Abstract
Gestures are an important social communication skill that infants and toddlers use to convey their thoughts, ideas, and intentions. Research suggests that early gesture use has important downstream impacts on developmental processes, such as language learning. However, autistic children are more likely to have challenges in their gestural development. The current study expands upon previous literature on the differences in gesture use between young autistic and non-autistic toddlers by collecting data using a parent-report questionnaire called the MCDI–Words and Gestures at three time points, 12, 18, and 24 months of age. Results (N = 467) showed that high-likelihood infants who later meet diagnostic criteria for ASD (n = 73 HL-ASD) have attenuated gesture growth from 12 to 24 months for both deictic gestures and symbolic gestures when compared to high-likelihood infants who later did not meet criteria for ASD (n = 249 HL-Neg) and low-likelihood infants who did not meet criteria for ASD (n = 145 LL-Neg). Other social communicative skills, like play behaviors and imitation, were also found to be impacted in young autistic children when compared to their non-autistic peers. Understanding early differences in social communication growth before a formal autism diagnosis can provide important insights for early intervention.
Keywords: ASD, gesture, MCDI, communication, development, infants
Lay Summary:
As infants learn to talk, they use gestures to communicate. In this study, we used a parent-report questionnaire to look at the group level differences of gestures in the first two years of life by comparing infants who later developed autism to infants who did not develop autism. We found that infants who later developed autism have fewer gestures and a slower rate of gesture growth compared to infants who did not develop autism. Play behaviors and imitation skills were also impacted in young autistic children when compared to their non-autistic peers.
Introduction
Symbols are an important hallmark of communication development (Deloache, 1995; Tomasello et al., 2005). Using symbols allows for representation of meaning (DeLoache, 2004). For example, in young children, a banana can represent a telephone during pretend play, or the act of waving hands can represent saying “goodbye.” Symbolic communication can take many forms: gestures, words, and even drawings. For children, immersion in the symbolic world begins early in development (Rakoczy et al., 2017). However, signs of emerging symbolic communication skills for young children are difficult to discern until they can demonstrate them through motor acts like gestures.
Development of Communicative Gestures and Representational Skills
In the first two years of life, gestures develop gradually. The first form of gestures that emerge are intentional communicative gestures (also called deictic gestures). Infants usually demonstrate deictic gestures at around 8 to 10 months of age (Bretherton & Bates, 1979). Deictic gestures include showing, pointing, giving, and reaching to communicate based on the environmental context (Iverson et al., 1994). These initial forms of gestures usually appear before or around the emergence of spoken language; hence, these type of gestures are often called prelinguistic gestures (Colonnesi et al., 2010; Iverson & Goldin-Meadow, 2005; Liszkowski, 2014; Spencer, 2011).
The mastery of deictic gestures sets the stage for more advanced forms of gestures, like symbolic gestures (Werner & Kaplan, 1964). Symbolic gestures require the symbolic system and representational skills. The symbolic system refers to the ability to use symbols to represent absent objects, and representational skill refers to the ability to link a symbol to a referent (Bates et al., 1979; Corrigan, 1983; Piaget, 1952). It is speculated that deictic gestures help build the foundation for the symbolic system because the development of symbolic understanding occurs when children’s pointing elicits referent-labeling of objects from caregivers (Goldin-Meadow, 2007). By connecting the labels to the referent, infants learn to understand what the referent symbolizes (e.g., pointing to a ball reflects that a spherical shape represents a ball). This type of learning is a precursor to the development of object-word pairings in young children.
As infants mature past 11 months, they begin to use play schemes to learn how to use symbols to communicate meaning (Fein, 1981; McCune, 1995, 2009). Play schemes are carried out when toddlers are using the functional aspects of the object (e.g., a telephone used as a telephone) and are manipulating the referent (e.g., banana as a telephone). Examples of play schemes are functional play, pretend play, and symbolic play. While these are not necessarily considered true forms of gestures, as they do not meet the communicative and symbolic criteria described by Acredolo and Goodwyn (1988), they do help children understand the relationship between two objects (Escalona, 1973; Thiemann-Bourque et al., 2019). Ultimately, this type of learning allows toddlers to carry out symbolic representation as they learn how to decontextualize sensorimotor schemes from the referents to create abstract symbols (McCune-Nicolich, 1981; Orr, 2020; Werner & Kaplan, 1964).
As toddlers practice deictic gestures and play schemes, they begin to develop the skill of combining mental representations of relationships between objects. At this stage, children begin to understand that objects, events, and people have properties that can be independent of their own actions (Corrigan, 1983). This leads to the emergence of symbolic gestures (also called representational gestures, Acredolo & Goodwyn, 1988). Symbolic gestures convey specific intentions, and the message of these gestures can be conveyed in different social and situational contexts (Iverson et al., 1994; Stefanini et al., 2009). These gestures include actions that represent a specific semantic meaning through an identified referent, like flapping arms to represent a bird, and cultural-bound gestures like nodding to say “yes” (Manwaring et al., 2019). Demonstrations of advanced representational skills occur when toddlers can create abstract symbols to communicate with a social partner (e.g., throwing action to represent “ball”).
Gestures Facilitate Language and Cognition
Gestures are an important developmental milestone for infants and toddlers because gestures facilitate language and cognitive development. Symbols in gestures are thought to share common origins with linguistic symbols, like spoken language (Piaget, 1962, p. 19). Symbolic skills in gestures and spoken language require mental representation (Piaget, 1952), and mental representation is a cognitive skill (Corrigan, 1983). Early gesture use enables growth in language and cognition due to the early training in linking symbols to referents, resulting in the overall growth of the symbolic system (Clough & Duff, 2020; Goldin-Meadow, 2015; Goldin-Meadow & Alibali, 2013; Tomasello, 2010). Hence, gestures are positively associated with language and cognition (Goldin-Meadow & Alibali, 2013; Golomb & Cornelius, 1977; Iverson & Goldin-Meadow, 2005; Kelly et al., 2010; Kuhn et al., 2014; McCune, 1995; McGillion et al., 2017; Orr & Geva, 2015; Özçalışkan et al., 2016; Rowe et al., 2008).
Parental Inventories of Gestures and Symbolic Abilities
The MacArthur-Bates Communicative Developmental Inventory – Words and Gestures (MCDI–Words and Gestures; Fenson et al., 2006) is a parent-report questionnaire that has been used by researchers to measure the development of communicative and symbolic gestures (Choi et al., 2020, 2021; Lucca & Wilbourn, 2018; Morin-Lessard et al., 2021; Talbott et al., 2015). Specifically, the instrument delves into the developmental milestones of symbolic gestures, from deictic gestures to early emerging symbolic gestures. As the questionnaire investigates the development of symbolic gestures, it also taps into the development of representational skills. While the MCDI–Words and Gestures is intended to capture the development of typical children between the ages of 8 and 18 months of age, researchers have used the instrument outside this age range to assess the gesture profiles of autistic children (Choi et al., 2020; Franchini et al., 2018; Iverson et al., 2018; West et al., 2020).
The MCDI–Words and Gestures is divided into two categories: Early Gestures and Later Gestures. Early Gestures consists of deictic gestures, conventional gestures (culturally bound gestures like nodding to say “yes”), and games and routines (e.g., playing patty cake). Demonstration of these social-communicative actions are examples of interacting with the social world (Vallotton & Ayoub, 2010), and these skills are necessary for the development of early symbolic abilities. On the other hand, Later Gestures represent the improvement of symbolic skills. Items in Later Gestures include imitating adult actions, knowledge of appropriate usage of items and actions (functional play), and using objects to represent other objects (symbolic play). To demonstrate Later Gestures, children need to understand the meaning and intention behind complex adult behaviors and object use (Acredolo & Goodwyn, 1988; Fenson et al., 2006). Items in Later Gestures are not considered “true” gestures; however, they are motor acts that facilitate symbolic understanding. Thus, Later Gestures is sometimes called symbolic gestures.
Challenges in Gestures for ASD Children
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a neurodevelopmental disorder that is associated with challenges in social communication and interactions as well as the presence of restricted and repetitive patterns of behavior (American Psychiatric Association, 2013; CDC, 2020). As gestures are a social behavior, challenges and delays in deictic and symbolic gestures use for ASD individuals are documented and likely to occur (Choi et al., 2020; González-Sala et al., 2021; Iverson, et al., 2018; Ramos-Cabo et al., 2019; Ramos-Cabo et al., 2021; West et al., 2020). These differences have been found using a variety of research measurements including parent-report questionnaires (Choi et al., 2020; Iverson et al., 2018), retrospective video recordings (Colgan et al., 2006; Watson et al., 2013a), behavioral coding (Delehanty & Wetherby, 2021, 2022; Thiemann-Bourque et al., 2012, 2019), and behavioral assessments (Hobson et al., 2015; Lee et al., 2016; Talbott et al., 2015; Veness et al., 2012; Ye et al., 2021).
Before 2 years of age, autistic children often produce fewer gestures and have a smaller inventory of gestures when compared to non-autistic children. Group differences in gesture use have been reported for infants who are at high likelihood for ASD (HL) and later meet diagnostic criteria for ASD (HL-ASD), group differences in gesture use have been reported (Choi et al., 2020; Franchini et al., 2018; Iverson et al., 2018; Manwaring et al., 2018; Mitchell et al., 2006). For example, Iverson et al. (2018) conducted a small study (n = 14 HL-ASD) that looked at the trajectory of gesture development from 8 to 14 months of age for infants with and without high likelihood for ASD by collecting the quantity of gestures with the MCDI–Words and Gestures. The researchers found that HL-ASD infants have attenuated gesture development from 8 to 14 months of age in both Early and Later Gestures when compared to three peer groups: infants with low likelihood for ASD who did not meet criteria for ASD (LL-Neg), infants with high likelihood for ASD who did not meet criteria for ASD (HL-Neg), and infants with high likelihood for ASD who showed signs of early language delays (HL-LD).
In another study including infants with a family history of autism, Franchini et al. (2018) collected longitudinal MCDI–Words and Gestures data on gesture use when infants were 9 to 24 months of age. Using a data-driven (i.e., agnostic to diagnostic outcome) approach, results revealed three developmental trajectories: high, intermediate, and low gesture inventories. In addition, results showed that there was a greater percentage of the HL-ASD group in the low trajectory group when compared to the percentage of HL-Neg and LL-Neg infants. However, when comparing across the HL-ASD infants, more HL-ASD infants fell into the intermediate trajectory than in the low or high trajectories.
Symbolic skills within the first two years were also found to be affected in autistic children (Christensen et al., 2010; Landa et al., 2007; Wetherby et al., 2004, 2007). Specifically, Christensen et al. (2010) and Landa et al. (2007) found that HL-ASD infants (respectfully) showed less functional and symbolic play than HL-Neg and LL-Neg infants. Using a population screening approach, Wetherby et al. (2004, 2007) found similar results. Toddlers who met criteria for autism at 30 months of age showed less functional and symbolic play compared to toddlers without autism. Overall indications in the literature suggest that autistic toddlers are more likely to be challenged in the mastery of symbolic skills.
These challenges in symbolic skills are persistent beyond the first two years of life for many autistic children (Hobson et al., 2013, 2015; Jarrold, 2003; Lee et al., 2016; Rutherford et al., 2007; Rutherford & Rogers, 2003; Strid et al., 2013; Thiemann-Bourque et al., 2019). Hobson et al. (2013) tested pretend play skills in autistic children aged 2-7 years and a language-matched group of non-autistic children with developmental delays. Hobson et al. found lower pretend play scores in the autistic group when compared to the developmental delay group — a pattern suggesting that challenges in the symbolic skills specific to pretend play may be uniquely impacted by autism. Lee et al. (2016) added more nuance to the symbolic difficulties in autistic children as they found that autistic children between the ages of 3 and 7 years had more difficulties generating symbolic play on their own than their non-autistic peers. Taken together, the results from previous studies suggest that challenges in symbolic skills are present early on and continue to be a challenge for some autistic children well into the school age years.
Goals of the Current Study
Despite a growing body of literature on the challenges many autistic children experience with communicative gestures and symbolic skills, there are limited longitudinal studies that investigate gesture use within the first two years of life. Of the existing literature, only two studies have looked at the development of symbolic gestures along with communicative gestures during infancy, and neither study included a time point beyond 14 months of age (Iverson et al., 2018; West et al., 2020). The goal of the current study is to use a prospective research design with a large sample of infants to assess the development of communicative and symbolic gestures in the first two years. To accomplish this goal, the MCDI–Words and Gestures measure (Fenson et al., 2006) was collected at 12, 18, and 24 months of age from parents of infants with an increased likelihood of autism who later meet diagnostic criteria for autism (HL-ASD, n = 73). Control groups included infants with a high likelihood of having autism who were not diagnosed with autism (HL-Neg, n = 249) and infants with a low likelihood of having autism who were not diagnosed with autism (LL-Neg, n = 145). We hypothesize that the HL-ASD group will have smaller Early and Later Gestures inventories at all three time points and show attenuated growth relative to the HL-Neg group and the LL-Neg group.
Methods
Participants
Data from this study were collected by the Infant Brain Imaging Study (IBIS) Network at four locations: the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, the University of Washington, The Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia, and Washington University in St. Louis. Families were enrolled in the study when infants were between 3 and 12 months of age, with follow-up assessments at 12, 18, and 24 months of age. Institutional Review Boards approved the study procedures at each of the four data collection sites. Written informed consent was obtained from the parent or legal guardian of all participants prior to enrollment. The LORIS data management platform served as the behavioral and clinical hub for data collection, curation, and archiving (Das et al., 2016).
The present study includes infants who (1) completed the MCDI–Words and Gestures (Fenson et al., 2006) at 12, 18, and/or 24 months and (2) completed assessments for clinical best estimate diagnosis of ASD at 24 months (N = 467; 274 Male and 193 Female; see Table 1). Participants who were outliers based on chronological age older than 27 months of age on the MCDI–Words and Gestures (N = 8; 2 LL-Neg, 5 HL-Neg and 1 HL-ASD) were excluded from the analyses because such data have the potential to influence longitudinal trajectories. Clinical best estimate diagnoses were made by licensed clinicians based at 24 months of age on DSM-IV TR criteria for autistic disorder or pervasive developmental disorder–not otherwise specified (hereafter ASD) using all available assessment data (Estes et al., 2015). Materials include the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule (Lord et al., 2000), the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised (Rutter et al., 2003), the Mullen Scales of Early Learning (Mullen, 1995), and the Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales II (Sparrow et al., 2005). A priori examiner training and administration and scoring reliability procedures were implemented to ensure comparability of data across sites. Further details of the assessment and diagnostic protocol can be found in Estes et al. (2015).
Table 1.
Participant Demographic
LL-Neg | HL-Neg | HL-ASD | |
---|---|---|---|
n | 145 | 249 | 73 |
Sex (%) | |||
Female | 60 (41%) | 116 (47%) | 17 (23%) |
Male | 85 (59%) | 133 (53%) | 56 (77%) |
Race (%) | |||
African American | 4 (3%) | 5 (2%) | 2 (3%) |
Asian | 2 (1%) | 4 (2%) | 1 (1%) |
More than one | 11 (8%) | 22 (9%) | 11 (16%) |
Unknown | 0 (0%) | 1 (0%) | 1 (1%) |
White | 128 (88%) | 217 (87%) | 58 (78%) |
Hispanic (%) | 6 (4%) | 15 (6%) | 4 (5%) |
Maternal Ed (%) | |||
Some college or less | 19 (13%) | 74 (30%) | 33 (45%) |
College degree | 61 (42%) | 113 (45%) | 23 (32%) |
Graduate degree | 65 (45%) | 62 (25%) | 17 (23%) |
Notes. HL-ASD, high likelihood for autism infants who were diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age; HL-Neg, high likelihood for autism infants who were not diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age; LL-Neg, low likelihood for autism infants who were not diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age.
Infants were separated into three groups: HL-ASD, HL-Neg and LL-Neg. Infants were considered HL if they had an older sibling with ASD confirmed with the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised and medical records. HL infants were grouped as HL-ASD if the infants themselves met the clinical best estimate for ASD at 24 months. HL infants were grouped as HL-Neg if the infants did not meet the clinical best estimate for ASD at 24 months. LL-Neg infants had siblings that were typically developing, no first-degree relatives with ASD, and did not receive a diagnosis of ASD themselves at 24 months.
Procedure
The MCDI–Words and Gestures is a parent-report questionnaire designed to capture early language and communication development for typically developing children between the ages of 8 and 18 months (Fenson et al., 2006; Hutchins, 2013). However, this inventory has been used in various clinical and non-clinical populations, with the same efficacy in assessing language and communication skills, including autistic children from 8 months of age up to 36 months (Charman et al., 2003; Iverson et al., 2018; Luyster et al., 2008; Mitchell et al., 2006; Smith et al., 2007). To measure the growth of gestures in the first two years of development and because it is expected that some HL infants may have language delays (HL-ASD and HL-Neg), the MCDI–Words and Gestures form was collected at 12, 18, and 24 months of age (Landa & Garrett-Mayer, 2006; Swanson et al., 2017). The current work focuses on the Actions and Gestures section of the MCDI, which includes sections on Early and Later Gestures. Early Gestures consists of “First Communicative Gesture” and “Games and Routines,” with a total possible raw score of 18. Later Gestures consists of “Actions with Objects,” “Pretending to be a Parent,” and “Imitating Other Adult Actions,” with a total possible raw score of 45.
Analysis
To longitudinally characterize the development of gesture inventories for each group, general linear mixed models were used to analyze both Early and Later Gestures with the lmerTest package (Kuznetsova et al., 2020) in R 4.1.0 (R Core Team, 2021). General linear mixed models were used because this program can analyze repeated dependent variables and can handle missing data through maximum likelihood estimation. Dependent variables for the models were Early and Later Gestures raw scores from the MCDI–Words and Gestures. Predictor variables included the diagnostic grouping at 24 months, chronological age in months, and the interaction between diagnostic grouping and chronological age. Covariates were selected a priori and included the sex of the infants, the data collection sites, and maternal education. These covariates were included in all models. A random intercept for each participant was included in the models to account for repeated measures. To compare the differences in gesture growth in Early and Later Gestures, estimated marginal slopes were used as a pairwise comparison with a Tukey correction. Estimated marginal slopes were estimated with the R package emmeans (Lenth et al., 2022).
Cross-sectional analysis with generalized linear models was used to look at the effect sizes for the main effects of diagnostic grouping and age and the effect sizes of pairwise differences within the diagnostic grouping at 12, 18, and 24 months. Generalized linear models for cross-sectional analyses were performed with the stats package in R (R Core Team, 2021). Dependent variables were Early and Later Gestures from the MCDI–Words and Gestures. Predictor variables were diagnostic grouping and chronological age in months. Covariates include the sex of the infants, the data collection sites, and maternal education. Effect sizes (eta-squared; η2) for the main effects of diagnostic grouping and chronological age in months were conducted with the effectsize R package (Ben-Shachar et al., 2020). Effect sizes, eta-squared, were estimated using the sum of squares of the main effects divided by the total sum of squares. Cohen’s d was used for effect sizes of the pairwise differences for the diagnostic grouping using the emmeans R package (Lenth et al., 2022). Cohen’s d was computed using the estimated pairwise differences divided by the standard deviation of the data.
Results
We examined the differences in longitudinal gesture inventories across three different groups: LL-Neg, HL-Neg, and HL-ASD. Table 2 gives the means and standard deviations of the different groups per visit. Supplementary Tables 1 and 2 show the full results of the general linear mixed models.
Table 2.
Age, MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventory - Words and Gestures Scores, and Mullen Scales of Early Learning: Expressive and Receptive Language Scores
LL-Neg | HL-Neg | HL-ASD | |
---|---|---|---|
Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | Mean (SD) | |
Age 12 months | |||
n | 112 | 199 | 57 |
Age (months) | 12.60 (0.65) | 12.57 (0.56) | 12.60 (0.63) |
Mullen Expressive | 12.83 (2.76) | 11.94 (2.94) | 10.34 (2.90) |
Mullen Receptive | 12.40 (1.93) | 11.14 (2.44) | 9.70 (2.52) |
Early Raw Scores (SD) | 10.70 (2.63) | 10.09 (2.94) | 8.26 (3.34) |
Later Raw Scores (SD) | 14.30 (7.46) | 11.23 (6.67) | 8.79 (6.34) |
Age 18 months | |||
n | 51 | 80 | 23 |
Age (months) | 18.45 (0.64) | 18.35 (0.92) | 18.38 (1.00) |
Early Raw Scores (SD) | 14.88 (2.22) | 14.18 (2.63) | 11.74 (4.03) |
Later Raw Scores (SD) | 29.46 (7.24) | 26.27 (7.44) | 20.23 (7.83) |
Age 24 months | |||
n | 108 | 183 | 60 |
Age (months) | 24.59 (0.73) | 24.64 (0.69) | 24.53 (0.78) |
Mullen Expressive | 25.49 (4.99) | 24.24 (5.29) | 18.17 (5.50) |
Mullen Receptive | 27.49 (3.98) | 26.08 (4.37) | 17.95 (7.77) |
Early Raw Scores (SD) | 16.44 (1.69) | 16.04 (2.00) | 12.33 (3.17) |
Later Raw Scores (SD) | 37.86 (5.53) | 34.38 (6.84) | 24.10 (8.22) |
Notes. HL-ASD, high likelihood for autism infants who were diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age; HL-Neg, high likelihood for autism infants who were not diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age; LL-Neg, low likelihood for autism infants who were not diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age; SD indicates standard deviation; Receptive and Expressive Language are collected from Mullen Scales of Early Learning and are scores based on age of equivalence. Mullen measure was only collected at 12 and 24 months of age.
Early Gestures
Figure 1 shows the longitudinal results of Early Gestures for which there was a significant interaction between diagnostic group and chronological age, F(2,459) = 9.44, p < .001 (Table 3). Estimated marginal slopes (Table 4) for the interaction between diagnostic group and chronological age indicated that HL-ASD infants had slower growth for Early Gestures when compared to HL-Neg and LL-Neg.
Figure 1. Early Gesture Growth Differed by Diagnostic Grouping.
Notes. Interaction between chronological age in months and diagnostic grouping in Early Gestures of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Developmental Inventory – Words and Gestures. HL-ASD, high likelihood for autism infants who were diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age; HL-Neg, high likelihood for autism infants who were not diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age; LL-Neg, low likelihood for autism infants who were not diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age.
Table 3.
ANOVA Table. Type III F tests with Kenward-Roger (Kenward & Roger, 1997) Degrees of Freedom
F | df | df residuals | p | |
---|---|---|---|---|
Early Gestures | ||||
Intercept*** | 1952.61 | 1 | 465.83 | < .001 |
Age*** | 424.23 | 1 | 543.19 | < .001 |
Diagnosis*** | 47.64 | 2 | 438.69 | < .001 |
DiagnosisAge*** | 9.44 | 2 | 524.47 | < .001 |
Maternal Ed | 1.49 | 2 | 441.01 | .226 |
Sex*** | 11.19 | 1 | 448.72 | < .001 |
Site | 2.62 | 3 | 442.87 | .050 |
Later Gestures | ||||
Intercept*** | 1125.30 | 1 | 440.91 | < .001 |
Age*** | 1030.42 | 1 | 482.91 | < .001 |
Diagnosis*** | 48.42 | 2 | 341.57 | < .001 |
DiagnosisAge*** | 23.47 | 2 | 474.63 | < .001 |
Maternal Ed | .87 | 2 | 431.34 | .421 |
Sex*** | 20.15 | 1 | 435.58 | < .001 |
Site | 2.08 | 3 | 430.97 | .103 |
Notes. References for each variable are Diagnosis as LL-Neg, Maternal Education as College Graduate, Sex as Female, Site as UNC-Chapel Hill; df indicates degrees of freedom; dfresiduals indicates residuals for degrees of freedom
Table 4.
Estimated Marginal Slopes
LL-Neg (a) | HL-Neg (b) | HL-ASD (c) | ||
---|---|---|---|---|
EMS (SE) | EMS (SE) | EMS (SE) | Pairwise Comparison | |
Early Gesture | ||||
Interaction | 2.75 (.13) | 2.80 (.10) | 1.93 (.18) | c < b, a |
Later Gesture | ||||
Interaction | 11.10 (.35) | 10.94 (.26) | 7.51 (.47) | c < b, a |
Notes. HL-ASD, high likelihood for autism infants who were diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age; HL-Neg, high likelihood for autism infants who were not diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age; LL-Neg, low likelihood for autism infants who were not diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age. Pairwise comparison indication is based on Tukey adjusted p-value of p < .05; EMS indicates estimated marginal slopes; SE indicates standard error.
Follow-up tests revealed significant main effects for the diagnostic grouping, with a medium effect size (η2) at 12 months, a medium effect size at 18 months, and a large effect size at 24 months (Table 5). The main effect of age was significant only at 12 months (η2 = .03, p < .001, CI [.01, .07]). Group pairwise comparisons are presented in Table 6 for each time point. Across all time points, the HL-ASD group had significantly fewer Early Gestures when compared to the LL-Neg and HL-Neg groups. In contrast, the HL-Neg and LL-Neg groups did not significantly differ on the number of Early Gestures at any time point.
Table 5.
Effect Sizes (eta-squared) for Main Effects of Diagnostic Grouping and Age
Gestures | Diagnostic Group | Age | ||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
η2 | 95% CI | p | η2 | [95% CI] | p | |
Early Gestures | ||||||
12 months | .07 | [.03, .12] | < .001 | .03 | [.01, .07] | < .001 |
18 months | .12 | [.05, .22] | < .001 | .00 | [.00, .05] | .182 |
24 months | .32 | [.25, .39] | < .001 | .00 | [.00, .03] | .167 |
Later Gestures | ||||||
12 months | .07 | [.03, .12] | < .001 | .10 | [.05, .16] | < .001 |
18 months | .15 | [.06, .25] | < .001 | .01 | [.00, .08] | < .05 |
24 months | .30 | [.23, .37] | < .001 | .00 | [.00, .03] | .253 |
Notes. p-values are p-values for the main effects Diagnostic Group and Age at the different time points. CI: confidence interval; η2 indicates generalized eta-squared: η2 of .01 represents a small effect, η2 of .06 represents a medium effect, η2 of .14 represents a large effect.
Table 6.
Effect Size (Cohen’s d; Cohen, 1988) for ANOVA Contrast in Diagnostic Grouping of Early Gesture
Pairwise Comparison for Diagnosis | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Early Gestures | EMM | SE | p | Cohen’s d | Cohen’s d 95% CI |
SE | df |
12 Months | |||||||
LL-Neg vs. HL-Neg | 0.77 | 0.35 | .073 | 0.27 | [0.03, 0.51] | 0.12 | 358 |
*** LL-Neg vs. HL-ASD | 2.44 | 0.49 | < .001 | 0.86 | [0.52, 1.20] | 0.17 | 358 |
*** HL-Neg vs. HL-ASD | 1.67 | 0.44 | < .001 | 0.59 | [0.28, 0.90] | 0.16 | 358 |
18 Months | |||||||
LL-Neg vs. HL-Neg | 0.92 | 0.50 | .154 | 0.34 | [0.02, 0.71] | 0.19 | 144 |
*** LL-Neg vs. HL-ASD | 2.98 | 0.71 | < .001 | 1.11 | [0.57, 1.65] | 0.27 | 144 |
** HL-Neg vs. HL-ASD | 2.05 | 0.66 | .006 | 0.77 | [0.27, 1.26] | 0.25 | 144 |
24 Months | |||||||
LL-Neg vs. HL-Neg | 0.37 | 0.27 | .350 | 0.18 | [0.08, 0.43] | 0.13 | 341 |
*** LL-Neg vs. HL-ASD | 3.83 | 0.36 | < .001 | 1.80 | [1.44, 2.16] | 0.18 | 341 |
*** HL-Neg vs. HL-ASD | 3.46 | 0.33 | < .001 | 1.63 | [1.30, 1.96] | 1.96 | 341 |
Notes. * p < .05, ** p < .01, ***p < .001. HL-ASD, high likelihood for autism infants who were diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age; HL-Neg, high likelihood for autism infants who were not diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age; LL-Neg, low likelihood for autism infants who were not diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age. SD indicates standard deviation. Pairwise comparison indication is based on Tukey adjusted p-value of p <.05; EMS indicates estimated marginal means; SE indicates standard error; df indicates degrees of freedom; CI indicates confidence interval; Cohen’s d effect size index: 0.2 represents a small effect, 0.5 represents a medium effect, 0.8 represents a large effect
Later Gestures
Figure 2 shows the longitudinal results of Later Gestures, for which there was a significant interaction between diagnosis and chronological age, F(2,451) = 23.47, p < .001 (see Table 3). Estimated marginal slopes (Table 4) for the interaction between diagnostic group and chronological age indicated that HL-ASD infants had slower growth in Later Gestures when compared to HL-Neg and LL-Neg.
Figure 2. Later Gesture Growth Differed by Diagnostic Grouping.
Notes. Interaction between chronological age in months and diagnostic grouping in Later Gestures of the MacArthur-Bates Communicative Developmental Inventory – Words and Gestures. HL-ASD, high likelihood for autism infants who were diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age; HL-Neg, high likelihood for autism infants who were not diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age; LL-Neg, low likelihood for autism infants who were not diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age.
Cross-sectional results revealed significant main effects for diagnostic grouping, with a medium effect size (η2) at 12 months and large effect sizes at 18 and 24 months (Table 5). In Later Gestures, the main effect of age was significant at 12 months (η2 = .10, p < .001, 95% CI [.04, .16]) and at 18 months (η2 = .01, p < .05, 95% CI [.00, .08]), but not at 24 months (η2 = 0, p = .25, 95% CI [.00, .03]). Group pairwise comparisons are presented on Table 7 by time point. Across all time points, the HL-ASD group had significantly fewer Later Gestures when compared to the LL-Neg group. The HL-ASD group also had significantly fewer Later Gestures when compared to the HL-Neg group at 18 and 24 months. The two groups were not significantly different from one another at 12 months (d = 0.37, p = .06, 95% CI [.05, .69]). The HL-Neg group had significantly fewer Later Gestures when compared to the LL-Neg group at 12 and 24 months, but the two groups did not significantly differ at 18 months (d = 0.39, p = .11, 95% CI [.00, .77]) (Table 7).
Table 7.
Effect Size (Cohen’s d) for ANOVA Contrast in Diagnostic Grouping of Later Gesture
Pairwise Comparison for Diagnosis | |||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Later Gestures | EMM | SE | p | Cohen’s d | Cohen’s d 95% CI |
SE | df |
12 Months | |||||||
*** LL-Neg vs. HL-Neg | 3.33 | 0.83 | < .001 | 0.52 | [0.26, 0.78] | 0.13 | 331 |
*** LL-Neg vs. HL-ASD | 5.70 | 1.14 | < .001 | 0.89 | [0.53, 1.25] | 0.18 | 331 |
HL-Neg vs. HL-ASD | 2.36 | 1.02 | .055 | 0.37 | [0.05, 0.69] | 0.16 | 331 |
18 Months | |||||||
LL-Neg vs. HL-Neg | 2.76 | 1.38 | .115 | 0.39 | [0.00, 0.77] | 0.20 | 130 |
*** LL-Neg vs. HL-ASD | 8.63 | 1.92 | < .001 | 1.21 | [0.67, 1.77] | 0.28 | 130 |
** HL-Neg vs. HL-ASD | 5.87 | 1.80 | .004 | 0.82 | [0.31, 1.33] | 0.26 | 130 |
24 Months | |||||||
*** LL-Neg vs. HL-Neg | 3.32 | 0.87 | < .001 | 0.51 | [0.25, 0.78] | 0.14 | 318 |
*** LL-Neg vs. HL-ASD | 11.78 | 1.15 | < .001 | 1.81 | [1.44, 2.19] | 0.19 | 318 |
*** HL-Neg vs. HL-ASD | 8.46 | 1.04 | < .001 | 1.30 | [0.97, 1.63] | 0.17 | 318 |
Notes. * p < .05, ** p < .01, ***p < .001. HL-ASD, high likelihood for autism infants who were diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age; HL-Neg, high likelihood for autism infants who were not diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age; LL-Neg, low likelihood for autism infants who were not diagnosed with ASD at 24 months of age. SD indicates standard deviation. Post-hoc comparison indication is based on Tukey adjusted p-value of p <.05; EMS indicates estimated marginal means; SE indicates standard error; df indicates degrees of freedom; CI indicates confidence interval; Cohen’s d effect size index: 0.2 represents a small effect, 0.5 represents a medium effect, 0.8 represents a large effect.
Discussion
Previous prospective studies show that autistic children have challenges in symbolic gestures as early as 12 months of age; however, there are few longitudinal studies that track gestures from infancy to 2 years of age. The current prospective study used a large sample size (N = 467; 73 HL-ASD, 249 HL-Neg, and 145 LL-Neg) to assess whether communicative and symbolic gestures differ between autistic children and non-autistic children in the first two years of development. Using the MCDI–Words and Gestures, longitudinal and cross-sectional analyses show that HL-ASD infants acquired and used fewer deictic and symbolic gestures from 12 to 24 months when compared to the HL-Neg and LL-Neg infants.
The Early Gestures inventory from the MCDI–Words and Gestures includes deictic gestures, conventional gestures, and games and routines. These types of gestures represent the proficiency of skillsets in intentional communication and social interactions with others. As a group, infants and toddlers with ASD have been reported to display fewer gestures that involve intentional communication and social interactions with others than their non-ASD peers (Stone et al., 1997; Volkmar et al., 2005; Watson et al., 2013; West et al., 2020; Wetherby et al., 1998; Zwaigenbaum et al., 2005). Previous research with small samples (n < 15 in the HL-ASD group) found that HL-ASD infants have slower early gesture development from 8 to 14 months of age than HL-Neg and LL-Neg infants (Iverson et al., 2018; West et al., 2020). The current results support this previous work using a larger sample and extends the findings by showing that challenges with early gesture development persist until at least 24 months of age.
The Later Gestures inventory from the MCDI–Words and Gestures taps into behaviors that are indicative of representational skills and symbolic abilities. Items include knowledge of appropriate usage of items and actions, imitating adult actions, and using objects to represent other objects (symbolic play). Previous and current results show that autistic toddlers and preschoolers produce fewer symbolic gestures than their peers (González-Sala et al., 2021; Rogers et al., 2003; Williams et al., 2001). This lack of production in symbolic gestures is most likely due to challenges in social communication skills and interests in demonstrating these actions rather than challenges in symbolic abilities (Ingersoll & Gergans, 2007; Jarrold, 2003; Smith & Bryson, 2007). Previous research supports this claim because autistic children are able to produce symbolic actions when prompted (Charman & Baron-Cohen, 1997; Hamilton et al., 2007; Jarrold, 2003; Wainwright et al., 2020). The current study contributes to this body of work and shows that a lack of participation in symbolic gestures can be assessed in the first year of development.
The MCDI–Words and Gestures is a parent-report measure that has consistently shown to be a reliable tool to assess gestures as an early behavioral indicator for later ASD, with multiple studies reporting group differences in the first year of life (Charman et al., 2003; Franchini et al., 2018; Iverson et al., 2018; Mitchell et al., 2006; Whitehouse et al., 2019). Data from our current study also shows that group-level differences in gesture use are apparent at 12 months of age, well before the infants were old enough for formal ASD evaluations. Overall, the MCDI–Words and Gestures contributes to the expanding research on early signs of ASD by showing that parental observations and concern for ASD arise before children are diagnosed with ASD (Ozonoff et al., 2009).
There is mounting evidence that the MCDI–Words and Gestures captures delays in gesture use before ASD diagnoses are established; hence, future efforts should evaluate the measure as part of a suite of early screening measures. For example, Doyle et al. (2021) recruited a large community sample of parents of toddlers and collected parent-report measures of toddler repetitive behavior, reciprocal social communication, and word production and gestures (via the MCDI–Words and Gestures). Using a data-driven, RDoC-inspired approach, factor mixture models identified a five-class solution, of which two classes were “high-risk.” Follow-up analyses indicated that these two classes also had elevated rates of externalizing symptoms and dysregulated behaviors. “Phenoscreening” addresses concerns around the generalizability of baby-sibling studies by providing a practical solution to community screening that is relatively low-cost and can be performed remotely.
Early Intervention to Support Communication
Early detection of potential markers for ASD can provide important insight into targets for early intervention. As infancy is a time of high neuroplasticity and potentially a sensitive period for language acquisition, early intervention provided during this period of development may be more effective in supporting communication skills than intervention during the preschool years (Zeanah et al., 2011). For example, intervention programs such as the Early Start Denver Model are more effective when delivered during toddlerhood than interventions delivered during school age (Vivanti et al., 2016). Another study that focused on the timing of interventions found that toddlers who entered a parent-implemented intervention called the Early Social Interaction Model at 18 months showed greater gains in social communication, including gesture use, and language during the intervention than toddlers who entered the intervention at 27 months of age (Guthrie et al., 2023).
There is a small body of research showing that gestures and symbolic skills are an actionable intervention target for toddlers with ASD. In single-subject designs, autistic toddlers who participated in gesture interventions showed an increase in gestures and in overall language and communication (Ingersoll et al., 2007; Ingersoll & Gergans, 2007; Ingersoll & Lalonde, 2010). Similarly, interventions in social engagement improve spontaneous social behaviors, including imitation and play schemes (Ingersoll et al., 2007; Ingersoll & Walton, 2013; Kasari et al., 2006; Whalen et al., 2006). While these studies are not preemptive interventions, they do indicate that early intervention for autism that focuses on developmentally appropriate deictic gestures and symbolic skills may support later language skills. Future studies should determine whether early gesture interventions, before ASD diagnosis, can improve later communication skills.
Limitations
The current study used a contemporaneous parent report to quantify gesture inventories, but direct tests of gesture production may yield different information. Another limitation is that we considered the HL-Neg group as a whole and did not derive subgroups based on language ability or some other variable (Franchini et al., 2018; Iverson et al., 2018; Mitchell et al., 2006). Sub-grouping the HL-Neg may provide a better understanding of gesture production within this specific group.
Infant-sibling designs are unique and have limitations. The current study sample has restricted diversity in terms of race and ethnicity—a problem which could impact the generalizability of the results. Additionally, since the infants have an older sibling with ASD, parents’ perceptions of their children’s skillsets may differ from the perception of parents of typically developing children.
Conclusion
Gesture use is an important communicative tool for early social communication and language. As a group, HL-ASD infants have attenuated gesture growth compared to the control groups. In both Early and Later Gestures of the MCDI–Words and Gestures, the HL-ASD group had smaller inventories compared to the LL-Neg group. In contrast, HL-ASD had smaller inventories compared to HL-Neg at almost all time points in Early and Later Gestures, except for Later Gestures at 12 months. Infants in the HL-Neg group did not differ in rate of gesture growth compared to infants in the LL-Neg group. While the HL-Neg group did not statistically differ from the LL-Neg group in the number of Later Gestures at 18 months, the effect size for this comparison was small. Results from this study indicate that parent-report questionnaires are sensitive to developmental differences in gesture skills well before toddlers are diagnosed with ASD. Understanding the early behavioral traits of ASD before formal diagnosis provides valuable information for pre-symptomatic intervention programs.
Supplementary Material
ACKNOWLEDGMENTS
This work was supported by grants through the National Institutes of Health (R00-MH108700 PI Swanson, R01-HD055741 PI Piven, R01-HD055741-S1 PI Piven, P30-HD003110 PI Piven, U54 EB005149 PI Kikinis) and the Simons Foundation (SFARI Grant 140209). We would also like to give thanks to all the families for their participation in the study. The funders had no role in study design, data collection, analysis, data interpretation, or the writing of the manuscript.
Footnotes
CONFLICT OF INTEREST STATEMENT
The authors declare no conflicts of interest.
ETHICS STATEMENT
The study was conducted in accordance with the US Federal Policy for the Protection of Human Subjects by receiving formal approval from the Institutional Board Review.
The Infant Brain Imaging Study (IBIS) Network is an NIH funded Autism Centers of Excellence project and consists of a consortium of 9 universities in the U.S. and Canada. Clinical Sites: University of North Carolina: J. Piven (IBIS Network PI), H. C. Hazlett, C. Chappell; University of Washington: S. Dager, A. Estes, D. Shaw; Washington University: K. Botteron, R. McKinstry, J. Constantino, J. Pruett: Children’s Hospital of Philadelphia: R. Schultz, J. Pandey: University of Alberta: L. Zwaigenbaum: University of Minnesota; J. T. Elison, J. Wolff. Data Coordinating Center: Montreal Neurological Institute: A. C. Evans, D. L. Collins, G. B. Pike, V. Fonov, P. Kostopoulos, S. Das, L. MacIntyre. Image Processing Core: New York University: G. Gerig: University of North Carolina: M. Styner. Statistical Analysis Core: University of North Carolina: H. Gu.
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.
References
- Acredolo L, & Goodwyn S (1988). Symbolic Gesturing in Normal Infants. Child Development, 59(2), 450–466. 10.2307/1130324 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- American Psychiatric Association. (2013). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders (Fifth Edition). 10.1176/appi.books.9780890425596 [DOI]
- Bates E, Benigni L, Bretherton I, Camaioni L, & Volterra V (1979). The Emergence of Symbols: Cognition and Communication in Infancy. Academic Press. [Google Scholar]
- Ben-Shachar MS, Lüdecke D, & Makowski D (2020). effectsize: Estimation of effect size indices and standardized parameters. Journal of Open Source Software, 5(56), 2815. 10.21105/joss.02815 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bretherton I, & Bates E (1979). The emergence of intentional communication. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1979(4), 81–100. 10.1002/cd.23219790407 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- CDC. (2020, March 25). Basics About Autism Spectrum Disorder (ASD) ∣ NCBDDD ∣ CDC. Centers for Disease Control and Prevention. https://www.cdc.gov/ncbddd/autism/facts.html [Google Scholar]
- Charman T, & Baron-Cohen S (1997). Brief report: Prompted pretend play in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(3), 325–332. 10.1023/A:1025806616149 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Charman T, Drew A, Baird C, & Baird G (2003). Measuring early language development in preschool children with autism spectrum disorder using the MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory (Infant Form). Journal of Child Language, 30(1), 213–236. 10.1017/S0305000902005482 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi B, Shah P, Rowe ML, Nelson CA, & Tager-Flusberg H (2020). Gesture development, caregiver responsiveness, and language and diagnostic outcomes in infants at high and low risk for autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 50(7), 2556–2572. 10.1007/s10803-019-03980-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi B, Wei R, & Rowe ML (2021). Show, give, and point gestures across infancy differentially predict language development. Developmental Psychology, 57(6), 851–862. 10.1037/dev0001195 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christensen L, Hutman T, Rozga A, Young GS, Ozonoff S, Rogers SJ, Baker B, & Sigman M (2010). Play and developmental outcomes in infant siblings of children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 40(8), 946–957. 10.1007/s10803-010-0941-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Clough S, & Duff MC (2020). The role of gesture in communication and cognition: Implications for understanding and treating neurogenic communication disorders. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 14, 323. 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00323 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cohen J. (1988). Statistical Power Analysis for the Behavioral Sciences (2nd ed.). Routledge. 10.4324/9780203771587 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Colgan SE, Lanter E, McComish C, Watson LR, Crais ER, & Baranek GT (2006). Analysis of social interaction gestures in infants with autism. Child Neuropsychology: A Journal on Normal and Abnormal Development in Childhood and Adolescence, 12(4–5), 307–319. 10.1080/09297040600701360 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Colonnesi C, Stams GJJM, Koster I, & Noom MJ (2010). The relation between pointing and language development: A meta-analysis. Developmental Review, 30(4), 352–366. 10.1016/j.dr.2010.10.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Corrigan R. (1983). The development of representational skills. New Directions for Child and Adolescent Development, 1983(21), 51–64. 10.1002/cd.23219832106 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Das S, Glatard T, MacIntyre LC, Madjar C, Rogers C, Rousseau M-E, Rioux P, MacFarlane D, Mohades Z, Gnanasekaran R, Makowski C, Kostopoulos P, Adalat R, Khalili-Mahani N, Niso G, Moreau JT, & Evans AC (2016). The MNI data-sharing and processing ecosystem. NeuroImage, 124, 1188–1195. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.08.076 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delehanty A, & Wetherby AM (2021). Rate of communicative gestures and developmental outcomes in toddlers with and without autism spectrum disorder during a home observation. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 30(2), 649–662. 10.1044/2020_AJSLP-19-00206 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delehanty A, & Wetherby AM (2022). Coordinated social communication in toddlers with and without autism spectrum disorder during a home observation. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments, 7, 23969415221121089. 10.1177/23969415221121089 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Deloache JS (1995). Early Understanding and Use of Symbols: The Model Model. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 4(4), 109–113. Scopus. 10.1111/1467-8721.ep10772408 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- DeLoache JS (2004). Becoming symbol-minded. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 8(2), 66–70. 10.1016/j.tics.2003.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Doyle CM, Lasch C, Vollman EP, Desjardins CD, Helwig NE, Jacob S, Wolff JJ, & Elison JT (2021). Phenoscreening: A developmental approach to research domain criteria-motivated sampling. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 62(7), 884–894. 10.1111/jcpp.13341 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Escalona SK (1973). Basic modes of social interaction: Their emergence and patterning during the first two years of life. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Behavior and Development, 19(3), 205–232. [Google Scholar]
- Estes A, Zwaigenbaum L, Gu H, St. John T, Paterson S, Elison JT, Hazlett H, Botteron K, Dager SR, Schultz RT, Kostopoulos P, Evans A, Dawson G, Eliason J, Alvarez S, & Piven J (2015). Behavioral, cognitive, and adaptive development in infants with autism spectrum disorder in the first 2 years of life. Journal of Neurodevelopmental Disorders, 7(1). 10.1186/s11689-015-9117-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Fein GG (1981). Pretend play in childhood: An Integrative Review. Child Development, 52(4), 1095–1118. 10.2307/1129497 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fenson L, Marchman VA, Thal DJ, Dale PS, Steven Reznick J, & Bates E (2006). The MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories User’s Guide and Technical Manual, Second Edition. Brookes. [Google Scholar]
- Franchini M, Duku E, Armstrong V, Brian J, Bryson SE, Garon N, Roberts W, Roncadin C, Zwaigenbaum L, & Smith IM (2018). Variability in verbal and nonverbal communication in infants at risk for autism spectrum disorder: Predictors and outcomes. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(10), 3417–3431. 10.1007/s10803-018-3607-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldin-Meadow S. (2007). Gesture with speech and without it. In Duncan SD, Cassel J, & Levy E (Eds.), Gesture and the dynamic dimension of language: Essays in honor of David McNeill (pp. 31–49). John Benjamins Publishing Company. https://www.torrossa.com/en/resources/an/5001141 [Google Scholar]
- Goldin-Meadow S. (2015). Gesture and cognitive development. In Handbook of child psychology and developmental science: Cognitive processes, Vol. 2, 7th ed (pp. 339–380). John Wiley & Sons, Inc. 10.1002/9781118963418.childpsy209 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Goldin-Meadow S, & Alibali MW (2013). Gesture’s role in speaking, learning, and creating language. Annual Review of Psychology, 64, 257–283. 10.1146/annurev-psych-113011-143802 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Golomb C, & Cornelius CB (1977). Symbolic play and its cognitive significance. Developmental Psychology, 13(3), 246–252. 10.1037/0012-1649.13.3.246 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- González-Sala F, Gómez-Marí I, Tárraga-Mínguez R, Vicente-Carvajal A, & Pastor-Cerezuela G (2021). Symbolic play among children with autism spectrum disorder: A scoping review. Children, 8(9), 801. 10.3390/children8090801 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Guthrie W, Wetherby AM, Woods J, Schatschneider C, Holland RD, Morgan L, & Lord CE (2023). The earlier the better: An RCT of treatment timing effects for toddlers on the autism spectrum. Autism, 13623613231159153. 10.1177/13623613231159153 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hamilton A. F. de C., Brindley RM, & Frith U (2007). Imitation and action understanding in autistic spectrum disorders: How valid is the hypothesis of a deficit in the mirror neuron system? Neuropsychologia, 45(8), 1859–1868. 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2006.11.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hobson JA, Hobson RP, Cheung Y, & Caló S (2015). Symbolizing as interpersonally grounded shifts in meaning: Social play in children with and without autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(1), 42–52. 10.1007/s10803-014-2122-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hobson JA, Hobson RP, Malik S, Bargiota K, & Caló S (2013). The relation between social engagement and pretend play in autism. The British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 31(Pt 1), 114–127. 10.1111/j.2044-835X.2012.02083.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hutchins T. (2013). MacArthur-Bates Communicative Development Inventories, Second Edition. In Volkmar FR (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders (pp. 1773–1779). Springer. 10.1007/978-1-4419-1698-3_769 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Ingersoll B, & Gergans S (2007). The effect of a parent-implemented imitation intervention on spontaneous imitation skills in young children with autism. Research in Developmental Disabilities, 28(2), 163–175. 10.1016/j.ridd.2006.02.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ingersoll B, & Lalonde K (2010). The impact of object and gesture imitation training on language use in children with autism. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research : JSLHR, 53(4), 1040–1051. 10.1044/1092-4388(2009/09-0043) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ingersoll B, Lewis E, & Kroman E (2007). Teaching the imitation and spontaneous use of descriptive gestures in young children with autism using a naturalistic behavioral intervention. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(8), 1446–1456. 10.1007/s10803-006-0221-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ingersoll B, & Walton K (2013). Play intervention. In Volkmar FR (Ed.), Encyclopedia of Autism Spectrum Disorders (pp. 2287–2291). Springer. 10.1007/978-1-4419-1698-3_1898 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Iverson JM, Capirci O, & Caselli MC (1994). From communication to language in two modalities. Cognitive Development, 9(1), 23–43. 10.1016/0885-2014(94)90018-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Iverson JM, & Goldin-Meadow S (2005). Gesture paves the way for language development. Psychological Science, 16(5), 367–371. 10.1111/j.0956-7976.2005.01542.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Iverson JM, Northrup JB, Leezenbaum NB, Parladé MV, Koterba EA, & West KL (2018). Early gesture and vocabulary development in infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 48(1), 55–71. 10.1007/s10803-017-3297-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Jarrold C. (2003). A review of research into pretend play in autism. Autism, 7(4), 379–390. 10.1177/1362361303007004004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kasari C, Freeman S, & Paparella T (2006). Joint attention and symbolic play in young children with autism: A randomized controlled intervention study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(6), 611–620. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2005.01567.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kelly SD, Özyürek A, & Maris E (2010). Two sides of the same coin: Speech and gesture mutually interact to enhance comprehension. Psychological Science, 21(2), 260–267. 10.1177/0956797609357327 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kenward MG, & Roger JH (1997). Small sample inference for fixed effects from restricted maximum likelihood. Biometrics, 53(3), 983–997. 10.2307/2533558 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuhn LJ, Willoughby MT, Wilbourn MP, Vernon-Feagans L, & Blair CB (2014). Early communicative gestures prospectively predict language development and executive function in early childhood. Child Development, 85(5), 1898–1914. 10.1111/cdev.12249 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen RHB, & Jensen SP (2020). lmerTest: Tests in Linear Mixed Effects Models (3.1-3) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=lmerTest [Google Scholar]
- Landa R, & Garrett-Mayer E (2006). Development in infants with autism spectrum disorders: A prospective study. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 47(6), 629–638. 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2006.01531.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Landa RJ, Holman KC, & Garrett-Mayer E (2007). Social and communication development in toddlers with early and later diagnosis of autism spectrum disorders. Archives of General Psychiatry, 64(7), 853. 10.1001/archpsyc.64.7.853 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lee Y-C, Chan P-C, Lin S-K, Chen C-T, Huang C-Y, & Chen K-L (2016). Correlation patterns between pretend play and playfulness in children with autism spectrum disorder, developmental delay, and typical development. Research in Autism Spectrum Disorders, 24, 29–38. 10.1016/j.rasd.2016.01.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Lenth RV, Buerkner P, Herve M, Love J, Miguez F, Riebl H, & Singmann H (2022). emmeans: Estimated Marginal Means, aka Least-Squares Means (1.7.2) [Computer software]. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=emmeans [Google Scholar]
- Liszkowski U. (2014). Two sources of meaning in infant communication: Preceding action contexts and act-accompanying characteristics. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 369(1651). 10.1098/rstb.2013.0294 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lord C, Risi S, Lambrecht L, Cook EH, Leventhal BL, DiLavore PC, Pickles A, & Rutter M (2000). The Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule—Generic: A standard measure of social and communication deficits associated with the spectrum of autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 30(3), 205–223. 10.1023/A:1005592401947 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lucca K, & Wilbourn MP (2018). Communicating to learn: Infants’ pointing gestures result in optimal learning. Child Development, 89(3), 941–960. 10.1111/cdev.12707 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Luyster RJ, Kadlec MB, Carter A, & Tager-Flusberg H (2008). Language assessment and development in toddlers with autism spectrum disorders. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 38(8), 1426–1438. 10.1007/s10803-007-0510-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Manwaring S, Stevens A, Mowdood A, & Lackey M (2018). A scoping review of deictic gesture use in toddlers with or at-risk for autism spectrum disorder. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments, 3, 239694151775189. 10.1177/2396941517751891 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Manwaring S, Swineford L, Mead DL, Yeh C-C, Zhang Y, & Thurm A (2019). The gesture–language association over time in toddlers with and without language delays. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments, 4, 2396941519845545. 10.1177/2396941519845545 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McCune L. (1995). A normative study of representational play at the transition to language. Developmental Psychology, 31(2), 198–206. Scopus. 10.1037/0012-1649.31.2.198 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McCune L. (2009). Developing symbolic abilities. In Wagoner B (Ed.), Symbolic Transformation: The Mind in Movement Through Culture and Society (pp. 173–192). Routledge. [Google Scholar]
- McCune-Nicolich L. (1981). Toward symbolic functioning: Structure of Early pretend games and potential parallels with language. Child Development, 52(3), 785–797. 10.2307/1129078 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- McGillion M, Herbert JS, Pine J, Vihman M, dePaolis R, Keren-Portnoy T, & Matthews D (2017). What paves the way to conventional language? The predictive value of babble, pointing, and socioeconomic status. Child Development, 88(1), 156–166. 10.1111/cdev.12671 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mitchell S, Brian J, Zwaigenbaum L, Roberts W, Szatmari P, Smith I, & Bryson S (2006). Early language and communication development of infants later diagnosed with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(2), S69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Morin-Lessard E, Hentges RF, Tough SC, & Graham SA (2021). Developmental pathways between infant gestures and symbolic actions, and children’s communicative skills at age 5: Findings from the all our families pregnancy cohort. Child Development, 92(3), 799–810. 10.1111/cdev.13567 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mullen EM (1995). Mullen Scales of Early Learning. Pearsons. [Google Scholar]
- Orr E. (2020). Object play as a mediator of the role of exploration in communication skills development. Infant Behavior and Development, 60, 101467. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2020.101467 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Orr E, & Geva R (2015). Symbolic play and language development. Infant Behavior and Development, 38, 147–161. 10.1016/j.infbeh.2015.01.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Özçalışkan Ş, Adamson LB, & Dimitrova N (2016). Early deictic but not other gestures predict later vocabulary in both typical development and autism. Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 20(6), 754–763. 10.1177/1362361315605921 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ozonoff S, Young GS, Steinfeld MB, Hill MM, Cook I, Hutman T, Macari S, Rogers SJ, & Sigman M (2009). How early do parent concerns predict later autism diagnosis? Journal of Developmental and Behavioral Pediatrics : JDBP, 30(5), 367–375. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Piaget J. (1952). Play, dreams and imitation in childhood (pp. ix, 296). W W Norton & Co. [Google Scholar]
- Piaget J. (1962). The stages of the intellectual development of the child. Bulletin of the Menninger Clinic, 26, 120–128. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- R Core Team. (2021). R: A language and environment for statistical computing [Computer software]. R Foundation for Statistical Computing. https://www.R-project.org/ [Google Scholar]
- Rakoczy H, Tomasello M, & Striano T (2017). How children turn objects into symbols: A cultural learning account. In Symbol Use and Symbolic Representation: Developmental and Comparative Perspectives (pp. 69–97). Scopus. [Google Scholar]
- Ramos-Cabo S, Vulchanov V, & Vulchanova M (2019). Gesture and language trajectories in early development: An overview from the autism spectrum disorder perspective. Frontiers in Psychology, 10. 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.01211 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ramos-Cabo S, Vulchanov V, & Vulchanova M (2021). Different ways of making a point: A study of gestural communication in typical and atypical early development. Autism Research, 14(5), 984–996. 10.1002/aur.2438 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rogers SJ, Hepburn SL, Stackhouse T, & Wehner E (2003). Imitation performance in toddlers with autism and those with other developmental disorders. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 44(5), 763–781. 10.1111/1469-7610.00162 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rowe ML, Özçalişkan Ş, & Goldin-Meadow S (2008). Learning words by hand: Gesture’s role in predicting vocabulary development. First Language, 28(2), 182–199. 10.1177/0142723707088310 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutherford MD, & Rogers SJ (2003). Cognitive underpinnings of pretend play in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 33(3), 289–302. 10.1023/a:1024406601334 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutherford MD, Young GS, Hepburn S, & Rogers SJ (2007). A longitudinal study of pretend play in autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(6), 1024–1039. 10.1007/s10803-006-0240-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rutter M, LeCouteur A, & Lord C (2003). Autism Diagnostic Interview—Revised: WPS edition. Western Psychological Services. [Google Scholar]
- Smith IM, & Bryson SE (2007). Gesture imitation in autism: II. Symbolic gestures and pantomimed object use. Cognitive Neuropsychology, 24(7), 679–700. 10.1080/02643290701669703 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith V, Mirenda P, & Zaidman, -Zait Anat. (2007). Predictors of Expressive vocabulary growth in children with autism. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 50(1), 149–160. 10.1044/1092-4388(2007/013) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sparrow SS, Cicchetti DV, & Balla D (2005). Vineland Adaptive Behavior Scales, Second Edition. American Guidance Service. https://psycnet.apa.org/doiLanding?doi=10.1037%2Ft15164-000 [Google Scholar]
- Spencer PE (2011). Prelinguistic Communication. In Goldstein S & Naglieri JA (Eds.), Encyclopedia of Child Behavior and Development (pp. 1145–1146). Springer US. 10.1007/978-0-387-79061-9_2218 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stefanini S, Bello A, Caselli MC, Iverson JM, & Volterra V (2009). Co-speech gestures in a naming task: Developmental data. Language and Cognitive Processes, 24(2), 168–189. 10.1080/01690960802187755 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Stone WL, Ousley OY, Yoder PJ, Hogan KL, & Hepburn SL (1997). Nonverbal communication in two- and three-year-old children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 27(6), 677–696. 10.1023/a:1025854816091 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strid K, Heimann M, & Tjus T (2013). Pretend play, deferred imitation and parent-child interaction in speaking and non-speaking children with autism. Scandinavian Journal of Psychology, 54(1), 26–32. 10.1111/sjop.12003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Swanson MR, Shen MD, Wolff JJ, Elison JT, Emerson R, Styner M, Hazlett HC, Truong K, Watson LR, Paterson S, Marrus N, Botteron K, Pandey J, Schultz RT, Dager S, Zwaigenbaum L, Estes AM, & Piven J (2017). Subcortical brain and behavior phenotypes differentiate infants with autism versus language delay. Biological Psychiatry. Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging, 2(8), 664–672. 10.1016/j.bpsc.2017.07.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Talbott MR, Nelson CA, & Tager-Flusberg H (2015). Maternal gesture use and language development in infant siblings of children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 45(1), 4–14. 10.1007/s10803-013-1820-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thiemann-Bourque K, Brady N, & Fleming K (2012). Symbolic play of preschoolers with severe communication impairments with autism and other developmental delays: more similarities than differences. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 42(5), 863–873. 10.1007/s10803-011-1317-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thiemann-Bourque K, Johnson L, & Brady NC (2019). Similarities in Functional Play and Differences in Symbolic Play of Children With Autism Spectrum Disorder. American Journal on Intellectual and Developmental Disabilities, 124(1), 77–91. 10.1352/1944-7558-124.1.77 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello M. (2010). Piagetian and vygotskian approaches to language acquisition. Human Development, 39(5), 269–276. 10.1159/000278478 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Tomasello M, Carpenter M, Call J, Behne T, & Moll H (2005). Understanding and sharing intentions: The origins of cultural cognition. The Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 28(5), 675–691; discussion 691-735. 10.1017/S0140525X05000129 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vallotton CD, & Ayoub CC (2010). Symbols build communication and thought: The role of gestures and words in the development of engagement skills and social-emotional concepts during toddlerhood. Social Development (Oxford, England), 19(3), 601–626. 10.1111/j.1467-9507.2009.00549.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Veness C, Prior M, Bavin E, Eadie P, Cini E, & Reilly S (2012). Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders at 12 and 24 months of age: A prospective, longitudinal comparative study. Autism, 16(2), 163–177. 10.1177/1362361311399936 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Vivanti G, Dissanayake C, & The Victorian ASELCC Team. (2016). Outcome for children receiving the Early Start Denver Model before and after 48 months. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46(7), 2441–2449. 10.1007/s10803-016-2777-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Volkmar F, Chawarska K, & Klin A (2005). Autism in Infancy and Early Childhood. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 315–336. 10.1146/annurev.psych.56.091103.070159 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wainwright BR, Allen ML, & Cain K (2020). The influence of labelling on symbolic understanding and dual representation in autism spectrum condition. Autism & Developmental Language Impairments, 5, 2396941520931728. 10.1177/2396941520931728 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson LR, Crais ER, Baranek GT, Dykstra JR, & Wilson KP (2013a). Communicative gesture use in infants with and without autism: A retrospective home video study. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 22(1), 25–39. 10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0145) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Watson LR, Crais ER, Baranek GT, Dykstra JR, & Wilson KP (2013b). Communicative gesture use in infants with and without autism: a retrospective home video study. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology / American Speech-Language-Hearing Association, 22(1), 25–39. 10.1044/1058-0360(2012/11-0145) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werner H, & Kaplan B (1964). Symbol formation: An organismic-developmental approach to language and the expression of thought. Language, 40(4), 566. 10.2307/411939 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- West KL, Roemer EJ, Northrup JB, & Iverson JM (2020). Profiles of early actions and gestures in infants with an older sibling with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 63(4), 1195–1211. 10.1044/2019_JSLHR-19-00013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wetherby AM, Watt N, Morgan L, & Shumway S (2007). Social communication profiles of children with autism spectrum disorders late in the second year of life. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 37(5), 960–975. 10.1007/s10803-006-0237-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wetherby AM, Woods J, Allen L, Cleary J, Dickinson H, & Lord C (2004). Early indicators of autism spectrum disorders in the second year of life. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 34(5), 473–493. 10.1007/s10803-004-2544-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wetherby A, Prizant B, & Hutchinson T (1998). Communicative, social/affective, and symbolic profiles of young children with autism and pervasive developmental disorders. American Journal of Speech-Language Pathology, 7, 79. 10.1044/1058-0360.0702.79 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Whalen C, Schreibman L, & Ingersoll B (2006). The collateral effects of joint attention training on social initiations, positive affect, imitation, and spontaneous speech for young children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 36(5), 655–664. 10.1007/s10803-006-0108-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whitehouse AJO, Varcin KJ, Alvares GA, Barbaro J, Bent C, Boutrus M, Chetcuti L, Cooper MN, Clark A, Davidson E, Dimov S, Dissanayake C, Doyle J, Grant M, Iacono T, Maybery M, Pillar S, Renton M, Rowbottam C, … Hudry K (2019). Pre-emptive intervention versus treatment as usual for infants showing early behavioural risk signs of autism spectrum disorder: A single-blind, randomised controlled trial. The Lancet Child & Adolescent Health, 3(9), 605–615. 10.1016/S2352-4642(19)30184-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Williams E, Reddy V, & Costall A (2001). Taking a closer look at functional play in children with autism. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 31(1), 67–77. 10.1023/a:1005665714197 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ye Q, Liu L, Lv S, Cheng S, Zhu H, Xu Y, Zou X, & Deng H (2021). The Gestures in 2–4-year-old children with autism spectrum disorder. Frontiers in Psychology, 12. 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.604542 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeanah CH, Gunnar MR, McCall RB, Kreppner JM, & Fox NA (2011). Sensitive periods. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development, 76(4), 147–162. 10.1111/j.1540-5834.2011.00631.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zwaigenbaum L, Bryson S, Rogers T, Roberts W, Brian J, & Szatmari P (2005). Behavioral manifestations of autism in the first year of life. International Journal of Developmental Neuroscience, 23(2–3), 143–152. 10.1016/j.ijdevneu.2004.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data that support the findings of this study are available on request from the corresponding author. The data are not publicly available due to privacy or ethical restrictions.