Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2024 Apr 3.
Published in final edited form as: J Fam Issues. 2023 Feb 12;45(3):674–696. doi: 10.1177/0192513x231155673

Re-Examining the Link Between Premarital Sex and Divorce

Jesse Smith 1, Nicholas H Wolfinger 2
PMCID: PMC10989935  NIHMSID: NIHMS1910902  PMID: 38571758

Abstract

Premarital sex predicts divorce, but we do not know why. Scholars have attributed the relationship to factors such as differences in beliefs and values, but these explanations have not been tested. It is further unclear how this relationship changes by number of sexual partners, or differs by gender. We re-examine this relationship with event history models using data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health. Models include measures of adolescent beliefs and values, religious background, and personal characteristics, as well as approximate number of premarital sexual partners in young adulthood. We find the relationship between premarital sex and divorce is highly significant and robust even when accounting for early-life factors. Compared to people with no premarital partners other than eventual spouses, those with nine or more partners exhibit the highest divorce risk, followed by those with one to eight partners. There is no evidence of gender differences.

Keywords: divorce/separation, family demography, sexuality, quantitative, life course


Premarital sex is linked to higher rates of divorce (Kahn and London, 1991; Paik, 2011), particularly when it involves partners other than one’s eventual spouse (Teachman, 2003), but the nature of this relationship is poorly understood. Three key questions remain unanswered, concerning explanatory mechanisms, variable effect sizes, and sex differences. Scholars have speculated that the effect of premarital sex on divorce may be related to beliefs and values about marriage and commitment, religious background, or learned relationship patterns (Paik, 2011; Teachman, 2003), but these hypotheses have not been tested. Past research has not examined how the effect of premarital sex on divorce varies by the number of sexual partners. Finally, we do not know whether the effect of premarital sex partners differs between men and women.

These represent key gaps in our understanding of the link between past and present intimate relationships, especially given the prevalence of premarital sex in the United States. Upwards of 90% of Americans first have sex prior to marriage, usually with someone other than their eventual spouse (Finer, 2007; Teachman, 2003; Wu, Martin, & England, 2017). The average number of premarital partners reported by women has increased in recent years (Wolfinger, 2016). These trends may have implications for divorce rates if premarital sex adversely affects marital stability. Given gender differences in sexual behaviors, motivations, and social attributions (England & Bearak, 2014; Regnerus & Uecker, 2011; Wells & Twenge, 2005), these implications may differ between men and women. An improved understanding of the link between premarital sex and divorce is therefore relevant for scholars of sexuality, marriage, and gender alike.

Previous research in this area has offered noteworthy insights, but has been limited by use of cross-sectional data and all-female samples. In this study, we build on existing scholarship by using longitudinal data on men and women from the Add Health survey to examine a) whether the effect of premarital sex on divorce can be explained by commonly cited factors such as early-life experiences or personal characteristics, b) how this effect changes according to the number of premarital partners, and c) whether this effect differs between men and women. We find no support for theorized explanations from past work—the effect of premarital sex on divorce is robust to controls for beliefs and values, religious background, family relationships, personality characteristics, and mental health in adolescence. Divorce risk is strongest for survey respondents with nine or more premarital partners, followed by those with one through eight partners, and lowest for those with none, thus indicating three “tiers” of divorce risk based on number of past partners. We find no evidence for differences between men and women in the relationship between premarital sex and marital dissolution.

Background

Although scholars have long been interested in the causes of divorce, the predictive role of premarital sexual partnerships has received relatively little research attention. What we do know is primarily based on three studies. Kahn and London (1991) compared women who reported any premarital sexual experience to those with none, and found higher divorce risk in the former group. Expanding on this finding, Teachman (2003) showed that there is elevated divorce risk only for those with premarital partners other than their eventual spouse, that is, sex with a future spouse alone does not predict divorce (but see Paik, 2011). Furthermore, he found that adverse effects of premarital cohabitation with someone other than a future spouse disappeared when controlling for premarital sex. Finally, Paik (2011) showed that the adverse effect of early sexual debut on marital stability can be explained by the fact that those who have sex earlier are more likely to have premarital sexual partners other than future spouses. Other studies also have demonstrated in passing that premarital sex predicts divorce (e.g., Heaton, 2002; Manning & Cohen, 2012). In each of these studies, the link between premarital sex and divorce is significant when controlling for a variety of individual characteristics.

These three studies have suggested various possible explanations for their findings, which largely mirror the literature on cohabitation and divorce (e.g., Rosenfeld & Roesler, 2019; Sassler & Lichter, 2020). Premarital sex may be an indicator of permissive attitudes toward sex and marriage, low religiosity, or a predilection for sexual variety, all of which are linked to higher divorce risk (Kahn & London, 1991; Paik, 2011; Vaaler, Ellison, & Powers, 2009). In this case, the premarital sex-divorce link is best explained by pre-existing differences between individuals. Alternatively, the experience of premarital sex itself, especially with multiple partners, may contribute to the development of more permissive attitudes toward sex or a greater awareness of sexual alternatives, either of which may serve to undermine marital stability (Paik, 2011; Teachman, 2003). In this case, the premarital sex-divorce link is better understood in terms of causality.

Extending Prior Research

Past research on premarital sex and divorce has relied almost entirely on data from the National Survey of Family Growth (NSFG), and thus presents with many of the same strengths and limitations. The NSFG contains detailed information on both marital timelines and sexual histories, and is therefore well suited to establishing the link between premarital sex and marriage dissolution. However, NSFG data are also limited in two key respects. First, as they are cross-sectional, they lack information on early-life factors which may contribute to both sexual behavior and marital outcomes. Second, they contain incomplete information on premarital sexual histories for men, leading to exclusive use of female samples in past research.

In this study, we rely on longitudinal data from the Add Health study to address three questions not resolved in past research. 1) To what extent is the effect of premarital sex on divorce explained by early-life factors such as beliefs and values about sex and marriage, religious background, personality traits, or parent–child relationships? 2) How does risk of divorce vary according to the number of premarital sexual partners? 3) Does the relationship between premarital sex and divorce differ between men and women?

Early-Life Factors and Personal Characteristics.

As identified above, past scholarship has argued that the link between premarital sex and divorce is best explained by beliefs and values about sex and marriage, low religiosity, or preferences for sexual variety (Kahn & London, 1991; Paik, 2011). Premarital sexual behavior is further associated with factors known to predict divorce, including depression (Khan et al. 2009; Wade & Pevalin, 2004), low risk-aversion (Hoyle, Fejfar, & Miller, 2000; Kelly & Conley, 1987), and poor family relationships (Amato & Patterson, 2017; Kerpelman, McElwain, Pittman, & Adler-Baeder, 2016). Given that these factors are variable over time and may be a consequence as well as a cause of sexual behaviors and marital experiences (Teachman, 2003), cross-sectional data are insufficient for untangling cause and effect. In this study, we address this concern by including measures of individual attributes during adolescence, prior to marriage and in relatively early stages of sexual development.

Variable Effect Size.

Past research has offered limited comparisons of divorce risk, contrasting virgins with people who only had premarital sex with a future spouse, and those with other premarital partners (Paik, 2011; Teachman, 2003). However, there is substantial variation in the number of premarital sexual partners reported by respondents, which may have as-yet-unexplored implications for divorce risk. For instance, recent NSFG estimates indicate a median of two premarital sexual partners among married women, and that over 10% report more than 10 partners (Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2020). The full extent of the relationship between premarital partner counts and divorce risk holds considerable practical relevance for our larger understanding of the link between past and present intimate relationships.

Prior research and theory offer relatively little guidance in predicting the functional form of the association between number of premarital sexual partners and marital dissolution (but see Wolfinger, 2016). Specifically, it is unclear whether we should expect the effect of premarital sex to become stronger or weaker as partners accumulate. On the one hand, it is plausible that having any premarital partners indicates a departure from idealized traditional linkages between sex and marriage, so that divorce risk should increase most strongly for lower-order counts of sexual partners and then level off. On the other hand, it may be that those with fewer premarital partners remain within a normative range and thus have relatively conventional attachments to institutional marriage or high-commitment relationship patterns, in which case we should see divorce risk rise most sharply for those with many partners.

Gender Differences.

Prior research is unclear as to whether the effects of premarital sex on divorce risk might be stronger for men or women. We do know that men have stronger preferences for sexual frequency and variety, and women’s sexual behavior is tied more strongly to a particular relationship (Baumeister, Catanese, & Vohs, 2001; Wells & Twenge, 2005). Paik (2011) claims that a predilection for sexual variety can undermine marital commitment (but see Perry, 2020). This suggests that the effect of premarital sex on divorce rates should be stronger for men given their predisposition to sexual variety.

But there’s also a strong counterargument. Sex has stronger adverse consequences for mental and emotional health for women than men (Regnerus & Uecker, 2011; Townsend & Wasserman, 2011). Furthermore, women are subjected to a sexual double-standard in which they may face negative evaluations for sexual behavior, resulting in greater social consequences (England & Bearak, 2014; Sprecher, Treger, & Sakaluk, 2013). For these reasons, it is possible premarital sex will have a stronger effect on marital functioning, and ultimately the risk of divorce, for women than men.

Data and Methods

The data for this study come from the first, third, and fourth waves of the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health (Add Health) (https://addhealth.cpc.unc.edu/) (Harris et al., 2019). The first wave of data collection occurred in 1994–1995 and consisted of 20,745 adolescents from grades 7–12, as well as 17,670 parents (mother if available, another adult in the household if not). Data for Wave III (N = 15,197) were collected in 2001–2002 when respondents were 18–26 years old. Wave IV took place in 2008–2009 and consisted of 15,701 respondents ages 24–32, with a response rate of 80.3%.

This study uses two separate samples from Add Health. The first sample is limited to those who had ever been married as of Wave IV (N = 7797), had a valid sample weight (N = 7393), and had sufficient data to calculate marital duration (N = 7286). We remove 67 cases where respondents, despite having been married, report no lifetime sexual partners, as this likely indicates an invalid response. An additional 166 cases were excluded due to reporting having no mother or father or any other household caregiver who serves this role at Wave I (e.g., grandparents, aunts/uncles), as some items regarding parental relationships are included in the analysis. A further 23 cases were removed due to reporting already being married as of Wave I. This leaves an analytic sample of 7030 respondents who had been in either opposite-sex (N = 6989) or same-sex (N = 41) marriages. The second sample is a subset of the 7030 which includes only respondents whose first marriages did not start until after Wave III (N = 3506).

Each sample contains distinct advantages and disadvantages. The first sample is larger, more representative, and offers a longer duration of observation of marriage, and is thus better suited to examining the effects of early-life factors and personal characteristics. The second sample is smaller and is selective of later marriages, but contains less ambiguity regarding the time ordering of sexual partners. This allows for greater approximation of the number of premarital partners needed in order to examine the shape of the association between sexual history and divorce risk.

Measures

Dependent Variable.

The dependent variable in this study is the duration of first marriage in months. In Wave IV, respondents were asked to provide information regarding what month and year their marriages started and ended. The survey items did not distinguish between separation and divorce but instead relied on self-report as to when respondents viewed their marriages as ending.1 If respondents reported their marriages were ongoing, the end date was set as the date of the Wave IV interview and observations were right-censored.

Independent Variables.

The key independent variables in this study are measures of premarital sexual partnerships. In Wave III, respondents were asked to provide their total lifetime number of vaginal sexual partners, as well as the total number of vaginal, oral, or anal partners they had had since 1995. In Wave IV, respondents were asked about their total lifetime number of sexual partners, including prior to the age of 18. These items, in conjunction with marital timelines, were used to develop a dichotomous indicator of whether respondents had any premarital sexual partners other than their spouse. For 434 cases (6%) in the full sample, there was insufficient information available to make this determination; values for these cases were imputed. Measures and coding criteria used to develop this indicator are shown in Table 1.

Table 1.

Coding Criteria for Premarital Sex Indicator.

Measures used
 Wave IV
  Total male + female lifetime vaginal, oral, or anal sexual partners
  Total male + female vaginal, oral, or anal sexual partners prior to age 18
  Divorce status
  Report of any concurrent sexual partners during first marriagea
  First marriage began prior to Wave III
  Age at first marriage
 Wave III
 Total lifetime vaginal sexual partners
 Total vaginal, oral, or anal sexual partners since the summer of 1995
 Start and end dates for sexual relationships since the summer of 1995a
Sufficient conditions to code respondents as having had premarital, nonspousal sexual partners (“I”)
 In first marriage at Wave IV, report more than one lifetime partner, no nonspousal partners concurrent with marriage
 Unmarried as of Wave III, more than one partner ever as of Wave III
 Married prior to Wave III, report any sexual relationships having ended prior to when marriage started
 Married after age 18, more than one partner prior to age 18
Sufficient conditions to code respondents as having had no premarital, nonspousal sexual partners (“0”)
 No more than one partner reported on any Wave IV or Wave III measure
 No more than one partner reported prior to Wave III, married by Wave III
 No more than one partner reported prior to age 18, married by age 18

Note: Above criteria allowed for logical certainty of categorization for 94% of respondents. Missing data for the remaining 6% of cases were addressed using multiple imputation (m = 10).

a

Measure only available for a subset of respondents.

For the subsample of those who did not marry until after Wave III, we first take the highest number of sexual partners reported across three items: lifetime vaginal partners as of Wave III, total sexual partners of any kind between 1995 and Wave III data collection, or partners of any kind prior to age 18 as reported at Wave IV. The partners counted in these three measures may overlap, so we use the highest number to capture as many partners as possible while ensuring no partner is counted more than once. We subtract one from this number (unless respondents report no partners) to account for the possibility that a single partner may be the respondent’s eventual spouse. Next we divide the sample intro three groups: 1) Those with no premarital partners (excluding future spouses) as of Wave III, 2) those with one to eight partners, and 3) those with nine or more. We selected these categories based on a series of analyses using dichotomous indicators of sexual partners, which showed divorce risk to increase substantially with one partner and again with nine, while remaining stable for partner counts between one and eight (see appendix). This approach also provides an improved model fit as measured by the deviance scores, compared to alternative approaches such as using quartiles or a continuous sexual partner variable.

It should be noted that because this second stage of analysis only includes sexual partners measured as of Wave III, any premarital partners in between Waves III and IVare not counted. This may result in some measurement error, leading to estimates biased downwards. Results from this analysis should thus be regarded as conservative.

Adolescent and Family Characteristics.

To account for early-life experiences and individual characteristics, we include several measures of respondents’ Wave I beliefs and values, religious involvement, and other traits. We measure religious identity using a modified version of Lehman and Sherkat’s (2018) recent typology (the key modification is that Latter-Day Saints are coded to the “Sectarian” category due to considerations of small cell sizes). Wave I religious attendance is included as a continuous variable. Unfortunately, Add Health respondents from Wave I who reported no religious affiliation were not asked about religious behaviors; these cases were coded as having no attendance.

We include two additional dichotomous variables to capture beliefs and values about premarital sex. The first indicates whether or not the respondent had ever taken a virginity pledge (Bearman & Brückner, 2001; Uecker, 2008). The second indicates whether adolescents report that in an ideal relationship, they would expect to have sex, but not get married, in the next year. Specifically, respondents were given a set of cards identifying different relationship activities (e.g., going out together in a group, holding hands, meeting partner’s parents, getting married), and asked to return to the interviewer any cards indicating activities they would not want to happen in the next year in their ideal relationship. If they kept the card for having sex, but returned the card for getting married, they were coded as a “1” on this indicator.2

The analysis also includes scales of Wave I depressive symptoms, delinquent behaviors, and decision-making style, as these all may be associated with both sexual behavior and marital outcomes. The depression scale uses 10 items on the frequency of experiencing various depressive symptoms, drawn from the CES-D depression inventory (e.g., “You felt you were too tired to do things”) (α = .72). The delinquency scale includes 15 items on frequency of various delinquent activities in the past year (e.g., damaging property, lying to parents about activities) (α = .83). Finally, adolescents were asked to assess their own personalities. We use five items pertaining to decision-making (e.g., “When you have a problem to solve, one of the first things you do is get as many facts about the problem as possible,”) to create a scale measuring a cautious approach to making decisions (α = .64) (Uecker, Regnerus, & Vaaler, 2007).

Two items are included to capture potential parental influences on children’s sexual and later marital outcomes. As positive parent–child relationships may serve as a protective factor against high-risk sexual behaviors (McElwain & Bub, 2018; Price & Hyde, 2009), we measure parent–child relationship quality using a scale of five items indicating the adolescent’s perception of closeness to each parent (e.g., “Most of the time, your mother is warm and loving toward you”) (α = .85). This scale used responses about mothers where available, fathers otherwise. We further include a cumulative measure of the number of activities respondents report doing with their parent in the past 4 weeks (e.g., gone shopping, played a sport).

Sociodemographic Controls.

Based on past studies, a number of controls are included to account for potential spuriousness in the relationship between premarital sex and marital stability. From Wave I, these include sex, race, and highest level of parent education in four categories: 1) Less than high school, 2) high school diploma/GED, 3) some post-high school education, and 4) college degree or higher. We further include measures of the number of family structure transitions experienced as of Waves I and II, using a variable constructed through procedures outlined in Gaydosh & Harris (2018; see also Martinson & Wu, 1992).3 From Wave IV, we add respondent education and indicators of the respondent’s relationship history. These include age at first marriage, any premarital birth, any premarital cohabitation, age at first sex in three categories (before age 16, at ages 16 or 17, and age 18 or later), and a binary indicator of the experience of nonconsensual sex as a minor.4

Analytic Strategy

In all analyses, we use discrete-time event history models to test the effects of premarital sex and other predictors on the rate of dissolution of first marriage. Specifically, we apply a complementary log-log estimator to data shaped to a marriage-month format, with one observation contributed for each month of first marriage of each respondent. Event history methods are ideal for studying divorce as they account for right censoring: some respondents will end their marriages subsequent to the most recent wave of data.

The first set of analyses includes the full sample of those who were ever married as of Wave IV. In a series of models, we examine the effects of having any premarital sexual partners on divorce risk in bivariate, with sociodemographic controls, and finally, with child and family characteristics. This serves as a test of the hypothesis that the effects of premarital sex are explained by early-life factors or personal characteristics. We proceed to conduct a second, nearly identical set of analyses on the subsample of those who were not married until after Wave III, the only difference being that we use a measure of three categories of sexual partners rather than the dichotomous variable. This provides an estimate of the shape of the relationship between premarital partners and divorce risk. Finally, we re-estimate all models with an interaction between premarital sex and gender in order to test for differences in effects between men and women. For all analyses, the baseline hazard is specified as the quadratic of marital duration in months. This produced substantively similar results to models specifying the baseline hazard with binary indicators for 1-year marriage intervals, so the quadratic in months was chosen for the sake of parsimony.

Missing data were handled via multiple imputation. Specifically, we used the mice package in R to create 10 imputed data sets for each subsample, conduct analyses for each data set, and pool the results, adjusting estimates for uncertainty in the imputed values (Van Buuren, 2018). All analyses include a survey weight and are adjusted for clustering and stratification.

Results

Descriptive statistics are shown in Table 2. Of respondents who had ever been married as of Wave IV, approximately 21% had a marriage which ended in divorce. Eighty-four percent of respondents reported having premarital, nonspousal sexual partners. For the late-marrying subsample, the proportion whose first marriage ended in dissolution is about 10%. Later marriage is a protective factor against dissolution (e.g., Teachman, 2002), and these marriages have shorter durations of observation, and hence a shorter period in which to observe divorce. In this group, about a quarter reported one or zero sexual partners as of Wave III, over half (57%) reported between one and eight partners, and 19% reported 9 or more.

Table 2.

Descriptive Statistics for Ever-Married Respondents as of Add Health Wave IV.

W1 (Full) W1 (Late-Marrying)
Variables M (SD) or proportion Range % imputed
Marital dissolution by Wave IVa .21 .10
Premarital sexual partners (excluding first spouse)a .84 6.2
 0 .24
 1–8 .57
 9+ .19
Child and family characteristics
 No religion .10 .10
 Sectarian .32 .28
 Catholic .26 .28
 Moderate/liberal protestant .27 .29
 Other religion .05 .05
 Religious attendance frequency 2.81 (1.18) 2.85 (1.18) 1–4 0.1
 Taken virginity pledge .15 .15 0.3
 Sex in perfect relationship .27 .27 0.8
 Depressive symptoms 0.52 (0.37) 0.49 (0.35) 0–3
 Delinquent behaviors 0.27 (0.33) 0.26 (0.32) 0–3 0.4
 Cautious decision-making style 3.67 (0.57) 3.67 (0.57) 1–5 0.1
 Parent–child relationship quality 4.39 (0.66) 4.44 (0.63) 1–5
 Parent–child activities in past 4 weeks 3.71 (1.90) 3.78 (1.91) 1–9 0.1
Sociodemographic controls
 Female .57 .55
 White .62 .62
 Black .13 .13
 Hispanic .16 .14
 Other race .09 .11
 Parent has < HS education .06 .07 3.4
 Parent has HS diploma/GED .16 .22 3.4
 Parent has some college .47 .29 3.4
 Parent has a college degree .32 .41 3.4
 Family structure transitions 0.76 (1.10) 0.69 (1.08) 0–5 0.5
 Respondent has < HS education .06 .04
 Respondent has HS diploma .16 .12
 Respondent has some post-HS .47 .42
 Respondent has BA or higher .32 .43
 Age at first marriagea 23.80 (3.17) 25.70 (2.31) 14.8–32.3
 Premarital birtha .21 .22 0.3
 Premarital cohabitationa .70 .75
 First sex before age 16 .38 .34 0.9
 First sex at ages 16–17 .32 .32 0.9
 First sex at age 18 or older .30 .34 0.9
 Coercive sex as minora .12 .10 0.8
 No. of observations 7030 3506
 Marriage-months 389,197 131,769

Note: Means and proportions are calculated prior to multiple imputation. The percent imputed refers to the full sample. The late-marrying sample is a subset of the full sample whose first marriages did not begin until after Wave III.

a

Wave IV measure.

Results for the first set of discrete-time models are shown in Table 3. The most important takeaway is that premarital sex is a highly significant predictor of divorce at the p < .001 level in every model. This effect remains robust even with the inclusion of the full set of early-life factors relating to beliefs or values, religious practice, family characteristics, individual attributes, and parent–child relationships. The effect size is both large and stable: across models, those with premarital sexual partners have more than twice the odds of divorce as do those without (ORs = 2.50—2.52). We thus find no evidence that the link between premarital sex and divorce is due to selectivity based on early-life religiosity or beliefs and values. In fact, although several variables significantly predict divorce in bivariate analyses (not shown), most do not predict divorce in full models, aside from those confirmed in past research: African Americans are at higher risk of divorce compared to whites, people with a college degree have lower divorce risk, experience of family transitions predicts higher risk of divorce, and age at marriage is strongly and negatively linked to divorce.

Table 3.

Odds Ratios from Discrete-Time Event History Models Predicting Dissolution of First Marriage.

Variable M1 M2 M3
Any premarital sexual partners 2.61 (0.12)*** 2.54 (0.14)*** 2.52 (0.13)***
Child and family characteristics
 Sectarian 0.89 (0.16)
 Moderate/liberal protestant 0.94 (0.15)
 Catholic 0.98 (0.17)
 Other religion 0.86 (0.20)
 Religious attendance frequency 0.98 (0.04)
 Taken virginity pledge 1.01 (0.10)
 Sex in perfect relationship 0.98 (0.09)
 Depressive symptoms 0.98 (0.10)
 Delinquent behaviors 1.10 (0.10)
 Cautious decision-making style 0.96 (0.06)
 Parent–child relationship quality 1.02 (0.06)
 Parent–child activities in past 4 weeks 0.99 (0.02)
Sociodemographic controls
 Female 0.90 (0.07) 0.92 (0.07)
 Black 1.21 (0.11) 1.27 (0.11)*
 Hispanic 0.89 (0.11) 0.86 (0.12)
 Other race 1.23 (0.14) 1.22 (0.14)
 Parent has HS diploma/GED 1.09 (0.11) 1.08 (0.12)
 Parent has some college 1.09 (0.11) 1.09 (0.11)
 Parent has a college degree 0.90 (0.14) 0.91 (0.14)
 Respondent has HS diploma 0.81 (0.14) 0.82 (0.14)
 Respondent has some post-HS 0.95 (0.11) 0.97 (0.12)
 Respondent has a BA 0.62 (0.16)** 0.64 (0.17)*
 Family structure transitions 1.07 (0.03)* 1.06 (0.03)
 Age at first marriage 0.87 (0.02)*** 0.87 (0.02)***
 Premarital birth 0.99 (0.09) 0.99 (0.09)
 Premarital cohabitation 1.04 (0.09) 1.02 (0.09)
 First sex at ages 16–17 0.90 (0.07) 0.91 (0.07)
 First sex at age 18 or older 0.95 (0.09) 0.98 (0.09)
 Coercive sex as minor 0.95 (0.10) 0.96 (0.10)
Months 1.02 (0.00)*** 1.02 (0.00)*** 1.02 (0.00)***
Months squared 1.00 (0.00)*** 1.00 (0.00)*** 1.00 (0.00)***
Deviance 19,964 19,637 19,630

Note: N = 389,197 (marriage-months). Standard errors are in parentheses. Reference categories: White, respondent has less than a high school diploma, parent has less than a high school diploma, first sex before age 16, no religion.

p < .1,

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001 (2-tailed tests).

Analyses of the subsample of those who did not marry until after Wave III are shown in Table 4. To reiterate, the purpose of using this sample is to be able to capture the variation in premarital sexual partners more effectively. The key results here are mostly consistent across models: those with the highest number of premarital sexual partners as of Wave III (nine or more) have about triple the odds of divorce compared to those with none (ORs = 2.65—3.20). Notably, this effect becomes stronger as controls are added to the model, indicating such hypothesized selection factors as sociodemographic or religious characteristics actually suppress, rather than help explain, the effect of premarital sex for those with the highest number of partners.

Table 4.

Odds Ratios from Discrete-Time Event History Models Predicting Dissolution of First Marriage among Later-Marrying Respondents.

Variable M1 M2 M3
1–8 premarital partners 1.50 (0.20)* 1.59 (0.22)* 1.64 (0.21)*
9+ partners 2.65 (0.20)*** 3.00 (0.24)*** 3.20 (0.24)***
Child and family characteristics
 Sectarian 0.38 (0.31)**
 Moderate/liberal protestant 0.45 (0.29)**
 Catholic 0.46 (0.27)**
 Other religion 0.70 (0.34)
 Religious attendance frequency 1.06 (0.10)
 Taken virginity pledge 1.22 (0.21)
 Sex in perfect relationship 0.78 (0.18)
 Depressive symptoms 0.76 (0.22)
 Delinquent behaviors 1.21 (0.21)
 Cautious decision-making style 0.95 (0.12)
 Parent–child relationship quality 1.04 (0.13)
 Parent–child activities in past 4 weeks 1.04 (0.04)
Sociodemographic controls
 Female 1.23 (0.16) 1.23 (0.17)
 Black 1.03 (0.22) 1.16 (0.22)
 Hispanic 0.77 (0.28) 0.79 (0.29)
 Other race 1.37 (0.24) 1.18 (0.23)
 Parent has HS diploma/GED 0.87 (0.28) 0.90 (0.27)
 Parent has some college 0.99 (0.25) 1.00 (0.25)
 Parent has a college degree 0.77 (0.29) 0.75 (0.29)
 Respondent has HS diploma 0.65 (0.30) 0.65 (0.29)
 Respondent has some post-HS 0.72 (0.24) 0.72 (0.23)
 Respondent has a BA 0.43 (0.30)** 0.43 (0.30)**
 Family structure transitions 1.05 (0.05) 1.04 (0.05)
 Age at first marriage 0.90 (0.04)** 0.91 (0.04)*
 Premarital birth 0.85 (0.18) 0.91 (0.19)
 Premarital cohabitation 1.44 (0.24) 1.42 (0.26)
 First sex at ages 16–17 0.89 (0.18) 0.90 (0.18)
 First sex at age 18 or older 1.35 (0.20) 1.37 (0.21)
 Coercive sex as minor 0.85 (0.22) 0.85 (0.22)
 Months 1.04 (0.01)*** 1.04 (0.01)*** 1.04 (0.01)***
 Months squared 1.00 (0.00)* 1.00 (0.00)* 1.00 (0.00)*
 Deviance 4999 4928 4889

Note: N = 131,769 (marriage-months). Standard errors are in parentheses. Reference categories: No premarital sexual partners as of Wave 3, White, respondent has less than a high school diploma, parent has less than a high school diploma, first sex before age 16, no religion.

p < .1,

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001 (2-tailed tests).

Those with one to eight partners are also at greater risk of divorce, though this coefficient is weaker than for those with nine or more partners. Specifically, in the full model the odds of divorce for those with one to eight partners are 64% higher than those with no premarital partners (ORs = 1.50—1.64). This effect is also not attenuated as controls are added to the model, reinforcing the finding that explanations based on early-life experiences and personal characteristics are not supported. Additional analyses show those in this middle category to have a significantly lower divorce risk than those with nine or more partners, indicating three distinct groups. Taken together, these results suggest that the relationship between number of premarital partners and marital dissolution is nonlinear. They point rather to three tiers of divorce risk, with the lowest risk for those with no premarital, nonspousal partners, a modest increase for those with some, and a sharp increase for those with many. These results are more consistent with the notion that the effect of premarital sex on divorce becomes stronger, not weaker, as sexual partners accumulate.

Finally, to test for differences in the effect of premarital sex on divorce between men and women, we repeat all analyses from Tables 3 and 4 and include an interaction between gender and premarital sex variables (not shown). The results are straightforward: we do not any evidence of sex differences in the link between premarital sex and divorce risk in any model for either sample. Although there is theoretical reason to expect such a difference, our results suggest otherwise.

Discussion

Past research has established a clear link between premarital sex, particularly with partners other than an eventual spouse, and the risk of marital dissolution (Kahn & London, 1991; Paik, 2011; Teachman, 2003). Thus far, however, we know little about the nature of this link. The purpose of this study was to shed light on three areas which have not been fully addressed in past literature: the potential role of early-life factors or personal characteristics in explaining this relationship, the possibility of nonlinear effects of premarital sexual partnerships on divorce risk, and whether the relationship varies between men and women. A better understanding of these factors contributes to our knowledge of the links between early-life and later-life relationship experiences and outcomes, an important focus of theory and research on the family life course (Sassler, 2010).

Previous research pointed to a variety of individual and social variables to explain the relationship between premarital sex and divorce, including nontraditional views on sex and marriage, weaker religious attachments, and lower-quality family relationships (Kahn & London, 1991; Paik, 2011). We find no support for these explanations, and show that the effect of premarital sex remains highly significant after accounting for a wide range of individual and social differences between respondents. Needless to say, our analysis does not rule out the possibility that unmeasured variables might be driving selection. Future research should endeavor to test for the potential causal effects of premarital sex on divorce, and to elaborate on possible selection processes which have not been considered in past scholarship.

As expected, we find evidence of a nonlinear relationship between the number of sexual partners and the risk of divorce. Those in the highest category of partners (9+) consistently show the highest divorce risk by a substantial margin, followed by those with one to eight partners, with the lowest risk for those with none. In other words, we find distinct tiers of divorce risk between those with no, some, or many premarital, nonspousal sexual partners. As over half the sample falls into the middle category, these results may be interpreted as indicating two distinctive groups. At one extreme, having no premarital partners serves as a protective factor against divorce risk. At the other, those in the top quintile experience especially high levels of marital dissolution. These findings highlight the importance of incorporating the variation in sexual history into research in this area, rather than relying on dichotomous indicators of premarital sex. A possible implication here is that the robust effect of premarital sex found in past studies is being driven largely by a minority of respondents with especially high levels of both sexual partners and divorce rates. This reflects the fact that although partner counts of eight or less have become increasingly normative, having more partners may indicate distinctive characteristics which are not conducive to marital stability.

We find no evidence of gender differences in the link between premarital sex and divorce. This is a surprising finding—the domains of sexuality and marriage are highly gendered (e.g., Monin & Clark, 2011; Okami & Shackelford, 2001; Oliver & Hyde, 1993), and there are many plausible theoretical pathways by which the premarital sex-divorce relationship might be expected to differ between men and women. Future research might consider if the mechanisms linking premarital sex and divorce function the same way for men and women.

This study has contributed to our understanding of the link between premarital sex and marital dissolution by taking advantage of the mixed-sex sample and longitudinal design of the Add Health study in order to address some of the limitations of past literature. Yet Add Health has its own limitations with respect to the aims of this study. As all respondents at Wave IV were 32 years old or younger, the sample is selective of those who marry younger, and the time to observe divorce is somewhat limited. The results are therefore most reflective of early marriages and divorces. Also, at the start of data collection, respondents ranged from early to late adolescence, and many had already had sex. On a related note, in some cases it is unclear whether sexual partnerships occurred before, during or after marriage. To ensure proper temporal ordering, tests of the effects of the number of sexual partners were only performed on a late-marrying subsample, and the measure used did not capture all partners. Nevertheless, it represents a more detailed measure than almost all prior studies have used. Needless to say, a more exact measure for the full sample would be ideal. Similarly, we relied on proxies for beliefs and values about marriage and divorce as well as marital quality, so more precise measures would be preferable. Finally, though Add Health data are well-suited to ruling out some of the hypothesized selection processes explaining the premarital sex-divorce link, they are less appropriate for examining possible causal mechanisms. This would require more detailed timelines of sexual histories and more frequent waves of data collection than Add Health provides.

This study makes a contribution to our understanding of the link between premarital sex and divorce, which has been heretofore limited in key respects. It reinforces the finding that the effect of premarital sex is robust, and shows that it applies equally to men as well as women. Moreover, our findings help to rule out common explanations offered by past research. The results here also serve to highlight the importance of conceptualizing sexual history more broadly, taking into account its extent and timing, when considering how it might affect marriages. As relationship experiences over the life course have become more varied and complex in recent decades (Sassler & Lichter, 2020), it is increasingly vital that theory and analysis adjust to take account of these developments.

Funding

The author(s) disclosed receipt of the following financial support for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article: This research was supported by funding from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development (NICHD) to the Population Research Institute at The Pennsylvania State University for Population Research Infrastructure (P2C HD041025) and Family Demography Training (T-32HD007514). The findings and conclusions do not necessarily reflect the views of the funding agency.

Appendix

Table A1.

Distribution of Premarital Sexual Partners (as of Wave III) by Sex.

Number of Partners Men Women All
0 368 (0.23) 475 (0.25) 843 (0.24)
1 194 (0.12) 245 (0.13) 439 (0.13)
2 159 (0.10) 232 (0.12) 391 (0.11)
3 132 (0.08) 174 (0.09) 306 (0.09)
4 147 (0.09) 176 (0.09) 323 (0.09)
5 81 (0.05) 109 (0.06) 190 (0.05)
6 63 (0.04) 98 (0.05) 161 (0.05)
7 54 (0.03) 69 (0.04) 123 (0.04)
8 23 (0.01) 38 (0.02) 61 (0.02)
9 81 (0.05) 87 (0.05) 168 (0.05)
10+ 274 (0.17) 223 (0.12) 497 (0.14)
Total 1576 (1.00) 1926 (1.00) 3502 (1.00)

Note: Proportion by column in parentheses. Sample is limited to those whose first marriages began after Wave III (N = 3502 after removing four missing observations). One partner is subtracted for each respondent to ensure only partners other than eventual spouses are counted.

Table A2.

Odds ratios from Discrete-Time Event History Models Predicting Dissolution of First Marriage among Late-Marrying Respondents—Alternative Measure of Sexual Partners.

Premarital Partner Count M1 M2 M3 M4 M5 M6 M7 M8 M9 M10 M11
0 0.56* 0.53* 0.76 0.76 0.60 0.47* 0.70 0.86 0.30*** 0.32***
1 1.77* 0.95 1.34 1.35 1.06 0.84 1.25 1.53 0.54 0.58
2 1.87* 1.05 1.41 1.41 1.11 0.88 1.31 1.60 0.56 0.60*
3 1.32 0.74 0.71 1.00 0.79 0.63 0.93 1.14 0.40* 0.43**
4 1.32 0.74 0.90 1.00 0.79 0.62 0.93 1.14 0.40* 0.43**
5 1.68 0.94 1.13 1.27 1.27 0.79 1.18 1.44 0.50 0.54
6 2.11* 1.19 0.76 1.60 1.60 1.26 1.49 1.82 0.64 0.69
7 1.42 0.80 0.76 1.08 1.08 0.85 0.67 1.22 0.43 0.46
8 1.16 0.65 0.62 0.88 0.88 0.69 0.55 0.82 0.35 0.37
9 3.32*** 1.87 1.78 2.51* 2.51* 1.98 1.57 2.33 2.86 1.07
10 3.08*** 1.73 1.65* 2.33** 2.33** 1.84 1.46 2.17 2.66 0.93
Months 1.04*** 1.04*** 1.04*** 1.04*** 1.04*** 1.04*** 1.04*** 1.04*** 1.04*** 1.04*** 1.04***
Months squared 1.00* 1.00* 1.00* 1.00* 1.00* 1.00* 1.00* 1.00* 1.00* 1.00* 1.00*

Note: N = 131,769 (marriage-months). Models include all controls from Model 3 in Table 4.

p < .1,

*

p < .05,

**

p < .01,

***

p < .001 (2-tailed tests).

Footnotes

Declaration of Conflicting Interests

The author(s) declared no potential conflicts of interest with respect to the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.

1.

The majority of spouses who separate will eventually divorce (Bumpass, Martin, & Sweet, 1991). Moreover, most marriages effectively end at the time of separation, not divorce (Ono, 1995).

2.

Wave I of Add Health also contains several items regarding perceptions of the likelihood and severity of negative consequences resulting from sex, particularly unprotected sex (e.g., “Imagine that sometime soon you were to have sexual intercourse with someone just once, but were unable to use any method of birth control for some reason. What is the chance that you would get [your partner] pregnant?”). These were excluded from the analyses reported here as they were only asked of older respondents, and their inclusion would lead to a considerably reduced and more selective sample. Their inclusion in supplementary analyses did not lead to substantively different conclusions.

3.

We do not include family structure at Wave I due to concerns of collinearity with the measure of number of family transitions. There is some evidence that with respect to marital outcomes, number of transitions is the more important factor (Wolfinger 2000).

4.

Recall that we subtract 1 from the count of sexual partners to account for the possibility that respondents will have had sex with a future spouse, which is less likely to predict divorce (Teachman 2003). It is therefore possible for respondents to have had a premarital birth or cohabitation while still being coded as “0” in our premarital sex indicator. We do not include respondent age due to concerns of collinearity with age at marriage in the late-marrying subsample (r = .63). Models with respondent age included produce substantively similar results.

References

  1. Amato PR, & Patterson SE (2017). The intergenerational transmission of union instability in early adulthood. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 79(3), 723–738. 10.1111/jomf.12384 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Baumeister RF, Catanese KR, & Vohs KD (2001). Is there a gender difference in strength of sex drive? Theoretical views, conceptual distinctions, and a review of relevant evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 5(3), 242–273. 10.1207/s15327957pspr0503_5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bearman PS, & Brückner H (2001). Promising the future: Virginity pledges and first intercourse. American Journal of Sociology, 106(4), 859–912. 10.1086/320295 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  4. Bumpass LL, Martin TC, & Sweet JA (1991). The impact of family background and early marital factors on marital disruption. Journal of Family Issues, 12(1), 22–42. 10.1177/019251391012001003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Center for Disease Control and Prevention (2020). National survey of family Growth, 2017–2019: Female respondent file codebook. Retrieved November 4, 2021 from https://www.cdc.gov/nchs/nsfg/nsfg_2017_2019_codebooks.htm#femresp
  6. England P, & Bearak J (2014). The sexual double standard and gender differences in attitudes toward casual sex among U.S. university students. Demographic Research, 30(46), 1327–1338. 10.4054/demres.2014.30.46 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  7. Finer LB (2007). Trends in premarital sex in the United States, 1954–2003. Public Health Reports, 122(1), 73–78. 10.1177/003335490712200110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Gaydosh L, & Harris KM (2018). Childhood family instability and young adult health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 59(3), 371–390. 10.1177/0022146518785174 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Harris Kathleen Mullan, Halpern Carolyn Tucker, Whitsel Eric A, Hussey Jon M, Killeya-Jones Ley A, Tabor Joyce, & Dean Sarah C (2019). Cohort Profile: The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health (Add Health). International Journal of Epidemiology, 48(5), 1415–1425. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Heaton TB (2002). Factors contributing to increasing marital stability in the United States. Journal of Family Issues, 23(3), 392–409. 10.1177/0192513x02023003004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  11. Hoyle RH, Fejfar MC, & Miller JD (2000). Personality and sexual risk taking: A quantitative review. Journal of Personality, 68(6), 1203–1231. 10.1111/1467-6494.00132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Kahn JR, & London KA (1991). Premarital sex and the risk of divorce. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 53(4), 845. 10.2307/352992 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  13. Kelly EL, & Conley JJ (1987). Personality and compatibility: A prospective analysis of marital stability and marital satisfaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 52(1), 27–40. 10.1037//0022-3514.52.1.27 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Kerpelman JL, McElwain AD, Pittman JF, & Adler-Baeder FM (2016). Engagement in risky sexual behavior: Adolescents’ perceptions of self and the parent-child relationship matter. Youth & Society, 48(1), 101–125. 10.1177/0044118x13479614 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  15. Khan MR, Kaufman JS, Pence BW, Gaynes BN, Adimora AA, Weir SS, & Miller WC (2009). Depression, sexually transmitted infection, and sexual risk behavior among young adults in the United States. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 163(7), 644–652. 10.1001/archpediatrics.2009.95 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Lehman D, & Sherkat DE (2018). Measuring religious identification in the United States. Journal for the Scientific Study of Religion, 57(4), 779–794. 10.1111/jssr.12543 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  17. Manning WD, & Cohen JA (2012). Premarital cohabitation and marital dissolution: An examination of recent marriages. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 74(2), 377–387. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2012.00960.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Martinson BC, & Wu LL (1992). Parent histories: Patterns of change in early life. Journal of Family Issues, 13(3), 351–377. 10.1177/019251392013003006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  19. McElwain AD, & Bub KL (2018). Changes in parent–child relationship quality across Early adolescence: Implications for engagement in sexual behavior. Youth & Society, 50(2), 204–228. 10.1177/0044118x15626843 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  20. Monin JK, & Clark MS (2011). Why do men benefit more from marriage than do women? Thinking more broadly about interpersonal processes that occur within and outside of marriage. Sex Roles, 65(5–6), 320–326. 10.1007/s11199-011-0008-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  21. Okami P, & Shackelford TK (2001). Human sex differences in sexual psychology and behavior. Annual Review of Sex Research, 12(1), 186–241. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Oliver MB, & Hyde JS (1993). Gender differences in sexuality: A meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 114(1), 29–51. 10.1037/0033-2909.114.1.29 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Ono H (1995). Expanding on explanations of recent patterns in U.S. Divorce rates. Los Angeles, CA: Doctoral dissertation. [Google Scholar]
  24. Paik A (2011). Adolescent sexuality and the risk of marital dissolution. Journal of Marriage and Family, 73(2), 472–485. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00819.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  25. Perry SL (2020). Does low-cost sexual gratification make men less eager to marry? Pornography use, masturbation, hookup sex, and desire to be married among single men. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 49(8), 3013–3026. 10.1007/s10508-020-01793-w [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Price MN, & Hyde JS (2009). When two isn’t better than one: Predictors of early sexual activity in adolescence using a cumulative risk model. Journal of Youth and Adolescence, 38(8), 1059–1071. 10.1007/s10964-008-9351-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Regnerus M, & Uecker J (2011). Premarital sex in America: How young Americans meet, mate, and think about marrying. Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  28. Rosenfeld MJ, & Roesler K (2019). Cohabitation experience and cohabitation’s association with marital dissolution. Journal of Marriage and Family, 81(1), 42–58. 10.1111/jomf.12530 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  29. Sassler S (2010). Partnering across the life course: Sex, relationships, and mate selection. Journal of Marriage and the Family, 72(3), 557–575. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2010.00718.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Sassler S, & Lichter DT (2020). Cohabitation and marriage: Complexity and diversity in union-formation patterns. Journal of Marriage and Family, 82(1), 35–61. 10.1111/jomf.12617 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  31. Sprecher Susan, Treger Stanislav, & Sakaluk John K. (2013). Premarital Sexual Standards and Sociosexuality: Gender, Ethnicity, and Cohort Differences. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 42(8), 1395–1405. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Teachman JD (2002). Stability across cohorts in divorce risk factors. Demography, 39(2), 331–351. 10.1353/dem.2002.0019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Teachman JD (2003). Premarital sex, premarital cohabitation, and the risk of subsequent marital dissolution among women. Journal of Marriage and Family, 65(2), 444–455. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2003.00444.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  34. Townsend JM, & Wasserman TH (2011). Sexual hookups among college students: Sex differences in emotional reactions. Archives of Sexual Behavior, 40(6), 1173–1181. 10.1007/s10508-011-9841-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Uecker JE (2008). Religion, pledging, and the premarital sexual behavior of married young adults. Journal of Marriage and Family, 70(3), 728–744. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2008.00517.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  36. Uecker JE, Regnerus MD, & Vaaler ML (2007). Losing my religion: The social sources of religious decline in early adulthood. Social Forces, 85(4), 1667–1692. 10.1353/sof.2007.0083 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  37. Vaaler ML, Ellison CG, & Powers DA (2009). Religious influences on the risk of marital dissolution. Journal of Marriage and Family, 71(4), 917–934. 10.1111/j.1741-3737.2009.00644.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  38. Van Buuren S (2018). Flexible imputation of missing data. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. [Google Scholar]
  39. Wade TJ, & Pevalin DJ (2004). Marital transitions and mental health. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 45(2), 155–170. 10.1177/002214650404500203 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Wells BE, & Twenge JM (2005). Changes in young people’s sexual behavior and attitudes, 1943–1999: A cross-temporal meta-analysis. Review of General Psychology, 9(3), 249–261. 10.1037/1089-2680.9.3.249 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Wolfinger NH (2000). Beyond the intergenerational transmission of divorce: Do people replicate the patterns of marital instability they grew up with? Journal of Family Issues, 21(8), 1061–1086. 10.1177/019251300021008006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  42. Wolfinger NH (2016, June, 6). Counterintuitive trends in the link between premarital sex and marital stability. Charlottesville, VA: Institute for Family Studies. https://ifstudies.org/blog/counterintuitive-trends-in-the-link-between-premarital-sex-and-marital-stability [Google Scholar]
  43. Wu LL, Martin SP, & England P (2017). The decoupling of sex and marriage: Cohort trends in who did and did not delay sex until marriage for US women born 1938–1985. Sociological Science, 4, 151–175. 10.15195/v4.a7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES