Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2024 Apr 2;12:RP88253. doi: 10.7554/eLife.88253

Natural variation in the Caenorhabditis elegans egg-laying circuit modulates an intergenerational fitness trade-off

Laure Mignerot 1,, Clotilde Gimond 1,, Lucie Bolelli 1, Charlotte Bouleau 1, Asma Sandjak 1, Thomas Boulin 2, Christian Braendle 1,
Editors: Patrick J Hu3, Piali Sengupta4
PMCID: PMC10987095  PMID: 38564369

Abstract

Evolutionary transitions from egg laying (oviparity) to live birth (viviparity) are common across various taxa. Many species also exhibit genetic variation in egg-laying mode or display an intermediate mode with laid eggs containing embryos at various stages of development. Understanding the mechanistic basis and fitness consequences of such variation remains experimentally challenging. Here, we report highly variable intra-uterine egg retention across 316 Caenorhabditis elegans wild strains, some exhibiting strong retention, followed by internal hatching. We identify multiple evolutionary origins of such phenotypic extremes and pinpoint underlying candidate loci. Behavioral analysis and genetic manipulation indicates that this variation arises from genetic differences in the neuromodulatory architecture of the egg-laying circuitry. We provide experimental evidence that while strong egg retention can decrease maternal fitness due to in utero hatching, it may enhance offspring protection and confer a competitive advantage. Therefore, natural variation in C. elegans egg-laying behaviour can alter an apparent trade-off between different fitness components across generations. Our findings highlight underappreciated diversity in C. elegans egg-laying behavior and shed light on its fitness consequences. This behavioral variation offers a promising model to elucidate the molecular changes in a simple neural circuit underlying evolutionary shifts between alternative egg-laying modes in invertebrates.

Research organism: C. elegans

Introduction

Reproductive strategies reflect higher-order phenotypes that emerge through integration of developmental, morphological, physiological, and behavioural phenotypes. Understanding how these different phenotypes integrate and co-evolve to explain the diversification in reproductive strategies is a central goal of life history research (Flatt and Heyland, 2011). One such fundamental aspect of reproduction in internally fertilizing animals is the duration of intra-uterine embryonic development, which can be highly variable and is influenced by an interplay of genetic and environmental factors. While oviparity and viviparity reflect opposite extremes, many taxa display intermediate modes by laying eggs containing embryos at variably advanced stages of development (sometimes termed ovoviviparity). The degree of such maternal retention of fertilized eggs may vary significantly between closely related species or even between genotypes of the same species, for example in insects, snails, or reptiles, and is further influenced by diverse environmental factors, such as mating status or food availability (Meier et al., 1999; Markow et al., 2009; Bleu et al., 2012; Kalinka, 2015). Viviparity – and prolonged egg retention in general – are thought to promote maternal control over the offspring environment, providing an extended opportunity for offspring resource provisioning as well as improved offspring protection against environmental fluctuations, stressors, pathogens or predators (Blackburn, 1999; Avise, 2013; Kalinka, 2015; Ostrovsky et al., 2016). In addition, laying of advanced-stage embryos or larvae may generate an advantage during intra- and interspecific competition by reducing external egg-to-adult developmental time, for example, in insects (Bakker, 1961; Kalinka, 2015; Mueller and Bitner, 2015). Despite these apparent benefits, prolonged egg retention and viviparity are generally presumed to incur fitness costs, expressed as reduced maternal survival and fecundity, for example, due to higher metabolic costs as a result of prolonged gestation (Kalinka, 2015). How variation in ecological niche drives the evolution of different egg-laying modes given these apparent trade-offs has been a major focus of evolutionary ecological research (Blackburn, 1999; Avise, 2013; Kalinka, 2015; Ostrovsky et al., 2016).

The study of evolutionary transitions between ovi- and viviparity has concentrated on comparisons between species, so that evidence is mainly correlative and the genetic changes underlying such transitions can only rarely be determined (Horváth and Kalinka, 2018; Recknagel et al., 2021). Relatively few studies have examined quantitative intraspecific variation in egg retention although this approach may facilitate disentangling its relative costs and benefits. Intraspecific analysis may further allow for identification of genomic loci contributing to this variation and potentially provide insights into the molecular changes during incipient stages of evolutionary transitions towards obligate viviparity. One such study was performed for natural quantitative variation in egg retention of fertilized Drosophila females (Horváth and Kalinka, 2018): Genome-wide association (GWA) mapping yielded 15 potential candidate genes harbouring natural polymorphisms contributing to variation in egg retention. These variants have not yet been validated and it remains unclear which particular traits, such as fecundity or egg-laying behaviour, contribute to observed variation in egg retention (Horváth and Kalinka, 2018). In this study, prolonged Drosophila egg retention was correlated with a reduction in fecundity, in line with the prediction that evolutionary transitions towards viviparity impose fitness costs (Clutton-Brock, 1991; Blackburn, 1999; Avise, 2013; Kalinka, 2015; Ostrovsky et al., 2016). Analysis of intraspecific variation may thus be valuable to determine genetic causes and fitness consequences of divergent egg retention, providing an entry point into understanding evolutionary transitions between ovi- and viviparity.

In our study, to characterize intraspecific differences in egg retention, we focused on natural variation in egg laying of the male-hermaphrodite (androdioecious) Caenorhabditis elegans. In optimal conditions, this primarily self-fertilizing nematode completes its life cycle within 3–4 days, and hermaphrodites generate around 150–300 offspring over a period of 2–3 days. During the reproductive phase, animals of the reference strain (N2) accumulate up to~15 fertilized eggs in the uterus, caused by a delay between ovulation and egg laying (Schafer, 2006). Embryonic development takes around 15 hours and occurs independently of whether eggs are laid or retained in the uterus. On average, embryos spend two to three hours in utero, reaching the early gastrula (~30-cell) stage when they are being laid (Sulston et al., 1983). At any given time, the number of eggs retained in utero will therefore depend on rates of both egg production and egg laying. Reduced egg-laying rates lead to the accumulation of advanced stage embryos in utero. Egg laying is a rhythmic behaviour that alternates between inactive (~20min) and active (~2min) phases (Waggoner et al., 1998), regulated by a structurally simple neural circuit (White et al., 1986). During active egg-laying phases, the neurotransmitter serotonin and neuropeptides signal via the hermaphrodite-specific neurons (HSN) to increase excitability of vulva muscle cells, causing the rapid sequential release of four to six eggs (Waggoner et al., 1998; Shyn et al., 2003; Zhang et al., 2008; Collins et al., 2016; Brewer et al., 2019). Additional neuromodulatory signals and mechanosensory feedback regulate temporal patterns of egg-laying activity, which are further modulated by physiological and environmental inputs (Trent et al., 1983; Schafer, 2005; Ringstad and Horvitz, 2008; Koelle, 2018; Fernandez et al., 2020; Ravi et al., 2021; Aprison et al., 2022; Medrano and Collins, 2023). Food signals are required for sustained egg-laying activity (Horvitz et al., 1982; Trent, 1982; Waggoner et al., 1998; Daniels et al., 2000; Dong et al., 2000), whereas diverse stressors, such as starvation, hypoxia, thermal stress, osmotic stress, or pathogens, inhibit egg laying (Trent, 1982; Aballay et al., 2000; Waggoner et al., 2000; Chen and Caswell-Chen, 2003; Schafer, 2005; Zhang et al., 2008; McMullen et al., 2012; Fenk and de Bono, 2015). Prolonged stress exposure will lead to intra-uterine egg retention and internal (matricidal) hatching, so that larvae develop inside their mother, often leading to premature maternal death (Maupas, 1900; Trent, 1982; Chen and Caswell-Chen, 2003; Chen and Caswell-Chen, 2004). Environmentally induced egg retention and internal hatching in C. elegans can be considered a plastic switch from oviparity to viviparity, also termed facultative viviparity (Chen and Caswell-Chen, 2004). In C. elegans, while eggs can no longer be provisioned after fertilization, larvae in utero may feed on decaying maternal tissues allowing them to develop into advanced larval stages, even in nutrient-scarce environments (Chen and Caswell-Chen, 2004). Stress-induced internal hatching in C. elegans may thus reflect adaptive phenotypic plasticity allowing for offspring provisioning, in particular, by enabling larvae to develop into the diapausing, starvation-resistant dauer larval stage (Chen and Caswell-Chen, 2004), which also represents the key dispersal developmental stage, able to colonize novel favourable environments (Félix and Braendle, 2010).

The detailed genetic understanding of C. elegans egg-laying behaviour has been obtained through the study of a single wild-type genotype, the laboratory strain N2, and its mutant derivatives (Schafer, 2006). It therefore remains unclear to what extent the egg-laying circuit harbours intraspecific variation. Although wild strains are highly isogenic due to a predominant self-fertilization (selfing) (Barrière and Félix, 2005; Lee et al., 2021), C. elegans retains considerable genetic (and phenotypic) diversity across the globe, with strains differing genetically, often in localized, highly divergent genomic regions (Andersen et al., 2012; Crombie et al., 2019; Lee et al., 2021; Gilbert et al., 2022). While egg-laying responses to diverse stimuli seem to be largely invariant across many C. elegans populations (Chen and Caswell-Chen, 2004; Chen et al., 2020; Vigne et al., 2021), wild strains may exhibit strongly divergent egg-laying behaviour (Vigne et al., 2021). These rare strains display constitutively strong egg retention (up to ~50 eggs in utero) and internal hatching irrespective of the environment (partial viviparity). A single, major-effect variant explains this derived phenotype: a single amino acid substitution (V530L) in KCNL-1, a small-conductance calcium-activated potassium channel subunit. This gain-of-function mutation causes vulval muscle hyperpolarization to reduce egg-laying activity, leading to constitutively strong egg retention and internal hatching (Vigne et al., 2021). The apparent evolutionary maintenance of the KCNL-1 V530L variant in natural C. elegans populations seems puzzling given its highly deleterious fitness effects caused by matricidal hatching. Competition experiments indicate, however, that this variant allele can be maintained in more natural conditions mimicking fluctuations in resource availability and/or if reproduction preferentially occurs during early adulthood (Vigne et al., 2021). Nevertheless, it remains unknown how constitutively strong egg retention may be beneficial in such conditions, and more generally, how different degrees of C. elegans egg retention affect alternative fitness components.

Here, extending our previous work (Vigne et al., 2021), we quantified the full spectrum of natural variation in C. elegans egg retention using a world-wide panel of strains covering much of the species’ genetic diversity (Lee et al., 2021). Our first aim was to characterize variation in egg laying, and to identify phylogenetic patterns and genomic regions (QTL) associated with observed phenotypic variation. We show that, while most strains differed only subtly in egg retention, a subset of strains exhibit deviant, strongly increased or reduced, egg retention. We provide evidence for repeated evolution of such extreme egg retention phenotypes through distinct genetic changes. We then characterized a subset of wild strains with divergent egg retention to determine how they differ in egg-laying behaviour and underlying neuromodulatory architecture. These results show that the C. elegans egg-laying system harbours surprisingly high natural diversity in the sensitivity to neuromodulators, such as serotonin, suggesting rapid evolution of the involved neural circuitry. In a second objective, we explored why variation in egg retention might be maintained in natural C. elegans populations. To address this question, we tested for the presence of fitness costs and benefits associated with variable egg retention of wild C. elegans strains. We experimentally demonstrate that strong egg retention usually reduces maternal fertility and survival, mostly due to frequent internal (matricidal) hatching. On the other hand, these genotypes with strong egg retention may be able to benefit from improved offspring protection against environmental insults and from a competitive advantage caused by a significantly reduced extra-uterine egg-to-adult developmental time. Observed natural variation in C. elegans egg-laying behaviour may therefore modify a trade-off between fitness components expressed in mothers versus offspring. Altogether, we present an integrative analysis of natural variation in C. elegans egg laying providing first insights into the genetic basis, fitness consequences and possible evolutionary ecological significance of this central reproductive behaviour.

Results

Natural variation in C. elegans egg retention

Examining a world-wide panel of 316 genetically distinct C. elegans wild strains, we found highly variable average egg number in utero (a proxy for egg retention) in self-fertilizing adult hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +48 hr), ranging from approximately 5–50 eggs retained in utero (Figure 1A–C). To simplify further analysis of natural variation in egg retention, we defined three phenotypic classes: most strains (81%, N=256) differed relatively subtly in the number of eggs in utero (Class II canonical egg retention: 10–25 eggs), including the laboratory reference strain N2. A small number of strains consistently showed either strongly reduced (Class I weak egg retention:<10 eggs, 15%, N=46) or increased egg retention (Class III: strong egg retention:>25 eggs or larvae in utero, 4%, N=14). Several Class III strains also exhibited internal hatching (Figure 1—source data 1).

Figure 1. Natural variation in C. elegans egg retention.

(A) The number of eggs in utero in hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +48 hr) of 316 genetically distinct strains (isotypes) often strongly deviated from values observed in the laboratory strain N2 (~15 eggs in utero). We defined three classes of strains with distinct levels of egg retention: Class I weak:<10 eggs in utero (N=34), Class II canonical: 10–25 eggs in utero (N=230), Class III strong:>25 eggs in utero (N=14). N=18–150 individuals per strain were scored. (B) Geographic distribution of 316 C. elegans wild strains. Strains with different degrees of egg retention are labelled in different colours. For a detailed comparison of geographic distribution of the three phenotypic Classes, see Figure 1—figure supplement 1A. (C) Nomarski microscopy images of adult hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +48 hr) in wild strains with divergent egg retention. Eggs (coloured) contain embryos at different stages of development.

Figure 1—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 1.
elife-88253-fig1-data1.xlsx (315.9KB, xlsx)

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Natural variation of C. elegans egg number in utero.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Geographic distribution of Class I strains:<10 eggs in utero, low retention (N=34); Class II strains: 10–25 eggs in utero, canonical retention (N=230); Class III strains:>25 eggs in utero, high retention (N=14). (B) Frequency of strains with different egg retention phenotypes (Class I to III) in each substrate category. Strain information was obtained from the CaeNDR website: caendr.org. N=311 (for five strains there was no substrate information available).
Figure 1—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 1—figure supplement 1.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2. Egg number in utero in swept versus divergent C. elegans strains.

Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

Strains, scored for egg number in utero at L4 +48 hr (Figure 1A), were categorized as swept (N=275) or as divergent strains (N=41) following previous classifications (Gilbert et al., 2022). In brief, strains are categorized as swept if any of chromosomes I, IV, V, or X contained greater than or equal to 30% of the same haplotype; strains not among the swept strains are classified as divergent (Andersen et al., 2012; Lee et al., 2021; Gilbert et al., 2022). Thirteen out of the 14 Class III strains exhibited a swept haplotype.
Figure 1—figure supplement 2—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 1—figure supplement 2.

Strains with extreme egg retention phenotypes at either end of the spectrum were reminiscent of mutants uncovered by screens for egg-laying-defects (egl mutants) with major alterations in neural function or neuromodulation (Trent, 1982; Trent et al., 1983; Desai and Horvitz, 1989; Schafer, 2006). All examined wild strains showed, however, some egg-laying activity and none of the Class III strains exhibited any obvious, penetrant defects in vulval development or morphogenesis.

To examine if natural variation in egg retention may be linked to strain differences in ecological niche, we tested for effects of geographical and climatic parameters (e.g. latitude/longitude, elevation) (Spearman rank correlations, all p>0.05; data not shown) (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A) and substrate type (Figure 1—figure supplement 1B) on mean egg retention but found no obvious evidence for such relationships. Strains of all three phenotypic Classes were found across the globe and may co-occur in the same locality, or even in the same microhabitat or substrate (Figure 1—source data 1), as previously reported (Vigne et al., 2021). A majority (11 out of 14) of Class III strains were isolated in Europe (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A), but this may be simply due to increased sampling efforts of this region.

Both common and rare genetic variants underlie natural differences in egg retention

The presence of significant variation in egg retention allows for mapping of genomic regions that contribute to this variation using GWA mapping. This GWA approach is limited to the detection of common variants as variants below 5% minor allele frequency are excluded from analysis (Cook et al., 2017; Widmayer et al., 2022). Performing GWA mapping using data for 316 unique strains (isotypes), mapping of mean egg retention yielded a single QTL located on chromosome III spanning 1.48 Mb (Figure 2A). This QTL explained 24% of the observed phenotypic variance (Table 1). Among the 257 genes found in this QTL region, five have known roles in the egg-laying system (https://www.wormbase.org) and display various natural polymorphisms (Table 2). However, we did not detect any obvious candidate polymorphisms in these genes that could explain variable levels of egg retention. Two additional QTL on chromosomes II and IV were detected for variance in egg retention (CV) (Figure 2B). The three QTLs do not align with any of the recently identified hyper-divergent regions of the genome (Lee et al., 2021). The phenotypic distributions and detection of several QTL by GWA indicates that natural variation in C. elegans egg retention is likely polygenic, influenced by multiple common variants.

Figure 2. Common and rare genetic variants underlie natural differences in egg retention.

Figure 2.

(A–B) Manhattan plots of single-marker based GWA mappings for C. elegans egg retention phenotypes (N=316). Each dot represents a SNV that is present in at least 5% of the assayed population. The genomic location of each single-nucleotide variant (SNV) is plotted on the X-axis against its log10(p) value on the y-axis. SNVs that pass the genome-wide EIGEN threshold (dotted line) or the Bonferroni threshold (solid line) are marked in red. (A) Manhattan plot of single-marker based GWA mapping region for mean number of eggs in utero. (B) Manhattan plot of single-marker-based GWA mapping region for the coefficient of variation (CV) (mean number of eggs in utero). (C) Neighbour-joining tree based on the 2 Mb region surrounding the kcnl-1 genomic region using a subset of 48 C. elegans wild strains. The four strains with the KCNL-1 V530L variant are shown in red.

Figure 2—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 2.

Table 1. QTL detected by GWA mapping for mean and coefficient of variation (CV) of egg number in utero (N=316 strains).

Trait Chromosome Interval (bp) Peak Log10(p) Variance explained (%)
Mean III 8,532,784–10,019,713 9,312,552 6.01 24.14
CV II 13,477,760–14,077,923 13,790,719 4.45 6.74
CV IV 15,491,767–16,170,655 15,885,539 4.81 6.74

Table 2. Potential candidate genes (with known roles in C. elegans egg laying) and variants in the QTL interval on chromosome III (GWA mapping for egg number in utero).

Potential high-impact variants are predicted to disrupt gene function, for example, through nonsense or frameshift mutations; low impact variants are predicted to have little or no impact on gene function, such as synonymous mutations (Cook et al., 2017).

Gene Chromosome Interval (bp) Number of variants(predicted high impact) Number of variants(predicted low impact)
pat-2 III 8,818,898–8,825,266 1 2
lin-12 III 9,060,220–9,071,472 0 2
ina-1 III 9,168,072–9,172,802 1 2
lin-52 III 9,824,082–9,824,750 0 1
cbp-2 III 9,923,173–9,924,800 20 11

Examining the phylogenetic distribution of extreme egg retention phenotypes based on whole-genome sequence data (Cook et al., 2017; Lee et al., 2021) indicates that Class I and III phenotypes have been derived multiple times. On average, egg retention did not differ between genetically divergent, likely ancestral, strains (mostly from Hawaii) (N=41) and strains with swept haplotypes (N=275) found across the globe (Crombie et al., 2019; Lee et al., 2021; Gilbert et al., 2022); however, most Class III strains (13 out of 14) exhibited swept haplotypes (Figure 1—figure supplement 2). Among the Class III strains with very strong egg retention, four strains (including the newly isolated strain NIC1832) are known to carry a single amino acid substitution (V530L) in KCNL-1 (Vigne et al., 2021). This variant has been shown to strongly reduce egg-laying activity (Vigne et al., 2021), thus explaining strong egg retention in these four French strains. As previously suggested (Vigne et al., 2021), we confirmed that the KCNL-1 V530L is likely derived from a single mutational event, as inferred by local haplotype analysis of the genomic region surrounding kcnl-1 (Figure 2C). In additional Class III strains, which do not carry this variant (N=10), the derived state of constitutively high egg retention must thus be explained by alternative genetic variants. Therefore, natural variation in C. elegans egg retention is shaped by both common variants and multiple rare variants, including KCNL-1 V530L, explaining extreme phenotypic divergence.

Temporal progression of egg retention and internal hatching

To better characterize natural variation in C. elegans egg retention, we focused on a subset of 15 strains from divergent phenotypic Classes I-III, with an emphasis on Class III strains exhibiting strong egg retention (at mid-L4 +30 h; Figure 3A and B). Class III strains were further distinguished depending on the absence (Class IIIA) or presence (Class IIIB) of the KCNL-1 V530L variant explaining strong egg retention (Vigne et al., 2021). In addition, we included the strain QX1430 (Class II), a derivative of the N2 reference strain, in which the major-effect, N2-specific npr-1 allele, impacting ovulation and egg laying (Andersen et al., 2014; Zhao et al., 2018), has been replaced by its natural version (Andersen et al., 2015); this allowed us to directly assess the effect of the N2 npr-1 allele on examined phenotypes. Note that this strain selection, especially concerning the largest Class II, is unlikely to reflect the overall strain diversity observed across the species.

Figure 3. Temporal progression of egg retention and internal hatching.

(A) Egg retention in a subset of 15 strains with divergent egg retention, divided into the three phenotypic classes. Class I weak:<10 eggs in utero (N=34), Class II canonical: 10–25 eggs in utero (N=230), Class III strong:>25 eggs in utero (N=14). Class III strains were further distinguished depending on the absence (Class IIIA) or presence (Class IIIB) of the KCNL-1 V530L variant explaining strong egg retention. Estimates of Class effects labelled with the same letter are not significantly different from each other (Tukey’s honestly significant difference, p>0.05) based on results of a Two-Way ANOVA, fixed effect Class: F3,577=710.38, p<0.0001, fixed effect Strain(nested in Class): F11,577=33.58, p<0.0001. (B) Geographic distribution of the 15 focal strains with divergent egg retention. (C) Temporal dynamics of offspring number in utero in the 15 focal strains. Number of eggs and larvae in utero at three stages covering the reproductive span of self-fertilizing hermaphrodites. N=28–96 individuals per strain per time point (except for JU2593: at mid-L4 +72 hr: only four individuals were scored as most animals were dead by this time point). (D) Age distribution of embryos retained in utero of hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +30 hr) in the 15 focal strains. Embryonic stages were divided into five age groups according to the following characteristics using Nomarski microscopy (Hall and Altun, 2007): 1–2 cell stage, 4–26 cell stage, 44 cell to gastrula stage, bean to two-fold stage, three-fold stage, L1 larva. (Data from same cohort of animals used for experiment shown in A). (E) Frequency of internal hatching across three time points of the reproductive span of self-fertilizing hermaphrodites (extracted from data shown in D). Red bars indicate the proportion of individuals carrying at least one L1 larva in the uterus; blue bars indicate the proportion of individuals carrying only embryos in the uterus. Dead mothers were excluded from analyses.

Figure 3—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 3, Figure 3—figure supplement 1A.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Temporal progression of egg retention and internal hatching.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A) Age distribution of embryos within eggs retained in utero of hermaphrodites (L4 +30 hr) in the 15 focal strains, including the N2-derived strain QX1430 (Andersen et al., 2015). Embryonic stages were divided into five age groups according to the following characteristics using Nomarski microscopy (Hall and Altun, 2007): 1–2 cell stage, 4–26 cell stage, 44 cell to gastrula stage, bean to two-fold stage, three-fold stage, L1 larva. N=36–40 individuals were scored per strain; data obtained from same individuals scored for number of eggs in utero (Figure 3A). (B) Egg size (volume) in the 15 focal strains with variable egg retention, measured on laid eggs of mixed-age adult populations. Strains differed significantly in egg size (Kruskal-Wallis Test, χ2=223.97, df = 14, p<0.0001). N=50–69 eggs per strain. (C) Body size (volume) measured in the 15 focal strains; selfing hermaphrodites at first fertilization (1–2 eggs in utero). Strains differed significantly in body size (Kruskal-Wallis Test, χ2=227.45, df = 14, p<0.0001). N=47–51 individuals per strain. (D) Marginally significant positive correlation between mean egg size and mean body size (volume; at first fertilization, 1–2 eggs in utero) across the 15 focal strains with divergent egg retention (ρSpearman=0.50, p=0.06). (E) No correlation between mean egg size and mean egg retention (at L4 +30 hr) across the 15 focal strains with divergent egg retention (ρSpearman=0.01, p=0.98). (F) No correlation between mean body size (volume; at first fertilization, 1–2 eggs in utero) and mean egg retention (at L4 +30 hr) across the 15 focal strains with divergent egg retention (ρSpearman=0.1, p=0.92).
Figure 3—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 3—figure supplement 1B–F.

The temporal dynamics of egg number in utero varied strongly across the hermaphrodite reproductive span (Figure 3C), in line with progeny production and presumptive ovulation rates, coupled to the number of remaining self-sperm (Ward and Carrel, 1979; McCarter et al., 1997; Kosinski et al., 2005; McMullen et al., 2012; Large et al., 2017; Zhao et al., 2018). Towards the end of the reproductive period (between mid-L4 +48 hr to mid-L4 +72 hr), surviving animals in all phenotypic Classes showed strongly reduced offspring numbers in utero (Figure 3C). Scoring the age distribution of progeny in utero of young (mid-L4 +30 hr) adult hermaphrodites confirmed that increased egg number in utero was linked to prolonged intra-uterine embryonic development (Figure 3D). At this developmental stage, most Class III strains contained more advanced embryonic stages (or L1 larvae) compared to Class I and II strains (Figure 3D, Figure 3—figure supplement 1A). Examining the temporal dynamics of internal hatching across the entire reproductive span, we found that internal hatching occurred consistently in the six strains with highest egg retention: all Class IIIB strains and in two Class IIIA strains (JU830, JU2829; Figure 3E).

Using the 15 focal strains, we further tested if strain differences in egg retention correlate with differences in body or egg size, that is, morphological characteristics likely modulating the capacity to retain eggs. Both egg and body size (of early adults at beginning of fertilization, containing 1–2 eggs in utero) showed significant differences across strains (Figure 3—figure supplement 1B and C); in addition, there was a trend for a correlation between body and egg size across strains (Figure 3—figure supplement 1D). However, egg and body size did not correlate with measures of mean egg retention across strains (Figure 3—figure supplement 1E and F), suggesting that the capacity to retain eggs is not simply a function of body or egg size.

Natural variation in egg-laying behaviour

Given that egg number in utero is modulated by temporal changes in rates of both ovulation and egg laying (Figure 3C), we wanted to determine to what extent natural variation in C. elegans egg retention can be attributed to differences in egg-laying behaviour. We first examined the temporal dynamics of egg-laying activity across the hermaphrodite reproductive span by measuring the number of eggs laid during a two-hour window at five distinct adult ages (Figure 4). In line with the temporal progression of egg retention (Figure 3C), egg-laying activity was highly dynamic, peaking between mid-L4 +24 hr and mid-L4 +30 hr for most strains, followed by a decline until cessation of reproduction (Figure 4). Most Class III strains exhibited a shortened time window of egg-laying activity, with few eggs being laid from mid-L4 +48 hr onwards (Figure 4).

Figure 4. Natural variation in egg-laying activity.

Figure 4.

Temporal dynamics of egg-laying activity in the 15 focal strains. Number of eggs laid (within a two-hour window) at five time points across the reproductive span of self-fertilizing hermaphrodites. N=20 individuals per strain per time point except for JU2593 (at mid-L4 +72 hr: only four individuals could be scored as most animals were dead by this time point). Note that several Class III strains laid eggs containing advanced-stage embryos, evidenced by L1 hatching within the two-hour window of the experiment (Figure 4—source data 1).

Figure 4—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 4.

We next quantified natural variation in behavioural patterns of egg laying during peak activity (mid-L4 +30 hr). Based on continuous video imaging of the reference strain N2, C. elegans egg laying has been described as a rhythmic two-state behaviour with inactive (~20 min) and active (~2 min) phases, during which multiple eggs are expelled (Waggoner et al., 1998). Here, we used a simplified, non-continuous (scan-sampling) method to detect variation in egg-laying behavioural patterns across the 15 focal strains: we scored the presence and number of eggs laid by isolated adults every 5 min over a 3-hr period (Figure 5A). This assay enables to estimate egg-laying frequency and to derive an approximate duration of prolonged inactive egg-laying periods, but it does not provide the means to determine the precise timing and structure of active egg-laying periods. Our assay results indicate the presence of significant natural variation in C. elegans egg-laying behaviour. The number of intervals with egg laying varied significantly between strains and Classes (Figure 5B and C). In line with their egg retention phenotype, Class I strains exhibited an increased number of intervals with egg laying, whereas Class IIIB strains showed a reduced number of such intervals (Figure 5C). In contrast, the number of intervals with egg laying did not significantly differ between Class II and IIIA strains (Figure 5C). Moreover, strains with strong egg retention tended to exhibit prolonged periods during which egg laying was inactive (Figure 5D). In addition, the reference strain N2 (Class II) expelled significantly more eggs per interval (with egg laying) compared to all other strains, including QX1430 – therefore, this atypical phenotype likely caused by the N2-specific npr-1 allele (Figure 5E). Taken together, these observations show that strain and Class differences in C. elegans egg-laying behaviour partly align with observed differences in egg retention.

Figure 5. Natural variation in egg-laying behaviour.

(A) Cartoon depicting the design of our scan-sampling experiment to quantify temporal patterns of C. elegans egg-laying behaviour in the 15 focal strains (B–E). We scored the presence and number of eggs laid by isolated adults (at mid-L4 +30 hr every 5 min across a 3-hr period, resulting in a total of 36 observations [intervals] per individual [N=17–18 individuals per strain]). Intervals with and without egg laying are marked in green and red, respectively. (B) Raster plots illustrating strain variation in temporal patterns of egg-laying behaviour across 3 hr of observation. Each horizontal line represents single individual and vertical bars indicate 5-min intervals during which one or more eggs were laid. For a detailed figure of the same data, see Figure 5—figure supplement 1A. (C) The number of 5-min intervals with egg laying differed significantly between strains and Classes. Two-Way ANOVA, fixed effect Class: F3,248=19.94, p<0.0001, fixed effect Strain(nested in Class): F11,248=5.20, p<0.0001. Estimates of Class effects labelled with the same letter are not significantly different from each other (Tukey’s honestly significant difference, p<0.05). Each dot represents the number of intervals with egg laying (out of a total of 37 intervals) per individual (N=17–18 individuals per strain). (D) The estimated mean duration of inactive periods (min) differed significantly between strains and Classes. Two-Way ANOVA, fixed effect Class: F3,248=8.46, p<0.0001, fixed effect Strain(nested in Class): F11,248=2.93, p=0.0012. Estimates of Class effects labelled with the same letter are not significantly different from each other (Tukey’s honestly significant difference, p>0.05). For each individual, this value was estimated as the mean time (corresponding to the number of intervals without egg laying) separating successive intervals with egg laying. Each dot represents the mean duration of inactive periods (min) per individual (N=17–18 individuals per strain). (E) The mean number of eggs laid per interval with egg laying differed between strains and Classes. Two-Way ANOVA, fixed effect Class: F3,248=12.41, p<0.0001, fixed effect Strain(nested in Class): F11,248=6.30, p<0.0001. Estimates of Class effects labelled with the same letter are not significantly different from each other (Tukey’s honestly significant difference, p>0.05). These significant effects are exclusively explained by the higher value of the N2 strain (Class II) relative to all other strains (p<0.05); none of the strains other than N2 did differ significantly from each other. Each dot represents the mean number of eggs laid per interval with egg laying per individual (N=17–18 individuals per strain). For data on total number eggs laid during the three-hour experiment, see Figure 5—figure supplement 1B.

Figure 5—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 5.
elife-88253-fig5-data1.xlsx (265.7KB, xlsx)

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Natural variation in egg-laying behaviour.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A) Detailed representation of data shown in Figure 5B: Representative raster plots of temporal patterns of egg-laying behaviour during a 3-hr interval, with each horizontal line representing a single individual; vertical bars indicate 5-min intervals during which one or more eggs were laid. The exact number of eggs laid in each interval is indicated above bars. (B) The total number of eggs laid during the three hours of observation differed significantly between strains and Classes. Two-Way ANOVA, fixed effect Class: F3,248=23.07, p<0.0001, fixed effect Strain(nested in Class): F11,248=3.73, p<0.0001. Estimates of Class effects labelled with the same letter are not significantly different from each other (Tukey’s honestly significant difference, p>0.05). Data from same experiment as shown in Figure 5B-E.

We next wanted to examine how strain differences in egg retention relate to possible differences in the initial onset of egg laying as a function of egg accumulation (Ravi et al., 2018). We therefore tested how egg accumulation in utero correlates with the beginning of egg-laying behaviour by measuring measured the timing and onset of the first egg-laying event in the 15 focal strains. Tracking individual hermaphrodites from mid-L4 onwards, we determined the time interval between the time points of first fertilization (presence of one or two eggs in utero) and first egg-laying event (when we also measured egg number in utero). Hermaphrodites of Class II strains laid their first egg after accumulating ~5–10 eggs in utero, that is approximately 1–2 hr after first fertilization, similar to what has been reported previously for the reference strain N2 (Ravi et al., 2018; Figure 6A and B). By comparison, the time points of the first egg-laying event for Class I and III strains were significantly advanced and delayed, respectively, with corresponding differences in the number of eggs accumulated in utero (Figure 6A and B).

Figure 6. Natural variation in egg retention at the first egg-laying event.

Figure 6.

(A) The time (min) between first fertilization and first egg-laying event differed between strains and Classes. Two-Way ANOVA, fixed effect Class: F3,261=102.38, p<0.0001, fixed effect Strain(nested in Class): F11,261=9.40, p<0.0001. Estimates of Class effects labelled with the same letter are not significantly different from each other (Tukey’s honestly significant difference, p>0.05). N=16–22 individuals per strain. (B) The number of eggs in utero at first egg-laying event differed between strains and Classes. Two-Way ANOVA, fixed effect Class: F3,261=198.73, p<0.0001, fixed effect Strain(nested in Class): F11,261=13.00, p<0.0001. Estimates of Class effects labelled with the same letter are not significantly different from each other (Tukey’s honestly significant difference, p>0.05). N=16–22 individuals per strain; measured in the same individuals as shown in (A).

Figure 6—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 6.

The above experiments demonstrate that a substantial portion of the natural variation in C. elegans egg retention can be attributed to differences in egg-laying behaviour. First, strain differences in egg retention are established prior to the onset of egg-laying behaviour through a differential delay of the first egg-laying event (Figure 6A and B). Hence, the sensitivity to stimuli that trigger the onset of egg-laying behaviour – probably mechanical stretch signals modulated by the accumulation of eggs in utero (Collins et al., 2016; Ravi et al., 2018; Ravi et al., 2021; Medrano and Collins, 2023) – seems to vary across different strains. Differential mechanosensory perception of egg accumulation could therefore represent a principal mechanism driving natural variation in egg retention. Second, we observed more subtle strain differences in certain behavioural phenotypes at the peak of egg-laying activity, such as the duration of inactive egg-laying periods, which in part correlated with differences in egg retention (Figure 5C and D). Consequently, natural variation in egg retention arises from variation in multiple phenotypes that manifest at distinct time points during adulthood, collectively defining C. elegans egg laying.

Natural variation in C. elegans egg-laying in response to neuromodulatory inputs

Observed strain differences in C. elegans egg-laying behaviour (Figure 5, Figure 6) potentially arise through genetic variation in the neuromodulatory architecture of the egg-laying circuit (Figure 7A). Multiple neurotransmitters, in particular, serotonin (5-HT), play a central role in synaptic and extrasynaptic neuromodulation of egg laying, with HSN-mediated serotonin bursts triggering egg laying (Trent et al., 1983; Waggoner et al., 1998; Shyn et al., 2003; Collins et al., 2016; Figure 7A). Exogenous application of serotonin, and other neurotransmitters and neuromodulatory drugs, have been extensively used to study their roles in regulating C. elegans egg-laying activity (Horvitz et al., 1982; Trent et al., 1983; Schafer, 2006). We therefore tested if the 15 focal strains differ in their response to exogenous serotonin known to stimulate C. elegans egg laying (Horvitz et al., 1982; Schafer, 2006). We used standard assays, in which animals are reared on NGM agar plates with bacterial food, and then at the start of the egg-laying assay, are transferred to liquid M9 buffer without bacterial food. This liquid treatment inhibits egg laying, but adding serotonin overrides inhibition and stimulates egg laying, as reported for the N2 reference strain (Horvitz et al., 1982; Trent et al., 1983; Weinshenker et al., 1995). We found exposure to a high dose of serotonin (25 mM) to generate a consistent increase in egg laying in both Class I and II strains (Figure 7B). In contrast, responses were highly variable in Class III strains: among the five Class IIIA strains without the kcnl-1 variant, serotonin stimulated egg laying in all strains except for JU2829, in which serotonin strongly inhibited egg laying as compared to basal egg laying in controls (Figure 7B). In addition, serotonin induced a much stronger egg-laying response in the strain ED3005 than in other Class IIIA strains with similar levels of egg retention (Figure 7B). ED3005 may thus exhibit serotonin hypersensitivity, which has been observed in certain egg-laying mutants where perturbed synaptic transmission impacts serotonin signalling (Schafer and Kenyon, 1995; Schafer et al., 1996). In Class IIIB strains carrying the KCNL-1 V530L variant allele, serotonin had no effect on, or a trend towards inhibiting, an already weak egg laying activity (Figure 7B). Strains within Classes I, II, and IIIA further showed subtle but significative quantitative differences in the egg-laying response triggered by serotonin despite showing grossly similar egg-laying behaviour in control conditions (Figure 7B). Stimulation of egg-laying activity by exogenous serotonin was thus strongly genotype-dependent. We conclude that genetic differences in neuromodulatory architecture of the C. elegans egg-laying circuit contribute to natural diversity in egg-laying behaviour.

Figure 7. Natural variation in C. elegans egg-laying in response to neuromodulatory agents.

(A) Cartoon of the neural circuit controlling C. elegans egg laying (Collins et al., 2016; Kopchock et al., 2021). The structure of the C. elegans egg-laying circuit is simple, containing two classes of motoneurons, the two serotonergic hermaphrodite-specific motoneurons (HSN) and the six ventral cord motoneurons (VC), which provide synaptic input to the egg-laying muscles (VM). Serotonin from HSN act through vulval muscle receptors to increase muscle excitability, which together with rhythmic signals from motor neurons, causes contractions of VM during egg laying (Collins et al., 2016; Kopchock et al., 2021). Mechanical feedback in response to egg accumulation favours exit from the inactive state (Collins et al., 2016; Medrano and Collins, 2023). Muscles are indicated by rectangles, neurons by circles, and neurosecretory cells (uv1) by triangles. Principal neurotransmitters released by neurons are indicated next to neurons (ACh: Acetylcholine). (B) Natural variation in egg-laying activity in response to exogenous serotonin, fluoxetine, and imipramine. Adult hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +30 hr) were placed in M9 buffer without food (control) or in M9 containing the indicated concentrations of serotonin, fluoxetine, and imipramine. The number of eggs laid were scored after two hours. Assays for each of the three treatments were carried out independently; the 15 strains were scored in parallel in both control and treatment conditions for each of the three assays. Serotonin: for each strain, 11–24 replicates (each containing 3.73±0.36 individuals on average) were scored for serotonin and control (M9 buffer) conditions. Align Rank Transform ANOVA, fixed effect Treatment: F1,390=432.62, p<0.0001, fixed effect Strain: F14,390=42.94, p<0.0001; interaction Treatment x Strain: F14,390=34.40, p<0.0001. Serotonin stimulated egg-laying in Class, II and III A strains but had no effect on egg laying in Class III B and JU2829 (Class III A) (Tukey’s honestly significant difference, ***p<0.0001; ns: not significant). Imipramine: for each strain, 6–18 replicates/wells (each containing 4.62±0.50 individuals on average) were scored for serotonin and control (M9 buffer) conditions. Align Rank Transform ANOVA, fixed effect Treatment: F1,222=562, p<0.0001, fixed effect Strain: F14,222=23.86, p<0.0001; interaction Treatment x Strain: F14,222=8.52, p<0.0001. Imipramine stimulated egg laying in strains from the 4 Class (Tukey’s honestly significant difference, ***p<0.0001; ns: not significant). Fluoxetine: for each strain, 12–24 replicates/wells (each containing 3.31±0.37 individuals on average) were scored for serotonin and control (M9) conditions. Align Rank Transform ANOVA, fixed effect Treatment: F1,378=1005, p<0.0001, fixed effect Strain: F14,378=30.09, p<0.0001; interaction Treatment x Strain: F14,378=16.35, p<0.0001. Imipramine stimulated egg-laying in strains from the 4 Class (Tukey’s honestly significant difference, ***p<0.0001; ns: not significant). For detailed statistical results, see Figure 7—source data 2. (C) Effects of exogenous serotonin (25 mM) on egg laying activity in strains with strongly divergent egg retention due to variation in a single amino acid residue of KCNL-1. Strains JU1200WT (canonical egg retention), JU751KCNL-1 L530V (CRISPR-Cas9-engineered, weak egg retention), JU1200KCNL-1 V530L (CRISPR-Cas9-engineered, strong egg retention) and JU751WT (strong egg retention). Adult hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +30 hr) were placed into M9 buffer without food (control) or M9 with serotonin (25 mM). Serotonin stimulated egg-laying in JU751KCNL-1 L530V and JU1200WT but inhibited egg laying in JU751WT and JU1200KCNL-1 V530L (Kruskal-Wallis Tests were performed separately for each strain to test for the effect of serotonin on the number of eggs laid; *p<0.05). For each strain, six replicates (each containing 5.50±0.92 individuals on average) were scored for serotonin and control conditions. For additional data, see Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

Figure 7—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 7B and C.
Figure 7—source data 2. Excel file containing statistical results for analyses of data shown in Figure 7B and C, Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

Figure 7.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1. Effects of exogenous serotonin, Imipramine and Fluoxetine on egg laying activity in strains with strongly divergent egg retention due to variation in a single amino acid residue of KCNL-1.

Figure 7—figure supplement 1.

Effects of exogenous serotonin, fluoxetine and imipramine on egg laying activity in strains with strongly divergent egg retention due to variation in a single amino acid residue of KCNL-1. Strains JU1200WT (canonical egg retention), JU1200KCNL-1 V530L (CRISPR-engineered, strong egg retention), JU751WT (strong egg retention) and JU751KCNL-1 L530V (CRISPR-engineered, low egg retention). Adult hermaphrodites (L4 +30 hr) were placed into M9 buffer without food (control) or M9 with serotonin (25 mM). In contrast to serotonin, imipramine and fluoxetine stimulated egg-laying activity in all four strains. For each strain, six replicates (each containing 5.50±0.92 individuals on average) were scored for each treatment and control condition. (For clarity, Figure 7C only shows the serotonin versus control conditions of this experiment).

The action of diverse drugs in clinical use, such as serotonin-reuptake inhibitors (SSRI) and tricyclic antidepressants, have been characterized through genetic analysis of their action on neurotransmitters regulating C. elegans egg-laying behaviour (Trent et al., 1983; Weinshenker et al., 1995; Weinshenker et al., 1999; Ranganathan et al., 2001; Dempsey et al., 2005; Kullyev et al., 2010; Branicky et al., 2014). As reported for the reference strain N2 (Dempsey et al., 2005), we find that the SSRI fluoxetine (Prozac) and the tricyclic antidepressant imipramine strongly stimulated egg-laying activity in all 15 focal strains, which only showed slight (although significant) quantitative differences in drug sensitivity (Figure 7B). Both drugs also stimulated egg laying in the Class IIIB strains and the Class IIIA strain JU2829 for which exogenous serotonin either inhibited egg laying or had no effect on it (Figure 7B). In the past, mutants unresponsive to serotonin yet responsive to other drugs, including fluoxetine and imipramine, have been interpreted as being defective in the serotonin response of vulval muscles (Trent et al., 1983; Reiner et al., 1995; Weinshenker et al., 1995). This is indeed the likely case of Class IIIB strains carrying the KCNL-1 V530L variant thought to specifically reduce excitability of vulval muscles (Vigne et al., 2021). Our results therefore suggest that JU2829 (Class IIIA) may exhibit a similar defect in vulval muscle activation via serotonin caused by an alternative genetic change. Overall, these pharmacological assays do not allow us to conclude if and how HSN function has diverged among strains because the mode of action and targets of tested drugs has not been resolved. Nevertheless, our results are consistent with previous models proposing that these drugs do not simply block serotonin reuptake but can stimulate egg laying, to some extent, through mechanisms independent of serotonergic signalling (Trent et al., 1983; Desai and Horvitz, 1989; Reiner et al., 1995; Weinshenker et al., 1995; Weinshenker et al., 1999; Dempsey et al., 2005; Kullyev et al., 2010; Branicky et al., 2014; Yue et al., 2018).

As shown above, serotonin may also inhibit, rather than stimulate, egg laying, specifically in the Class IIIA strain JU2829, and a similar tendency was observed for Class IIIB strains (Figure 7B). This result is in line with past research reporting a dual role of serotonin signalling in C. elegans egg laying by generating both excitatory and inhibitory inputs (Carnell et al., 2005; Dempsey et al., 2005; Hobson et al., 2006; Hapiak et al., 2009). In the reference wild type strain (N2), serotonin-mediated inhibitory inputs are masked by its stronger excitatory inputs and are only revealed when function of certain serotonin receptors has been mutationally disrupted (Carnell et al., 2005; Hobson et al., 2006; Hapiak et al., 2009). In analogous fashion, our observations suggest that serotonin-mediated inhibitory effects become only visible in wild strains whose egg-laying response cannot be stimulated by serotonin. In the case of Class IIIB strains, the KCNL-1 V530L variant likely hyperpolarizes vulval muscles to such an extent that exogenous serotonin is insufficient to induce successful egg laying while the negative serotonin-mediated inputs are able to exert their effects, likely through HSN, as previously shown for N2 (Carnell et al., 2005; Hobson et al., 2006; Hapiak et al., 2009). To test if KCNL1- V530L is indeed sufficient to unmask inhibitory inputs of serotonin, we examined the effects of this variant introduced into a Class II genetic background (strain JU1200); the resulting CRISPR-Cas9-engineered strain (JU1200KCNL-1 V530L) recapitulates reduced egg laying and strong egg retention (Vigne et al., 2021). Very similar to what we found for JU751 (Class IIIB), serotonin had a negative effect on egg laying in JU1200KCNL-1 V530L, suggesting that altering vulval muscle excitability by means of KCNL-1 V530L was indeed sufficient to abrogate positive but not negative serotonin inputs acting in the C. elegans egg-laying system (Figure 7C). In addition, both fluoxetine and imipramine stimulated egg laying in JU1200KCNL-1 V530L (Figure 7—figure supplement 1), very similar to what we observed in JU751 and other Class IIIB strains (Figure 7B). In addition, introducing the canonical KCNL-1 sequence into JU751 (strain JU751KCNL-1 L530V) fully restores egg-laying responses as for the ones observed in JU1200 (Figure 7C, Figure 7—figure supplement 1). These results thus further confirm that variation in a single amino acid residue of KCNL-1 is sufficient to explain drastic differences in the C. elegans egg-laying system and its response to various neuromodulatory inputs.

CRISPR-Cas9-mediated manipulation of endogenous serotonin levels uncovers natural variation in the neuromodulatory architecture of the C. elegans egg-laying system

To consolidate the finding that C. elegans harbours natural variation in the neuromodulatory architecture of the egg-laying system, we examined how an identical genetic modification – which increases availability of endogenous serotonin levels – affects egg-laying activity in different genetic backgrounds. The C. elegans genome encodes a single serotonin transporter (SERT), mod-5, which is involved in serotonin re-uptake (Ranganathan et al., 2001). We introduced a point mutation corresponding to the mod-5(n822) loss-of-function allele, increasing endogenous serotonin signalling (Ranganathan et al., 2001; Dempsey et al., 2005; Kullyev et al., 2010; Jafari et al., 2011), into 10 wild strains of the three classes with divergent egg retention using CRISPR-Cas9 genome editing (Figure 8A). Consistent with past reports, we found that mod-5(n822) tended to increase basal egg-laying activity in liquid M9 buffer without food. However, the size of this effect was genotype-dependent, and several strains did not show a significantly increased egg-laying response (Class I: JU3166, QG2873, Class IIIA: JU830, Class IIIB: JU751) (Figure 8B). The effect of mod-5 was also quantitatively different for certain strains within the same phenotypic Class (Class II and Class IIIA), indicating that serotonin sensitivity of the egg laying system varies genetically despite generating similar behavioural outputs (Figure 8B).

Figure 8. Manipulating endogenous serotonin levels uncovers natural variation in the neuromodulatory architecture of the C. elegans egg-laying system.

(A) Cartoon outlining the experimental design to introduce (by CRISPR-Cas9 technology) the loss-of-function mutation mod-5(n822) (Ranganathan et al., 2001) into 10 C. elegans strains with divergent egg-laying behaviour. (B) Natural variation in the effect of mod-5(lf) on egg laying (adult hermaphrodites, mid-L4 +30 hr). mod-5(lf) increased basal egg-laying activity relative to wild type in the absence of food (M9 buffer), except for the two Class I strains and the Class III strains JU830 and JU751. The stimulatory effect of mod-5(lf) on egg laying also varied quantitatively between strains within the same Class, that is, within Class II and Class IIIA. Align Rank Transform ANOVA, fixed effect mod-5: F1,709=175.55, p<0.0001, fixed effect Background: F9,709 = 20.98, p<0.0001; interaction mod-5 x Background: F9,709=5.34, p<0.0001. N=23–60 replicates per strain, with each replicate containing 3.15±0.18 individuals on average were scored. (C) Natural variation in egg laying in response to a gradient of low exogeneous serotonin concentrations in wild type and mod-5(n822) animals. Adult hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +30 hr) were placed into M9 buffer without food containing four different concentrations of serotonin (0.0 mM, 3.7 mM, 6.0 mM, 12.3 mM). Strains differed strongly in sensitivity to specific concentrations of exogenous serotonin and these effects of genetic background were further contingent on the presence of mod-5(lf) as indicated by the significant three-way interaction term genetic background x mod-5 allele x serotonin treatment (Table 3). For complete results of statistical analyses, see Table 3; for an alternative representation of the same data, see Figure 8—figure supplement 1B. For each concentration, 8–10 replicates (each containing 3.15±0.22 individuals on average) were scored per strain.

Figure 8—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 8, Figure 8—figure supplement 1A and B.
Figure 8—source data 2. Excel file containing statistical results for analyses of data shown in Figure 8B and Figure 8—figure supplement 1A.

Figure 8.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1. Manipulating endogenous serotonin levels uncovers natural variation in the neuromodulatory architecture of the C. elegans egg-laying system.

Figure 8—figure supplement 1.

(A) Natural variation in serotonin sensitivity of mod-5(lf) on egg laying (adult hermaphrodites, L4 +30 hr). Align Rank Transform ANOVA, fixed effect mod-5: F1,582=0.29, p=0.59, fixed effect Background: F7,582 = 277.73, p<0.0001; interaction mod-5 x Background: F7,582=3.57, p<0.0001. mod-5(lf) did not increase egg-laying when exposed to a high dose of exogenous serotonin (25 mM), except for a slight increase observed in ED3005 (Tukey’s honestly significant difference, ***p<0.0001; ns: not significant). For each strain, 24–60 replicates (each containing 3.08±0.14 individuals on average) were scored. For complete results of statistical analyses, see Figure 8—source data 2. (B) Alternative representation of data shown in Figure 8C: Natural variation in egg laying in response to a gradient of low exogeneous serotonin concentrations in wild type and mod-5(n822) animals. Adult hermaphrodites (L4 +30 hr) were placed into M9 buffer without food containing four different concentrations of serotonin (0.0 mM, 3.7 mM, 6.0 mM, 12.3 mM). Strains differed strongly in sensitivity to specific concentrations of exogenous serotonin and these effects of genetic background were further contingent on the presence of mod-5(lf) as indicated by the significant 3-way interaction term genetic background x mod-5 allele x serotonin treatment (see Table 3 for statistical results). For complete results of statistical analyses, see Table 3. For each concentration, 8–10 replicates (each containing 3.15±0.22 individuals on average) were scored per strain. For detailed results of statistical analyses, see Figure 8—source data 1.

Introducing the mutation mod-5(n822) did not further increase egg laying when wild strains were exposed to 25 mM serotonin (except for the strain ED3005), likely because egg-laying activity was already at its maximum at such a high dose of serotonin (Figure 8—figure supplement 1A). In contrast, exposure to a range of lower serotonin concentrations uncovered significant strain differences in sensitivity to exogenous serotonin and these effects of genetic background were further contingent on the presence of mod-5(lf) (Figure 8C, Figure 8—figure supplement 1B, Table 3). Low exogenous serotonin concentrations strongly stimulated egg laying in Class I strains and ED3005 (Class IIIA), and this stimulation was significantly amplified in the presence of mod-5(lf) (Figure 8C), consistent with the previously observed serotonin hypersensitivity of ED3005 (Figure 7B). By contrast, in Class II strains, including N2, low exogenous serotonin only marginally increased egg-laying, and mod-5(lf) had little effect (Figure 8C). As in previous experiments (Figure 7B, Figure 8B), we find again that strains sharing the same egg retention phenotype may differ strongly in egg-laying behaviour in response to modulation of both exo- and endogenous serotonin levels (Class IIIA: ED3005 and JU2829) (Figure 8C, Figure 8—figure supplement 1B).

Table 3. Natural variation in egg laying in response to a gradient of low exogenous serotonin concentrations in wild type and mod-5(n822) animals (Figure 8C, Figure 8—figure supplement 1B).

Results for statistical analyses testing for the effects of and interactions between genetic background, presence of mod-5(lf) and Treatment (concentration of serotonin) on egg laying (ANOVA).

Source DF Sum of Squares F Ratio p
mod-5 1 67.45 186.42 <.0001*
Background 7 149.21 58.91 <.0001*
Treatment 3 120.21 110.73 <.0001*
mod-5 x Background 7 8.94 3.53 0.0010*
mod-5 x Treatment 3 15.77 14.53 <.0001*
Background x Treatment 21 95.77 12.60 <.0001*
mod-5 x Background x Treatment 21 13.31 1.75 0.021*
Error 510 184.55

C. elegans wild strains differ in their egg-laying activity in response to the same dose of exogenous serotonin and in response to the same genetic alteration that causes an increase in endogenous serotonin levels. Such significant differences between genotypes in effect sizes of a focal allele are evidence for epistasis (non-linear genetic interactions) (Gibson and Dworkin, 2004). Hence, C. elegans harbours natural variation in its genetic architecture affecting sensitivity to neuromodulatory inputs of the egg-laying circuit. This genetic variation can occur in elements of the egg-laying circuit, such as the only known natural variant (KCNL-1 V530L) affecting vulval muscle excitability (Vigne et al., 2021), but likely also in components acting outside of the core circuit, including head neurons and signalling pathways mediating sensory inputs affecting egg-laying activity (Aprison et al., 2022; Schafer, 2006).

Evaluating potential costs and benefits of variation in egg retention

The existence of pronounced natural variation in C. elegans egg-laying phenotypes raises the questions of why this variation is maintained and how it might impact alternative fitness components. We therefore experimentally explored potential fitness costs and benefits associated with variable egg retention of C. elegans wild strains. We previously reported an apparent fitness cost of strong egg retention but only for a single strain (JU751, Class IIIB), in which mothers die prior to the end of the reproductive span due to internal (matricidal) hatching (Vigne et al., 2021). Here, using comparisons between multiple focal strains with variable degrees of egg retention, we tested if increased egg retention consistently generates negative fitness effects. Consistent with this hypothesis, strains with stronger egg retention generally showed reduced lifetime self-fertility and reduced survival, with strongest reductions observed in Class IIIB strains (Figure 9A-C). Class III strains showed frequent internal hatching, with mothers dying prematurely, sometimes before the end of the reproductive span (Figure 10A-C). Internal hatching in Class I and II strains was absent or occurred only in rare instances at the end of the reproductive period (Figure 10C), similar to what has been previously observed for the strain N2 (Pickett and Kornfeld, 2013).

Figure 9. Strains with stronger egg retention show reduced lifetime self-fertility and survival.

Figure 9.

(A) Dynamics of lifetime offspring production in self-fertilizing hermaphrodites of the 15 focal strains with divergent egg retention. Hermaphrodites at the mid-L4 stage were isolated to individual NGM plates and their offspring production was scored every 24 hr until reproduction had ceased (~mid-L4+96 hr). N=28–30 individuals per strain. (B) Significant differences in total lifetime offspring number between the 15 focal strains (Kruskal-Wallis Test, χ2=290.79, df=14, p<0.0001). Same experiment as shown in (A), N=28–30 individuals per strain. (C) Significant negative correlation between mean lifetime offspring number and mean egg retention across the 15 focal strains with divergent egg retention (at mid-L4 +30 hr) (ρSpearman=-0.85, p<0.0001).

Figure 9—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 9.

Figure 10. Strains with strong egg retention show reduced survival and increased internal hatching.

Figure 10.

(A) Hermaphrodite survival in the 15 focal strains with divergent egg retention, separated by phenotypic classes. Survival was scored every 24 hr across the first eight days of adulthood. For each strain, two to three replicates were scored, with each replicate containing 30–36 individuals. On day 5, the fraction of surviving individuals was significantly different between all four Classes (Tukey’s honestly significant difference, all p<0.05) (Two-Way ANOVA, fixed effect Class: F3,29=178.49, p<0.0001, fixed effect Strain(nested in Class): F11,29=6.41, p<0.0001). (B) Significant negative correlation between mean percentage of survival (day 5) and mean egg retention across the 15 focal strains with divergent egg retention (at mid-L4 +30 hr) (ρSpearman=-0.84, p=0.00013). (C) Temporal progression of internal hatching during the survival assay (from data shown in (A)) in the 15 focal strains, measured as the cumulative percentage of dead mothers containing one or more internally hatched larva. For each strain, two to three replicates were scored, and each replicate consisted of 30–36 individuals. No individuals were censored.

Figure 10—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 10.

To further test when and how strong egg retention and internal hatching may perturb self-fertilization, we screened DAPI-stained hermaphrodites at different adult stages to count remaining self-sperm and internally hatched larvae, and we scored animals for physical damage of the maternal germline and soma. First, a fraction of adults in multiple Class III (but not Class I or II) strains had self-sperm remaining at the time of internal hatching (Figure 11), confirming that internal hatching can indeed occur during the reproductive span, potentially disrupting self-fertilization. Consistent with this latter scenario, in the Class IIIB strain JU2593, the total number of self-sperm greatly exceeded the number of lifetime progeny (Figure 11—figure supplement 1A). Second, internally hatched larvae in live mothers with remaining self-sperm (limited to Class III strains) caused apparent physical damage to the maternal gonad and somatic tissues due to larval movement, sometimes disrupting the uterine wall (Figure 11—figure supplement 1B). Third, larval movement also appeared to scatter sperm away from the uterine regions adjacent to spermathecae, which likely reduces fertilization efficacy as certain Class III strains had large numbers of unfertilized oocytes in the uterus before self-sperm was depleted (Figure 11—figure supplement 1C and D).

Figure 11. Internal hatching may occur during the reproductive span of hermaphrodites.

Quantifying hermaphrodite self-sperm numbers in select focal strains with divergent egg retention and testing for the co-occurrence of internally hatched larvae and self-sperm. Adult hermaphrodites derived from age-synchronized populations were collected across multiple time points of their reproductive span, then stained with DAPI to visualize and count spermatids (N=46–175 individuals per strain). Sperm and internally hatched larvae were found to co-occur in multiple Class III strains but not in strains of Class I or II.

Figure 11—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 11.

Figure 11.

Figure 11—figure supplement 1. Internal hatching may have deleterious effects on germline integrity and reproduction.

Figure 11—figure supplement 1.

(A) The Class IIIB strain JU2593 shows a strong discordance between total self-sperm number (N=20 individuals) and lifetime offspring production (N=19 individuals (ANOVA, F1,37=27.66, p<0.0001)). Self-sperm was counted in DAPI-stained worms that had just reached reproductive maturity (0–5 eggs in the uterus). (B) Internal larval hatching increases the incidence of gonad/germline damage in the Class IIIB strain JU2593 (hermaphrodites at midL4 +40 hr and midL4 +48 hr were fixed and stained with DAPI to visualize germlines, N=105). The number of larvae in utero was higher in individuals with damaged gonads versus individuals with intact gonads (Kruskal-Wallis Test, χ2=41.74, df=1, p<0.0001). (C) and (D) Uterine accumulation of unfertilized oocytes precedes self-sperm depletion in the Class III strains JU830 and NIC1832 (but not in the Class II strain N2). For each strain, hermaphrodites (at L4 +48 hr and L4 +72 hr) were fixed and stained with DAPI to visualize and count remaining (C) self-sperm and (D) unfertilized oocytes in the uterus. At L4 +48 h, JU830 and NIC1832 have generated large numbers of unfertilized oocytes despite the presence of self-sperm (N2: N=30, JU830: N=37, NIC1832: N=33). At L4 +72 h, all strains have depleted their self-sperm and have unfertilized oocytes in utero (N2: N=40, JU830: N=34, NIC1832: N=40).
Figure 11—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 11—figure supplement 1.

The above experiments show that strong egg retention (and internal hatching) correlates with reduced self-fertility (Figure 9C) and reduced maternal survival (Figure 10B). Class IIIB strains exhibited the most drastic reduction in reproductive output, only using a fraction of available self-sperm. In contrast, in Class IIIA strains (ED3005, JU440, NIC1786) internal hatching occurred mainly after reproduction had ceased (Figure 10C), suggesting that strong egg retention has limited detrimental fitness effects in these strains.

Strong egg retention may provide a competitive advantage in resource-limited environments

Given its frequent deleterious effects on survival and fecundity, we asked if there could be any counteracting beneficial effects associated with increased egg retention. Strong egg retention reflects prolonged intra-uterine embryonic development (Figure 3D), which results in laying of eggs holding advanced-stage embryos, as confirmed by scoring the age distribution of embryos contained in eggs laid by young adults of the 15 focal wild strains (Figure 12A). Within laid eggs of Class I and II strains, embryos rarely exceeded the 26-cell stage, whilst in Class III, many embryos had started to differentiate, containing hundreds of cells, including late-stage embryos, sometimes close to hatching (Figure 12A). In the context of the rapid C. elegans life cycle (~80 hr egg-to-adult developmental time), observed strain differences in embryonic age are considerable: it takes approximately 2–3 hr to reach the 26-cell stage but around 9–12 hr to develop into late-stage embryos (Hall and Altun, 2007). Prolonged egg retention should thus result in earlier hatching ex utero, so that intraspecific differences in the timing of hatching could potentially affect competitive fitness when different genotypes compete for the same resource. Specifically, rapid external hatching may provide an advantage when exploiting resource-limited environments as only a few hours of ‘head start’ in larval development can be decisive for competitive outcomes, for example, as shown in Drosophila flies (Bakker, 1961; Mueller and Bitner, 2015).

Figure 12. Strong egg retention may provide a competitive advantage in resource-limited environments.

Figure 12.

(A) Age distribution of embryos contained within eggs laid by hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +40 hr) of the 15 focal strains. Embryonic stages were divided into five age groups according to the following characteristics using Nomarski microscopy (Hall and Altun, 2007): 1–2 cell stage, 4–26 cell stage, 44 cell to gastrula stage, bean to two-fold stage, three-fold stage, L1 larva. N=45–72 eggs per strain. (B) Significant negative correlation between hatching time of laid eggs and mean egg retention across the 15 focal strains with divergent egg retention (at mid-L4 +30 hr) (ρSpearman=-0.92, p<0.0001). Values are estimates of the time point at which 50% of the eggs had hatched. For each strain, 10–20 adult hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +30 hr) were allowed to lay eggs within an 1-hr window (N=48–177 eggs per strain). The fraction of hatched eggs was scored every hour until all eggs had hatched. (C) Significant negative correlation between egg-to-adult developmental time and mean egg retention across the 15 focal strains with divergent egg retention (at mid-L4 +30 hr) (ρSpearman=-0.69, p=0.0041). Values are estimates of the time point at which 50% of individuals had reached reproductive maturity (one or two eggs in utero). For each strain, 10–20 adult hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +30 hr) were allowed to lay eggs within a one-hour window (N=77–318 eggs per strain). After removal of adults, eggs were allowed to hatch and after 45 hr of development, populations were surveyed every two hours to count the fraction of adults that had reached reproductive maturity. (D) Short-term competition of JU1200WT and JU1200KCNL-1 V530L against a GFP-tester strain (myo-2::gfp) with a genotype starting frequencies of 50:50. The strain JU1200KCNL-1 V530L (strong egg retention, Class III phenotype) outperformed JU1200WT (regular egg retention, Class II phenotype). For each replicate, 20 laid eggs of either genotype were mixed with 20 laid eggs of the GFP-tester strain on a NGM plate; after 4–5 days (when food became exhausted) the fraction of GFP-positive adult individuals was determined. Relative to the GFP-tester strain, JU1200KCNL-1 V530L showed a significantly higher fraction of adults compared to JU1200WT (Kruskal-Wallis Test, χ2=11.57, df=1, p=0.0007). N=10 replicates per genotype. (E) Short- term competition of JU751 (Class IIIB, strong egg retention) against each of five wild strains (Class II, canonical egg retention) isolated from the same locality. For each of the five strains, 20 freshly laid eggs were mixed with 20 freshly laid eggs from JU751 and allowed to develop. Adult population size and genotype frequencies were determined after 4–5 days (when food became exhausted). In each of the five competition experiments, JU751 showed a significantly higher number of adults (Wilcoxon signed-rank test for matched pairs, all p<0.05). N=10 replicates per strain.

Figure 12—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 12.

Here, we tested to what extent strain differences in egg retention (of young adults) affect external egg-to-adult developmental time and competitive ability. First, comparing the average timing of larval hatching between the 15 focal strains with variable egg retention, we found that eggs of all Class IIIB strains and some Class IIIA strains hatched on average ~2–5 hours earlier compared to Class I and II strains (Figure 12B). Second, to test if earlier larval hatching indeed translates into corresponding earlier onset of reproductive maturity in the 15 focal strains, we measured the time interval between egg laying and reproductive maturity, defined as the onset of fertilization, that is by the presence of 1–2 eggs in utero. Although many Class III strains with strong egg retention reached age at maturity (~2–6 hr) earlier than Class I and II strains, some Class III strains did not maintain their initial head start in larval development (Figure 12C). Overall, these measurements confirm that strains with prolonged egg retention benefit from a relatively shorter developmental time from laying to reproductive maturity, which might improve competitive ability. The above experiment only examined eggs laid by young adults (mid-L4 +30 hr); in Class III strains, the observed head start in larval development should thus be further amplified in older hermaphrodites (from mid-L4 +48 hr onwards), containing not only advanced-stage embryos but also larvae as shown earlier (Figure 3E).

To quantify the possible effects of egg retention on short-term competitive ability in the absence of confounding genetic variation, we compared the Class II strain JU1200 (wild type) to the engineered JU1200KCNL-1 V530L strain, i.e. two genetically identical strains with the exception of the engineered KCNL-1 V530L mutation in the latter strain, causing very strong egg retention and laying of advanced-stage embryos: JU1200WT (~15 eggs in utero) versus JU1200KCNL-1 V530L (~40 eggs in utero; Vigne et al., 2021). We competed each strain separately against a green fluorescent protein (GFP)-tester strain by inoculating NGM plates with 20 freshly laid eggs from either genotype. Estimating adult population number and genotype frequencies relative to the GFP-tester strain 5 days after inoculation (around the time point of food exhaustion), the KCNL-1 V530L variant performed significantly better (Figure 12D). Given that the principal phenotypic effect of KCNL-1 V530L is reduced egg laying and consequently increased egg retention (Vigne et al., 2021), we conclude that strong egg retention can improve competitive ability in food-limited environments.

Finally, to mimic scenarios of ecologically relevant short-term competition between naturally co-occurring wild strains with divergent egg retention, we compared the competitive ability of the Class IIIB strain JU751 (strong egg retention caused by the variant KCNL-1 V530L; ~40 eggs in utero at mid-L4 +30 hr) against strains isolated around the same time from the same habitat (compost heap, Le Perreux-sur-Marne, France) (Barrière and Félix, 2007) but exhibiting canonical egg retention (Class II phenotype; hermaphrodites at mid-L4 +30 h; Vigne et al., 2021). For each of five of these strains, 20 freshly laid eggs were mixed with 20 freshly laid eggs from JU751 and allowed to develop. We then estimated adult population size and genotype frequencies 5 days after inoculation (around the time point of food exhaustion). In all five cases, JU751 reached higher adult population sizes at this stage (Figure 12E). These experimental results offer preliminary evidence (bearing in mind that our analysis was primarily centered on a single genetic background) that laying of advanced-stage embryos may enhance intraspecific competitive ability, particularly in scenarios where multiple genotypes compete for colonization and exploitation of limited, patchily distributed resources. Similar to our experiments, increased egg-to-adult developmental time has previously been shown to be disadvantageous for C. elegans population growth when analysing mutants with increased hermaphrodite sperm production, which incurs a cost as the onset of reproductive maturity will be delayed; thus, although these mutants have the potential to produce overall many more self-progeny, they will be rapidly outcompeted because of delayed maturity whenever resources are limited (Hodgkin and Barnes, 1991; Barker, 1992; Cutter, 2004). Together with theoretical and experimental evidence in insects (Bakker, 1961; Mueller and Bitner, 2015; Horváth and Kalinka, 2018), these observations indicate that maternal features accelerating offspring development after oviposition can confer fitness benefits in ephemeral habitats with rapidly decaying resources.

Strong egg retention improves offspring protection when facing sudden environmental stress

Prolonged intra-uterine embryonic development, as observed in viviparous organisms, is thought to provide improved protection of developing embryos against deleterious environmental fluctuations and stressors (Blackburn, 1999; Kalinka, 2015; Horváth and Kalinka, 2018). Whether prolonged egg retention in C. elegans can increase such protection is unclear, in particular, because embryos seem already well protected: they are encapsulated within a multi-layered egg shell that is highly resistant to diverse environmental insults, such as osmotic stress or pathogen infections (Schierenberg and Junkersdorf, 1992; Johnston and Dennis, 2012; Stein and Golden, 2018; Sandhu et al., 2021). Nevertheless, eggs developing ex utero are still vulnerable to a variety of abiotic and biotic stressors (Van Voorhies and Ward, 2000; Garsin et al., 2001; Padilla et al., 2002; Burton et al., 2021; Fausett et al., 2021). If eggs in utero are indeed more effectively shielded against environmental insults compared to eggs laid in the external environment, genotypes with strong egg retention may thus benefit from improved offspring protection. Here, we tested this hypothesis by comparing the effects of environmental perturbations on eggs developing in utero versus ex utero using a subset of wild strains with different levels of egg retention.

We first examined the effects of short-term, acute exposure to high concentrations of ethanol and acetic acid, chemicals that are present in decaying plant matter, that is the natural C. elegans habitat (Félix and Braendle, 2010). Across strains, most eggs removed from mothers and directly exposed to these chemical treatments were killed, whereas eggs inside mothers exhibited significantly higher survival in most strains, particularly when exposed to acetic acid (Figure 13A and B). Eggs in utero were thus partly protected by the body of the mothers, even though mothers instantly died upon treatment exposure. Consequently, increased egg retention resulted in overall higher numbers of surviving offspring per mother in both stress treatments (Figure 13A and B).

Figure 13. Strong egg retention improves progeny protection when facing sudden environmental stress.

(A) Differences in survival of eggs developing ex utero versus in utero when exposed to a high concentration of ethanol (96%, 10-min exposure). A subset of the 15 focal strains with divergent egg retention was selected to compare the number of surviving eggs ex utero (extracted by dissection) versus in utero (eggs retained in mothers) exposed to ethanol. Overall, the number of surviving eggs tended to be greater when exposed to ethanol in utero compared to ex utero (Kruskal-Wallis Tests performed separately for each strain to compare the number of surviving offspring in utero versus ex utero when exposed to ethanol; *p<0.05, ns: not significant). Class III strains tended to have a higher number of surviving offspring than Class I and II strains. N=5–10 replicates per genotype and treatment (10 hermaphrodites at mid-L4 +30 hr per replicate). See Figure 13—figure supplement 1A, for additional (control) data of the experiment. (B) Differences in survival of eggs developing ex utero versus in utero when exposed to a high concentration of acetic acid (10 M, 15-min exposure). A subset of the 15 focal strains with divergent egg retention was selected to compare the number of surviving eggs ex utero (extracted by dissection) versus in utero (eggs retained in mothers) exposed to acetic acid. For all strains, the number of surviving eggs was significantly greater when exposed to acetic acid in utero compared to ex utero (Kruskal-Wallis Tests performed separately for each strain to compare the number of surviving offspring in utero versus ex utero when exposed to acetic acid; *p<0.05, ns: not significant). Class III strains tended to have a higher number of surviving offspring than Class I and II strains. N=5 replicates per genotype and treatment (10 hermaphrodites at mid-L4 +30 hr per replicate). See Figure 13—figure supplement 1B for additional (control) data of the experiment.

Figure 13—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 13, Figure 13—figure supplement 1A and B.

Figure 13.

Figure 13—figure supplement 1. Additional data for experiment shown in Figure 13A and B, including data for control conditions (M9 buffer).

Figure 13—figure supplement 1.

(A) Additional data for the experiment shown in Figure 13A: Differences in survival of eggs developing ex utero versus in utero when exposed to a high concentration of ethanol (96%, 10-min exposure). A subset of the 15 focal strains with divergent egg retention was selected to compare embryonic survival of eggs ex utero (extracted by dissection) versus in utero (eggs retained in mothers) exposed to ethanol. In addition to data shown in Figure 13A, this figure includes control data for survival of eggs ex utero exposed to control conditions (M9 buffer). N=5–10 replicates (each containing 10 mothers) per genotype and treatment (hermaphrodites at L4 +30 hr). (B) Additional data for the experiment shown in Figure 13B: Differences in survival of eggs developing ex utero versus in utero when exposed to a high concentration of acetic acid (10 M, 15-min exposure). A subset of the 15 focal strains with divergent egg retention was selected to compare embryonic survival of eggs ex utero (extracted by dissection) versus in utero (eggs retained in mothers) exposed to acetic acid. In addition to data shown in Figure 13B, this figure includes control data for survival of eggs ex utero exposed to control conditions (M9 buffer). N=5 replicates (each containing 10 mothers) per genotype and treatment (hermaphrodites at L4 +30 hr).

To corroborate this result, we tested if the Class III strain JU751 displays better maternal protection relative to strains with a canonical egg retention phenotype (Class II) from same habitat, isolated around the same time (Barrière and Félix, 2007). Exposure to acetic acid confirmed that higher egg retention of JU751 increased the number of surviving offspring compared to strains with lower retention (Figure 14A). In the same fashion, we found the number of surviving offspring in the Class II strain JU1200WT (~15 eggs in utero) to be much reduced compared to JU1200KCNL-1 V530L (~40 eggs in utero) when exposed to acetic acid (Figure 14B). In addition, larvae in utero present at the time of treatment were also efficiently protected (Figure 14C). Offspring in utero may therefore benefit from maternal protection, so that strains with constitutively higher egg retention will protect overall more progeny when confronting a sudden environmental insult.

Figure 14. Differences in survival of eggs developing ex utero versus in utero when exposed to environmental stress.

(A) Differences in survival of eggs developing ex utero versus in utero when exposed to acetic acid (10 M, 15-min exposure). Comparison of the strain JU751 (Class IIIB, strong retention) to four strains (Class II, canonical retention) isolated from the same locality. Examining only data for eggs exposed in utero, JU751 exhibited a significantly higher number of surviving offspring compared to all other strains (ANOVA, effect Strain: F4,20=15.96, p<0.0001; Tukey’s honestly significant difference, all p<0.05). N=5 replicates per genotype and treatment (10 hermaphrodites at mid-L4 +30 hr per replicate). See Figure 14—figure supplement 1 for additional (control) data of the experiment. (B) Comparing the Class II strain JU1200WT (canonical retention) and the JU1200KCNL-1 V530L strain (strong retention): differences in the number of surviving eggs ex utero (extracted by dissection) versus in utero (eggs retained in mothers) exposed to a high concentration of acetic acid (10 M, 15-min exposure). JU1200KCNL-1 V530L exhibited a significantly higher number of surviving offspring in utero when exposed to acetic acid compared to JU1200WT (Kruskal-Wallis Test, χ2=14.35, df=1, p=0.0002). N=10 replicates per genotype per treatment. (C) Differences in the number of surviving internally hatched larvae exposed to acetic acid (10 M, 15-min exposure) using the strain JU1200KCNL-1 V530L: ex utero (extracted by dissection) versus in utero (larvae retained in mothers). The number of live larvae per mother was significantly higher in utero compared to ex utero (Kruskal-Wallis Test, χ2=13.89, df=1, p=0.0002). N=10 replicates per genotype per treatment.

Figure 14—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 14, Figure 14—figure supplement 1.

Figure 14.

Figure 14—figure supplement 1. Additional data for the experiment shown in Figure 14A.

Figure 14—figure supplement 1.

Differences in survival of eggs developing ex utero versus in utero when exposed to a high concentration of acetic acid (10 M, 15-min exposure). Comparison of the strain JU751 (Class IIIB, high retention) to four strains (Class II, canonical retention) isolated from the same locality. In addition to data shown in Figure 14A, this figure includes control data for survival of eggs ex utero exposed to control conditions (M9 buffer). N=5 replicates (each containing 10 mothers) per genotype and treatment (hermaphrodites at L4 +30 hr).

To test how exposure to milder environmental perturbations affects the reproductive fitness of descendants from eggs exposed in utero versus ex utero, we focused on a single strain (JU751, Class IIIB) with constitutively strong egg retention. First, we selected an osmotic stress condition (15-hr exposure to 0.3 M NaCl), which caused relatively low embryonic mortality when eggs were exposed to this treatment ex utero (Figure 15A). We then followed surviving individuals to score their lifetime reproductive success under selfing. We found that fertility of individuals derived from stress-exposed eggs ex utero was reduced compared to the ones derived from eggs in utero (Figure 15B). Second, we treated eggs in utero versus ex utero with a low concentration of acetic acid (1 M) for 15 min. This treatment was lethal for mothers but did not cause any mortality of embryos laid ex utero. Yet, individuals derived from ex utero eggs developed more slowly and exhibited significantly delayed reproductive maturity (but not reduced fertility) compared to individuals that hatched from eggs in utero (Figure 15C and D). In addition, larvae in utero at the time of stress exposure were also protected by their mother’s body and produced as many offspring as animals in control conditions (Figure 15C). In contrast, larvae were killed instantly if exposed to this stress ex utero. Together, our experimental results suggest that constitutively strong egg retention can enhance maternal protection of offspring by protecting a relatively larger number of eggs in utero when facing a sudden environmental perturbation. Prolonged egg retention will also reduce the total time of external exposure of the immobile embryonic stage, thus likely lowering risks associated with diverse environmental threats, including exposure to predators and pathogens.

Figure 15. Increased egg retention protects offspring viability and fertility under mild environmental stress.

Figure 15.

(A) Effects of osmotic stress on survival of embryos from eggs developing ex utero in the strain JU751. Eggs were extracted from adult hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +36 hr) by dissection and exposed to variable concentrations of NaCl (and M9 control condition) for 15 hr. Embryonic survival was estimated by counting the fraction of live larvae 24 hr later. N=120–288 eggs per treatment. (B) Differences in total lifetime offspring production of selfing JU751 animals derived from surviving eggs developing ex utero (dissected from adults at mid-L4 +36 hr) versus in utero when exposed to mild osmotic stress (0.3 M NaCl) for 15 hr; in parallel, ex utero eggs (dissected from adults at mid-L4 +36 hr) were exposed to a control treatment (M9 buffer) for 15 hr. 24 hr after the treatment, larvae from each of the three treatments were allowed to develop and reproduce for 3 days and scored for total offspring production. Animals derived from eggs developing in utero showed significantly higher fertility than animals derived from eggs developing ex utero when exposed to osmotic stress (Kruskal-Wallis Test, χ2=4.66, df=2, p=0.03). N=18–19 animals per treatment. (C) Differences in developmental time of JU751 animals derived from surviving eggs developing ex utero (dissected from adults at mid-L4 +36 hr) versus in utero when exposed to a low concentration of acetic acid (1 M) for 15 min; in parallel, ex utero eggs (dissected from adults at mid-L4 +36 hr) were exposed to a control treatment (M9 buffer). Eggs were then allowed to hatch and develop for 45 hr, at which time we determined their developmental stages. Each stage was assigned a score of development as follows: L3=1; early mid-L4=2; midL4=3; lateL4=4; Adult=5. Each dot represents the mean score reached by offspring produced by one single mother (N=30) mothers per treatment, producing between surviving 9–57 larval offspring. Animals derived from in utero eggs had a significantly higher mean developmental time score, that is they exhibited accelerated development compared to animals derived from ex utero eggs (Kruskal-Wallis Test, χ2=38.93, df=1, p<0.0001). (D) Differences in total lifetime offspring production of selfing JU751 animals derived from eggs developing ex utero versus eggs and L1 larvae in utero when exposed to a low concentration of acetic acid (1 M) for 15 min; in parallel, ex utero eggs (dissected from adults at mid-L4 +36 hr) were exposed to a control treatment (M9 buffer). Note that L1 larvae directly exposed to 1 M acetic acid died immediately. Twenty-four hr after the treatment, larvae from each of the four treatments were allowed to develop and reproduce for four days until cessation of reproduction; total offspring production was then scored 24 hr later. There were no significant differences in mean fertility between the four different treatment groups (Kruskal-Wallis Test, χ2=4.00, df=3, p=0.26). N=15 animals per treatment. Eggs were dissected from adults at mid-L4 +36 hr and L1 larvae at mid-L4 +48 hr.

Figure 15—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 15.

Natural variation in Caenorhabditis elegans egg laying modulates an intergenerational fitness trade-off

Taken together, our experimental results suggest that the degree of C. elegans egg retention alters the interplay of antagonistic effects on maternal versus offspring fitness. On the one hand, strong egg retention reduces maternal survival and reproduction (Figure 9C, Figure 10B); on the other, by prolonging intra-uterine embryonic development, offspring may benefit from improved competitive ability (Figure 12) and protection against environmental insults (Figure 13, Figure 14, Figure 15). Therefore, variable egg retention, as observed in natural C. elegans populations, may reflect genetic differences in the adjustment of an intergenerational fitness trade-off. In extreme cases of strong constitutive egg retention, C. elegans strains exhibit partial viviparity, associated with significantly reduced maternal reproduction and survival. Overall, observed genetically variable duration of intra-uterine development in C. elegans thus seems to align well with past reports showing that transitions towards viviparity incur maternal fitness costs as observed for diverse invertebrate and vertebrate taxa with both inter- and intraspecific differences in egg-laying modes (Avise, 2013; Blackburn, 2015; Kalinka, 2015; Ostrovsky et al., 2016; Horváth and Kalinka, 2018; Whittington et al., 2022). To what extent this apparent intergenerational fitness trade-off in C. elegans could involve parent-offspring conflict (Trivers, 1974) is uncertain. However, given the nearly exclusive selfing mode of reproduction in C. elegans (Barrière and Félix, 2005; Lee et al., 2021), we expect none to very little genetic disparity between hermaphrodite mothers and offspring, indicating much reduced opportunities for intergenerational conflict. Observed natural differences in C. elegans egg retention are therefore predicted to reflect properties of genetically distinct (effectively clonally propagating) genotypes, likely resulting from adaptation to distinct ecological niches differing in nature, fluctuations, and predictability of resource availability (Blackburn, 1999; Meier et al., 1999; Trexler and DeAngelis, 2003; Kalinka, 2015; Mueller and Bitner, 2015; Dey et al., 2016; Horváth and Kalinka, 2018).

Discussion

Our study has quantified and characterized natural variation in C. elegans egg retention and egg-laying behaviour, resulting in the following main findings: (1) C. elegans exhibits quantitative variation in egg retention, with rare instances of extreme phenotypes at either end of the spectrum. (2) Both common and rare genetic variants underlie observed phenotypic variation. (3) Strain differences in egg retention can be largely explained by differences in egg-laying behaviour, such as the onset of egg laying activity and timing of the rhythm between active and inactive egg-laying phases. (4) These behavioural differences map, at least partly, to genetic differences in the sensitivity to various neuromodulators, indicative of natural variation in the C. elegans egg-laying circuitry. (5) Modified egg-laying behaviour causing strong egg retention – linked to frequent larval hatching in utero – negatively impacts maternal fitness, but (6) prolonged intra-uterine embryonic development may benefit offspring by improving their competitive ability and protection against environmental insults. (7) Hence, variation in C. elegans egg retention may reflect variation in a trade-off between antagonistic effects acting on maternal versus offspring fitness.

C. elegans egg retention is a quantitative (complex) trait: observed phenotypic distribution and GWA mapping indicate that this trait is likely influenced by multiple loci of small effect (Mackay et al., 2009). In addition, large-effect variants, such as KCNL-1 V530L (Vigne et al., 2021), occur but they seem to be relatively rare. Although variation in reproductive processes, such as self-sperm production and ovulation rates, contributes to observed variation in egg number in utero, we also present clear evidence for natural variation in egg-laying behaviour and involved neuromodulatory processes. How such behavioural differences arise through modification of specific cellular and physiological processes and how these, in turn, are mediated by specific molecular alterations of genes in the C. elegans egg-laying system remains to be elucidated. The genetics of the C. elegans egg-laying circuit has been studied for decades, and dozens of genes have been identified by mutations, which either reduce or increase egg-laying activity (Trent et al., 1983; Schafer, 2006). Some of the identified genes encode ion channels regulating cell and synaptic electrical excitability, others encode components of G-protein signalling pathways that act in specific cells of the circuit. Many more genes transmitting environmental and physiological signals via sensory processing are known to regulate C. elegans egg laying (Schafer, 2006; Ringstad and Horvitz, 2008; Fenk and de Bono, 2015; Banerjee et al., 2017). Recent research has further uncovered an important role of muscle-directed mechanisms regulating egg laying through sensation of internal stretch mediated by egg accumulation in utero (Collins et al., 2016; Ravi et al., 2018; Ravi et al., 2021; Medrano and Collins, 2023). This stretch-dependent homeostat directly targets vulval muscles and is sufficient to trigger egg laying when synaptic transmission is reduced or defective, for example, in the absence of HSN command neurons (Ravi et al., 2018; Ravi et al., 2021; Medrano and Collins, 2023). As indicated by our results (Figure 6B), changes in such muscle-directed signalling components in response to egg accumulation are likely key factors driving the evolution of C. elegans egg retention. Future experiments should therefore be aimed at understanding how such modifications in mechanosensory feedback interact with other potential changes in synaptic transmission to explain the natural diversity in the C. elegans egg-laying neural circuit. In addition, we cannot exclude that natural variation in egg-laying behaviour may arise through differential activity of developmental genes generating variation in the neuroanatomy of the egg-laying circuit itself, for example as observed between different Caenorhabditis species (Loer and Rivard, 2007).

Currently, the only known natural variant modulating C. elegans egg laying is the major-effect variant KCNL-1 V530L, which reduces egg laying through likely hyperpolarization of vulval muscles (Vigne et al., 2021), so that stimuli, such as mechanosensory inputs, are insufficient to trigger regular and successful egg-laying events. Our new results show that other unknown variants must cause the strong reduction of egg-laying activity in Class IIIA strains. Although our experimental results do not hint at any variants in specific candidate genes or processes, we detected strong differences in the serotonin response between Class IIIA strains (Figure 7B). Exogenous serotonin stimulated egg laying in all Class IIIA strains except in JU2829, where it inhibited egg laying as observed for Class IIIB strains (Figure 7B). This suggests that JU2829 might carry a variant with effects like KCNL-1 V530L, whereas variants with distinct functional effects would explain reduced egg-laying activity in other Class IIIA strains. The detection of both strong and subtle strain variation in egg-laying activity (also among strains within the same Class), as revealed by our pharmacological assays and when analysing the effects of mod-5(lf) in different strains (Figure 8), indicates the presence of many different natural variants that modulate egg-laying circuitry in C. elegans. Molecular identification of specific variants will thus be essential to explore how the nematode egg-laying circuit evolves at the intraspecific level.

Consistent with previous reports on the apparent costs of transitioning from oviparity to obligate and facultative viviparity (Avise, 2013; Kalinka, 2015; Horváth and Kalinka, 2018), we found that increased C. elegans egg retention lowers maternal survival and fecundity. We then show that fitness costs associated with strong egg retention are potentially offset by improved offspring competitive ability and protection against environmental perturbations. To what extent our highly simplified experimental conditions recapitulate relevant ecological scenarios encountered by natural C. elegans populations remains to be explored. The likely ecological factors shaping natural variation in C. elegans egg retention are thus unknown and we have not found any obvious links between the degree of egg retention and habitat parameters, such as substrate type or climate. However, the likely ancestral (divergent) strains only very rarely exhibited a Class III phenotype, which could indicate that derived state of strong egg retention may have been favoured by the exploitation of novel microhabitats during the historically recent expansion of C. elegans reflected by globally distributed, swept haplotypes (Lee et al., 2019; Lee et al., 2021).

The typical C. elegans habitat is highly ephemeral in nature, exhibiting extreme fluctuations in nutrient availability and occurrence of diverse abiotic and biotic variables, including stressors and pathogens (Frézal and Félix, 2015; Schulenburg and Félix, 2017). A critical omission in our study is therefore the analysis of plasticity in egg retention and egg-laying behaviour in different or fluctuating environments. C. elegans egg laying is known to be highly plastic, strongly modulated by subtle and very diverse environmental factors, so that our analyses in standard laboratory conditions can only capture fractions of the complexity. Most prominently, diverse environmental stimuli (food quantity and quality, osmotic or hypoxic stress, pathogens, etc.) have inhibitory effects on egg laying and long-term exposure to these stimuli will lead to strong egg retention and matricidal hatching (Trent, 1982; Aballay et al., 2000; Waggoner et al., 2000; Chen and Caswell-Chen, 2003; Schafer, 2005; Zhang et al., 2008; McMullen et al., 2012; Fenk and de Bono, 2015; Vigne et al., 2021). Future research will therefore require to carefully examine how such environmental effects modulate egg laying and fitness consequences in strains with the here reported differences in constitutive egg retention. In particular, plastically induced egg retention and matricidal hatching by prolonged starvation may allow for nutrient provisioning of internally hatched larvae as they can consume debris stemming from the decaying mother, hence allowing for developmental growth in the absence of external food (Chen and Caswell-Chen, 2003; Chen and Caswell-Chen, 2004). Future experiments should therefore specifically test if internally hatched larvae in Class III strains (Figure 3E) may gain a further advantage through maternal provisioning while developing in utero, specifically when exposed to long-term conditions inhibiting egg laying, such as starvation.

The existence of pronounced natural variation in C. elegans egg retention and egg-laying behaviour generates novel opportunities to dissect the molecular genetic basis of intraspecific variation in egg retention, providing an entry point to understand the proximate mechanisms underlying evolutionary transitions between invertebrate ovi- and viviparity. In contrast to vertebrate taxa with intraspecific variation in egg-laying mode, such as certain lizards (Recknagel et al., 2021; Whittington et al., 2022), the C. elegans model allows for rapid and powerful genetic analysis, for example through linkage mapping of natural variants and subsequent functional validation of variants by CRISPR-Cas9 gene editing in multiple strains (Evans et al., 2021). Identifying the precise changes in C. elegans egg-laying circuitry that have led to variation in egg retention may also generate insights into the genetic basis of evolutionary diversification of egg-laying modes across species and genera. Nematodes display frequent transitions from oviparity to obligate viviparity in many distinct genera (Sudhaus, 1976; Ostrovsky et al., 2016), including in the genus Caenorhabditis, with at least one viviparous species, C. vivipara (Stevens et al., 2019). Although evidence exists for the evolution of egg-laying circuitry across oviparous Caenorhabditis species (Loer and Rivard, 2007), the specific cellular and genetic changes responsible for the transition to obligate viviparity in C. vivipara have yet to be examined. Resolving the genetic basis of intraspecific variation in C. elegans egg retention, including partial or facultative viviparity, may thus shed light on the molecular changes underlying the initial steps of evolutionary transitions from oviparity to obligate viviparity in invertebrates.

Materials and methods

Materials availability statement

Requests for additional information, data and strains generated in this study (Supplementary file 1) should be directed to the lead contact, Christian Braendle (braendle@unice.fr).

C. elegans strains and culture conditions

A complete list of strains used in this study is provided in Supplementary file 1. C. elegans stocks were maintained on 2.5% agar NGM (Nematode Growth Medium) plates (55 mm diameter) seeded with E. coli strain OP50 at 15 °C or 20 °C (Stiernagle, 2006). All strains were decontaminated by hypochlorite treatment (Stiernagle, 2006) in the third generation after thawing and kept in ad libitum food conditions on NGM plates. Detailed information for C. elegans wild strains used in this study is available at the CaeNDR website (https://caendr.org/) (Crombie et al., 2023). Most experiments also included the strain QX1430, a derivative of the N2 reference strain, in which the major-effect and N2-specific npr-1 allele has been replaced by its natural version (Andersen et al., 2015). For experiments, all strains were maintained on 2.5% agar NGM (Nematode Growth Medium) plates (55 mm diameter) at 20 °C unless noted otherwise. All data are reported for exclusively selfing (self-fertilizing) hermaphrodites. Liquid assays were performed in 0.1 ml M9 buffer in 96-well microplates (Greiner Bio-One, Ref: 655180); each well was treated as an independent replicate, containing approximately three worms on average. During the incubation period, plates were mildly agitated on a shaker.

Method details

Age-synchronization and identification of developmental stages

Mixed-age hermaphrodite stock cultures were hypochlorite-treated to obtain age-synchronized, arrested L1 larval populations (Stiernagle, 2006). Hermaphrodites were then picked at the mid-L4 stage based on the morphology of the vulval invagination (Mok et al., 2015).

Quantification of progeny number in utero and determination of embryonic stages

Progeny number (eggs and larvae) in utero was measured in age-synchronized hermaphrodites, mounted directly on microscopy slides and gently squashed with a coverslip. The number of offspring was then counted using a 20 x DIC microscope objective. An individual was considered to exhibit internal hatching when one or more larvae were visible in the uterus. Embryonic stages in utero were determined using DIC microscopy, and we distinguished six stages: 2 cell stage, 3–20 cell stage, early gastrula stage (21+ cells), comma stage, twofold stage, threefold stage, and L1 larva. To determine the age distribution of embryos contained in laid eggs (Figure 12A), age-synchronized hermaphrodite populations (mid-L4 +30 hr) were allowed to lay eggs for 30 min, after which embryonic stages were determined within 20 min.

Egg size measurements (Figure 3—figure supplement 1)

Laid eggs of age-synchronized hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +30 hr) were collected in M9 buffer and mounted on 4% agar pads on glass slides. Microscopy images were acquired with a 40 x DIC microscope objective using Fiji (Schindelin et al., 2012). Length and width of eggs were measured to calculate egg volume with the ellipsoid volume function: 4/3 x π x (length/2) x (width/2)2 (Fausett et al., 2021).

Body size measurements (Figure 3—figure supplement 1)

Hermaphrodite body size was measured at the early adult stage prior to the accumulation of eggs in utero, i.e., when they contained 1 or 2 eggs in utero. Animals were collected in M9 and mounted on 4% agar pads on glass slides and microscopy images (10 X objective) were acquired and processed using Fiji (Schindelin et al., 2012). As an estimate of body size, we measured the length of each animal from the tip of the head to the end of the tail using the segmented line tool and width was measured at the site of the vulval opening. The volume of the animal was calculated as that of a cylinder: π × (width/2)2×length (Vielle et al., 2016).

Quantification of egg-laying behaviour (Figure 5)

Unlike the reference behavioural protocol based on continuous video imaging (Waggoner et al., 1998), we used a non-continuous, scan-sampling method (Vigne et al., 2021) to estimate variation in egg-laying behavioural patterns across the 15 focal strains. Individual animals at peak activity of egg laying (mid-L4 +30 hr) were isolated onto seeded NGM plates. Using a dissecting microscope, we then scored the presence and number of eggs laid by isolated adults every 5 min over a 3-hr period. We therefore screened a total of 36 successive 5-min intervals (N=17–18 individuals per strain). This assay allows for the estimation of egg-laying frequency and the derivation of an approximate duration for extended inactive egg-laying periods. However, this assay does not have sufficient resolution to determine the exact timing and structure of active egg-laying periods.

Serotonin, imipramine, and fluoxetine assays (Figures 6 and 7)

Experiments testing for the effects of serotonin (5-hydroxytryptamine creatinine sulfate monohydrate, Sigma, Ref: H7752), fluoxetine (Fluoxetine hydrochloride, Sigma, Ref: PHR1394) and imipramine (imipramine hydrochloride, Sigma, Ref: I0899) on egg laying were based on previously established liquid culture protocols and drug concentrations (Trent et al., 1983; Weinshenker et al., 1995; Weinshenker et al., 1999; Ranganathan et al., 2001; Dempsey et al., 2005; Kullyev et al., 2010; Branicky et al., 2014). Approximately three age-synchronized adults (mid-L4 +30 hr) were transferred to individual wells of a 96-well microplate containing serotonin or drugs dissolved in 100 μl of M9 buffer (without bacterial food); in parallel, control animals were transferred to wells containing only M9 buffer. The number of eggs released was scored after 2 hr at 20 °C.

Introduction of mod-5(n822) into wild strains using CRISPR-Cas9 gene editing (Figure 8)

To alter endogenous serotonin levels (Ranganathan et al., 2001; Dempsey et al., 2005; Kullyev et al., 2010), we introduced a point mutation corresponding to the mod-5(n822) loss-of-function allele using CRISPR-Cas9 genome editing. This mutation changes cysteine 225 (codon TGT) to an opal stop codon (TGA) (Ranganathan et al., 2001). Gene editing was performed according to previously described procedures (Ben Soussia et al., 2019). In brief, the in vitro-synthetized crRNA crNB040 (GAAGUUCCGUGGGCGUCAUG) was used to target purified Cas9 protein to the mod-5 locus. The single-strand DNA oligonucleotide oNB322 (TCTAGATATTGTCACGTTGAGGTCATCTGAGCATCTCGGaGTgTTCCAgGGgTTtgCtCATGACGCCCACGGAACTTCGGAATCCCAAATTTTctgaaattttattgataaattgaacaa) was co-injected as a repair template with the Cas9 complex to introduce the Cys225Opal nonsense mutation. oNB322 also carries silent polymorphisms used for PCR detection using the oNB325/oNB328 (GCGTCATGaGcaAAcCCc/CAGACGACTGTGGACCCTTC) nucleotide pair. Final sequence validation was performed by Sanger sequencing on homozygous lines using the oNB327/oNB328 (ATCATCGCTCAAGCCGTCTA /CAGACGACTGTGGACCCTTC) primer pair.

Determination of reproductive schedules and lifetime offspring production (Figure 9)

Lifetime production of (viable) offspring was analysed by isolating mid-L4 hermaphrodites onto individual NGM plates and transferring them daily to fresh NGM plates until egg-laying ceased. The number of live larvae was counted 24–36 hr after each transfer. Larvae in dead mothers were counted once they had exited the maternal body. Alternatively, mothers were squashed between slide and coverslip to count offspring in the uterus. All strains were scored in parallel.

Analysis of lifespan and internal hatching (Figure 10)

Young adult hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +24 hr) were transferred to NGM plates seeded with fresh E. coli OP50. Individuals were transferred daily onto fresh plates until the end of egg laying, and every 3 days afterwards. Animals were scored as dead if they failed to respond to gentle touch with a platinum wire (N=90–102 individuals per strain, except for QG2873 with N=60).

Quantification of hermaphrodite self-sperm number (Figure 11—figure supplement 1)

The total number of self-sperm was quantified in age-synchronized young adult hermaphrodites of the JU2593 strain containing between one to six eggs in utero. A single, usually the anterior, spermatheca was imaged at ×60 magnification by performing Z-sections (1 µm) covering the entire gonad (Gimond et al., 2019). Sperm number was determined by identifying condensed sperm nuclei visible in each focal plane using the Fiji plugin Cell Counter (Schindelin et al., 2012). For a given individual, sperm count was assessed for a single gonad arm and then multiplied by two to extrapolate the total sperm count. (N=19 individuals). In parallel, a cohort of individuals in the same experiment was used to count lifetime offspring production.

Effects of internal hatching on physical integrity of the germline and scoring the presence of sperm (Figure 11, Figure 11—figure supplement 1)

Gravid hermaphrodites were fixed in methanol at different stages of reproductive adulthood and stained with DAPI to check for germline damage and the potential concomitant presence of sperm, unfertilized oocytes, and hatched larvae. Animals were fixed in cold methanol (–20 °C) for at least 30 min, washed three times with PBTw (PBS: phosphate-buffered saline, 137 mM NaCl, 2.7 mM KCl, 10 mM Na2HPO4, 2 mM KH2PO4, pH 7.4 containing 0.1% Tween 20) and squashed on a glass slide with Vectashield mounting medium containing 4,6-diamidino-2-phenylindole (DAPI; Vector Laboratories, Newark, California, USA). Observations were performed using an Olympus BX61 microscope (N=45–193 individuals per strain).

Measuring the time until hatching of laid eggs (Figure 12B)

For each strain, 10–20 adult hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +30 hr) were allowed to lay eggs for one hour on NGM plates seeded with E. coli OP50 and then removed from plates. The initial number of laid eggs was counted (N=30–170 eggs per strain) and the proportion of non-hatched eggs was determined every 60 min until all eggs had hatched. Mean values represented as bars correspond to the time at which 50% of eggs have hatched.

Measuring egg-to-adult developmental time of laid eggs (Figure 12C)

For each strain, 10–20 adult hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +30 hr) were allowed to lay eggs for one hour on NGM plates seeded with E. coli OP50 (N=77–318 eggs per strain) and then removed from plates. Starting at 40 hr after egg laying, developmental progression of resulting progeny was surveyed using a high-resolution dissecting microscope to determine age at reproductive maturity, defined as the time point at which an animal had one to two eggs in the uterus. Animals that had reached reproductive maturity were progressively eliminated from the plate. Populations were scanned for mature adults every 1–2 hr until all individuals had reached reproductive maturity, that is for a maximum period of 16 hr. Mean values shown in Figure 12C correspond to the time at which 50% of individuals have reached maturity.

Short-term competition assays (Figure 12D and E)

Short-term competition experiments of JU1200WT and JU1200KCNL-1 V530L against a GFP-tester strain with genotype starting frequencies of 50:50 were carried out in parallel (Figure 12D). The two strains were competed separately against the GFP-tester strain PD4790, containing an integrated transgene [mls12 (myo-2::GFP, pes-10::GFP, F22B7.9::GFP)] in the N2 background, expressing green fluorescent protein (GFP) in the pharynx (Figure 12D). For each replicate, 20 laid eggs of either genotype were mixed with 20 laid eggs of the GFP-tester strain on a NGM plate; eggs were derived from a one-hour egg-laying window of adult hermaphrodites (midL4 +30 hr). After 4–5 days (when food became exhausted), the fraction of GFP-positive adult individuals was estimated by quantifying the fraction of GFP-positive individuals among a subpopulation of ~200–400 individuals per replicate using a fluorescence dissecting microscope. N=10 replicates per genotype.

Direct competition between JU751 (containing the V530L KCNL-1 variant) and other wild strains (JU651, JU660, JU814, JU820, JU822) isolated from the same compost substrate in Le Perreux-sur-Marne, France (Barrière and Félix, 2007; Billard et al., 2020; Vigne et al., 2021; Fausett et al., 2022; Figure 12E). For each the five strains, 20 freshly laid eggs were mixed with 20 freshly laid eggs from JU751 on a NGM plate; eggs were derived from a 1-hr egg-laying window of adult hermaphrodites (midL4 +30 hr). After 4–5 days (when food became exhausted), all adults present on each plate were transferred to fresh plates to allow for growth for 24 hr, after which the strong egg retention phenotype of JU751 was used to infer genotype frequencies (Vigne et al., 2021). N=10 replicates per strain.

Effect of strong egg retention on progeny protection in response to strong environmental stress (Figures 13 and 14)

We examined the differences in survival of eggs and/or larvae developing ex utero versus in utero in response to different environmental stressors. For ex utero exposure, 10 synchronized mid-L4 +36 hr gravid mothers were dissected to extract eggs, which were transferred together to a single spot next to (but not directly on) the bacterial lawn on a single fresh NGM plate. A drop (20 microlitres) of M9 (control) or M9 containing ethanol or acetic acid in indicated concentrations was then deposited directly on eggs, which was absorbed by the agar in ~15 min. For in utero exposure (in parallel to ex utero exposure), 10 synchronized mid-L4 +36 hr gravid mothers were deposited into a drop (20 microlitres) of M9 containing ethanol or acetic acid in indicated concentrations (which was absorbed by the agar in ~15 min). Mothers were killed within a few minutes; no M9 control treatment as live adults can leave the drop rapidly. Forty-eight hr after the treatment, the number of live larval offspring was counted. For each strain per treatment (ex utero versus in utero), 5–10 replicates were established (Figure 13A and B, Figure 13—figure supplement 1, Figure 14A, Figure 14—figure supplement 1). For experiment shown in Figure 14B and C, single mothers (instead of 10) were picked to individual plates (N=10 per treatment). For the experiment shown in Figure 14C, we used older hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +48 hr) containing larger numbers of internally hatched larvae to compare larval survival ex utero (extracted by dissection) versus in utero (larvae retained in mothers).

Effect of strong egg retention on progeny development and reproduction in response to mild environmental stressors (Figure 15)

Eggs from adult JU751 hermaphrodites (mid-L4 +36 hr) or L1 larvae from mid-L4 +48 hr mothers were exposed either directly (ex utero) or indirectly (in utero) to mild environmental stressors: hyperosmotic stress (0.3M to 1M NaCl) (Figure 15A and B) and 1 M acetic acid (Figure 15C and D). Eggs were then allowed to hatch and develop for 45 hr, at which time we determined their developmental stages. To calculate differences in developmental maturation, we used a semi-quantitative scoring system (Poullet et al., 2016) to assign each developmental stage a specific index as follows: L3=1; early mid-L4=2; midL4=3; lateL4=4; Adult = 5. Lifetime offspring production of individuals descending from eggs and L1 larvae that had survived environmental insult and reached maturity was assayed as described in the material and methods section for Figure 9.

Genome-wide association mapping

GWA mapping was performed using the NemaScan pipeline (Cook et al., 2017; Widmayer et al., 2022; Crombie et al., 2023).

Local haplotype analysis

A neighbour-joining network of a region extending 1 Mb on either side of kcnl-1 gene were established based on the VCF file dataset of CeNDR release 20220216 including 15 focal strains of a total of 48 isotypes. The region flanking the kcnl-1 gene was extracted using the vcftools command line and the 48 isotypes from were then extracted using vcfselectsamples command line from galaxy (https://usegalaxy.org). The distance matrix and unrooted neighbour-joining trees were generated using the vk phylo tree nj command-line from the VCF-kit tools (Cook et al., 2017; Widmayer et al., 2022). Tree figures were made using the software iTOL (v5.6.3) (Letunic and Bork, 2019).

Statistical analyses

Statistical tests were performed using R (R Development Core Team, 2018) and JMP 16.0 software. Data for parametric tests were transformed where necessary to meet the requirements for ANOVA procedures. Box-plots: The median is shown by the horizontal line in the middle of the box, which indicates the 25th to 75th quantiles of the data. The 1.5 interquartile range is indicated by the vertical line.

Acknowledgements

C. elegans strains and substrate samples were kindly provided by the Caenorhabditis Natural Diversity Resource (CaeNDR), Marie-Anne Félix and Jean-Antoine Lepesant. We thank all students and technicians, including Céline Ferrari, Alex Lassagne, Ghada Bouzouida, Nicolas Schwartz, Matthieu Duval, for contributing to egg retention measurements. We thank the team of Erik Andersen, particularly Sam Widmayer, for helping with GWA mapping procedures. For helpful discussion and comments, we thank Kevin M Collins, Henrique Teotónio and the reviewers and editors. We would also like to thank CaeNDR (https://caendr.org) and WormBase (https://wormbase.org/) for providing resources and tools without which the analyses performed here would not have been possible.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Christian Braendle, Email: braendle@unice.fr.

Patrick J Hu, Vanderbilt University Medical Center, United States.

Piali Sengupta, Brandeis University, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • Agence Nationale de la Recherche ANR-22-CE13-0030-02 to Thomas Boulin, Christian Braendle.

  • Agence Nationale de la Recherche ANR-17-CE02-0017 to Christian Braendle.

  • Agence Nationale de la Recherche ANR-15-IDEX-01 to Laure Mignerot, Christian Braendle.

  • Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique to Laure Mignerot, Clotilde Gimond, Lucie Bolelli, Charlotte Bouleau, Asma Sandjak, Thomas Boulin, Christian Braendle.

  • Université Côte d’Azur to Laure Mignerot, Clotilde Gimond, Lucie Bolelli, Charlotte Bouleau, Asma Sandjak, Christian Braendle.

  • Institut National de la Santé et de la Recherche Médicale to Thomas Boulin, Christian Braendle.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review and editing.

Investigation, Methodology.

Investigation, Methodology.

Investigation, Methodology.

Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Resources, Formal analysis, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Project administration.

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. A table of strains used in this study.
elife-88253-supp1.xlsx (17.7KB, xlsx)
MDAR checklist

Data availability

All data generated or analysed in this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source data files have been provided for all figures.

References

  1. Aballay A, Yorgey P, Ausubel FM. Salmonella typhimurium proliferates and establishes a persistent infection in the intestine of Caenorhabditis elegans. Current Biology. 2000;10:1539–1542. doi: 10.1016/S0960-9822(00)00830-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Andersen EC, Gerke JP, Shapiro JA, Crissman JR, Ghosh R, Bloom JS, Félix M-A, Kruglyak L. Chromosome-scale selective sweeps shape Caenorhabditis elegans genomic diversity. Nature Genetics. 2012;44:285–290. doi: 10.1038/ng.1050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Andersen EC, Bloom JS, Gerke JP, Kruglyak L. A variant in the neuropeptide receptor npr-1 is A major determinant of Caenorhabditis elegans growth and physiology. PLOS Genetics. 2014;10:e1004156. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1004156. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Andersen EC, Shimko TC, Crissman JR, Ghosh R. A powerful new quantitative genetics platform, combining Caenorhabditis elegans high-throughput fitness assays with a large collection of recombinant strains. G3: Genes, Genomes, Genetics. 2015;5:911–920. doi: 10.1534/g3.115.017178. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Aprison EZ, Dzitoyeva S, Ruvinsky I. The serotonin circuit that coordinates germline proliferation and egg laying with other reproductive functions in Caenorhabditis elegans. Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 2022;289:20220913. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2022.0913. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Avise J. Evolutionary Perspectives on Pregnancy. Columbia University Press; 2013. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bakker K. An analysis of factors which determine success in competition for food among larvae of Drosophila melanogaster. Archives Néerlandaises de Zoologie. 1961;14:200–281. doi: 10.1163/036551661X00061. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  8. Banerjee N, Bhattacharya R, Gorczyca M, Collins KM, Francis MM. Local neuropeptide signaling modulates serotonergic transmission to shape the temporal organization of C. elegans egg-laying behavior. PLOS Genetics. 2017;13:e1006697. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006697. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Barker DM. Evolution of sperm shortage in a selfing hermaphrodite. Evolution; International Journal of Organic Evolution. 1992;46:1951–1955. doi: 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1992.tb01181.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Barrière A, Félix MA. High local genetic diversity and low outcrossing rate in Caenorhabditis elegans natural populations. Current Biology. 2005;15:1176–1184. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2005.06.022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Barrière A, Félix MA. Temporal dynamics and linkage disequilibrium in natural Caenorhabditis elegans populations. Genetics. 2007;176:999–1011. doi: 10.1534/genetics.106.067223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Ben Soussia I, El Mouridi S, Kang D, Leclercq-Blondel A, Khoubza L, Tardy P, Zariohi N, Gendrel M, Lesage F, Kim EJ, Bichet D, Andrini O, Boulin T. Mutation of a single residue promotes gating of vertebrate and invertebrate two-pore domain potassium channels. Nature Communications. 2019;10:787. doi: 10.1038/s41467-019-08710-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Billard B, Vigne P, Braendle C. A natural mutational event uncovers a life history trade-off via hormonal pleiotropy. Current Biology. 2020;30:4142–4154. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2020.08.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Blackburn DG. Viviparity and Oviparity: Evolution and Reproductive Strategies. Academic Press; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  15. Blackburn DG. Evolution of vertebrate viviparity and specializations for fetal nutrition: A quantitative and qualitative analysis. Journal of Morphology. 2015;276:961–990. doi: 10.1002/jmor.20272. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bleu J, Heulin B, Haussy C, Meylan S, Massot M. Experimental evidence of early costs of reproduction in conspecific viviparous and oviparous lizards. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2012;25:1264–1274. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2012.02518.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Branicky R, Miyazaki H, Strange K, Schafer WR. The voltage-gated anion channels encoded by clh-3 regulate egg laying in C. elegans by modulating motor neuron excitability. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2014;34:764–775. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3112-13.2014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Brewer JC, Olson AC, Collins KM, Koelle MR. Serotonin and neuropeptides are both released by the HSN command neuron to initiate Caenorhabditis elegans egg laying. PLOS Genetics. 2019;15:e1007896. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1007896. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Burton NO, Willis A, Fisher K, Braukmann F, Price J, Stevens L, Baugh LR, Reinke A, Miska EA. Intergenerational adaptations to stress are evolutionarily conserved, stress-specific, and have deleterious trade-offs. eLife. 2021;10:e73425. doi: 10.7554/eLife.73425. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Carnell L, Illi J, Hong SW, McIntire SL. The G-protein-coupled serotonin receptor SER-1 regulates egg laying and male mating behaviors in Caenorhabditis elegans. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2005;25:10671–10681. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3399-05.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Chen J, Caswell-Chen EP. Why Caenorhabditis elegans adults sacrifice their bodies to progeny. Nematology. 2003;5:641–645. doi: 10.1163/156854103322683355. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  22. Chen J, Caswell-Chen EP. Facultative vivipary is a life-history trait in Caenorhabditis elegans. Journal of Nematology. 2004;36:107–113. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Chen YC, Seyedsayamdost MR, Ringstad N. A microbial metabolite synergizes with endogenous serotonin to trigger C. elegans reproductive behavior. PNAS. 2020;117:30589–30598. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2017918117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Clutton-Brock TH. The Evolution of Parental Care. Princeton University Press; 1991. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Collins KM, Bode A, Fernandez RW, Tanis JE, Brewer JC, Creamer MS, Koelle MR. Activity of the C. elegans egg-laying behavior circuit is controlled by competing activation and feedback inhibition. eLife. 2016;5:e21126. doi: 10.7554/eLife.21126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Cook DE, Zdraljevic S, Roberts JP, Andersen EC. CeNDR, the Caenorhabditis elegans natural diversity resource. Nucleic Acids Research. 2017;45:D650–D657. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkw893. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Crombie TA, Zdraljevic S, Cook DE, Tanny RE, Brady SC, Wang Y, Evans KS, Hahnel S, Lee D, Rodriguez BC, Zhang G, van der Zwagg J, Kiontke K, Andersen EC. Deep sampling of Hawaiian Caenorhabditis elegans reveals high genetic diversity and admixture with global populations. eLife. 2019;8:e50465. doi: 10.7554/eLife.50465. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Crombie TA, McKeown R, Moya ND, Evans KS, Widmayer SJ, LaGrassa V, Roman N, Tursunova O, Zhang G, Gibson SB, Buchanan CM, Roberto NM, Vieira R, Tanny RE, Andersen EC. CaeNDR, the caenorhabditis natural diversity resource. Nucleic Acids Res. 2023;45:D650–D657. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkad887. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Cutter AD. Sperm-limited fecundity in nematodes: how many sperm are enough? Evolution. 2004;58:651–655. doi: 10.1111/j.0014-3820.2004.tb01687.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Daniels SA, Ailion M, Thomas JH, Sengupta P. egl-4 acts through a transforming growth factor-beta/SMAD pathway in Caenorhabditis elegans to regulate multiple neuronal circuits in response to sensory cues. Genetics. 2000;156:123–141. doi: 10.1093/genetics/156.1.123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Dempsey CM, Mackenzie SM, Gargus A, Blanco G, Sze JY. Serotonin (5HT), fluoxetine, imipramine and dopamine target distinct 5HT receptor signaling to modulate Caenorhabditis elegans egg-laying behavior. Genetics. 2005;169:1425–1436. doi: 10.1534/genetics.104.032540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Desai C, Horvitz HR. Caenorhabditis elegans mutants defective in the functioning of the motor neurons responsible for egg laying. Genetics. 1989;121:703–721. doi: 10.1093/genetics/121.4.703. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Dey S, Proulx SR, Teotónio H. Adaptation to temporally fluctuating environments by the evolution of maternal effects. PLOS Biology. 2016;14:e1002388. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.1002388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Dong MQ, Chase D, Patikoglou GA, Koelle MR. Multiple RGS proteins alter neural G protein signaling to allow C. elegans to rapidly change behavior when fed. Genes & Development. 2000;14:2003–2014. doi: 10.1101/gad.14.16.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Evans KS, van MH, Mcgrath PT, Andersen EC, Sterken MG. From QTL to gene: C. elegans facilitates discoveries of the genetic mechanisms underlying natural variation. Trends Genet. 2021;37:933–947. doi: 10.1016/j.tig.2021.06.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Fausett S, Poullet N, Gimond C, Vielle A, Bellone M, Braendle C. Germ cell apoptosis is critical to maintain Caenorhabditis elegans offspring viability in stressful environments. PLOS ONE. 2021;16:e0260573. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0260573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Fausett SR, Sandjak A, Billard B, Braendle C. Higher-order epistasis shapes natural variation in germ stem cell niche activity. bioRxiv. 2022 doi: 10.1101/2022.11.04.515229. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  38. Félix MA, Braendle C. The natural history of Caenorhabditis elegans. Current Biology. 2010;20:R965–R969. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2010.09.050. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Fenk LA, de Bono M. Environmental CO2 inhibits Caenorhabditis elegans egg-laying by modulating olfactory neurons and evokes widespread changes in neural activity. PNAS. 2015;112:E3525–E3534. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1423808112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Fernandez RW, Wei K, Wang EY, Mikalauskaite D, Olson A, Pepper J, Christie N, Kim S, Weissenborn S, Sarov M, Koelle MR. Cellular expression and functional roles of all 26 neurotransmitter GPCRs in the C. elegans egg-laying circuit. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2020;40:7475–7488. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1357-20.2020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Flatt T, Heyland A. Mechanisms of Life History Evolution: The Genetics and Physiology of Life History Traits and Trade-Offs. Oxford University Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  42. Frézal L, Félix MA. C. elegans outside the Petri dish. eLife. 2015;4:e05849. doi: 10.7554/eLife.05849. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Garsin DA, Sifri CD, Mylonakis E, Qin X, Singh KV, Murray BE, Calderwood SB, Ausubel FM. A simple model host for identifying Gram-positive virulence factors. PNAS. 2001;98:10892–10897. doi: 10.1073/pnas.191378698. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Gibson G, Dworkin I. Uncovering cryptic genetic variation. Nature Reviews. Genetics. 2004;5:681–690. doi: 10.1038/nrg1426. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Gilbert KJ, Zdraljevic S, Cook DE, Cutter AD, Andersen EC, Baer CF. The distribution of mutational effects on fitness in Caenorhabditis elegans inferred from standing genetic variation. Genetics. 2022;220:iyab166. doi: 10.1093/genetics/iyab166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Gimond C, Vielle A, Silva-Soares N, Zdraljevic S, McGrath PT, Andersen EC, Braendle C. Natural variation and genetic determinants of Caenorhabditis elegans sperm size. Genetics. 2019;213:615–632. doi: 10.1534/genetics.119.302462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Hall DH, Altun ZF. C. elegans Atlas. Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory Press; 2007. [Google Scholar]
  48. Hapiak VM, Hobson RJ, Hughes L, Smith K, Harris G, Condon C, Komuniecki P, Komuniecki RW. Dual excitatory and inhibitory serotonergic inputs modulate egg laying in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics. 2009;181:153–163. doi: 10.1534/genetics.108.096891. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Hobson RJ, Hapiak VM, Xiao H, Buehrer KL, Komuniecki PR, Komuniecki RW. SER-7, a Caenorhabditis elegans 5-HT7-like receptor, is essential for the 5-HT stimulation of pharyngeal pumping and egg laying. Genetics. 2006;172:159–169. doi: 10.1534/genetics.105.044495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Hodgkin J, Barnes TM. More is not better: brood size and population growth in a self-fertilizing nematode. Proceedings. Biological Sciences. 1991;246:19–24. doi: 10.1098/rspb.1991.0119. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Horváth B, Kalinka AT. The genetics of egg retention and fertilization success in Drosophila: One step closer to understanding the transition from facultative to obligate viviparity. Evolution; International Journal of Organic Evolution. 2018;72:318–336. doi: 10.1111/evo.13411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Horvitz HR, Chalfie M, Trent C, Sulston JE, Evans PD. Serotonin and octopamine in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Science. 1982;216:1012–1014. doi: 10.1126/science.6805073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Jafari G, Xie Y, Kullyev A, Liang B, Sze JY. Regulation of extrasynaptic 5-HT by serotonin reuptake transporter function in 5-HT-absorbing neurons underscores adaptation behavior in Caenorhabditis elegans. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2011;31:8948–8957. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1692-11.2011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Johnston WL, Dennis JW. The eggshell in the C. elegans oocyte-to-embryo transition. Genesis. 2012;50:333–349. doi: 10.1002/dvg.20823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Kalinka AT. How did viviparity originate and evolve? Of conflict, co-option, and cryptic choice. BioEssays. 2015;37:721–731. doi: 10.1002/bies.201400200. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Koelle MR. Neurotransmitter signaling through heterotrimeric G proteins: insights from studies in C. elegans. WormBook. 2018;2018:1–52. doi: 10.1895/wormbook.1.75.2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Kopchock RJ, Ravi B, Bode A, Collins KM. The sex-specific vc neurons are mechanically activated motor neurons that facilitate serotonin-induced egg laying in C. elegans. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2021;41:3635–3650. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2150-20.2021. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Kosinski M, McDonald K, Schwartz J, Yamamoto I, Greenstein D. C. elegans sperm bud vesicles to deliver a meiotic maturation signal to distant oocytes. Development. 2005;132:3357–3369. doi: 10.1242/dev.01916. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Kullyev A, Dempsey CM, Miller S, Kuan CJ, Hapiak VM, Komuniecki RW, Griffin CT, Sze JY. A genetic survey of fluoxetine action on synaptic transmission in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics. 2010;186:929–941. doi: 10.1534/genetics.110.118877. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Large EE, Padmanabhan R, Watkins KL, Campbell RF, Xu W, McGrath PT. Modeling of a negative feedback mechanism explains antagonistic pleiotropy in reproduction in domesticated Caenorhabditis elegans strains. PLOS Genetics. 2017;13:e1006769. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006769. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Lee D, Zdraljevic S, Cook DE, Frézal L, Hsu JC, Sterken MG, Riksen JAG, Wang J, Kammenga JE, Braendle C, Félix MA, Schroeder FC, Andersen EC. Selection and gene flow shape niche-associated variation in pheromone response. Nature Ecology & Evolution. 2019;3:1455–1463. doi: 10.1038/s41559-019-0982-3. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Lee D, Zdraljevic S, Stevens L, Wang Y, Tanny RE, Crombie TA, Cook DE, Webster AK, Chirakar R, Baugh LR, Sterken MG, Braendle C, Félix MA, Rockman MV, Andersen EC. Balancing selection maintains hyper-divergent haplotypes in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature Ecology & Evolution. 2021;5:794–807. doi: 10.1038/s41559-021-01435-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Letunic I, Bork P. Interactive Tree Of Life (iTOL) v4: recent updates and new developments. Nucleic Acids Research. 2019;47:W256–W259. doi: 10.1093/nar/gkz239. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Loer CM, Rivard L. Evolution of neuronal patterning in free-living rhabditid nematodes I: Sex-specific serotonin-containing neurons. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2007;502:736–767. doi: 10.1002/cne.21288. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Mackay TFC, Stone EA, Ayroles JF. The genetics of quantitative traits: challenges and prospects. Nature Reviews Genetics. 2009;10:565–577. doi: 10.1038/nrg2612. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Markow TA, Beall S, Matzkin LM. Egg size, embryonic development time and ovoviviparity in Drosophila species. Journal of Evolutionary Biology. 2009;22:430–434. doi: 10.1111/j.1420-9101.2008.01649.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Maupas E. Modes et Formes de Reproduction Des Nématodes. Worldcat; 1900. [Google Scholar]
  68. McCarter J, Bartlett B, Dang T, Schedl T. Soma-germ cell interactions in Caenorhabditis elegans: multiple events of hermaphrodite germline development require the somatic sheath and spermathecal lineages. Developmental Biology. 1997;181:121–143. doi: 10.1006/dbio.1996.8429. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. McMullen PD, Aprison EZ, Winter PB, Amaral LAN, Morimoto RI, Ruvinsky I. Macro-level modeling of the response of C. elegans reproduction to chronic heat stress. PLOS Computational Biology. 2012;8:e1002338. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1002338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Medrano E, Collins KM. Muscle-directed mechanosensory feedback activates egg-laying circuit activity and behavior in Caenorhabditis elegans. Current Biology. 2023;33:2330–2339. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2023.05.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Meier R, Kotrba M, Ferrar P. Ovoviviparity and viviparity in the Diptera. Biological Reviews. 1999;74:199–258. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-185X.1999.tb00186.x. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Mok DZL, Sternberg PW, Inoue T. Morphologically defined sub-stages of C. elegans vulval development in the fourth larval stage. BMC Developmental Biology. 2015;15:26. doi: 10.1186/s12861-015-0076-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Mueller LD, Bitner K. The evolution of ovoviviparity in a temporally varying environment. The American Naturalist. 2015;186:708–715. doi: 10.1086/683661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Ostrovsky AN, Lidgard S, Gordon DP, Schwaha T, Genikhovich G, Ereskovsky AV. Matrotrophy and placentation in invertebrates: a new paradigm. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society. 2016;91:673–711. doi: 10.1111/brv.12189. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Padilla PA, Nystul TG, Zager RA, Johnson ACM, Roth MB. Dephosphorylation of cell cycle-regulated proteins correlates with anoxia-induced suspended animation in Caenorhabditis elegans. Molecular Biology of the Cell. 2002;13:1473–1483. doi: 10.1091/mbc.01-12-0594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Pickett CL, Kornfeld K. Age-related degeneration of the egg-laying system promotes matricidal hatching in Caenorhabditis elegans. Aging Cell. 2013;12:544–553. doi: 10.1111/acel.12079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Poullet N, Vielle A, Gimond C, Carvalho S, Teotónio H, Braendle C. Complex heterochrony underlies the evolution of Caenorhabditis elegans hermaphrodite sex allocation. Evolution; International Journal of Organic Evolution. 2016;70:2357–2369. doi: 10.1111/evo.13032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Ranganathan R, Sawin ER, Trent C, Horvitz HR. Mutations in the Caenorhabditis elegans serotonin reuptake transporter MOD-5 reveal serotonin-dependent and -independent activities of fluoxetine. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2001;21:5871–5884. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-16-05871.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Ravi B, Garcia J, Collins KM. Homeostatic feedback modulates the development of two-state patterned activity in a model serotonin motor circuit in Caenorhabditis elegans. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38:6283–6298. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3658-17.2018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Ravi B, Zhao J, Chaudhry SI, Signorelli R, Bartole M, Kopchock RJ, Guijarro C, Kaplan JM, Kang L, Collins KM. Presynaptic Gαo (GOA-1) signals to depress command neuron excitability and allow stretch-dependent modulation of egg laying in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics. 2021;218:iyab080. doi: 10.1093/genetics/iyab080. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. R Development Core Team . Vienna, Austria: R Foundation for Statistical Computing; 2018. https://www.r-project.org/ [Google Scholar]
  82. Recknagel H, Carruthers M, Yurchenko AA, Nokhbatolfoghahai M, Kamenos NA, Bain MM, Elmer KR. The functional genetic architecture of egg-laying and live-bearing reproduction in common lizards. Nature Ecology & Evolution. 2021;5:1546–1556. doi: 10.1038/s41559-021-01555-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Reiner DJ, Weinshenker D, Thomas JH. Analysis of dominant mutations affecting muscle excitation in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics. 1995;141:961–976. doi: 10.1093/genetics/141.3.961. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Ringstad N, Horvitz HR. FMRFamide neuropeptides and acetylcholine synergistically inhibit egg-laying by C. elegans. Nature Neuroscience. 2008;11:1168–1176. doi: 10.1038/nn.2186. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Sandhu A, Badal D, Sheokand R, Tyagi S, Singh V. Specific collagens maintain the cuticle permeability barrier in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics. 2021;217:iyaa047. doi: 10.1093/genetics/iyaa047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Schafer WR, Kenyon CJ. A calcium-channel homologue required for adaptation to dopamine and serotonin in Caenorhabditis elegans. Nature. 1995;375:73–78. doi: 10.1038/375073a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Schafer WR, Sanchez BM, Kenyon CJ. Genes affecting sensitivity to serotonin in Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics. 1996;143:1219–1230. doi: 10.1093/genetics/143.3.1219. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Schafer WR. Egg-laying. WormBook. 2005;1:7. doi: 10.1895/wormbook.1.38.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Schafer WR. Genetics of egg-laying in worms. Annual Review of Genetics. 2006;40:487–509. doi: 10.1146/annurev.genet.40.110405.090527. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Schierenberg E, Junkersdorf B. The role of eggshell and underlying vitelline membrane for normal pattern formation in the early C. elegans embryo. Roux’s Archives of Developmental Biology. 1992;202:10–16. doi: 10.1007/BF00364592. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Schindelin J, Arganda-Carreras I, Frise E, Kaynig V, Longair M, Pietzsch T, Preibisch S, Rueden C, Saalfeld S, Schmid B, Tinevez JY, White DJ, Hartenstein V, Eliceiri K, Tomancak P, Cardona A. Fiji: an open-source platform for biological-image analysis. Nature Methods. 2012;9:676–682. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Schulenburg H, Félix MA. The natural biotic environment of Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics. 2017;206:55–86. doi: 10.1534/genetics.116.195511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Shyn SI, Kerr R, Schafer WR. Serotonin and Go modulate functional states of neurons and muscles controlling C. elegans egg-laying behavior. Current Biology. 2003;13:1910–1915. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2003.10.025. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Stein KK, Golden A. The C. elegans eggshell. WormBook. 2018;2018:1–36. doi: 10.1895/wormbook.1.179.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Stevens L, Félix MA, Beltran T, Braendle C, Caurcel C, Fausett S, Fitch D, Frézal L, Gosse C, Kaur T, Kiontke K, Newton MD, Noble LM, Richaud A, Rockman MV, Sudhaus W, Blaxter M. Comparative genomics of 10 new Caenorhabditis species. Evolution Letters. 2019;3:217–236. doi: 10.1002/evl3.110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Stiernagle T. Maintenance of C. elegans. WormBook. 2006;1:11. doi: 10.1895/wormbook.1.101.1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Sudhaus W. Vergleichende Untersuchungen Zur Phylogenie, Systematik, Ökologie, Biologie Und Ethologie Der Rhabditidae (Nematoda) Zoologica; 1976. [Google Scholar]
  98. Sulston JE, Schierenberg E, White JG, Thomson JN. The embryonic cell lineage of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Developmental Biology. 1983;100:64–119. doi: 10.1016/0012-1606(83)90201-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Trent C. Genetic and Behavioral Studies of the Egg-Laying System of Caenorhabditis elegans. Massachusetts Institute of Technology; 1982. [Google Scholar]
  100. Trent C, Tsuing N, Horvitz HR. Egg-laying defective mutants of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Genetics. 1983;104:619–647. doi: 10.1093/genetics/104.4.619. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Trexler JC, DeAngelis DL. Resource allocation in offspring provisioning: an evaluation of the conditions favoring the evolution of matrotrophy. The American Naturalist. 2003;162:574–585. doi: 10.1086/378822. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Trivers RL. Parent-Offspring Conflict. American Zoologist. 1974;14:249–264. doi: 10.1093/icb/14.1.249. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  103. Van Voorhies WA, Ward S. Broad oxygen tolerance in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. The Journal of Experimental Biology. 2000;203:2467–2478. doi: 10.1242/jeb.203.16.2467. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Vielle A, Callemeyn-Torre N, Gimond C, Poullet N, Gray JC, Cutter AD, Braendle C. Convergent evolution of sperm gigantism and the developmental origins of sperm size variability in Caenorhabditis nematodes. Evolution. 2016;70:2485–2503. doi: 10.1111/evo.13043. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Vigne P, Gimond C, Ferrari C, Vielle A, Hallin J, Pino-Querido A, El Mouridi S, Mignerot L, Frøkjær-Jensen C, Boulin T, Teotónio H, Braendle C. A single-nucleotide change underlies the genetic assimilation of A plastic trait. Science Advances. 2021;7:eabd9941. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abd9941. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Waggoner LE, Zhou GT, Schafer RW, Schafer WR. Control of alternative behavioral states by serotonin in Caenorhabditis elegans. Neuron. 1998;21:203–214. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)80527-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. Waggoner LE, Hardaker LA, Golik S, Schafer WR. Effect of a neuropeptide gene on behavioral states in Caenorhabditis elegans egg-laying. Genetics. 2000;154:1181–1192. doi: 10.1093/genetics/154.3.1181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Ward S, Carrel JS. Fertilization and sperm competition in the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Developmental Biology. 1979;73:304–321. doi: 10.1016/0012-1606(79)90069-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Weinshenker D, Garriga G, Thomas JH. Genetic and pharmacological analysis of neurotransmitters controlling egg laying in C. elegans. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1995;15:6975–6985. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.15-10-06975.1995. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Weinshenker D, Wei A, Salkoff L, Thomas JH. Block of an ether-a-go-go-like K(+) channel by imipramine rescues egl-2 excitation defects in Caenorhabditis elegans. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1999;19:9831–9840. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-22-09831.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. White JG, Southgate E, Thomson JN, Brenner S. The structure of the nervous system of the nematode Caenorhabditis elegans. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. B, Biological Sciences. 1986;314:1–340. doi: 10.1098/rstb.1986.0056. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Whittington CM, Van Dyke JU, Liang SQT, Edwards SV, Shine R, Thompson MB, Grueber CE. Understanding the evolution of viviparity using intraspecific variation in reproductive mode and transitional forms of pregnancy. Biological Reviews of the Cambridge Philosophical Society. 2022;97:1179–1192. doi: 10.1111/brv.12836. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Widmayer SJ, Evans KS, Zdraljevic S, Andersen EC. Evaluating the power and limitations of genome-wide association studies in Caenorhabditis elegans. G3. 2022;12:jkac114. doi: 10.1093/g3journal/jkac114. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Yue X, Zhao J, Li X, Fan Y, Duan D, Zhang X, Zou W, Sheng Y, Zhang T, Yang Q, Luo J, Duan S, Xiao R, Kang L. TMC proteins modulate egg laying and membrane excitability through a background leak conductance in C. elegans. Neuron. 2018;97:571–585. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2017.12.041. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Zhang M, Chung SH, Fang-Yen C, Craig C, Kerr RA, Suzuki H, Samuel ADT, Mazur E, Schafer WR. A self-regulating feed-forward circuit controlling C. elegans egg-laying behavior. Current Biology. 2008;18:1445. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2008.08.047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Zhao Y, Long L, Xu W, Campbell RF, Large EE, Greene JS, McGrath PT. Changes to social feeding behaviors are not sufficient for fitness gains of the Caenorhabditis elegans N2 reference strain. eLife. 2018;7:e38675. doi: 10.7554/eLife.38675. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

eLife assessment

Patrick J Hu 1

This important work provides a thorough and detailed analysis of natural variation in C. elegans egg-laying behavior. The authors present convincing evidence to support their hypothesis that variations in egg-laying behavior are influenced by trade-offs between maternal and offspring fitness. This study establishes a framework for elucidating the molecular mechanisms underlying this paradigm of behavioral evolution.

Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

Anonymous

Mignerot et al. performed a Herculean effort to measure and describe natural variation in C. elegans egg-laying behavior and egg retention. The paper is well written and organized. The authors show wild strains vary in egg retention with some extremes that appear phenotypically similar to species with viviparity (or live birth / internal hatching of offspring). They previously published a rare variant in the gene kcnl-1 that plays a role in egg retention but identify common variants in this study. They classify wild strains based on egg-retention to separate out the extremely different isolates. Egg laying has been extensively studied in the laboratory strain N2, but rarely addressed in natural strains. The authors investigate egg-laying behaviors using standard assays and find that their classified egg-laying groups have differences in sub-behaviors suggesting diverse roles in the ultimate egg-laying output. Then, they turn to the egg-laying circuit using both exogenous serotonin (5-HT), 5-HT modulatory drugs (e.g. SSRIs), and even genome editing to test epistasis with the mod-5 5-HT reuptake. The effects of 5-HT modulation and mutants are not predictive based on the basal behaviors and egg-retention phenotypes with the most extreme egg-retention strains differing in their responses. Interestingly, strains with more egg retention have decreased fitness (in their laboratory) measures but also provide a protective environment for offspring when exposed to common "natural" stressors. Their final conclusion that egg retention could be a trade-off between antagonistic effects of maternal vs. offspring fitness is supported well and sets the stage for future mechanistic studies across Caenorhabditis.

Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

Anonymous

Mignerot et al. study variations in egg retention in a large set of wild C. elegans strains use detailed analysis of a subset of these strains to those that these variations in egg retention appear to arise from variations in egg-laying behavior. The authors then take advantage of the advanced genetic technology available in C. elegans, and the fact that the cellular and molecular mechanisms that drive egg-laying behavior in the N2 laboratory strain of C. elegans have been studied intensely for decades. Thus, they demonstrate that variations multiple genetic loci appear to drive variations in egg laying across species, although they are unable to identify the specific genes that vary other than a potassium channel already identified in a previous study from some of these same authors (Vigne et al., 2021). Mignerot et al. also present evidence that variations in response of the egg-laying system to the neuromodulator serotonin appear to underlie variations in egg-laying behavior across species. Finally, the authors present a series of studies examining how the retention of eggs in utero affects the fertility and survival of mothers versus the survival of their progeny in a variety of adverse conditions, including limiting food, and the presence of acute environmental insults such as alcohol or acid. The results suggest that variations in egg-laying behavior evolved as a response to adverse environmental conditions that impose a trade-off between survival of the mothers versus their progeny.

Strengths:

The analysis of variations in egg laying by a large set of wild species significantly extends the previous work of Vigne et al. (2021), who focused on just one wild variant strain. Mignerot find that variations in egg laying are widespread across C. elegans strains and result from changes in multiple genetic loci.

To determine why various strains vary in their egg-laying behavior, the authors take advantage the genetic tractability of C. elegans and the huge body of previous studies on the cellular and molecular basis of egg-laying behavior in the laboratory N2 strain. Since serotonin is one signal that induces egg laying, the authors subject various strains to serotonin and to drugs thought to alter serotonin signaling, and they also use CRISPR induced gene editing to mutate a serotonin reuptake transporter in some strains. The results are largely consistent with the idea that variations across strains alter how the egg-laying system responds to serotonin.

The final figures in the paper presents a far more detailed analysis than did Vigne et al. (2021) of how variations in egg retention across species can affect fitness under various environmental stresses. Thus, Mignerot et al. look at competition under conditions of limiting food, and response to acute environmental insults, and compare the ability of adults, in utero eggs, and ex vivo eggs to survive. The results lead to an interesting discussion of how variations in behavior result in a trade-off in survival of mothers versus their progeny. The authors in their Discussion do a good job describing the challenges in interpreting the relevance of these laboratory results to the poorly-understood environmental conditions that C. elegans may experience in the wild. The Discussion also had an excellent section about how the ability of a single species to strongly regulate egg-laying behavior in response to its environment, and how this ability could be adaptive. Overall, these were the strongest and most interesting aspects of Mignerot et al.

Weaknesses

The specific potassium channel variation studied by Vigne et al. (2021) has by far the strongest effect on egg laying seen in the Mignerot et al. study and remains the only genetic variation that has been molecularly identified. So, Mignerot et al. were not able to identify any additional specific genes that vary across species to cause changes in egg laying, and this limited their ability to generate new insights into the specific cellular and molecular mechanisms that have changed across species to result in changes in egg laying behavior.

The authors' use of drug treatments and CRISPR to alter serotonin signaling yielded some insights into mechanistic variations in how the egg-laying system functions across strains, but these experiments only allow very indirect inferences into what is going on. The analysis in Figures 4 and 5 generates a complex set of results that are not easy to interpret. The clearest result seems to be that strains carrying the KCNL-1 point mutation lay eggs poorly and exogenous serotonin inhibits rather than stimulates egg laying in these strains. This basic result was to a large extent reported previously in Vigne et al. 2021.

The analysis of how differences between strains mechanistically result in changes in egg-laying behavior and egg retention, while excellent in concept, is only modestly successful. The analysis of the temporal pattern egg-laying behavior in Figure 3B-3F is relatively weak. Whereas the state of the art in analyzing this behavior is to take videos of animals and track exactly when they lay eggs, analyzing 40 or more hours of behavior per strain, the authors used a lower-tech method of just examining how many eggs were laid within 5-minute intervals over a period of just three hours per strain. While this analysis was sufficient to demonstrate some statistically significant differences in the pattern of egg laying in some strains, it is unclear to what extent these differences could be sufficient to explain the differences in accumulation of unlaid eggs between these strains. In contrast, the variations in age of the onset of egg-laying behavior in Fig 3G and 3H between strains were very strong and may be more likely to reflect mechanistic differences in how egg laying is controlled that could result in the differences in retention of unlaid eggs seen among the strains tested. In the Discussion, the authors extensively write about the work of the Collins lab showing that retained eggs stretch the uterus to produce a signal that activates egg-laying muscles. Could it be that really this mechanism is the main one that varies between strains, leading to the observed variations in time to laying the first egg as well as variations in the number of retained eggs throughout adulthood?

eLife. 2024 Apr 2;12:RP88253. doi: 10.7554/eLife.88253.3.sa3

Author Response

Laure Mignerot 1, Clotilde Gimond 2, Lucie Bolelli 3, Charlotte Bouleau 4, Asma Sandjak 5, Thomas Boulin 6, Christian Braendle 7

The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

Reviewer #1:

1. Can the authors statistically define the egg-laying classes? In some parts of the manuscript, the division between the different classes could be more ambiguous. I understand that the class III strains are divided by the kcnl-1 genotype, but given the different results for diverse traits, it could be more clear to keep them as one class. Also, overall, the authors choose a collection of 15 strains across the different classes to phenotype for many traits and perform genome edits. It is understandable that they cannot test all strains, but given the variation across traits and classes, it might be good to add a few more caveats about how these strains might not be representative of all strains across the species.

Response: The egg-laying classes were defined as in Figure 1A by arbitrarily chosen cut-offs (at 10, 10-25, and 25 eggs in utero) to simplify subsequent analyses. We added this explanation to the first paragraph of the results section. However, the differences in average egg retention are significantly different between the four defined classes using the 15 selected strains (Fig. 2A).

We think that the distinction between Class IIIA and IIIB strains is important and justified because the two Classes significantly differ in mean egg retention (Fig. 2A) and because Class IIIB harbour the large-effect variant KCNL-1 V530L whereas Class IIIA do not.

We agree that the 15 selected strains are not necessarily representative of all strains across the species. We have added a note of caution regarding this point to the first paragraph of the section “Temporal progression of egg retention and internal hatching”: “Note that this strain selection, especially concerning the largest Class II, is unlikely to reflect the overall strain diversity observed across the species". In addition, we have reworded the first sentence of this paragraph as follows: “ To better characterize natural variation in C. elegans egg retention, we focused on a subset of 15 strains from divergent phenotypic Classes I-III, with an emphasis on Class III strains exhibiting strong egg retention (at mid-L4 + 30h) (Fig. 2A and 2B).”

2. For the GWAS experiments, the authors should describe if any of the QTL overlap with hyper-divergent regions in the strain set. The QTL could be driven by these less well defined regions.

Response: We have added the following sentence: “The three QTLs do not align with any of the recently identified hyper-divergent regions of the genome (Lee et al., 2021).

3. The authors should look at correlations between the mod-5(n822) edit phenotypes and the exogenous 5-HT and SSRI phenotypes to demonstrate how the traits can differ. Some correlation plots might help that point as well.

Response: We examined all possible correlations as suggested: none are significant and strain effects on trait differences are idiosyncratic, as written in our results section. The correlational analyses remain of limited value due to small samples: N=10 for mean strain values for measured phenotypes. We therefore feel that these analyses do not provide any additional insights beyond our figures (4C, 4D, 5C, 5D, S5A-C ) and our statement on page 15: “As in previous experiments (Fig. 4C and 5C), we find again that strains sharing the same egg retention phenotype may differ strongly in egg-laying behaviour in response to modulation of both exo- and endogenous serotonin levels (Class IIIA: ED3005 and JU2829) (Fig. 5D and S5C).”

4. Figure 6D, was there any censoring of the data? Normally, these types of studies are plagued by an increase in censored animals that can decrease significance. The effects among the classes seem large, but statistical comparisons might help as well.

Response: There was no censoring of animals (censoring of animals in lifespan studies is usually done by removing “bags of worms”, which here was our study phenotype). We now mention this in the corresponding figure legend. We also added a statistical analysis showing that mean survival was significantly different between all Classes.

5. Many of the traits, edits, and deeper analyses are performed on the JU751 genetic background. This choice is sensible, otherwise, the work can increase exponentially. However, the authors should add a caveat about how these results might be limited to JU751 and other strains might respond differently.

Response: For certain experiments, it was not feasible to include multiple strains from all phenotypic classes, so we selected JU751 (Class IIIB) and JU1200 (Class II), for which we had established CRISPR-engineered lines to modulate the egg retention phenotype by a single amino acid change in KCNL-1. To emphasize that these experimental observations cannot be generalized, we added the following statement in the relevant results section: “These experimental results offer preliminary evidence (bearing in mind that our analysis was primarily centered on a single genetic background) that laying of advanced-stage embryos may enhance intraspecific competitive ability, particularly in scenarios where multiple genotypes compete for colonization and exploitation of limited, patchily distributed resources.”

6. The authors argue that evolution could be acting on specific parts of the egg-laying machinery (e.g., muscledirected signaling components). It might be useful to look at levels of standing variation and selection at groups of loci compared to genomic controls to see if this conclusion can be strengthened.

Response: This is a good idea but how to select pertinent candidate loci is unclear (there are over 300 genes with effects on egg laying, http://www.wormbase.org). In addition, the genetics of muscle-directed signalling components in egg laying is only starting to be explored, with no specific candidate genes having been identified (Medrano & Collins, 2023, Curr Biol). We therefore think that such an analysis is currently not possible.

7. Completely optional: The authors present a compelling and interesting case for transitions and trade-offs between oviparity and viviparity. The C. vivipara species has a different egg-laying mode than other Caenorhabditis species. The authors could add a short section describing their expectations about the neuronal morphology, 5-HT circuits, and muscle function in this species given their results. What genes or circuits should be the focus of future studies to address this question in Caenorhabditis. Also, Loer and Rivard present some similar ideas based on the differences in 5-HT staining neurons across diverse nematodes. Those results can be incorporated and discussed as well.

Response: Our current research focuses on the evolution of egg laying in different Caenorhabditis species. So far, however, it remains difficult to provide specific hypotheses on how the egg-laying circuit has changed in C. vivipara. We rephrased the final paragraph of the discussion to incorporate some of the reviewer’s suggestions: “Nematodes display frequent transitions from oviparity to obligate viviparity in many distinct genera (Sudhaus, 1976; Ostrovsky et al., 2015), including in the genus Caenorhabditis, with at least one viviparous species, C. vivipara (Stevens et al., 2019). Although evidence exists for the evolution of egg-laying circuitry across oviparous Caenorhabditis species (Loer and Rivard, 2007), the specific cellular and genetic changes responsible for the transition to obligate viviparity in C. vivipara have yet to be examined. Resolving the genetic basis of intraspecific variation in C. elegans egg retention, including partial or facultative viviparity, may thus shed light on the molecular changes underlying the initial steps of evolutionary transitions from oviparity to obligate viviparity in invertebrates.”

Specific edits:

1. Perhaps a silly point, but "parity" (to my knowledge) does not have a biological meaning on its own. I suggest "egg-laying mode" or "birth mode".

Response: This term has been used previously in the literature(e.g.https://onlinelibrary.wiley.com/doi/10.1111/jeb.13886 or https://doi.org/10.1101/2023.10.22.563505). However, as the referee rightly points out, this is not a standard term. We therefore replaced “parity mode” with “egg-laying mode”.

2. "Against fluctuating environmental fluctuations" is a bit strange

Response: Corrected.

3. The first publications of Egl mutants were by the Horvitz lab so some citations are not in all of the first descriptions of the trait (early in Results)

Response: We have added the relevant work (Trent 1982, Trent 1983, Desai & Horvitz 1989) to this paragraph in the early results section.

4. "Strong egg retention usually strongly..." is a bit strange

Response: Corrected.

5. Figure 8G font looks smaller than the others.

Response: Corrected.

Reviewer #2:

1. In Figure 1A, I infer that in the graph class I measurements are represented by dark blue dots and class II by purple dots. I am having a really hard time distinguishing between these two colors in the graph. In the pie chart I have no problem, but in the graph the black lines around the colored dots seem to obscure the colors. Not sure how to fix this graphical problem, but it is preventing the graph from communicating the results effectively.

Response: We have changed the colours, spacing and format of this figure to resolve this problem.

2. The behavioral analysis of Figure 3B-3F is problematic. The experimental methods used and the interpretation of the results each have issues. This is cause for concern since this is the most direct analysis of the actual variations in egg-laying behavior across strains presented in this paper.

This experiment is modeled after the work of Waggoner et al. 1998, who recorded egg laying events of individual worms on video over several hours and noted the exact time of individual egg laying events. Waggoner et al. found in the reference C. elegans strain N2 that egg-laying events occurred in ~2 minute clusters ("active phases") separated by ~20 minute silent periods ("inactive phases"). Mignerot et al. did not take continuous videos of animals, but rather examined plates bearing a single worm only every 5 minutes and noted the number of new eggs that appeared on the plate in each 5-minute interval. From these data, the authors claim they have measured the intervals between "egg-laying phases" (the term used in the Figure 3 legend). In the Results, the authors explicitly claim they are measuring the timing and frequency of actual active and inactive egg-laying phases. Apparently, all the eggs laid within one 5-minute interval are considered to have been laid in a single active phase, and the time between 5-minute intervals containing egg laying events is considered an "inactive phase" and is measured only with a resolution of 5 minutes. It is not explained anywhere how the authors handle the situation of seeing eggs laid in two consecutive 5-minute intervals. Is that one active phase that is 10 minutes long, or is that two separate active phases with a 5-minute active phase in between? Because of this ambiguity in how they define active and inactive phases, I find it impossible to understand and judge the data presented in Fig. 3D-3F. The authors in the results state that "Class I and Class IIIB displayed significantly accelerated and reduced egg laying activity respectively (Fig. 3C to 3E)" . I assume they are referring to the statistical analysis described in the figure legend, which is quite difficult to understand. Frankly, just looking at the graphs in Fig. 3D3F, it is hard for the reader to identify specific features shown in the graphs can explain why, for example, Class I strains have fewer retained eggs than Class III strains. So, I found this analysis very unsatisfying.

I also feel the authors are making an unwarranted assumption that their non-N2 strains will have distinguishable active and inactive phases of egg-laying behavior analogous to those seen in the N2 strain. Given the possibly large variations in egg-laying behavior in the various strains examined, that assumption should be questioned. Thus, framing the entire analysis of behavior patterns in terms of the length of active and inactive phases might not be appropriate.

Response: This comment validly highlights important problems and limitations of our scan-sampling method to quantify strain differences in egg-laying behaviour. We acknowledge that we failed to present the data with due diligence, and clarity regarding terminology and interpretation. However, we think that some of these results are still of value after revised presentation. Our biggest mistake was to use the terms “active and inactive phase”, as coined by Waggoner et al. 1998. We are aware that our measures are not equivalent to these previously defined measures but have been sloppy with terminology. We therefore carefully reworded this entire results section, using clear definitions to indicate differences between the Waggoner assay and our assay (including a graphical representation of our assay design in the revised Fig. 3B). In brief, our simplified assay is useful to estimate the frequency and approximate duration of prolonged inactive periods of egg laying because we can unambiguously determine intervals in which eggs were laid or not. In contrast, as pointed out by the reviewer, we cannot determine if multiple active phases occurred within a 5-min interval, nor can we estimate the duration of an active “phase”. We now state this limitation explicitly in the manuscript. What our results do show is that the number of intervals during which egg laying occurred is significantly different between strains and Classes: Class I (low retention) have a higher number of intervals with egg-laying events, whereas Class IIIB showed a reduced number of such events (Fig. 3D). We can therefore also roughly estimate the mean time (per individual) between two egg-laying intervals, giving us a proxy for prolonged periods when egg-laying is inactive (Fig. 3E); we note that our estimate for N2 is very close to what has been previously measured (~20 min). Therefore, we can confidently conclude that there are natural strains which have both shorter (Class I) and longer (Class IIIB) inactive periods of egg laying. These results partly align with observed variation in egg retention. However, we agree with the reviewer – as we had stated both in results and discussion sections – that these behavioural differences act together with differences in the sensing of egg accumulation in utero (as suggested by results shown in Fig. 3G and 3H). We also agree that it seems very plausible that the observed behavioural differences, as revealed by scan-sampling, may only have a secondary role in accounting for natural variation in egg retention. We will be testing these hypotheses specifically in our future research.

Note: The statistical analyses are nested ANOVAs to ask (a) does the value differ between strains within a given class and (b) does the value differ between Classes? Classes labelled with different letters in the figures therefore significantly differ in their mean values, demonstrating that measured behavioural phenotypes consistently differ between some (but not all) phenotypic classes, yet largely in line with their egg retention phenotypes (Fig. 3D and 3E).

3. Figure 4A is a schematic diagram of how the egg-laying circuit works based on previous literature, and the authors cite Collins et al. 2015 and Kopchock et al. 2021 as their sources. One feature of this figure seems unwarranted, namely the part indicating that egg accumulation acts on the UM muscles, and the statement in the legend that "mechanical excitation of uterine muscles (UM) in response to egg accumulation favours exit from the inactive state (Collins et al., 2016)". I believe Collins et al. 2016 showed that egg accumulation favors egg laying and may have speculated that it does so by stretching the um muscles, but this idea remains speculative and has not been established by any experimental data. I point out this issue,in particular, because it may bear on the nice data the authors of this manuscript show in Figure 3G and 3H, which show that some strains accumulate many eggs in the uterus before they initiate egg laying.

Also, in Figure 4A and 4B, the legend does not explain the logic of the green areas labeled "egg-laying active phase" and the yellow area labeled "egg-laying inactive state". I was not sure what sure how to interpret these features of the graphics.

Response: The input from uterine muscles remains indeed hypothetical, and we have corrected the figure accordingly, now simply referring to the feedback of egg accumulation on egg laying activity, as recently characterized in more detail by Medrano & Collins (2023, Curr Biol).

The green/yellow backgrounds shown in figures 4A (and 4B) are not useful and we have removed them.

5. Results, page 11: "We used standard assays, in which animals are reared in liquid M9 buffer without bacterial food." In the standard assays, animals are reared on NGM agar plates with bacterial food, and then at the start of the egg-laying assay, are transferred to liquid M9 buffer without bacterial food. I assume that is what these authors did, and they should correct the language of the text to make it more accurate.

Response: The reviewer is correct. We have incorporated this change to improve accuracy.

6. The authors note that "serotonin induced a much stronger egg-laying responds in the Class IIIA strain ED3005 than in other strains (Fig. 4C)". I would like to point out to the authors that strains such as ED3005 that have a very large number of unlaid eggs in their uterus are prone to lay a very large number of eggs when treated with exogenous serotonin, simply for the trivial reason that they have more eggs to release. This was previously seen in, for example, in Desai and Horvitz (1989) in certain egg-laying defective mutants.

Response: This is an important point and our comparison of ED3005 to ALL other strains is problematic. We changed this result description by stating that ED3005 shows possible serotonin hypersensitivity compared to strains with similar levels of egg retention (Class IIIA): “In addition, serotonin induced a much stronger egg-laying response in the strain ED3005 than in other Class IIIA strains with similar levels of egg retention (Fig. 4B). ED3005 may thus exhibit serotonin hypersensitivity, which has been observed in certain egg-laying mutants where perturbed synaptic transmission impacts serotonin signalling (Schafer and Kenyon, 1995; Schafer et al., 1996).”

7. In Figure 4 the authors show that all strains lay eggs in response to fluoxetine and imipramine, but some strains (Class IIIB) do not lay eggs in response to serotonin. They then cite a series of papers, starting with Trent et al. 1983, that they claim show that this specific phenotype demonstrates that the HSN neurons are functionally releasing serotonin (bottom of page 11). This statement needs to be removed - it is incorrect. It is true that egg laying in response to fluoxetine and/or imipramine AS WELL AS egg laying in response to serotonin has been interpreted as indicating the presence of HSN neurons that functionally release serotonin to stimulate egg laying (these were referred to as Category C by Trent et al., 1983). However, the mutants that Mignerot et al. are talking about (those that don't respond to serotonin but do respond to imipramine/fluoxetine) were called Category D by Trent et al., 1983, and to my knowledge these have never been interpreted as necessarily having functionally intact HSN neurons. Mutants such as these that can lay eggs in some circumstances but cannot lay eggs in response to exogenous serotonin have usually been interpreted as having egg-laying muscles that are defective in responding to serotonin.

How can we interpret strains that respond to imipramine/fluoxetine and not serotonin? Mignerot et al. cite some of the papers (Kullyev et al. 2010; Wenishenker et al., 1999; Yue et al., 2018) showing that imipramine and fluoxetene have off-target effects and can stimulate egg laying by acting through proteins other than the serotonin-reuptake inhibitor. The authors later in their discussion at the top of Page 24 also cite Dempsey et al 2005, a paper that also argues that imipramine and fluoxetene act via off target effects. However, currently in Figure 4B Mignerot et al. emphasize that the serotonin reuptake inhibitor is the target of these drugs. Since the results presented for Class IIIB strains are not in accord with this interpretation, this seems misleading to me. The bottom line for me is that class IIIB strains cannot respond to exogenous serotonin, but can lay eggs in other conditions, so perhaps there is something specifically wrong with their ability to respond to serotonin.

Response: We thank the reviewer for this important comment – we misinterpreted some of these past findings and our statements were either inexact or incorrect. We have revised this section accordingly: “Both drugs also stimulated egg laying in the Class IIIB strains and the Class IIIA strain JU2829 for which exogenous serotonin either inhibited egg laying or had no effect on it (Fig. 4B). In the past, mutants unresponsive to serotonin yet responsive to other drugs, including fluoxetine and imipramine, have been interpreted as being defective in the serotonin response of vulval muscles (Trent et al., 1983; Reiner et al., 1995; Weinshenker et al., 1995). This is indeed the likely case of Class IIIB strains carrying the KCNL-1 V530L variant thought to specifically reduce excitability of vulval muscles (Vigne et al., 2021). Our results therefore suggest that JU2829 (Class IIIA) may exhibit a similar defect in vulval muscle activation via serotonin caused by an alternative genetic change. Overall, these pharmacological assays do not allow us to conclude if and how HSN function has diverged among strains because the mode of action and targets of tested drugs has not been fully resolved. Nevertheless, our results are consistent with previous models proposing that these drugs do not simply block serotonin reuptake but can stimulate egg laying, to some extent, through mechanisms independent of serotonergic signaling (Trent et al., 1983; Desai and Horvitz, 1989; Reiner et al., 1995; Weinshenker et al., 1995, 1999; Dempsey et al., 2005;Kullyev et al., 2010; Branicky et al., 2014; Yue et al., 2018).”

We removed the oversimplified Fig. 4B to avoid any misinterpretation.

8. In Figure 7B and 7C, the authors should add some type of error bars to the graphs to and give the readers an idea of whether the differences between strains that they write about are statistically significant or not.

Response: These are frequency data to describe temporal dynamics of hatching (N=45-72 eggs per strain) (Fig. 7B) and development in single cohorts (N=48-177 eggs per strain) (Fig. 7C), hence, the absence of error bars.

We agree that this representation of the data is not very telling. We therefore changed the data representation in these two figures to show that there are clear, statistically significant, negative correlations between egg retention and time to hatching / egg-to-adult developmental time.

9. When the authors reference a list of papers in a single list, e.g. "(Burton et al., 2021; Fausett et al., 2021; Garsin et al., 2001; Padilla et al., 2002; Van Voorhies and Ward, 2000)" they seem to do so in alphabetical order by the first author's last name. I believe the usual practice is to list references by year of publication, with the earliest first.

Response: We corrected citation style according to eLIFE format.

10. At the top of page 24, the authors write "It seems unlikely, however, that any of these variants strongly alter central function of HSN and HSN-mediated signalling because fluoxetine and imipramine, known to act via HSN (Dempsey et al., 2005; Trent et al., 1983; Weinshenker et al., 1995), triggered a robust stimulatory effect on egg laying in all examined strains (Fig. 4C)." I believe that the Weinshenker paper in fact showed that imipramine does not act via the HSN, and the Dempsey paper suggested that both drugs can act at least in part independently of the HSN. Therefore, the authors should revise their statement.

Response: We have removed the sentence.

Reviewing Editor:

Minor suggestions:

1. p. 2, fifth line from bottom: "lead" instead of "leads";

1. p. 2, last line: "muscle" instead of "muscles";

1. p. 3, first full paragraph, 17th line: "populations" instead of "population";

1. p. 5, fourth line from bottom: Delete first comma;

1. p. 6, Figure 1D: "of" instead of "off";

1. p. 7, fifth line: "KCNL-1";

1. p. 9, third paragraph, second line: please clarify "late mid-L4";

1. p. 16, first line: "exogenous";

1. p 20, first paragraph, beginning of second sentence: "Whether" instead of "If";

1. p. 22, ninth line from bottom: delete "shaped by";

1. p. 23, last paragraph, third and eighth lines from bottom: change "between" to "among"

Response: Thank you. All corrected.

Additional changes:

Figure 5A: We removed figure 5A showing a cartoon of mod-5/SERT and its effects on serotonin signalling. This figure was incorrectly showing that MOD-5 is expressed in HSN (Jafari et al 2011 J. Neuroscience, Hammarlund et al 2018 Neuron).

Abstract: We reworded the abstract to reduce its length.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 1.
    elife-88253-fig1-data1.xlsx (315.9KB, xlsx)
    Figure 1—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 1—figure supplement 1.
    Figure 1—figure supplement 2—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 1—figure supplement 2.
    Figure 2—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 2.
    Figure 3—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 3, Figure 3—figure supplement 1A.
    Figure 3—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 3—figure supplement 1B–F.
    Figure 4—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 4.
    Figure 5—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 5.
    elife-88253-fig5-data1.xlsx (265.7KB, xlsx)
    Figure 6—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 6.
    Figure 7—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 7B and C.
    Figure 7—source data 2. Excel file containing statistical results for analyses of data shown in Figure 7B and C, Figure 7—figure supplement 1.
    Figure 8—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 8, Figure 8—figure supplement 1A and B.
    Figure 8—source data 2. Excel file containing statistical results for analyses of data shown in Figure 8B and Figure 8—figure supplement 1A.
    Figure 9—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 9.
    Figure 10—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 10.
    Figure 11—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 11.
    Figure 11—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 11—figure supplement 1.
    Figure 12—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 12.
    Figure 13—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 13, Figure 13—figure supplement 1A and B.
    Figure 14—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 14, Figure 14—figure supplement 1.
    Figure 15—source data 1. Excel file containing source data for Figure 15.
    Supplementary file 1. A table of strains used in this study.
    elife-88253-supp1.xlsx (17.7KB, xlsx)
    MDAR checklist

    Data Availability Statement

    All data generated or analysed in this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source data files have been provided for all figures.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES