Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2024 Apr 18;19(4):e0302439. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0302439

Validity of self-reported weight and height among female young adults in the United Arab Emirates

Dalia Haroun 1,*, Aseel Ehsanallah 1
Editor: Preeti Kanawjia2
PMCID: PMC11025931  PMID: 38635733

Abstract

Self-reported weight and height serve as important metrics in estimating overweight and obesity prevalence within epidemiological studies, primarily due to their cost and time efficiency. However, the accuracy and reliability of these self-reported measures remain controversial, with conflicting reports emerging from different regions. This study aims to compare self-reported weight and height with measured values among young female adults in the United Arab Emirates. A cross-sectional study of 131 female university students aged 17–27 reported their weight and height on a self-administered questionnaire and on the same day had their height and weight measured. Body Mass Index (BMI) values of both self-reported and measured weight and height were calculated and categorized according to the World Health Organization’s cut-off points. Overall, 87% of students had a resultant self-reported BMI value within their actual BMI category. The mean differences between self-reported and measured weight and height in the present study were -0.92 kg and 0.38 cm, respectively. Results indicated strong agreement between self-reported and direct measurements, as demonstrated by weighted Kappa statistics (kappa = 0.87). Bland & Altman plots illustrated that the majority of values fell within the limits of agreement (2 SD), with no systemic bias detected. BMI calculated from self-reported data demonstrates high sensitivity and specificity. Linear regression analyses revealed that self-reported weight (r2 = 0.973; p<0.001), height (r2 = 0.902; p<0.001), and BMI (r2 = 0.964; p<0.001) accurately predicted measured weight, height, and BMI. The study’s results highlight the ability of female university students in the UAE to accurately provide self-reports of their weight and height. This finding provides further support for the utilization of self-reported data on height and weight as a valid method for collecting anthropometric information.

Introduction

In the United Arab Emirates (UAE), overweight and obesity are major public health concerns and are key contributors to chronic illnesses including Type II diabetes, cardiovascular, respiratory, and gallbladder diseases, and some types of cancers [1, 2]. Addressing these challenges requires a foundation of reliable data which is essential for designing effective interventions, conducting precise risk assessments, and developing specific healthcare strategies. Accurate information not only aids in the development of preventive measures but also leads to improved health outcomes.

The “gold standard” in obtaining accurate results of weight and height is to have appropriately trained and monitored personnel who perform direct measurements of these anthropometrics using standardized and well-managed equipment and methods [3]. However, the application of this “gold standard” is not feasible, particularly in large-scale epidemiological studies, which commonly use self-reported weight and height data due to constraints such as time, financial resources, and available personnel [46]. Therefore, healthcare practitioners utilize self-reported height and weight to calculate BMI, providing a consistent method for assessing obesity and overweight trends in populations [7]. Such a method allows for quick, easy, and convenient data collection that can be completed through face-to-face or telephone interviews or self-administered questionnaires at a minimal cost and resources, especially for large-scale studies [1, 3, 8, 9]. Policymakers rely on this information to allocate resources and establish healthcare priorities, emphasizing the need to evaluate its precision and reliability [4]. These anthropometric measurements serve as primary factors of investigation and potential variables that might introduce confounding influences. They are fundamental in nutritional status assessments, predicting functional limitations, disease risks, and overall mortality [10]. Despite the inherent challenges, the use of self-reported data remains integral to understanding public health patterns and informing healthcare strategies.

The use of self-reported data in research is questionable and it can introduce limitations related to recall bias of participants who overestimate or underestimate their weight or height [1, 9] or who simply cannot recall their actual weight or height [11]. Bias in self-reporting can result in inaccuracies when evaluating nutritional status, ultimately compromising the precise evaluation of overweight/obesity prevalence within a community [12]. The literature shows substantial differences between the subjectively and objectively determined BMI. The subjective BMI tends to underestimate the objective BMI which consequently results in the underestimation of the prevalence of overweight and obesity [13, 14]. The lack of concordance between the subjectively and objectively determined BMI is due to the fact that weight tends to be under-reported, and height is often over-reported [8, 11, 15, 16]. Variations in the accuracy of self-reported weight and height among populations depend on some factors including age, gender, weight status, race [9, 15], and cultural factors [17]. There is a tendency of some individuals with these different factors to report weight and height values that are idealistic from their own or society’s perspective [18].

Cultural factors and backgrounds can play a role in the variations between self-reported and measured weight and height. A study of the European Union that focused on the relationship between the subjective and objective BMI among the European countries found that the degree of correlation between the subjective and objective BMI differed from one country to another thus, comparable estimates of the prevalence of overweight and obesity could not be made based on the subjective BMI [19]. This difference between the actual BMI and the perceived one can be a result of different views and perceptions on beauty and ideal body image that vary from one culture to another, leading individuals to misreport data in an attempt to attain a culturally valued body image [14]. Cultural factors and race go hand in hand because different ethnic groups have different cultural perceptions on ideal weight or height which may influence their tendencies to report data accurately. For example, white individuals are more likely to overestimate their height than individuals from other ethnic groups including black and Hispanic individuals [20]. Evaluating the accuracy of self-reported weight and height values requires a direct comparison between the self-reported data and the measured values within the target population. This step is crucial to determine the extent of the biases among populations, influenced by cultural norms and societal factors [12, 21].

While young adulthood is typically seen as a period associated with peak health and well-being, recent data reveals a notable change in the distribution of BMI. This shift is marked by a decline in the proportion of individuals falling into the ’normal’ BMI category and a simultaneous increase in those categorized as ’overweight and obese’ [22]. Adolescents are more likely to report their weight and height inaccurately compared to adults due to the rapid growth period they are undergoing that leads to substantial physical changes. Thus, they tend to lack knowledge about their current weight and height [14, 15]. Some aspects may influence the accuracy of self-reported data among adolescents, these aspects include body image and social desirability which both can lead to reporting values that are considered ideal or socially acceptable [23].

In both women and individuals struggling with overweight and obesity, there is a common tendency to underestimate their actual body weight [22, 24]. Overweight and obese individuals usually underreport their weight compared to underweight and normal-weight individuals [1, 9, 25]. This may be because of the stigma that is attached to being heavy which leads those who are overweight or obese to underestimate their actual weight [8]. Females are more likely than males to underestimate their weight [1, 3, 9, 26, 27]. This may be attributable to the role of media and advertisement that highlight women’s status mostly on the basis of their appearance which may influence females to report data that is desired by society [28]. Underestimation of weight among males is applicable only to those who are overweight or obese [28]. Males, in general, are more likely to overestimate their height than females [1]. Societal pressures and media influence lead many, especially women and overweight individuals, to underestimate their weight which can compromise the validity of self-reported anthropometric measurements.

The topic of the relationship between self-reported and measured weight, height, and BMI has been studied internationally including the United States and several European countries, but the literature shows no results regarding the UAE. Hence, this study aims to assess the validity of self-reported anthropometric measurements (weight and height) and BMI classification amongst female university students in the UAE.

Materials and methods

Ethical approval was obtained from the research ethics committee at Zayed University (ZU15_101_F). This research was performed as a part of a cross-sectional study investigating the caffeine and energy drink consumption among university students in the UAE. Data was collected between 25th February and 17th March 2016. Convenience sampling was used. The sample size was not calculated for this study as it was a post-hoc analysis. Based on results from a similar study [29], using 0.55kg as the mean weight difference and 2.03kg as the SD of the difference a sample size of 109 would be needed to achieve a power of 80% at 0.05 level of significance [30]. Our sample size of 131 is therefore comparable to what was used in similar studies [22].

All female students studying at Zayed University in Dubai were eligible to participate in the study. Those who were pregnant or had electronic medical implants were excluded. Researchers explained the study to participants and written informed consent was obtained prior to the study.

First, participants self-completed the questionnaire, that was available in both Arabic and English language. The questionnaire was used to obtain demographic data (age, ethnicity, college, year of study). Participants were also asked to report their weight in kilograms and height in centimeters. Subsequently, participants’ weight and height were measured by trained researchers, holding an undergraduate degree in Public Health and Nutrition, according to standard protocol. Participants were instructed to remove their shoes and socks and were requested to remove any heavy objects from their pockets (e. g. mobile phones, keys, key chains, wallets, and heavy accessories). Height was measured standing upright facing forward with back, buttocks, and heels vertically aligned against the scale. Additionally, feet and heels were placed together, and the movable head plate rested firmly on the top of participants’ crown. Height was measured to the nearest 0.1cm using the portable stadiometer (Charder, HM-200P) and was set up on a flat, secure, stable surface against a wall. Weight was measured to the nearest 0.1kg using the portable Tanita Body Composition Analyser (BC-420MA). This study aims to assess the validity of self-reported anthropometric measurements (weight and height) and BMI classifications amongst female university students in the UAE.

Data analysis

Self-reported and measured BMI were calculated as weight in kilograms divided by height in meters squared (kg/m2). Participants were classified as either underweight (<18.5 kg/m2), healthy weight 18.5–24.9 kg/m2, overweight 25–29.9 kg/m2, or obese ≥ 30 kg/m2 using World Health Organization cut-off points [31]. Descriptive statistics were used to analyze demographic data. Means and SD for weight, height, and BMI were computed for self-reported and measured data. Paired sample t-tests were used to determine the differences between self-reported and measured anthropometrics for the whole sample and then stratified by BMI category. The mean difference was calculated as self-reported values minus measured values. Correlations between the methods were tested using Pearson Correlations. Bland-Altman plots were performed to assess the agreement between self-reported and directly measured weight and height [32]. Means of self-reported and measured values were computed. The differences between self-reported and measured values were plotted against their means with a mean difference plus or minus 1.96 times its standard deviation. The Interclass Correlation Coefficient (ICC) was used to derive a summary measure of absolute agreement between self-reported and measured weight and height, where ICC >0.75 indicates good reliability [33]. Kappa statistics were calculated to assess the degree of agreement between BMI categorization derived from self-reported data versus that derived from measured data, where a kappa >0.8 indicates a strong strength of agreement [34, 35]. The effectiveness of self-reported weight and height data in accurately identifying underweight, overweight, and obesity was assessed through various measures, including sensitivity, specificity, positive predictive values (PPV), and negative predictive values (NPV). Sensitivity determines how accurately self-reported data identify individuals with underweight, overweight, or obesity, while specificity measures how accurately it excludes those without these conditions. PPV indicates the proportion of reported cases confirmed, while NPV shows the proportion of non-reported cases confirmed [15]. Linear regression analyses (adjusted for age) were used to assess the accuracy of self-reported weight, height, and BMI in predicting measured values. Data was analyzed with SPSS software, version 29 for Windows (IBM, Armonk, NY, USA) and significance was set at p-values <0.05.

Results

A total of 131 female participants aged between 19 and 27 years provided self-reports of their weight and height, and had their measurements taken. The mean age (standard deviation) of participants was 19.7 (1.9) years. The majority of participants were Emirati (93.9%). The sample was evenly distributed across the different years of study and came from a variety of colleges. Additional information on participants’ demographics can be found in Table 1.

Table 1. Characteristics of the study participants (n = 131).

Variables n (%)
Age
17–19
20–27

68 (51.9)
63 (48.1)
Nationality
Emirati
Non-Emirati
Missing

124 (93.9)
5 (3.8)
3 (2.3)
Marital status
Single
Engaged
Married
Divorced

116 (88.5)
6 (4.6)
8 (6.1)
1 (0.8)
Has children
Yes
No

2 (1.5)
129 (98.5)
Year of study
Foundation year
1st year
2nd year
3rd year
4th year

23 (17.6)
34 (26.0)
17 (13.0)
33 (25.2)
24 (18.3)
College
Academic Bridge Program
University College
Art and Creative Enterprises
Business Sciences
Communication and Media Science
Education
Sustainability Sciences and Humanities
Technological Innovation
Missing

23 (17.6)
43 (32.8)
5 (3.8)
5 (3.8)
11(8.4)
4 (3.1)
28 (21.4)
10 (7.6)
2 (1.5)
Took nutrition courses
Yes
No

33 (25.2)
98 (74.8)

Approximately half the participants (42.7%) were healthy weight, 22.9% overweight and 14.5% obese. Self-reported and measured anthropometric data were significantly correlated with weight (r = 0.997; p<0.001) height (r = 0.988; p<0.001) and BMI (r = 0.996; p<0.001). Furthermore, there was a strong agreement between self-reported values for height, weight, and the calculated BMI, where ICC values were 0.848, 0.982, and 0976 respectively (p<0.001).

Table 2 presents the number and proportion of female participants (n = 131) categorized into different BMI classifications based on both self-reported and measured values of height and weight. The data reveals distinct patterns in participants’ self-perception of their weight status compared to their actual measured BMI categories. According to the measured data, 19.8% of participants were classified as underweight, with a slight under-reporting observed as only 17.6% self-reported being underweight. In the normal weight category, 42.7% of participants were categorized based on measured BMI, whereas 49.6% self-identified as normal weight, indicating a tendency to perceive oneself as normal weight despite measured differences. Notably, 22.9% were classified as overweight using measured BMI, contrasting with the 19.8% who self-reported being overweight, indicating underreporting among this group. In the obese category, 14.4% of participants were classified based on measured BMI versus 12.9% based on self-reported measurements. The high Kappa value of 0.87 (95% CI: 0.81–0.93) demonstrated a statistically significant strong agreement between self-reported and measured BMI categories among the participants.

Table 2. The number and proportion of female participants (n = 131) were categorized into different BMI classifications based on both self-reported and measured values of height and weight.

BMI-category based on Measured BMI
Underweight n (%) Normal weight n (%) Overweight n (%) Obese n (%) Total n (%) Kappa (95% CI)
BMI-category based on Self-reported BMI Underweight 21 (16) 2 (1.5) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 23 (17.6)
0.87 (0.81, - 0.93)
Normal weight 5 (3.8) 53 (40.5) 7 (5.3) 0 (0.0) 65 (49.6)
Overweight 0 (0.0) 1 (0.8) 23 (17.6) 2 (1.5) 26 (19.8)
Obese 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 17 (12.9) 17 (12.9)
Total 26 (19.8) 56 (42.7) 30 (22.9) 19 (14.4) 131 (100)

Table 3 illustrates the mean difference between self-reported versus measured weight, height, and BMI of the total sample (n = 131). Overall, participants significantly underreported their weight by 0.92 kg (p = 0.001). Height was significantly over-reported by an average of 0.38 cm (p = 0.013). Underestimating weight and overestimating height resulted in a significant underestimation of BMI by an average of 0.47 kg/m2 (p<0.001).

Table 3. Self-reported versus measured anthropometrics in all participants (n = 131).

Self-reported
(mean ±SD)
Measured
(mean ±SD)
Mean difference*
(95% CI)
P-value
Weight (kg) 59.5 ±15.8 60.4 ±17.1 -0.92
(-1.45 to -0.40)
0.001
Height (cm) 158.7 ±5.4 158.3 ±5.5 0.38
(0.08 to 0.69)
0.013
BMI (kg/m2) 23.5 ±5.9 24 ±6.4 -0.47
(-0.69 to -0.25)
<0.001

* Mean difference = self-reported—measured values.

BMI (Body Mass Index); SD (Standard Deviation); CI (Confidence Interval)

Table 4 demonstrates the difference between self-reported versus measured weight, height, and BMI by BMI category. Underweight students significantly over-reported weight by 0.50 kg (p = 0.033). Normal-weight students significantly underreported weight by 0.51 kg (p = 0.044), consequently BMI was significantly under-estimated by 0.28 kg/m2 (p = 0.029). Overweight students underestimated weight by 1.05 kg (p = 0.071) but significantly over-reported height the most by 0.74 cm (p = 0.044) resulting in a significant underestimation of BMI by 0.64 kg/m2 (p = 0.007). Obese students significantly underreported weight and BMI by 3.89 kg (p = 0.004) and 1.65 kg/m2 (0.002), respectively.

Table 4. Agreement between self-reported and measured values for height, weight, and BMI stratified by BMI category.

Self-reported
(mean ±SD)
Measured (mean ±SD) Mean difference*
(95% CI)
P-value
Underweight
26 (19.8%)
Weight (kg) 41.8 ±4.2 41.3 ±3.9 0.50
(0.04 to 0.96)
0.033
Height (cm) 156.1 ±4.8 156.1 ±5.0 0.05
(-0.52 to 0.61)
0.868
BMI (kg/m2) 17.1 ±1.3 16.9 ±1.1 0.19
(-0.05 to 0.42)
0.110
Normal weight
56 (42.7%)
Weight (kg) 54.3 ±6.2 54.8 ±6.5 -0.51
(-1.00 to -0.01)
0.044
Height (cm) 159.2 ±5.3 158.8 ±5.6 0.34
(-0.23 to 0.92)
0.237
BMI (kg/m2) 21.4 ±1.9 21.7 ±1.9 -0.28
(-0.53 to -0.03)
0.029
Overweight
30 (22.9%)
Weight (kg) 66.7 ±6.0 67.7 ±5.7 -1.05
(-2.20 to 0.09)
0.071
Height (cm) 159.8 ±5.1 159.0 ±5.2 0.74
(0.26 to 1.22)
0.004
BMI (kg/m2) 26.1 ±1.7 26.7 ±1.1 -0.64
(-1.10 to -0.19)
0.007
Obese
19 (14.5%)
Weight (kg) 87.4 ±12.3 91.3 ±13.9 -3.89
(-6.33 to -1.45)
0.004
Height (cm) 159.2 ±6.2 158.7 ±6.3 0.41
(-0.27 to 1.09)
0.222
BMI (kg/m2) 34.5 ±4.9 36.2 ±5.0 -1.65
(-2.59 to -0.71)
0.002

* Mean difference = self-reported—measured values.

Table 5 summarizes the diagnostic values of self-reported height and weight to determine underweight, normal weight, overweight, and obesity among female participants. The results demonstrate high sensitivity and specificity of self-reported BMI compared to measured BMI. The sensitivity for the overweight category was slightly lower at 76.7% with a specificity of 97%, compared to the obese category which exhibited higher sensitivity and specificity (89.5% and 100%, respectively). Moreover, the PPV, was 88.5% for overweight and 100% for obesity, representing the proportion of females that correctly reported their anthropometric measures. The corresponding NPV for overweight and obesity were 93.3% and 98.2%, respectively, indicating the proportion of non-reported cases confirmed.

Table 5. Sensitivity, specificity, positive predictive value, and negative predictive value of self-reported BMI classifications.

Sensitivity (%) Specificity (%) PPV (%) NPV (%)
Underweight 80.8 98 91.3 95.4
Normal weight 94.6 84 81.5 95.5
Overweight 76.7 97 88.5 93.3
Obesity 89.5 100 100 98.2

The agreement between self-reported and directly measured weight, height, and BMI at an individual level is illustrated graphically in the Bland and Altman plots (Fig 1). The 95% limits of agreement (LOA) for weight (+4.98 to -6.85), height (+3.80 to -3.03), and BMI (+2.01 to -2.95) were quite far from zero indicating an overall discrepancy between self-reported and measured values. Some participants had an extreme difference between self-reported and measured anthropometrics that fell outside the 95% LOA. There were five values that under-reported weight by more than 10 kg most of which were near 100 kg. There was one value that over-reported height by more than 10 cm and five values that under-reported BMI by more than 4 kg/m2 most of which were in the obese range. The variability of the difference increases for larger weight and BMI, however, this was observed only for a small number of individuals. Therefore, excluding those outliers we can see from the graph that there was a good overall agreement between individual measures.

Fig 1.

Fig 1

Bland and Altman plot (B&A) of self-reported versus measured (a) weight, (b) height, and (c) BMI. Dark line: Mean difference between self-reported and measured anthropometrics. Dotted line: 95% limits of agreement (LOA), in which the upper line is +1.96 SD and lower line is -1.96 SD from mean difference (red line).

Additionally, linear regressions analyses showed that self-reported weight, height, and BMI were accurate in predicting measured weight (r2 = 0.973; p<0.001), height (r2 = 0.902; p<0.001), and BMI (r2 = 0.964; p<0.001). Furthermore, there was no systematic bias observed over the range of measurements for weight (r = 0.451; p <0.001) height (r = 0.065; p = 0.458), or BMI (r = 0.387; p <0.001).

Discussion

The present study was the first to be carried out in the UAE. It examined the difference between self-reported and measured weight, height, and BMI generally and specifically by weight status in a total of 131 young female students.

The findings presented in this study emphasize the complexity of body image perception and its impact on self-reported and measured BMI categories among young female participants. The substantial agreement observed between self-reported and measured BMI categories, as indicated by a high Kappa value of 0.87 (95% CI: 0.81–0.93), suggests a strong alignment between participants’ self-perception and their actual weight status. This level of agreement is crucial in understanding the accuracy of self-reported data. The study reveals interesting patterns of self-perception across different BMI categories. While a slight under-reporting was observed in the underweight category, participants showed a tendency to perceive themselves as normal weight, even when their measured BMI suggested otherwise. Notably, there was a consistent trend of under-reporting in the overweight and obese categories.

The general trend of weight under-reporting and height over-reporting found in this study is consistent with other studies carried out among female college students, yet the mean differences vary. The mean differences between self-reported and measured weight and height in the present study were -0.92 kg and 0.38 cm, respectively. Weekly body weight fluctuations are around 0.35% (equivalent to 0.2 kg in this study) [36]. Quick et al. (2015) found mean differences of -0.27 kg for weight and 0.51 cm for height among female students from eight universities in the U.S., which is lower for weight and larger for height compared to findings from this study [37]. Even higher differences were seen among female college students in Italy as weight was under-reported by 1.9 kg and height was over-reported by 2.8 cm [16]. A possible reason for the inconsistency of mean differences is the diverse cultural perspectives of ideal body size that can affect the extent of weight underestimation and height overestimation from one region to another as discussed earlier in the introduction [14].

The results of this study found that self-reported BMI demonstrates high sensitivity and specificity, which suggests that self-reported height and weight can effectively classify individuals into the different BMI categories, with reasonably accurate results. High sensitivity (89.5%) and specificity (100%) values for obesity were observed in this study. These findings closely resemble those of a previous study by Lee et al. (2011), which reported a sensitivity of 83.6% and a specificity of 98% for the prevalence of obesity [10].

The Bland & Altman plots illustrated limited disparities between self-reported weight, height, and BMI at an individual level. Estimations for body weight, height, and BMI were found to be within the pre-defined limits of accuracy, indicating that self-reported measurements can be considered a reliable tool for estimating a person’s weight, height, and BMI in this sample population.

This study found that normal-weight, overweight, and obese students underreported their weight. The underestimation increased as weight increased which implies that obese and overweight students under-reported weight to a higher extent than normal-weight students. In comparison with prior research among female college students by Gunnare et al. (2013) in the U.S. and Larsen et al. (2008) in the Netherlands weight was under-reported only among overweight and obese subjects, but not among those who were normal weight [8, 26]. A possible reason for this might be that these normal-weight students in the U.S. and Netherlands were more health-aware compared to the participants in this study. This can be alarming and a possible sign of an eating disorder or extreme dieting because despite being within a healthy weight range, these students failed to recognize their own health status. Additionally, these normal-weight students might have thought that they needed to lose weight, thus underreporting their weight. Concerning obese and overweight students, it is speculated that their stronger inclination toward thinness could be influenced by societal and media standards to a greater extent than normal-weight students. Notably, a study has linked weight under-reporting among heavier subjects to depression. Sherry, et al. (2007) pointed out that heavier individuals were more prone to depression, leading them to under-report their weight [9]. Another probable interpretation for weight underestimation among overweight and obese students could be their reluctance to acknowledge their heavy-weight status. If this is indeed the case, it is concerning, as it implies a lack of awareness about the health risks associated with excess weight. Moreover, it raises questions about their willingness to adopt necessary dietary and lifestyle changes to manage their weight effectively.

The present study is the first among female college students to report a slight weight overestimation of underweight participants. Only one study in Sweden assessing adolescents with a mean age of 16 years supported this finding [23]. Over-reporting of weight amongst underweight students might be a positive indicator of acknowledging that their thinness was unhealthy, and their willingness to be in a healthy weight range. On the contrary, weight over-reporting could have a negative meaning just in the case of eating disorders particularly anorexia, in which individuals who are extremely thin perceive themselves as heavy.

Variations in height were observed among students of different weight statuses, with individuals classified as heavyweight showing the highest tendency to over-report their height. Earlier studies amongst female college students have not examined the relationship between self-reported and measured height by BMI categories. The sole exception was the Swedish adolescent study, which demonstrated an increasing trend in height overestimation with higher BMI, aligning with the results of this study [23]. This implies that overweight and obese students might be more inclined to overstate their height, potentially as a way to compensate for their excess weight and avoid appearing as heavy as their actual weight suggests.

The outcomes suggest a high percentage of correct BMI classification when relying on self-reported weight and height, with only 18 out of 131 (13.7%) individuals being misclassified. In comparison, Lasren, et al. (2008) found a larger percentage of BMI misclassification when using self-reports as 50% of overweight and obese female college students in the Netherlands were misclassified as normal weight [8]. A possible reason for the inconsistency of BMI misclassification may be due to sample size as Larsen’s et al., study included 209 students while this study included 131 students.

The findings of this study suggest that self-reported values can be considered when determining the prevalence of unhealthy body weight for targeting weight loss or gain interventions. Female university students in the UAE were generally able to provide self-reports of their weight and height. While self-reported data showed small discrepancies from measured values, with weight and BMI tending to be underestimated and height overestimated, the overall trend indicated that self-reports could still be utilized. It is important to note that the accuracy of self-reports decreased with higher BMI, leading to a skewed prevalence of overweight and obesity. Given the convenience and minimal cost of self-reports, they will continue to be utilized, particularly for large-scale studies. However, it is recommended to prioritize direct measurements whenever feasible. Strategies to minimize self-reporting errors include employing a two-method measurement approach, involving direct measurements for a small portion of the sample, to estimate accuracy for the entire population. This method allows researchers to estimate the accuracy of values for the entire sample, reducing bias [13]. Additionally, participants can be encouraged, if possible, to measure themselves before completing self-administered questionnaires for enhanced accuracy [15]. Ultimately, to increase the reliability of self-reported data, it is important to establish a mandatory body size surveillance system for students, which periodically screens for cases of underweight, overweight, and obesity among college students, fostering greater awareness of weight and height among the student population.

The results stress the need for several intervention programs at Zayed University in Dubai including overweight and obesity controlling programs, programs for weight gain of underweight students as well as screening and intervention programs for potential eating disorders. The efforts of these programs should be directed to sustainable and easy-to-follow changes including dietary, physical activity, and behavioral modifications. Needless to say, all of these changes will require an effective team of dieticians, health counselors, and educators who can help in one-to-one sessions as well as deliver messages to the students as a whole.

A limitation of this study is that the sample size was not calculated a priori, as it was a post-hoc analysis. This may have impacted the statistical power and generalizability of the findings. The generalizability of the current study may also be limited by the recruitment of a convenience sample of only female participants. The absence of male participants may limit the generalizability of the findings to the broader population. A strength of this study is the absence of a time gap between self-reported data and direct measurements, effectively reducing the potential for weight fluctuations often seen in young adults [37]. Future research should examine the validity of self-reported measures among male participants and examine the relationship between self-reported and measured weight height, and BMI by gender and weighing frequency amongst college students from other emirates in the UAE. Additionally, further research is needed to determine the extent to which self-reporting might change in the later stages of adulthood. Lastly, it would be interesting to study this topic among the Arab countries to determine cultural and race variations in self-reports.

Conclusion

This study among female university students in Dubai offers valuable insights into the complex dynamics of body image perception and its impact on self-reported and measured weight, height, and BMI. The findings underscore the importance of precise measurements and individualized interventions to address weight-related concerns effectively. These insights are essential for intervention programs, emphasizing the need for tailored initiatives focused on obesity prevention, healthy weight gain, and interventions for potential eating disorders. Additionally, the study’s outcomes reveal a high level of agreement between self-reported and measured data, highlighting the reliability of self-perception in determining weight status among this sample of young female adults. This finding provides further support for the utilization of self-reported data on height and weight as a valid method for collecting anthropometric information when direct measurements are not possible.

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript.

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Bowring AL, Peeters A, Freak-Poli R, Lim MS, Gouillou M, Hellard M. Measuring the accuracy of self-reported height and weight in a community-based sample of young people. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2012. Nov 21;12:175. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-12-175 ; PMCID: PMC3561081. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Belal AM. Nutrition-related chronic diseases epidemic in UAE: can we stand to STOP it? Sudanese J Public Health. 2009. Oct;(4):383–392. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Kreatsoulas C, Hassan A, Subramanian S, Fleegler AE. Accuracy of Self-Reported Height and Weight to Determine Body Mass Index Among Youth. Child Adolesc Behav. 2014;(2):126–128. [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Al-Hazzaa HM, Al-Awadhi BA, Al-Dashti YA, Alajmi FA, Almansour FD, Al-Haifi AR. Adolescent’s self-reported weight and its association with media impact on decision to lose weight and body thinness perception. Sci Rep. 2022. Apr 8;12(1):5908. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-09909-z ; PMCID: PMC8993922. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Mori S, Asakura K, Sasaki S, Nishiwaki Y. Misreporting of height and weight by primary school children in Japan: a cross-sectional study on individual and environmental determinants. BMC Public Health. 2023. Apr 27;23(1):775. doi: 10.1186/s12889-023-15682-z ; PMCID: PMC10134671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Van Dyke, Drinkwater EJ, Rachele JN. Improving the accuracy of self-reported height and weight in surveys: an experimental study. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2022;22(1):241. doi: 10.1186/s12874-022-01690-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Fry JB, Temple JB. Discrepancies in self-reported and measured anthropometric measurements and indices among older Australians: prevalence and correlates. BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):1–1928. 10.1186/s12889-022-14326-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Larsen JK, Ouwens M, Engels RC, Eisinga R, van Strien T. Validity of self-reported weight and height and predictors of weight bias in female college students. Appetite. 2008. Mar-May;50(2–3):386–9. doi: 10.1016/j.appet.2007.09.002 Epub 2007 Sep 20. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Sherry B, Jefferds ME, Grummer-Strawn LM. Accuracy of adolescent self-report of height and weight in assessing overweight status: a literature review. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2007. Dec;161(12):1154–61. doi: 10.1001/archpedi.161.12.1154 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Lee DH, Shin A, Kim J, Yoo KY, Sung J. Validity of self-reported height and weight in a Korean population. J Epidemiol. 2011;21(1):30–6. doi: 10.2188/jea.je20100058 Epub 2010 Oct 9. ; PMCID: PMC3899514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Rasmussen M, Holstein BE, Melkevik O, Damsgaard MT. Validity of self-reported height and weight among adolescents: the importance of reporting capability. BMC Med Res Methodol. 2013. Jun 27;13:85. doi: 10.1186/1471-2288-13-85 ; PMCID: PMC3711890. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Chia YC, Ching SM, Ooi PB, Beh HC, Chew MT, Chung FFL, et al. Measurement accuracy and reliability of self-reported versus measured weight and height among adults in Malaysia: Findings from a nationwide blood pressure screening programme. PLoS One. 2023. Jan 17;18(1):e0280483. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0280483 ; PMCID: PMC9844876. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Dalton WT 3rd, Wang L, Southerland JL, Schetzina KE, Slawson DL. Self-reported versus actual weight and height data contribute to different weight misperception classifications. South Med J. 2014. Jun;107(6):348–55. doi: 10.14423/01.SMJ.0000450708.52011.7c ; PMCID: PMC4141645. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kurth BM, Ellert U. Estimated and measured BMI and self-perceived body image of adolescents in Germany: part 1—general implications for correcting prevalence estimations of overweight and obesity. Obes Facts. 2010 Jun;3(3):181–90. doi: 10.1159/000314638 Epub 2010 Jun 11. ; PMCID: PMC6452159. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.De Vriendt T, Huybrechts I, Ottevaere C, Van Trimpont I, De Henauw S. Validity of self-reported weight and height of adolescents, its impact on classification into BMI-categories and the association with weighing behaviour. Int J Environ Res Public Health. 2009 Oct;6(10):2696–711. doi: 10.3390/ijerph6102696 Epub 2009 Oct 20. ; PMCID: PMC2790101. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Danubio ME, Miranda G, Vinciguerra MG, Vecchi E, Rufo F. Comparison of self-reported and measured height and weight: implications for obesity research among young adults. Econ Hum Biol. 2008. Mar;6(1):181–90. doi: 10.1016/j.ehb.2007.04.002 Epub 2007 Apr 8. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Zhou X, Dibley MJ, Cheng Y, Ouyang X, Yan H. Validity of self-reported weight, height and resultant body mass index in Chinese adolescents and factors associated with errors in self-reports. BMC Public Health. 2010. Apr 12;10:190. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-10-190 ; PMCID: PMC2864211. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Griebeler ML, Levis S, Beringer LM, Chacra W, Gómez-Marín O. Self-reported versus measured height and weight in Hispanic and non-Hispanic menopausal women. J Womens Health (Larchmt). 2011. Apr;20(4):599–604. doi: 10.1089/jwh.2009.1850 Epub 2011 Mar 17. ; PMCID: PMC3115416. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Madrigal H, Sánchez-Villegas A, Martínez-González MA, Kearney J, Gibney MJ, Irala J, et al. Underestimation of body mass index through perceived body image as compared to self-reported body mass index in the European Union. Public Health. 2000. Nov;114(6):468–73. . [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Brener ND, Mcmanus T, Galuska DA, Lowry R, Wechsler H. Reliability and validity of self-reported height and weight among high school students. J Adolesc Health. 2003. Apr;32(4):281–7. doi: 10.1016/s1054-139x(02)00708-5 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rios-Leyvraz M, Ortega N, Chiolero A. Reliability of Self-Reported Height and Weight in Children: A School-Based Cross-Sectional Study and a Review. Nutrients. 2022. Dec 23;15(1):75. doi: 10.3390/nu15010075 ; PMCID: PMC9824624. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Davies A, Wellard-Cole L, Rangan A, Allman-Farinelli M. Validity of self-reported weight and height for BMI classification: A cross-sectional study among young adults. Nutrition. 2020. Mar;71:110622. doi: 10.1016/j.nut.2019.110622 Epub 2019 Oct 21. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Ekström S, Kull I, Nilsson S, Bergström A. Web-based self-reported height, weight, and body mass index among Swedish adolescents: a validation study. J Med Internet Res. 2015. Mar 18;17(3):e73. doi: 10.2196/jmir.3947 ; PMCID: PMC4382564. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Kamuk. The Accuracy of Body Mass Index Estimated from Self-Reported Height and Weight in Turkish Adults. Cyprus J Med Sci. 2022;7(5):669–677. 10.4274/cjms.2020.2017. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Vartanian LR, Germeroth LJ. Accuracy in estimating the body weight of self and others: Impact of dietary restraint and BMI. Body Image. 2011. Sep;8(4):415–8. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2011.06.007 Epub 2011 Aug 12. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Gunnare NA, Silliman K, Morris MN. Accuracy of self-reported weight and role of gender, body mass index, weight satisfaction, weighing behavior, and physical activity among rural college students. Body Image. 2013. Jun;10(3):406–10. doi: 10.1016/j.bodyim.2013.01.006 Epub 2013 Feb 16. . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Lin CJ, DeRoo LA, Jacobs SR, Sandler DP. Accuracy and reliability of self-reported weight and height in the Sister Study. Public Health Nutr. 2012;15(6):989–999. doi: 10.1017/S1368980011003193 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Wang Z, Patterson CM, Hills AP. A comparison of self-reported and measured height, weight and BMI in Australian adolescents. Aust N Z J Public Health. 2002. Oct;26(5):473–8. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-842x.2002.tb00350.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Pursey K, Burrows TL, Stanwell P, Collins CE. How accurate is web-based self-reported height, weight, and body mass index in young adults?. Journal of medical Internet research. 2014. Jan 7;16(1):e4. doi: 10.2196/jmir.2909 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Dhand NK, Khatkar MS. Statulator: an online statistical calculator. Sample size calculator for comparing two paired means [Internet]. 2019. [cited 2023 Oct 30]. Available from: http://statulator.com/SampleSize/ss2PM.html. [Google Scholar]
  • 31.WHO. About the BMI database: BMI classification [Internet]. Geneva: World Health; Organization ; 2016. March 31 [cited 2023 Oct 30]. Available from: http://apps.who.int/bmi/index.jsp?introPage=intro_3.html [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Bland JM, Altman DG. Measuring agreement in method comparison studies. Stat Methods Med Res. 1999;8:135–60. doi: 10.1177/096228029900800204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Koo TK, Li MY. A guideline of selecting and reporting intraclass correlation coefficients for reliability research. Journal of chiropractic medicine. 2016; 15(2):155–63. doi: 10.1016/j.jcm.2016.02.012 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Jakobsson U, Westergren A. Statistical methods for assessing agreement for ordinal data. Scandinavian journal of caring sciences. 2005; 19(4):427–31. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-6712.2005.00368.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Landis JR, Koch GG. The measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. Biometrics. 1977. Mar;33(1):159–74. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Gow RW, Trace SE, Mazzeo SE. Preventing weight gain in first year college students: an online intervention to prevent the "freshman fifteen". Eat Behav. 2010. Jan;11(1):33–9. doi: 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2009.08.005 Epub 2009 Aug 29. ; PMCID: PMC2790428. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Quick V, Byrd-Bredbenner C, Shoff S, White AA, Lohse B, Horacek T, et al. Concordance of self-report and measured height and weight of college students. J Nutr Educ Behav. 2015. Jan-Feb;47(1):94–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jneb.2014.08.012 Epub 2014 Oct 12. ; PMCID: PMC4274244. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Preeti Kanawjia

23 Feb 2024

PONE-D-23-35645Validity of self-reported weight and height among female young adults in the United Arab EmiratesPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Haroun,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. Please submit your revised manuscript by Apr 08 2024 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Preeti Kanawjia, MD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Note from Emily Chenette, Editor in Chief of PLOS ONE, and Iain Hrynaszkiewicz, Director of Open Research Solutions at PLOS: Did you know that depositing data in a repository is associated with up to a 25% citation advantage (https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230416)? If you’ve not already done so, consider depositing your raw data in a repository to ensure your work is read, appreciated and cited by the largest possible audience. You’ll also earn an Accessible Data icon on your published paper if you deposit your data in any participating repository (https://plos.org/open-science/open-data/#accessible-data).

3. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors present an interesting study entitled "Validity of self-reported weight and height among female young adults in the United Arab Emirates". The study presents the findings on the validity of self-reported height and weight measures in a small group of female students within a defined age range. In its current form, the manuscript provides insufficient information for additional replication, and may be of limited utility given its small sample size.

Major issues:

1. Kindly provide the formula and/or calculation justifying why the sample size of 131 is sufficient for a study of this nature.

2. Kindly provide more details on the scale used for height measurements - specifying the make and model of the scale, and describe how it was set up to assure accuracy.

3. Line 158 states that Bland-Altman plots were performed to assess the agreement between self-reported and directly measured weight and height. Figure 1 only shows 1 Bland-Altman plot, comparing measured to self-reported weight. Please provide both the weight and height BA plots.

4. The conclusions of the study are incongruous with the reported results. The authors conclude that "The findings of this study propose that self-reported values should not be taken into account when determining the prevalence of unhealthy body weight for targeting weight loss or gain interventions. In general, female university students in the UAE struggled to provide accurate self-reports of their weight and height, leading to inaccurate BMI scores." In fact, the results show strong ICC values where calculated, and that the mean discrepancies between self-reported and measured height and weight were low (Table 4). For instance, a discrepancy of 0.5 - 1 kg in body weight is not out of the ordinary, given fluctuations in body weight throughout the day and during the menstrual cycle - particularly pertinent in this group of participants who were all female. Furthermore, discrepancies of less than 1 cm in height is negligible, and fall within the margin of error for height measurements. The discrepancies in the BA plot appear to be driven entirely by a small number of individuals, for whom there may be other factors leading to them providing inaccurate self-reported weight values. Kindly justify the conclusions made.

5. There appears to be substantial similarities between the text in this article and similar published articles. Please run this text through a plagiarism detection software and correct wherever appropriate.

6. There were no pre-defined range of acceptable limits of height and weight measure discrepancies, which should be defined a priori. Please refer to the following: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4470095/

7. Kindly justify the statement "However, if these values were to be used to determine the prevalence

368 of underweight or overweight, or target students for nutritional interventions programs, then

369 approximately one fifth of those who were underweight or overweight would be misclassified as

370 normal weight." 5 participants self-reported as normal but were underweight, whereas 7 overweight individuals self-reported as normal. 12 of 131 participants to not make up one fifth. Additionally, these findings should include the caveat that these findings were based on a very small number of individuals.

Minor issues:

1. SD was not fully defined at first mention.

2. Spelling error on line 158: testes in place of tests.

Reviewer #2: I would like to applaud the efforts of the authors for conducting a study to validate the self-reported measures of height and weight. Please see my comments below to improve the content and validity of the manuscript.

•Line 147 Add the hypothesis of the study

•Line 131-133 Is it a post-hoc analysis, then add it under the discussion section while discussing methodological considerations and limitations of the study

•Line 135 Any other contraindication for bioelectrical impedance analysis?

•Line 140 Explain more about “trained researchers” – their qualifications, specialization, and relevant experience

•Line 144 Add the scale/portable stadiometer details used for measuring the height of participants

•Line 147 Add the reliability and validity of the portable Tanita Body Composition Analyser (BC-420MA)

•Were the participants assessed at a fasting state in the morning using the portable Tanita Body Composition Analyser (BC-420MA)?

•Remove zero (dotted) line for the Bland-Altman plots

•Add a brief explanation of the absences of heteroscedasticity in the plots

•Add a linear regression analysis to explain the absence of a proportional bias in the plots

•Future recommendations should include similar studies on male participants

Thank you for the opportunity to review the manuscript

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2024 Apr 18;19(4):e0302439. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0302439.r003

Author response to Decision Letter 0


13 Mar 2024

Editor Comments

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

Response: We have checked that the manuscript meets PLOS one’s style requirements.

2. Note from Emily Chenette, Editor in Chief of PLOS ONE, and Iain Hrynaszkiewicz, Director of Open Research Solutions at PLOS: Did you know that depositing data in a repository is associated with up to a 25% citation advantage (https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230416)? If you’ve not already done so, consider depositing your raw data in a repository to ensure your work is read, appreciated and cited by the largest possible audience. You’ll also earn an Accessible Data icon on your published paper if you deposit your data in any participating repository (https://plos.org/open-science/open-data/#accessible-data).

Response: We have not deposited our dataset in a repository, however we would be happy to provide this upon request from readers.

3. Please include captions for your Supporting Information files at the end of your manuscript, and update any in-text citations to match accordingly. Please see our Supporting Information guidelines for more information: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/supporting-information.

Response: Not Applicable. All tables are embedded within the manuscript, and as per the PLOS one requirement, the figure has been uploaded separately which will then be embedded within the manuscript.

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer #1: The authors present an interesting study entitled "Validity of self-reported weight and height among female young adults in the United Arab Emirates". The study presents the findings on the validity of self-reported height and weight measures in a small group of female students within a defined age range. In its current form, the manuscript provides insufficient information for additional replication, and may be of limited utility given its small sample size.

Major issues:

1. Kindly provide the formula and/or calculation justifying why the sample size of 131 is sufficient for a study of this nature.

Response: As this was a post-hoc analysis, sample size calculation prior to conducting the study was not done. This information has been added to the manuscript. Furthermore, the calculation used to justify the sample size along with the references have been included in the manuscript.

2. Kindly provide more details on the scale used for height measurements - specifying the make and model of the scale, and describe how it was set up to assure accuracy.

Response: Information detailing the scale used for height measurements has been added.

3. Line 158 states that Bland-Altman plots were performed to assess the agreement between self-reported and directly measured weight and height. Figure 1 only shows 1 Bland-Altman plot, comparing measured to self-reported weight. Please provide both the weight and height BA plots.

Response: I think the reviewer has missed seeing the other 2 figures, as they were on different pages. The figure shows 3 bland-Altman plots (for weight, for height and for BMI. We have re-uploaded the BA plots into a different format document for clarity.

4. The conclusions of the study are incongruous with the reported results. The authors conclude that "The findings of this study propose that self-reported values should not be taken into account when determining the prevalence of unhealthy body weight for targeting weight loss or gain interventions. In general, female university students in the UAE struggled to provide accurate self-reports of their weight and height, leading to inaccurate BMI scores." In fact, the results show strong ICC values where calculated, and that the mean discrepancies between self-reported and measured height and weight were low (Table 4). For instance, a discrepancy of 0.5 - 1 kg in body weight is not out of the ordinary, given fluctuations in body weight throughout the day and during the menstrual cycle - particularly pertinent in this group of participants who were all female. Furthermore, discrepancies of less than 1 cm in height is negligible, and fall within the margin of error for height measurements. The discrepancies in the BA plot appear to be driven entirely by a small number of individuals, for whom there may be other factors leading to them providing inaccurate self-reported weight values. Kindly justify the conclusions made.

Response: We have revised the conclusions of the study to be more in line with the reported findings of the study.

5. There appears to be substantial similarities between the text in this article and similar published articles. Please run this text through a plagiarism detection software and correct wherever appropriate.

Response: We have run the text through a plagiarism detection software and have revised it accordingly.

6. There were no pre-defined range of acceptable limits of height and weight measure discrepancies, which should be defined a priori. Please refer to the following: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4470095/

Response: The Bland and Altman Plots illustrate differences between measured and self-reported measures of weight and height. Individual differences within 2SD of the mean are considered acceptable levels of errors. These have already been included in the manuscript.

Is the reviewer here referring to acceptable limits of measures that are of clinical significance? If so, we have not seen in the literature what would be considered acceptable. What we found are studies reporting differences in measured and self-reported- similar to what we have included on our study, and what we also found are studies that assess the reliability of height and weight measures between different observers. Furthermore, we found studies reporting weight fluctuations (over the week/seasonal etc.) and have added a reference to that in the discussion section. As there was no time lag of self-reported and measured, this limits the time for weight fluctuations and has been added as a strength of our study.

7. Kindly justify the statement "However, if these values were to be used to determine the prevalence

368 of underweight or overweight, or target students for nutritional interventions programs, then

369 approximately one fifth of those who were underweight or overweight would be misclassified as

370 normal weight." 5 participants self-reported as normal but were underweight, whereas 7 overweight individuals self-reported as normal. 12 of 131 participants to not make up one fifth. Additionally, these findings should include the caveat that these findings were based on a very small number of individuals.

Response: This is based on the sensitivity results obtained in relation to the underweight and overweight categories that were shown in table 5. For the underweight category it was 80.8% and for overweight category sensitivity was 76.7%. This means approximately 20% (or one fifth) would be classified incorrectly within each category. As this may have been confusing, we have now rephrased and clarified this finding in the discussion section to report on total sample that were misclassified (18 out of 131). The sample size has been added as a limitation to the study. Furthermore, the conclusions of the study have been revised accordingly.

Minor issues:

1. SD was not fully defined at first mention.

Response: This has been added.

2. Spelling error on line 158: testes in place of tests

Response: This has been corrected.

Reviewer #2: I would like to applaud the efforts of the authors for conducting a study to validate the self-reported measures of height and weight. Please see my comments below to improve the content and validity of the manuscript.

•Line 147 Add the hypothesis of the study

Response: The aim of the study has been added.

•Line 131-133 Is it a post-hoc analysis, then add it under the discussion section while discussing methodological considerations and limitations of the study

Response: This is a post-hoc analysis and has been added in the sample size calculation part. We have also added it as a limitation to the study.

•Line 135 Any other contraindication for bioelectrical impedance analysis?

Response: Yes, those who had medical implants were also excluded from the study. This has been added to the manuscript.

•Line 140 Explain more about “trained researchers” – their qualifications, specialization, and relevant experience

Response: Information on the qualifications of the researchers has been added to the manuscript.

•Line 144 Add the scale/portable stadiometer details used for measuring the height of participants

Response: Information on the stadiometer has been added.

•Line 147 Add the reliability and validity of the portable Tanita Body Composition Analyser (BC-420MA)

Response: For the purpose of this study, we have used the TANITA Body Composition Analyser only for the reporting of weight to the nearest 0.1kg, and not to assess body composition. Tanita has been reported to be reliable and valid in estimating body composition in epidemiological studies, However, a reference to that has not been added in our manuscript as body composition measures were not included in this study.

•Were the participants assessed at a fasting state in the morning using the portable Tanita Body Composition Analyser (BC-420MA)?

Participants were not fasting when assessed. For the purpose of this study, body composition parameters (such as % fat, fat mass and fat-free mass) were not reported. The Tanita body composition analyser was used to report participants measured weight to the nearest 0.1KG on the same day participants self-reported their weight. Therefore, information on fasting was not included as it was irrelevant to the aim of the study.

•Remove zero (dotted) line for the Bland-Altman plots

Response: The zero dotted lines have been removed from all the BA plots.

•Add a brief explanation of the absences of heteroscedasticity in the plots

Response: Regression analysis and a brief explanation was added to further explain this.

•Add a linear regression analysis to explain the absence of a proportional bias in the plots

Response: Linear regression analyses on the difference and mean of the measurements was conducted in investigate if there was a bias over the range of measurements. This was added to the results section of the manuscript.

•Future recommendations should include similar studies on male participants

Response: This has been added as a limitation to the study and a recommendation.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

pone.0302439.s002.docx (24.6KB, docx)

Decision Letter 1

Preeti Kanawjia

4 Apr 2024

Validity of self-reported weight and height among female young adults in the United Arab Emirates

PONE-D-23-35645R1

Dear Dr. Haroun,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice will be generated when your article is formally accepted. Please note, if your institution has a publishing partnership with PLOS and your article meets the relevant criteria, all or part of your publication costs will be covered. Please make sure your user information is up-to-date by logging into Editorial Manager at Editorial Manager® and clicking the ‘Update My Information' link at the top of the page. If you have any questions relating to publication charges, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Preeti Kanawjia, MD

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The authors present an interesting study entitled "Validity of self-reported weight and height among female young adults in the United Arab Emirates". The study presents the findings on the validity of self-reported height and weight measures in a small group of female students within a defined age range. The authors have addressed my previous comments or acknowledged the limitations of their study in the text.

There remains several serious issues with the submission:

Major issues:

1. The resolution of the BA plots are of far too low resolution in the given file to be assessed. Only one file is provided to authors, in which the figures are embedded. Please ensure the figures provided to the journal are of high quality.

2. Limitations that should be stated and discussed are that the sample size was small, and that the observed group was of a limited demographic.

Minor errors:

1. Line 224: 0.976 was missing a decimal point.

2. Line 156: Are these undergraduate studies or do they already hold a Bachelor's degree in Public Health and Nutrition?

3. Figure 1 is not cross-referenced in the text.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

Acceptance letter

Preeti Kanawjia

8 Apr 2024

PONE-D-23-35645R1

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Haroun,

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now being handed over to our production team.

At this stage, our production department will prepare your paper for publication. This includes ensuring the following:

* All references, tables, and figures are properly cited

* All relevant supporting information is included in the manuscript submission,

* There are no issues that prevent the paper from being properly typeset

If revisions are needed, the production department will contact you directly to resolve them. If no revisions are needed, you will receive an email when the publication date has been set. At this time, we do not offer pre-publication proofs to authors during production of the accepted work. Please keep in mind that we are working through a large volume of accepted articles, so please give us a few weeks to review your paper and let you know the next and final steps.

Lastly, if your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at customercare@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Preeti Kanawjia

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Author 1.docx

    pone.0302439.s001.docx (12.8KB, docx)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    pone.0302439.s002.docx (24.6KB, docx)

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES