al 202225. Briefly, operant chambers contained 3 chambers separated by 2 motorized doors, one motorized pellet dispenser and trough, and 3 separate retractable levers (one for each type of reward). For experiments using partner-only presentation (Figure S1), only one social chamber was used, and a slotted divider was affixed next to the motorized door to allow social interaction while blocking social chamber entry. Chambers were constructed from a mix of laser cut acrylic and 3D printed ABS plastic. A bill of materials and chamber designs can be accessed at https://github.com/donaldsonlab/Operant-Cage/tree/main/V2.
The apparatus was controlled via custom scripts and code (https://github.com/dprotter/RPi_Operant) run on Raspberry Pi computers (Raspberry Pi Foundation). Servos were controlled via an Adafruit HAT (Adafruit 2327). Each apparatus was controlled by a corresponding Raspberry Pi. Food rewards were 20 mg pellets (Dustless Precision Pellets Rodent Grain-Based Diet; VWR 89067–546) delivered to a trough. Pellet dispensing and retrieval was detected by an IR beam break in the trough. Tones were generated via PWM on the Raspberry Pi (pigpio), and played through an amplified speaker (Adafruit 3885).
Voles were paired more than 14 days prior to the onset of operant training, sufficient time for stable pair bonding to occur 25,34. We used three different training paradigms, each described below, adapted to the goals of each experiment. Voles were not food restricted. During partner separation, animals were socially isolated.
Food Magazine Training
Food magazine training was only included for GRABDA experiments. Animals underwent 6 days of magazine training with 15 trials per day (Figure S2A), the goal of which was to learn associations between the lever, tone, and food reward. For each trial, a tone was played to indicate the start of the trial (5,000 Hz, 1s). The food lever was then extended for 2 seconds, a pellet cue (2,500 Hz, 1s) was played, and a single pellet was delivered to the trough. The lever was retracted 2 seconds later. If an animal pressed within the first 2 seconds of lever access, a pellet was immediately delivered. No more than 1 pellet was delivered per trial. Total trial time was 90s.
Operant food delivery
Operant food delivery was only performed for GRABDA experiments. Animals underwent 20 trials per day for 8 days (Figure S2A). During the first two days (training), pellet delivery was not contingent on lever pressing. During each trial, a tone was played to indicate the start of the trial (5,000 Hz, 1s). The food lever was then extended for 30 seconds. After 30 seconds, the lever was retracted if the vole did not press the lever, a pellet cue (2,500 Hz, 1s) was played, and a pellet was delivered to the trough. If the vole pressed the lever within 30 seconds, a pellet cue (2,500 Hz, 1s) was played, and a pellet was immediately delivered to the trough. A single pellet was dispensed on every trial after 30s of lever presentation, but lever pressing elicited an immediate pellet dispense. During days 3–8 of training, pellet delivery was contingent on lever pressing. The lever was extended for a maximum duration of 120s. During each trial, a tone was played to indicate the start of the trial (5,000 Hz, 1s). After 120 seconds, the lever was retracted if the vole did not press the lever and no pellet was dispensed. If the vole pressed the lever within 120 seconds, a pellet cue (2,500 Hz, 1s) was played, and a pellet was delivered to a trough. To provide a window to observe anticipatory behavior and dopamine release, animals experienced a delay between lever pressing and food reward as follows: days 1–5: no delay, days 6–8: 5 second delay. The intertrial interval for all trials was 45 sec.
Operant social access
For single-chamber social operant training (Figure S1), animals had 20 trials per day where they were given the option to press a single lever to gain access to a partner through a slotted barrier. The trial structure was as follows: a tone was played to indicate the start of the trial (5,000 Hz, 1s) followed by the extension of a single lever. If the lever was pressed within 300 seconds, the lever was retracted, and a tone was played to indicate a successful lever press. Then a door opened for 30 seconds, allowing limited access to the partner through a slotted barrier. After 30 seconds a door closing tone was played (7,000 Hz, 1s) and the door was closed. If the lever was not pressed after 300 seconds, the lever was retracted. All trials had an intertrial interval of 45 seconds.
For dual chamber social operant experiments, pair bonded voles underwent 20 trials of social training per day, in which they were given access to 2 levers, one lever gave access to the partner and another lever gave access to a novel opposite sex vole. Experimental voles were given alternating sets of 5 trials for each lever, starting with the partner lever (i.e. 5x partner, 5x novel, 5x partner, 5x novel) (Figure 2A). The partner and novel stimulus animals were tethered at opposite ends of the apparatus and farthest from the doors in a similar fashion to the partner preference test (below). The tethering location of partner and novel voles remained consistent across days. To avoid a potential unintended bias in lever pressing, we assign the lever farthest from the door to provide access to the partner or novel, respectively (Figure 2A). A new novel vole was used each day of operant social access and during the probe trial after separation. On all training days, a tone was played to indicate the start of the trial (5,000 Hz, 1s). After 120 seconds, the lever was retracted. If the lever was pressed within 120 seconds, social access was granted, and at the end of the trial, a door close tone was played (7,000 Hz, 1s) and the door was closed. If needed, subjects were manually returned to the center chamber immediately after the chamber closed. The duration of social interaction received was dependent on how quickly the vole pressed the lever. Each trial was a maximum of 120 seconds, and the amount of social interaction was 120 seconds minus the latency to press the lever. All trials had an intertrial interval of 45 seconds. In Figure 2, the door was opened with no delay following the lever press on days 1 – 8 and a 5 second delay on days 8 – 10. In Figure 3–4, the door was opened without any delay after lever press on days 1–3 and following a 5 second delay on days 4–6 and during the probe test after partner separation. For experiments with GRABDA recording, animals received food magazine and operant food training prior to social operant training.
Barrier climbing task
The standard partner preference chamber was adapted so that a one-sided metal wire mesh barrier (5 mm holes, other side clear acrylic, Figure 2L) was placed between the middle and a side chamber with the mesh facing towards the middle chamber. The third chamber was blocked off by a solid acrylic barrier. Female subjects were placed in the middle chamber with access to the mesh side of the barrier, while their male partner was placed in the other chamber under a pencil cup. Before the mesh barriers were added, the subject was given a 2-minute habituation period where they could freely explore both chambers and their partner under the cup.
Climbing attempts were defined as instances when all the subject’s feet left the ground while climbing the mesh barrier. Attempts were counted as successful when the animal landed on the other side of the barrier. Success rate was calculated as successes/attempts. After each successful attempt, the subject was given a 30 second period where they were able to interact with their partner under the cup before being returned to the middle chamber. Each session lasted 17 minutes, including the habituation period. All female subjects were initially given an 8.5 cm barrier (less than one body length). To avoid a potential ceiling effect, the barrier height was varied; upon reaching 90% success rate, we introduced a 17 cm barrier, and if the vole reached > 90% success rate again, we introduced a 34 cm barrier. Barrier height remained static while determining baseline crossing rate.
Partner preference test
Partner preference tests were performed as described in Scribner et al. 202049. Briefly, both partner and novel animals were tethered to the end walls of three-chamber plexiglass arenas (76.0 cm long, 20.0 cm wide, and 30.0 cm tall). Tethers consisted of an eye bolt attached to a chain of fishing swivels that slid into the arena wall. Animals were briefly anesthetized with isoflurane and attached to the tether using a zip tie around the animal’s neck. Two pellets of rabbit chow were given to each tethered animal and water bottles were secured to the wall within their access while tethered. After tethering the partner and novel animals, experimental animals were placed in the center chamber of the arena. At the start of the test, the opaque dividers between the chambers were removed, allowing the subject to move freely about the arena for three hours. Overhead cameras (Panasonic WVCP304) were used to video record eight tests simultaneously.
The movement of all three animals in each test was scored using TopScan High-Throughput software v3.0 (Cleversys Inc) using the parameters from Ahern et al., 200950. Behavior was analyzed using a Python script developed in-house (https://github.com/donaldsonlab/Cleversys_scripts) to calculate the time spent huddling with the partner or novel. The partner preference score was calculated as (partner huddle time/[partner huddle time + novel huddle time]) × 100%. We report the analysis of preference score in the text using a one-way t-test relative to a null hypothesis of 50% (no preference). We also performed a paired t-test and/or RM-ANOVA comparing partner versus novel huddle times in the figure legends and Supplementary Statistics Table. The latter is less rigorous as it uses data that violates assumptions of independence as the test animal cannot interact with both the partner and novel simultaneously.
Pharmacological receptor blockade
To test the role of dopamine on behavior, we administered D1- or D2-class receptor antagonists during social operant, barrier climbing, and partner preference tests. To test the role of D1- and D2-class receptor inhibition on behavior, we administered intraperitoneally, 0.5mg/kg SCH-23390 hydrochloride, 2mg/kg Eticlopride hydrochloride (Tocris Bioscience), or saline, 5 minutes prior to test onset. To test for potential order effects, we counterbalanced dopamine class antagonists and vehicle administration across operant testing days shown in Figure S1G,H (vehicle data is always presented first in figures). There was no effect of testing day or drug order on the number of lever presses (reported in supplementary Statistics Table). Finally, we assessed effects only during the first hour of the partner preference test, comparable to the amount of time animals performed the operant task post-administration.
Fiber Photometry
GRABDA-mediated measurement of nucleus accumbens dopamine during social operant
Subjects were habituated to the patch cable for 6 days in an open field chamber prior to the onset of operant training. Subjects were briefly anesthetized (<30s) to attach patch cables prior to recording and allowed 10 minutes to recover prior to operant testing.
Fluorescence was acquired using the Neurophotometrics (NPM) V2 system with 200uM core optical fibers purchased from Doric Lenses. Data was acquired using Bonsai44. During photometry recordings, light was delivered alternating between 470 nm, 560 nm, and 415 nm at a framerate of 180 frames per second. The LED power for each wavelength was set to 50uW at the optical fiber tip to reduce photobleaching. Signals were analyzed using a MATLAB script. To correct photobleaching and motion artifacts, the 560 nm signal was fit to the 470 nm, then this fit was subtracted from the 470 nm signal. Z-Scores were calculated as (fitted signal – baseline)/(baseline standard deviation) where the baseline for all events and behaviors was −8 to −3 seconds prior to lever extension (during the intertrial interval). The area under the curve was calculated as the average Z-Score values 2 sec after the event.
We time-locked operant events (extension of levers, lever pressing, chamber opening, chamber entry, pellet dispense, pellet retrieval) to the fluorescence signal using two microcontrollers. Bonsai cannot run on the Raspberry Pi 3 B+ operating system that is used to control the operant chamber hardware, so we directed a Raspberry Pi to send a serial signal to an Arduino Uno microcontroller, which has communication functionality with Bonsai.
All behaviors that occurred after crossing into the chamber were hand scored using BORIS, a behavior event scoring software43. Behaviors performed by the subject directed towards the partner or novel on days 4–6 of social operant access were hand scored. We examined the following behaviors: non-contact investigation, head sniffing, body sniffing, anogenital sniffing, huddling, allogrooming, defensive posture, and attacking. Head, body, and anogenital sniffing were aggregated into a direct-contact investigation metric. Huddling and allogrooming were combined as highly prosocial behaviors. Non-contact investigation consisted of the test animal attending to the stimulus animal without physical touching, a type of assessment behavior with reduced risk of agonistic interaction. Animals displayed little to no defensive posture/attack, so these behaviors were omitted from subsequent analyses.
Brain collection
Upon completion of experimental sessions, voles were transcardially perfused with 4% paraformaldehyde in phosphate buffered saline. The head was removed with the ferrule intact and post-fixed for 24 hours in 4% paraformaldehyde before extracting ferrule and brain. The brain was equilibrated in 30% sucrose, sectioned in 50 μm slices using a sliding freezing microtome (Leica), and mounted on slides. Ferrule placement was drawn onto corresponding mouse atlas sections.
QUANTIFICATION AND STATISTICAL ANALYSES
Data are shown as means ± standard error of the mean (SEM). Statistical significance α was set as 0.05. All n values represent the number of animals. All statistical analyses were carried out using Graphpad PRISM 9.3.1 and SPSS 29.0.0.0. As appropriate, one- or two-way repeated measure ANOVAs were employed to examine the effects of day, stimulus animal (partner or novel vole), and/or antagonist administration on operant behaviors with specific tests indicated in the results section. Mixed-model ANOVAs were used when individual data points were missing (for instance due to failure to enter a social chamber). Z-scored fluorescence area under the curve comparisons for day 1 and day 6 (Figure 1) or partner and novel (Figures 2, 3, 4) were analyzed using a paired or unpaired t-test (latter only Figure 5I). Differences in Z-scored fluorescence area under the curve elicited by food and social operant events were analyzed using a one-way repeated measures ANOVA with a Tukey’s post-hoc test (Figure S2). Partner preference was analyzed using a one sample t-test relative to an expected null 50 percent preference (no preference) (Figures 1B, 1F, 2B, 2M, and 4B). Differences in durations of behaviors demonstrated towards partners and novels were analyzed using a 2-way ANOVA. Finally, cumulative bouts of behaviors demonstrated towards partners and novels were analyzed using a Log-rank (Mantel-Cox) test. See Supplement table 1 for a full list of statistics information. Five animals were excluded from fiber photometry experiments due to placement outside of the NAc or misalignment of the ferrule with viral expression.
Supplementary Material
Data S1. Comprehensive statistical results including effect sizes for all analyses. Excel file containing one tab per figure. Related to Figures 1–5, S1 – S3.
KEY RESOURCES TABLE
REAGENT or RESOURCE | SOURCE | IDENTIFIER |
---|---|---|
Viruses | ||
AAV1-hSyn-DA4.3 M205T | Plasmid gifted from Yulong Li30; Packaged at Vigene Biosciences | NA |
AAV2-hsyn-mCherry | Addgene | CAT# 114472-AAV2 |
Chemicals | ||
SCH-23390 hydrochloride | Tocris Bioscience | CAT# 0925 |
Eticlopride hydrochloride | Tocris Bioscience | CAT# 1847 |
Deposited data | ||
Data for figures | This paper | Github: https://github.com/donaldsonlab/Pierce2023_Currentbiology |
Experimental models: Organisms/strains | ||
Prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster) | University of Colorado-Boulder Colony | NA |
Hardware and Software | ||
Operant Chambers Hardware | Brusman et al 202225 and this paper | Github https://github.com/donaldsonlab/Operant-Cage/tree/main/V2 |
Operant Chambers Software | Brusman et al 202225 and this paper | Github: https://github.com/dprotter/RPi_Operant |
Partner Preference and Barrier Climbing Chambers | This paper | Github: https://github.com/donaldsonlab/PPT-Chamber |
Software for scoring Partner Preference Test behavior | CleverSys | NA |
Software for hand scoring behaviors | Behavioral Observation Research Interactive Software (BORIS)43 | NA |
Acquisition of events from operant chamber and fiber photometry data | Bonsai44 | NA |
Fiber photometry equipment | ||
Fiber photometry system | Neurophotometrics | FP3002 |
Ferrules | Doric Lenses Inc. | MFC_200/230-0.37_5mm_ZF2.5_FLT Mono Fiberoptic Cannula **Fiber Photometry** |
Patch cables | Doric Lenses Inc. | MFP_200/220/LWMJ-0.37_3m_FC-ZF2.5_LAF Mono Fiberoptic Patchcord - Low Autofluorescence |
Other | ||
Dustless Precision Pellets Rodent Grain-Based Diet | Avantor/VWR | Cat# 89067-546 |
Rotarod | IITC Life Science Inc | Series 8 |
Highlights:
Partner preference in an existing bond does not require D1- or D2-class signaling
Social seeking in voles requires dopamine D1—but not D2—class receptor activity
Accumbal dopamine release is greater for pair-bonded partners than unknown voles
Long-term separation erodes enhanced partner-associated dopamine release
Acknowledgments
Yulong Li provided the GRABDA plasmid. We thank the animal care staff at the University of Colorado Boulder. David Root helped pilot early fiber photometry experiments and Emma Culter helped with histology and imaging. Linda Watkins provided the rotarod. Kelly Winther, Katie Gallagher, and Kresil Gordon managed the animal colony and provided experimental support. Sarah Litz contributed to the scripts used to run the operant chambers, and Lena Obaid helped with device assembly. We took histology images on the Stem Cell Research and Technology Resource Center microscope. Kelly Winther performed tissue preparation and histology. Sage Aronson provided advice on fiber photometry experiments. Finally, we thank the rest of the Donaldson lab for their feedback and support and the voles for their sacrifice. This work was supported by NIH awards R36MH129127 (to A.F.P.) & Whitehall and Dana Foundation awards, and DP2MH119421, UF1NS122124, U01NS131406. IOS-2045348 (to Z.R.D.).
Inclusion and Diversity Statement
One or more of the authors of this paper self-identifies as an underrepresented ethnic minority in their field of research or within their geographical location. One or more of the authors of this paper self-identifies as living with a disability. One or more of the authors of this paper self-identifies as a gender minority in their field of research.
Footnotes
Declaration of Interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.
References
- 1.Gunaydin LA, Grosenick L, Finkelstein JC, Kauvar IV, Fenno LE, Adhikari A, Lammel S, Mirzabekov JJ, Airan RD, Zalocusky KA, et al. (2014). Natural neural projection dynamics underlying social behavior. Cell 157, 1535–1551. 10.1016/j.cell.2014.05.017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hu RK, Zuo Y, Ly T, Wang J, Meera P, Wu YE, and Hong W. (2021). An amygdala-to-hypothalamus circuit for social reward. Nat. Neurosci 24, 831–842. 10.1038/s41593-021-00828-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Dai B, Sun F, Tong X, Ding Y, Kuang A, Osakada T, Li Y, and Lin D. (2022). Responses and functions of dopamine in nucleus accumbens core during social behaviors. Cell Rep. 40, 111246. 10.1016/j.celrep.2022.111246. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Anstrom KK, Miczek KA, and Budygin EA (2009). Increased phasic dopamine signaling in the mesolimbic pathway during social defeat in rats. Neuroscience 161, 3–12. 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.03.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Schwerdt HN, Shimazu H, Amemori K, Amemori S, Tierney PL, Gibson DJ, Hong S, Yoshida T, Langer R, Cima MJ, et al. (2017). Long-term dopamine neurochemical monitoring in primates. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci 114, 13260–13265. 10.1073/pnas.1713756114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Carter CS, and Getz LL (1993). Monogamy and the prairie vole. Sci. Am 268, 100–106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Kenkel WM, Gustison ML, and Beery AK (2021). A Neuroscientist’s Guide to the Vole. Curr. Protoc 1, e175. 10.1002/cpz1.175. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Aragona BJ, Liu Y, Yu YJ, Curtis JT, Detwiler JM, Insel TR, and Wang Z. (2006). Nucleus accumbens dopamine differentially mediates the formation and maintenance of monogamous pair bonds. Nat. Neurosci 9, 133–139. 10.1038/nn1613. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Aragona BJ, Liu Y, Curtis JT, Stephan FK, and Wang Z. (2003). A critical role for nucleus accumbens dopamine in partner-preference formation in male prairie voles. J Neurosci 23, 3483–3490. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Liu Y, and Wang ZX (2003). Nucleus accumbens oxytocin and dopamine interact to regulate pair bond formation in female prairie voles. Neuroscience 121, 537–544. 10.1016/S0306-4522(03)00555-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Gingrich B, Liu Y, Cascio C, Wang Z, and Insel TR (2000). Dopamine D2 receptors in the nucleus accumbens are important for social attachment in female prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Behav. Neurosci 114, 173–183. 10.1037//0735-7044.114.1.173. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Wang Z., Yu G., Cascio C., Liu Y., Gingrich B., and Insel TR. (1999). Dopamine D2 receptor-mediated regulation of partner preferences in female prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster): A mechanism for pair bonding? Behav. Neurosci 113, 602–611. 10.1037/0735-7044.113.3.602. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Resendez SL, Keyes PC, Day JJ, Hambro C, Austin CJ, Maina FK, Eidson LN, Porter-Stransky KA, Nevárez N, McLean JW, et al. (2016). Dopamine and opioid systems interact within the nucleus accumbens to maintain monogamous pair bonds. eLife 5, e15325. 10.7554/eLife.15325. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Salamone JD, and Correa M. (2012). The mysterious motivational functions of mesolimbic dopamine. Neuron 76, 470–485. 10.1016/j.neuron.2012.10.021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Salamone JD, Correa M, Yohn S, Lopez Cruz L, San Miguel N, and Alatorre L. (2016). The pharmacology of effort-related choice behavior: Dopamine, depression, and individual differences. Behav. Processes 127, 3–17. 10.1016/j.beproc.2016.02.008. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Ikemoto S, and Panksepp J. (1996). Dissociations between appetitive and consummatory responses by pharmacological manipulations of reward-relevant brain regions. Behav. Neurosci 110, 331–345. 10.1037/0735-7044.110.2.331. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Rusk IN, and Cooper SJ (1994). Parametric studies of selective D1 or D2 antagonists: effects on appetitive and feeding behaviour. Behav. Pharmacol 5, 615. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Roth BL, Tandra S, Burgess LH, Sibley DR, and Meltzer HY (1995). D4 dopamine receptor binding affinity does not distinguish between typical and atypical antipsychotic drugs. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 120, 365–368. 10.1007/BF02311185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Hall H, Köhler C, and Gawell L. (1985). Some in vitro receptor binding properties of [3H]eticlopride, a novel substituted benzamide, selective for dopamine-D2 receptors in the rat brain. Eur. J. Pharmacol 111, 191–199. 10.1016/0014-2999(85)90756-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Chien EYT, Liu W, Zhao Q, Katritch V, Han GW, Hanson MA, Shi L, Newman AH, Javitch JA, Cherezov V, et al. (2010). Structure of the human dopamine D3 receptor in complex with a D2/D3 selective antagonist. Science 330, 1091–1095. 10.1126/science.1197410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Cousins MS, Wei W, and Salamone JD (1994). Pharmacological characterization of performance on a concurrent lever pressing/feeding choice procedure: effects of dopamine antagonist, cholinomimetic, sedative and stimulant drugs. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 116, 529–537. 10.1007/BF02247489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Salamone J, Correa M, Mingote S, Weber S, and Farrar A. (2006). Nucleus Accumbens Dopamine and the Forebrain Circuitry Involved in Behavioral Activation and Effort-Related Decision Making: Implications for Understanding Anergia and Psychomotor Slowing in Depression. Curr. Psychiatry Rev 2, 267–280. 10.2174/157340006776875914. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Salamone JD., Steinpreis RE., McCullough LD., Smith P., Grebel D., and Mahan K. (1991). Haloperidol and nucleus accumbens dopamine depletion suppress lever pressing for food but increase free food consumption in a novel food choice procedure. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 104, 515–521. 10.1007/BF02245659. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Salamone JD (1994). The involvement of nucleus accumbens dopamine in appetitive and aversive motivation. Behav Brain Res 61, 117–133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Brusman LE, Protter DSW, Fultz AC, Paulson MU, Chapel GD, Elges IO, Cameron RT, Beery AK, and Donaldson ZR (2022). Emergent intra‐pair sex differences and organized behavior in pair bonded prairie voles ( Microtus ochrogaster ). Genes Brain Behav. 21. 10.1111/gbb.12786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Vahaba DM, Halstead ER, Donaldson ZR, Ahern TH, and Beery AK (2022). Sex differences in the reward value of familiar mates in prairie voles. Genes Brain Behav. 21, e12790. 10.1111/gbb.12790. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Beery AK, Lopez SA, Blandino KL, Lee NS, and Bourdon NS (2021). Social selectivity and social motivation in voles. eLife 10, e72684. 10.7554/eLife.72684. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kohl J, Babayan BM, Rubinstein ND, Autry AE, Marin-Rodriguez B, Kapoor V, Miyamishi K, Zweifel LS, Luo L, Uchida N, et al. (2018). Functional circuit architecture underlying parental behaviour. Nature 556, 326–331. 10.1038/s41586-018-0027-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Swart JM, Grattan DR, Ladyman SR, and Brown RSE (2021). Changes in maternal motivation across reproductive states in mice: A role for prolactin receptor activation on GABA neurons. Horm. Behav 135, 105041. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2021.105041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Sun F, Zhou J, Dai B, Qian T, Zeng J, Li X, Zhuo Y, Zhang Y, Wang Y, Qian C, et al. (2020). Next-generation GRAB sensors for monitoring dopaminergic activity in vivo. Nat. Methods 17, 1156–1166. 10.1038/s41592-020-00981-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Day JJ, Roitman MF, Wightman RM, and Carelli RM (2007). Associative learning mediates dynamic shifts in dopamine signaling in the nucleus accumbens. Nat. Neurosci 10, 1020–1028. 10.1038/nn1923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Amo R, Matias S, Yamanaka A, Tanaka KF, Uchida N, and Watabe-Uchida M. (2022). A gradual temporal shift of dopamine responses mirrors the progression of temporal difference error in machine learning. Nat. Neurosci 25, 1082–1092. 10.1038/s41593-022-01109-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Kutlu MG, Zachry JE, Melugin PR, Cajigas SA, Chevee MF, Kelley SJ, Kutlu B, Tian L, Siciliano CA, and Calipari ES (2021). Dopamine release in the nucleus accumbens core signals perceived saliency. Curr. Biol. CB 31, 4748–4761.e8. 10.1016/j.cub.2021.08.052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Harbert KJ, Pellegrini M, Gordon KM, and Donaldson ZR (2020). How prior pair-bonding experience affects future bonding behavior in monogamous prairie voles. Horm. Behav 126, 104847. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2020.104847. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Sun P, Smith AS, Lei K, Liu Y, and Wang Z. (2014). Breaking bonds in male prairie vole: Long-term effects on emotional and social behavior, physiology, and neurochemistry. Behav. Brain Res 265, 22–31. 10.1016/j.bbr.2014.02.016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.DeVries AC, and Carter CS (1999). Sex differences in temporal parameters of partner preference in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Can. J. Zool 77, 885–889. 10.1139/z99-054. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Yang J-H., Presby RE., Jarvie AA., Rotolo RA., Fitch RH., Correa M., and Salamone JD. (2020). Pharmacological studies of effort-related decision making using mouse touchscreen procedures: effects of dopamine antagonism do not resemble reinforcer devaluation by removal of food restriction. Psychopharmacology (Berl.) 237, 33–43. 10.1007/s00213-019-05343-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Carratalá-Ros C, López-Cruz L, SanMiguel N, Ibáñez-Marín P, Martínez-Verdú A, Salamone JD, and Correa M. (2020). Preference for Exercise vs. More Sedentary Reinforcers: Validation of an Animal Model of Tetrabenazine-Induced Anergia. Front. Behav. Neurosci 13. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Golden SA, Jin M, Heins C, Venniro M, Michaelides M, and Shaham Y. (2019). Nucleus Accumbens Drd1-Expressing Neurons Control Aggression Self-Administration and Aggression Seeking in Mice. J. Neurosci. Off. J. Soc. Neurosci 39, 2482–2496. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2409-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Maltese M, March JR, Bashaw AG, and Tritsch NX (2021). Dopamine differentially modulates the size of projection neuron ensembles in the intact and dopamine-depleted striatum. eLife 10, e68041. 10.7554/eLife.68041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Resendez SL, and Aragona BJ (2013). Aversive motivation and the maintenance of monogamous pair bonding. Rev. Neurosci 24, 51–60. 10.1515/revneuro-2012-0068. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Barr HJ, Wall EM, and Woolley SC (2021). Dopamine in the songbird auditory cortex shapes auditory preference. Curr. Biol 31, 4547–4559.e5. 10.1016/j.cub.2021.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Friard O, and Gamba M. (2016). BORIS: a free, versatile open‐source event‐logging software for video/audio coding and live observations. Methods Ecol. Evol 7, 1325–1330. 10.1111/2041-210X.12584. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Lopes G, Bonacchi N, Frazão J, Neto JP, Atallah BV, Soares S, Moreira L, Matias S, Itskov PM, Correia PA, et al. (2015). Bonsai: an event-based framework for processing and controlling data streams. Front. Neuroinformatics 9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.David Protter, Ryan Cameron, mupaulson, and ryantcameron (2023). dprotter/RPI_operant: final release. Version v1.2.0 (Zenodo) 10.5281/ZENODO.10234416 10.5281/ZENODO.10234416. [DOI]
- 46.David Protter, Ryan Cameron, and sarah-litz (2023). dprotter/RPI_Operant2: Working release for publication. Version v1.0.0 (Zenodo) 10.5281/ZENODO.10234395 10.5281/ZENODO.10234395. [DOI]
- 47.Murphy KZ, Haile ED, McTigue AD, Pierce AF, and Donaldson ZR (2023). PhAT: A Flexible Open-Source GUI-Driven Toolkit for Photometry Analysis. Curr. Protoc 3, e763. 10.1002/cpz1.763. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Ulloa M, Portillo W, Díaz NF, Young LJ, Camacho FJ, Rodríguez VM, and Paredes RG (2018). Mating and social exposure induces an opioid-dependent conditioned place preference in male but not in female prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster). Horm. Behav 97, 47–55. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2017.10.015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Scribner JL, Vance EA, Protter DSW, Sheeran WM, Saslow E, Cameron RT, Klein EM, Jimenez JC, Kheirbek MA, and Donaldson ZR (2020). A neuronal signature for monogamous reunion. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci 117, 11076–11084. 10.1073/pnas.1917287117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Ahern TH, Modi ME, Burkett JP, and Young LJ (2009). Evaluation of two automated metrics for analyzing partner preference tests. J Neurosci Methods 182, 180–188. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2009.06.010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data S1. Comprehensive statistical results including effect sizes for all analyses. Excel file containing one tab per figure. Related to Figures 1–5, S1 – S3.
Data Availability Statement
Data used in each figure panel have been deposited in GitHub: https://github.com/donaldsonlab/Pierce2023_Currentbiology and are publicly available as of the date of publication. Any additional information required to reanalyze the data reported in this paper is available from the lead contacts upon request.