Skip to main content
PLOS One logoLink to PLOS One
. 2024 Apr 29;19(4):e0301765. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0301765

An examination of early socioeconomic status and neighborhood disadvantage as independent predictors of antisocial behavior: A longitudinal adoption study

Shelley A Gresko 1,2,*, Laura K Hink 1,2, Robin P Corley 2, Chandra A Reynolds 1,2,3, Elizabeth Muñoz 4, Soo Hyun Rhee 1,2
Editor: Bárbara Oliván-Blázquez5
PMCID: PMC11057761  PMID: 38683790

Abstract

The present study examined early socioeconomic status (SES) and neighborhood disadvantage (ND) as independent predictors of antisocial behavior (ASB) and addressed the etiology of the associations (i.e., genes versus the environment) using a longitudinal adoption design. Prospective data from the Colorado Adoption Project (435 adoptees, 598 nonadopted children, 526 biological grandparents of adoptees, 481 adoptive parents, and 617 nonadoptive parents including biological parents of unrelated siblings of adoptees) were examined. SES and ND were assessed during infancy and ASB was evaluated from ages four through 16 using parent and teacher report. Associations between predictors and ASB were compared across adoptive and nonadoptive families and sex. Early SES was a nominally significant, independent predictor of antisocial ASB, such that lower SES predicted higher levels of ASB in nonadoptive families only. ND was not associated with ASB. Associations were consistent across aggression and delinquency, and neither SES nor ND was associated with change in ASB over time. Nominally significant associations did not remain significant after controlling for multiple testing. As such, despite nonsignificant differences in associations across sex or adoptive status, we were unable to make definitive conclusions regarding the genetic versus environmental etiology of or sex differences in the influence of SES and ND on ASB. Despite inconclusive findings, in nonadoptees, results were consistent—in effect size and direction—with previous studies in the literature indicating that lower SES is associated with increased risk for ASB.

Introduction

Antisocial behavior (ASB) lies on a continuum and encompasses several psychological diagnoses and characteristics including oppositional defiant disorder, conduct disorder, antisocial personality disorder, and externalizing problems. These behaviors have far reaching negative implications for society and the individual, including crime [1], risk for high school dropout [2], early substance use initiation [3], other psychiatric disorders [4, 5], and premature mortality [57], and additional research clarifying the etiology of ASB is needed.

Overall, twin and adoption studies demonstrate that approximately 50% of the variance of ASB is explained by genetic influences, but that ASB is more significantly influenced by the shared environment compared to other behavioral traits [8, 9], with shared environmental influences explaining approximately 20% of the variance [10, 11]. Comprehensive research on the putative environmental influences on ASB is needed to prevent and mitigate ASB effectively.

Evidence for putative environmental predictors of ASB: Parental socioeconomic status and neighborhood disadvantage

Many studies have investigated parental socioeconomic status (SES) and neighborhood disadvantage (ND) as putative environmental influences on childhood ASB. Two meta-analyses examining the associations between parents’ SES and their children’s ASB have reported statistically significant, albeit small effects; for example, one review reported a Fisher’s Z = −0.099, p < .001 [12], and another reported that low SES was associated with a 0.28 standard deviation increase in child externalizing behavior [13]. In the first meta-analysis, the sample’s SES variance did not significantly moderate associations between SES and ASB. Several studies and one meta-analysis [14] also reported a positive association between higher ND (e.g., neighborhood-level poverty, social housing levels, and unemployment) and various ASB constructs [1524], beginning as early as toddlerhood [25], although a threshold of high ND may need to be surpassed before children are at increased risk for ASB, specifically in boys [25]. Hypothesized mechanisms of these associations include behavioral modeling (e.g., disadvantaged environments may provide models for ASB [17] and psychological maladjustment to stressors unique to disadvantaged environments [17, 26].

Associations between SES and ASB could be confounded by ND and vice versa, as ND and SES are significantly correlated and families with low SES likely live in more disadvantaged neighborhoods [22]. However, studies indicate that lower SES is an independent predictor of ASB after controlling for ND [21, 23, 24, 2729] and that higher ND is an independent predictor after controlling for SES [16, 21, 23, 24, 27, 2933]. In addition, SES and ND may influence ASB independently in toddlerhood, middle childhood, and adolescence; however, most studies have examined predictors and ASB concurrently [19, 23, 27, 32]. Addressing the longitudinal influence of early SES and ND on later ASB is important: socioeconomic adversity may cause severe and prolonged stress during early childhood, in part due to decreased parental resources and negative impacts on parent-infant attachment [34]. Additionally, this toxic stress may impact brain development and the long-term functioning of the biological stress system (i.e., stress response modulation and behavioral regulation) permanently via increased allostatic load [3537]. Changes in the functioning of the biological stress system mediate the connection between early adversity and negative psychological and behavioral outcomes, such as the poor self-regulation and violence characteristic of ASB [35, 38].

Associations between SES and ND and ASB across sex and type of ASB

Overall, ASB is more prevalent in boys than girls, with boys displaying more physical or overt aggression and girls displaying more relational aggression [3941]. Despite these mean differences in ASB prevalence across sex, associations between SES and ASB [12] and ND and ASB [21, 42] are consistent across sex [12]. However, few studies have examined sex differences in the independent associations across SES, ND, and ASB.

Also, SES and ND may predict ASB differentially depending on the type of ASB examined. For example, one study found that ND predicted nonaggressive (e.g., delinquency), but not aggressive, behaviors after controlling for familial SES [43]. A meta-analysis found no evidence for differences in associations between SES and ASB across aggression and delinquency [12] and included studies that did not control for ND. As the literature on this topic is sparse, more research is needed to clarify discrepancies in the independent influences of SES and ND on aggression versus delinquency.

Mechanisms influencing associations between parental SES, ASB, and ND: Genetic versus environmental influences

Two hypotheses explicate the mechanisms by which parental SES and ND may influence ASB. The sociogenic or social causation hypothesis [44] implies environmental mediation: that ASB is caused by the stress and adversity associated with lower SES environments. The social selection or downward “drift” hypothesis [45] implies passive rGE; i.e., parental genetic predispositions influence associations between the family environment and adolescent characteristics [4648]. In this context, ASB would lead to diminished occupational and educational gains over time such that lower SES and higher ND is a byproduct of familial ASB history. These two hypotheses are not mutually exclusive, and both may explain associations between SES and ND and ASB.

Support for environmental mediation is suggested by natural and randomized experimental studies concluding that individuals who receive additional income or who experience a change in SES report fewer ASB symptoms [41, 4952]. Similarly, a randomized experimental study found support for environmental mediation, such that relocation from high to low poverty neighborhoods for adolescents in low-income families was associated with reduced arrests for violent offenses although the authors did not control for change in family SES [53].

Utilizing adoption studies to distinguish between passive rGE versus environmental mediation

It is often assumed that putatively environmental variables such as SES and ND influence ASB purely via the environment. However, environmental exposures and genetic background may be confounded. Adoption studies are a unique way to account for this confound by addressing the role of passive rGE versus environmental mediation. Because adoptive parents share only the familial environment with adoptees, similarities between adoptive parents and adoptees are assumed to have a purely environmental etiology. In contrast, similarities between biological parents and their children may be due to both genetic and environmental influences. If associations between predictors and ASB are only due to environmental mediation, we expect to see comparable associations between predictors and outcomes in adoptive and nonadoptive groups. If the association is due to passive rGE only, we would expect to see associations in only nonadoptive families in the absence of selective placement. If both processes influence associations, we would expect associations to be significant in both groups, but higher in nonadoptive families. Neither evocative rGE (where environmental responses are evoked by a child’s genetically influenced behavior) nor active rGE (where children seek environments aligned with their genetic predispositions) are likely explanations for the influence of ND and parental SES in early childhood [4648] in adoptive or biological families.

Present study

The present study addressed the independent influence of early parental SES and ND, assessed in children’s infancy, on ASB assessed from early childhood through adolescence, using data from the Colorado Adoption Project [54]. Many studies have examined the influence of SES and ND on concurrent ASB, but very few studies have examined the associations between these predictors measured in early infancy with later ASB, despite the well-documented connections between early adversity, toxic stress, and later maladaptive psychological functioning [55, 56]. First, we aimed to replicate previous findings that SES and ND independently predict ASB [21, 23, 24, 27, 29] and extend these findings to association with SES and ND measured in infancy (Aim 1). We also tested whether environmental mediation, passive rGE, or both influence these associations, to clarify a gap in the existing literature, which has yet to define the mechanisms by which early SES and ND influence ASB. Second, we explored whether early SES and ND independently predict change in ASB over time (Aim 2). One study on this topic found that boys in deprived neighborhoods were less likely to exhibit declines in ASB compared to girls [21] and another found that low familial SES was associated with an increase in aggression across childhood [57]; however, overall, prior research on the connection between SES, ND, and ASB trajectories is minimal and warrants further examination. In Aim 3, we examined differences in patterns of associations between predictors and aggression (e.g., physical fighting) versus delinquency (e.g., theft and truancy), as these two aspects of ASB are distinct, although there is not conclusive evidence that these have different predictors and etiology [12, 43, 58, 59]. Aims one to three were exploratory, as previous research on these topics is limited with mixed results. Finally, we examined sex differences in associations between predictors and ASB (Aim 4). We did not expect to see significant etiological differences, given lack of reliable sex differences in the existing literature [12, 41, 42].

Method

Sample

Participants were from The Colorado Adoption Project (CAP), an ongoing longitudinal adoption study that began in 1975 at the Institute of Behavioral Genetics in Boulder, Colorado. The full sample includes 245 adoptive families, 245 biological parent dyads of adoptees (although most data was on biological mothers only), and 245 control families that were matched on sex of the adoptive child, age and occupational status of the father, and number of children in the family. The present study includes data from 427 adoptees (from 181 girls, 203 boys; either the initial adoptees recruited from the 245 adoptive families or their adopted siblings), 598 nonadopted children (either biological children within adoptive families or children in nonadoptive families; from 284 girls, 314 boys), 526 biological grandparents of adoptees (from 267 grandmothers, 259 grandfathers, including data from grandparents of adopted siblings of initial probands), 486 adoptive parents (from 241 mothers, 245 fathers), and 617 biological parents of nonadoptees (306 mothers, 311 fathers). Prospective recruitment began on January 1st, 1976, and ended on September 30th, 1987. Biological mothers of adoptees were recruited from two large adoption agencies in Colorado. Their children were placed into their adoptive family homes within one year after birth. Social workers matched adoptees with adoptive families on non-proximity of location and similarity of height between adoptive and biological parents. No additional explicit selective placement practices were followed. Seventy-five percent of adoptive parents recruited for the study agreed to participate. Matched control families were recruited from local hospitals. Sibling enrollment and recruitment ended with the last longitudinally followed sibling. Biological and adoptive parents were assessed with a comprehensive battery of psychological measures when children were one year old or younger. Adoptees and nonadoptees were assessed approximately annually either through home or lab visits or telephone interviews from age one year to 16 years. The sample is over 90% White and is demographically representative of the Denver Metropolitan area at the time of recruitment [60]. Participants self-reported their race as one of five categories: Alaskan Native/American Indigenous (1.4%), Asian (4.7%), Black (0.6%), White (91.6%), and more than one race (0.9%), or unknown or not reported (0.9%). Probands were, on average, adopted within one month of birth, mitigating potential confounds between genetic and environmental influences. Importantly, adoptive and control parents were more likely to be older and in the upper half of SES and upper two-thirds of ND distributions compared to the general population. This project is approved by the University of Colorado Boulder Office of Research Integrity’s Institutional Review Board (protocol number 14–0421). Parents completed written consent and children aged seven and older provided written or verbal assent and children provided written consent at age 16. Only participants who provided consent for researchers to geocode first addresses were included in this study. For a detailed description of the recruitment and assessment protocols, refer to Plomin and DeFries [60] and Rhea et al. [54]. Data were collected beginning in 1976. The socioeconomic status and antisocial behavior data were accessed by the authors on April 27th, 2021, and the neighborhood disadvantage data were finalized and accessed on January 20th, 2023 (see Methods section for more details on the neighborhood disadvantage variable). De-identified data and code used in analyses are available upon request and, at the time of publication, data are in the process of being added to the Harvard Dataverse.

Measures

Socioeconomic status

SES variables were collected for biological maternal grandparents, adoptive parents, and biological parents of nonadoptees upon entering the study. For adoptive parents and biological parents of nonadoptees, three assessments of familial SES were used: maternal and paternal education (i.e., number of years of schooling completed) and paternal occupational ratings based on the National Opinion Research Center (NORC) 1970 occupational rating scale [61]. Only paternal occupational status was examined; given that the adoption agencies required that one parent be an at-home parent and given the limited number of mothers working outside of the home when the study was initiated, including occupational ratings for mothers would not have meaningfully added to the assessment of familial SES during infancy.

Neighborhood disadvantage

The U.S. Census Bureau’s Batch Address Geocoder converted participants’ addresses at intake to latitude and longitude coordinates and obtained geoidentifiers (GEOID) with resolution to a Census Block. Addresses not located with the U.S. Census tool were entered into Google Maps to identify coordinates. Census tracts respond to population growth; consequently, they change across decennial surveys. Thus, to retain tract boundaries consistent with the 2010 census, we used the Longitudinal Tract Database (LTDB) [62]. First addresses between 1976 and 1989 were matched to the 1980 census which represented 99.7% of the CAP sample, and the remaining .3% to either the 1990 or 2000 census.

We developed an ND composite for addresses using the following Census tract indicators clustered by decennial year (either 1980 or 1990+2000) using the CAP sample and a parallel sample of twins from the Colorado Longitudinal Twin Study (LTS) that were aligned with the 1980 or 1990 census. Each tract was scored on a scale from 0–9 corresponding to that tract’s value on a given indicator: percent with a high school degree or less; percent of female headed families; percent unemployment; percent in poverty; median household income (reverse scored); percent owner occupied units (reverse scored); median contract rent (reverse scored). ND was the mean of decile scores across the seven indicators.

Antisocial behavior

Parent-reported ASB was assessed with Achenbach’s Child Behavior Checklist (CBCL) [63] externalizing scale at age 4, 7, and yearly from 9–16 years (frequencies and descriptives in S1 and S2 Tables). Teacher-reported ASB was assessed via the Teacher Report Form (TRF) [64] externalizing scale yearly at ages 7–16 years. The externalizing scales consisted of a sum of subscales for aggression (characterized by the sum score of questions such as “physically attacks people” and “threatens people”) and delinquency (captured by the sum score of questions such as “truancy, skips school” and “vandalism”). Because the externalizing scale was significantly skewed, the items were binned into ordinal variables, maximizing the number of categories while avoiding small cell sizes (frequencies in S2 Table). This method of transforming non-normally distributed variables into ordinal categories reduces bias by assuming an underlying continuous normal liability distribution [65].

To examine aggression, delinquency, and overall ASB, we conducted confirmatory factors analyses (CFA). CFA were conducted using MPlus Version 8.4 [66]. We used the χ2 statistic to assess model fit but due to its sensitivity to sample size, we also examined the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) [67] and Bentler’s Comparative Fit Indices (CFI) [68]. An RMSEA < .06 and a CFI > .95 are indicative of good model fit [69]. The p-value of the z-statistic, which is the ratio of the parameter estimate to its standard error, was used to determine statistical significance of individual parameter estimates.

For overall ASB, we created a latent hierarchical factor with loadings on a parent reported ASB and teacher reported ASB factors, which in turn had loadings on composite ASB scores for each year of assessment, with auto-residual correlations to account for higher correlations between items in closer temporal proximity (Fig 1). We used CFA instead of exploratory factor analysis (EFA) to capture a broad construct of ASB across childhood and adolescence. The latent hierarchical ASB factor model fit the data well, χ2(806) = 898.55, p = .01, RMSEA = 0.02, CFI = 0.99. We also created separate delinquency and aggression factors comprised of hierarchical latent factors with loadings on parent and teacher reported aggression and delinquency scales. CFA, as opposed to EFA, were conducted for aggression and delinquency because these are well-established characteristics of antisocial behavior [70, 71]. Model fit for the aggression, χ2(729) = 806.45, p = .02, RMSEA = 0.03, CFI = 0.99, and delinquency, χ2(759) = 855.10, p = .01, RMSEA = 0.03, CFI = 0.98, factors were good.

Fig 1. Hierarchical latent ASB factor.

Fig 1

Note: unstandardized factor loadings presented. All factor loadings significant at p < .001. Model fit: χ2(806) = 898.55, p = .01, RMSEA = 0.02, CFI = 0.99.

Statistical analyses

Statistical analyses were conducted in Mplus version 8.4 [66] utilizing structural equation modeling. As mentioned above, a non-significant χ2 statistic, RMSEA < .06, and CFI > .95 were indicators of good model fit. The weighted least square mean and variance (WLSMV) estimation method was used, since all analyses included ordinal variables, and missing data were handled using pairwise deletion. Data from individuals within the same family were analyzed using the TYPE = COMPLEX option, which accounts for nonindependence when calculating standard errors and model fit. To correct for multiple testing, we used the False Discovery Rate (FDR) for all regression analyses, which controls for the expected proportion of falsely rejected null hypotheses [72, 73]. For all models, we used an alpha level of .05.

For Aim 1, we evaluated associations between SES, ND, and general ASB (measured by a hierarchical latent variable of parent and teacher reported ASB) with structural equation modeling. For models with nominally significant associations (i.e., p < .05 prior to correcting for multiple testing), we tested whether associations are best explained by environmental mediation versus passive rGE. Passive rGE would be indicated if associations are significantly higher in nonadoptive, compared to adoptive families. Environmental mediation would be suggested by any significant associations in adoptive families. We also examined differences in associations across sex assigned at birth. To examine differences across sex and adoption status, alternative models fixing associations to be equal across adoptive status (e.g., adopted and nonadopted girls; adopted and nonadopted boys) or sex (e.g., nonadopted girls and boys; adopted girls and boys) were compared to models where associations were freed across groups, via a χ2 difference test.

We used latent basis growth modeling (an efficient method to estimate change over time) to examine associations between predictors and ASB initial levels and change (Fig 2) [74]. ASB loadings were fixed to zero and 1.0 for the first and last timepoint, respectively [75]. Intermediate factor loadings represented percent change between first and last timepoint and were freely estimated. The latent intercept factor measured common variance across timepoints and had an unstandardized loading at 1.0 (see S5 Table for growth model parameters). Lastly, we examined associations separated by aggression and delinquency. Models were run separately for aggression and delinquency and effect sizes and significance of associations were compared.

Fig 2. Parent reported ASB latent growth factor regressed on adoptive parent SES (for adoptees) and Biological Parent SES (for nonadoptees) and ND.

Fig 2

Note: * = freely estimated loadings; “SES” = socioeconomic status; “ND” = neighborhood disadvantage. Unstandardized factor loadings, correlations, and regression coefficients reported. All factor loadings significant at p < .05. Model fit: χ2(296) = 368.34, p = 0.00, RMSEA = .03, CFI = .99, TLI = .99. Estimates presented as: adopted girls/adopted boys nonadopted girls/nonadopted boys.

Results

Sample description

Socioeconomic status

Given that biological parents of adoptees were younger on average (mothers’ mean age = 19.4; fathers’ mean age = 21.1) than biological parents of nonadoptees or adoptive parents, they had less opportunity for career or educational attainment. Therefore, for the biological parents of adoptees, the adoptees’ maternal grandmothers’ and grandfathers’ (i.e., the adoptees’ grandparents) educational attainment and maternal grandfathers’ NORC scores were examined. Results of a one-way ANOVA indicated that there were statistically significant differences in composite SES variables across adoptive parents, biological grandparents, and biological parents of nonadoptees (F(2,727) = [50.47], p < .001). Tukey’s HSD Test for multiple comparisons indicated that biological grandparent SES was significantly lower compared to SES for adoptive parents and biological parents of nonadoptees (p < .001), and that there were no differences across the SES of adoptive parents and biological parents of nonadoptees. Cohort effects in education may be partly responsible for the significantly lower SES scores for biological grandparents of adoptees, given that biological grandparents of adoptees were older than adoptive parents and biological parents of nonadoptees.

Education and NORC scores were transformed into z-scores, then averaged to create a composite SES variable. Table 1 shows the means and standard deviations for the SES measures and S3 and S4 Tables show correlations between individual measures of SES. Correlations between SES variables were significant at p < .001 and ranged from .32 to .60, except for a nonsignificant correlation between mother’s education and father’s NORC score in nonadopted boys (S4 Table).

Table 1. Descriptive Statistics for Parental SES Variables and ND.
Adoptees N M SD Range
 Biological GF’s Number of Years of Schooling Completed 259 13.53 2.99 5–21
 Biological GM’s Number of Years of Schooling Completed 267 13.01 2.26 8–21
 Biological GF’s NORC Score 254 47.25 14.41 17.3–81.2
 Adoptive Father’s Number of Years of Schooling Completed 233 15.67 2.46 9–21
 Adoptive Mother’s Number of Years of Schooling Completed 238 14.69 2.10 10–21
 Adoptive Father’s NORC Score 243 51.88 13.23 16.4–81.2
 Adoptive Parent’s ND 139 4.03 2.05 0–8.86
Nonadoptees N M SD Range
 Biological Father’s Number of Years of Schooling Completed 301 15.69 2.33 6–21
 Biological Mother’s Number of Years of Schooling Completed 306 14.85 2.09 8–21
 Biological Father’s NORC Score 311 51.13 12.05 19.3–81.2
 Biological Parent’s ND 200 4.71 1.87 0–8.9

Note: “Biological GF” = biological maternal grandfather of adoptees; “biological GM” = biological maternal grandmother of adoptees; “ND” = neighborhood disadvantage. Limitations in the 1980 census tract data led to decreased Ns of ND compared to SES data.

Neighborhood disadvantage

Overall, the mean ND score, which was the mean of decile scores across the seven indicators, was 4.51 (SD = 1.98). Table 1 shows mean and standard deviations for ND in adoptive versus nonadoptive parents and S3 and S4 Tables show correlations between ND and SES domains. Results of an independent samples between-subjects two-tailed t-test indicated that ND for nonadoptive parents was significantly higher than ND for adoptive parents, t (297) = [−4.35], p < .001.

Correlations

We first examined correlations between SES measures and ND. As expected, SES measures were negatively correlated with ND, although correlations were not significant (S3 and S4 Tables). We then examined correlations between predictors and ASB domains (Table 2). ND and ASB were not significantly correlated, and its association was not consistent in direction or magnitude across groups. In nonadoptees, parental SES was significantly negatively correlated with ASB. In general, parental SES was not significantly correlated with ASB in adoptees. Only biological parent SES and aggression were significantly, positively correlated in adopted girls.

Table 2. Correlations between ND, parent SES and ASB domains.

Adoptees
Biological Parent SES Adoptive Parent SES ND
Girls Boys Girls Boys Girls Boys
r [CI] p r [CI] p r [CI] p r [CI] p r [CI] p r [CI] p
ASB Hierarchical Factor .21 [-.02, .44] .07 .01 [-.22, .24] .93 .03 [-.19, .25] .79 -.07 [-.27, .13] .49 -.05 [-.32, .23] .72 .15 [-.10, .40] .23
Aggression Hierarchical Factor .28* [.05, .52] .02 .01 [-.24, .26] .94 .07 [-.17, .31] .55 -.06 [-.26, .15] .59 -.06 [-.35, .24] .76 .14 [-.14, .42] .32
Delinquency Hierarchical Factor .27 [-.01, .55] .06 .04 [-.18, .26] .70 -.08 [-.34, .19] .58 -.11 [-.31, .10] .31 -.06 [-.36, .25] .72 .15 [-.08, .38] .19
Nonadoptees
Biological Parent SES ND
Girls Boys Girls Boys
r [CI] p r [CI] p r [CI] p r [CI] p
ASB Hierarchical Factor -.18 [-.38, .02] .55 -.19* [-.36, -.01] .04 .07 [-.16, .31] .07 .05 [-.18, .29] .66
Aggression Hierarchical Factor -.17 [-.39, .05] .12 -.24* [-.45, -.02] .03 .05 [-.21, .30] .70 .06 [-.21, .32] .68
Delinquency Hierarchical Factor -.21* [-.41, -.01] .04 -.15 [-.34, .04] .11 -.01 [-.24, .22] .93 .03 [-.18, .25] .76

*p < .05

Overall, the positive correlations between biological parent SES and ASB domains were opposite in direction from expected in adopted girls. To ensure that correlations were not due to coding errors, we examined correlations between individual ASB measures and individual biological parent SES domains (i.e., maternal grandmother’s education, maternal grandfather’s education, and maternal grandfather’s occupation score). Associations across SES domains and ASB items were positive, consistent with correlations between the biological parent SES composite variable and ASB factors (results available upon request).

Aim 1: Are SES and ND independently associated with ASB?

In adoptees, we found no evidence for independent effects of predictors on ASB (Table 3). In nonadoptees, biological parent SES nominally predicted ASB in boys, but associations were not significant after FDR correction. Associations between SES and ASB were not significantly different across sex or adoption status, Δχ2(2) = 0.488, p = .784 and Δχ2(2) = 2.560, p = .278, respectively.

Table 3. ASB hierarchical factor regressed on parent SES and ND.

Adoptees
N = 419 Adoptive Parent SES ND
β [CI] SE p β [CI] SE p
Girls .04 [-.18, .25] .11 .75 -.05 [-.33, .23] .14 .71
Boys -.08 [-.28, .13] .10 .47 .16 [-.09, .40] .13 .22
N = 389 Biological Parent SES ND
β [CI] SE p β [CI] SE p
Girls .21 [-.02, .44] .12 .07 -.04 [-.31, .24] .14 .80
Boys .04 [-.20, .28] .12 .75 .16 [-.10, .42] .13 .23
Nonadoptees
N = 591 Biological Parent SES ND
β [CI] SE p β [CI] SE p
Girls -.17 [-.38, .03] .10 .10 .05 [-.20, .29] .12 .71
Boys -.19* [-.36, -.01] .09 .04 .05 [-.19, .28] .12 .70

*non-FDR corrected p < .05

Note: β = standardized regression coefficient; “CI” = confidence interval; “SE” = standard error

Model fit for model examining adoptive parent SES of adoptees and nonadoptive parent SES of nonadoptees: χ2(950) = 1046.72, p = 0.02, RMSEA = .02, CFI = .99.

Model fit for model examining biological parent SES of adoptees: χ2(440) = 479.43, p = 0.10, RMSEA = .02, CFI = .99.

Aim 2: Do early SES and ND predict change in ASB over time?

Teacher-reported ASB did not show consistent patterns of change over time, likely because ASB was assessed yearly by a different teacher. As such, we only investigated change over time for parent reported items. Parent-reported ASB decreased significantly over time in all groups (S5 Table).

Predictors were not correlated with the intercept (which captures stability with the initial level) of parent reported ASB of parent reported ASB (S6 and S7 Tables) and remained non-significant in multiple regression analyses (Table 4). Only SES of the biological parents of nonadoptees was significantly correlated with the ASB slope (in nonadopted girls), such that higher SES was correlated with less decrease in ASB (S6 and S7 Tables). This association remained nominally significant after controlling for ND but was no longer significant after correcting for multiple testing (Table 4). We found no significant sex or adoptive status differences in associations between parental SES and ASB slope, Δχ2(2) = 0.947, p = 0.623 and Δχ2(2) = 3.392, p = 0.066, respectively.

Table 4. Parent-reported ASB slope and intercept regressed on parent SES and ND.

Adoptees
Intercept Regression Slope Regression
N = 415 Adoptive Parent SES ND Biological Parent SES ND
β [CI] SE p β [CI] SE p β [CI] β [CI]
Girls -.05 [-.26, .15] .11 .62 .03 [-.21, .26] .12 .82 .01 [-.21, .22] .11 .96 -.17 [-.42, .09] .13 .20
Boys -.19 [-.39, .01] .10 .07 .03 [-.19, .26] .12 .78 .09 [-.19, .37] .14 .53 .21 [-.18, .61] .20 .29
N = 385 Biological Parent SES ND Biological Parent SES ND
β [CI] SE p β [CI] SE p β [CI] SE p β [CI] SE p
Girls .13 [-.12, .37] .12 .30 .02 [-.22, .26] .12 .87 .19 [-.07, .45] .13 .16 -.19 [-.47, .08] .14 .16
Boys .04 [-.16, .24] .10 .71 .05 [-.18, .28] .12 .68 -.05 [-.36, .25] .16 .73 .23 [-.21, .66] .22 .31
Nonadoptees
Intercept Regression Slope Regression
N = 591 Biological Parent SES ND Biological Parent SES ND
β [CI] SE p β [CI] SE p β [CI] SE p β [CI] SE p
Girls .01 [-.21, .24] .11 .93 .15 [-.10, .39] .12 .24 -.28* [-.52, -.05] .12 .02 -.12 [-.42, .18] .15 .43
Boys -.02 [-.22, .17] .10 .81 .04 [-.16, .23] .10 .73 -.14 [-.38, .10] .12 .26 .05 [-.19, .30] .13 .67

*non-FDR corrected p < .05

Note: β = standardized regression coefficient; “CI” = confidence interval; “SE” = standard error

Model fit for model examining adoptive parent SES of adoptees and nonadoptive parent SES of nonadoptees: χ2(296) = 368.34, p = 0.00; RMSEA = .03, CFI = .99, TLI = .99.

Model fit for model examining biological parent SES of adoptees: χ2(143) = 170.76, p = 0.06, RMSEA = .03, CFI = 1.00.

Aim 3: Are there differences in patterns of associations between SES, ND, and aggression and delinquency scales?

Aggression

ND was neither significantly correlated nor independently associated with aggression (Table 2). In adopted girls, biological parent SES was positively correlated with aggression. Biological parent SES of adoptees remained an independent predictor after controlling for ND (Table 5). There were no significant sex differences in these associations, Δχ2(1) = 1.951, p = .163. In nonadopted boys, biological parent SES and aggression were significantly, negatively correlated. This association remained nominally significant after controlling for ND. These associations did not significantly differ across sex or adoption status, Δχ2(2) = 0.818, p = .665 and Δχ2(2) = 3.301, p = .192, respectively. No nominally significant associations were significant after correcting for multiple testing.

Table 5. Aggression and delinquency factors regressed on parent SES and ND.
Adoptees
Aggression Factor
N = 389 Adoptive Parent SES ND
β [CI] SE p β [CI] SE p
Girls .08 [-.16, .32] .12 .52 -.06 [-.37, .24] .16 .68
Boys -.06 [-.27, .15] .11 .57 .14 [-.13, .41] .14 .31
N = 356 Biological Parent SES ND
β [CI] SE p β [CI] SE p
Girls .28* [.05, .51] .12 .02 -.05 [-.34, .25] .15 .75
Boys .04 [-.22, .29] .13 .79 .15 [-.14, .44] .15 .31
Delinquency Factor
N = 389 Adoptive Parent SES ND
β [CI] SE p β [CI] SE p
Girls -.07 [-.34, .19] .14 .60 -.05 [-.35, .25] .15 .75
Boys -.11 [-.32, .10] .11 .30 .16 [-.07, .39] .12 .18
N = 356 Biological Parent SES ND
β [CI] SE p β [CI] SE p
Girls .27 [-.01, .55] .14 .06 -.04 [-.33, .26] .15 .81
Boys .08 [-.15, .31] .12 .50 .17 [-.07, .41] .12 .16
Nonadoptees
Aggression Factor
N = 591 Biological Parent SES ND
β [CI] SE p β [CI] SE p
Girls -.17 [-.39, .05] .12 .14 .03 [-.24, .29] .13 .85
Boys -.23* [-.45, -.02] .11 .03 .05 [-.22, .31] .13 .73
Delinquency Factor
N = 591 Biological Parent SES ND
β [CI] SE p β [CI] SE p
Girls -.22* [-.42, -.02] .10 .03 -.04 [-.28, .20] .12 .73
Boys -.15 [-.33, .04] .09 .11 .03 [-.19, .25] .11 .80

*non-FDR corrected p < .05

Note: β = standardized regression coefficient; “CI” = confidence interval; “SE” = standard error

Aggression: model fit for model examining adoptive parent SES of adoptees and nonadoptive parent SES of nonadoptees: χ2(873) = 961.81, p = 0.02, RMSEA = 0.02, CFI = 0.99.

Aggression: model fit for model examining biological parent SES: χ2(415) = 440.74, p = 0.18, RMSEA = 0.02, CFI = 1.00.

Delinquency: model fit for model examining adoptive parent SES of adoptees and nonadoptive parent SES of nonadoptees: χ2(903) = 987.74, p = 0.03, RMSEA = 0.02, CFI = 0.98.

Delinquency: model fit for model examining biological parent SES: χ2(425) = 434.85, p = 0.36, RMSEA = 0.01, CFI = 1.00.

Delinquency

ND was neither significantly correlated nor independently associated with delinquency (Table 2). Only biological parent SES in nonadopted girls was significantly correlated with delinquency. This association remained significant after controlling for ND but was no longer significant after multiple testing correction (Table 5). We found no significant differences across sex or adoption status, Δχ2(2) 0.357, p = .836 and sex, Δχ2(2) = 0.810, p = .667, respectively.

Sensitivity and attrition analyses

Participants provided consent for analyses of early ND data many years after initial assessment. We conducted attrition and sensitivity analyses to determine whether missingness for ND predicted differences in early SES, ASB, or associations across predictors. Independent samples t-tests demonstrated that individuals with ND data had significantly higher parental SES t(488) = [2.83], p = .005, but no significant differences in ASB, t(735) = [.23], p = .82. Additionally, we conducted analyses including only individuals with ND data. Results were similar in analyses including only individuals with ND data and those including all participants (S8S14 Tables), including slope estimates for latent growth curve models and effect sizes for regression of ASB slope and hierarchical ASB factor on SES and ND.

Discussion

The present study utilized a longitudinal adoption design to examine the etiology of associations between SES, ND, and ASB. We aimed to replicate previous findings suggesting independent influences of SES and ND on ASB and to add to extant literature by addressing whether these are due to environmental mediation, passive rGE, or a combination of the two processes. We examined the influence of predictors on ASB over time, tested for sex differences in all associations, and evaluated the magnitude of associations between SES, ND, and aggressive versus delinquent ASB.

Contrary to prior research [16, 21, 23, 24, 27, 2931], our analyses for Aim 1 did not demonstrate that ND independently predicted ASB in any group. Effect sizes were small, not significant, and inconsistent in direction. Additionally, most previous research tested associations between ND assessed in middle childhood and ASB in middle childhood to adolescence, although one study did find long-lasting effects of ND assessed during infancy on later ASB [32]. The nonsignificant associations between ND and ASB may be in part explained by the limited variability of early ND in the present sample, as the association between ND and ASB may be strongest at highest levels of ND [30].

In contrast to our results regarding ND, associations between SES and ASB in nonadoptees were consistent with direction of results in the extant literature [12, 13, 23, 24]. Overall, SES for biological parents of nonadoptees was negatively associated with ASB. Additionally, effect sizes in nonadoptees (standardized betas ranging from -.15 to -.28) were comparable to or larger than those reported in existing studies, as a meta-analyses of associations between SES and ASB have found small but significant overall effects [12, 13]. In general, children whose parents had lower education and occupation scores were at higher risk for overall ASB in addition to aggressive and delinquent behavior. These results further illustrate the negative contribution of lower SES on the psychological health of children and adolescents and demonstrate that this influence can begin as early as infancy. Additionally, prior research has demonstrated that familial factors generally have a greater magnitude of influence on ASB compared to neighborhood level factors [24] and our results may reflect this phenomenon.

Alignment between results of the present study and prior research did not extend to results in adoptees. The direction of effects between adoptive parent SES and ASB were inconsistent, and associations were not significant. Counterintuitively, associations between SES of biological parents of adoptees and ASB were generally positive in direction and nominally significant (specifically for associations between biological parent SES and ASB in adopted girls). These results should be regarded with caution and interpreted in the context of the number of tests conducted. If these results indicate a broader pattern of positive associations between SES and ASB between biological parents and their children, we would likely have seen similar associations between nonadoptive parents and nonadoptees.

Regarding Aims 1 and 4, we found no evidence for differences in associations across sex or adoptees (in which adopted children and adoptive parents share only their environment) and nonadoptees (in which nonadopted children share both genes and environment with their parents). Overall, our findings regarding sex differences align with our hypotheses and prior research [11, 41, 76, 77]. However, our results did not replicate a previous adoption study that observed sex differences in the mechanisms by which SES influenced ASB (i.e., stronger environmental influences for boys than girls; [78] and we were unable to conclusively determine whether associations were primarily due to environmental influences or passive rGE.

ASB significantly decreased over time in our sample, consistent with expectations [79, 80], but we did not find evidence that differences in decreases in ASB were due to SES or ND (Aim 3). Higher SES for biological parents of nonadoptees nominally predicted less ASB decline in girls only. Prior research on this topic is minimal and found that high ND and lower SES predicted less decline and increase in ASB, respectively [21, 57]. Additional research on predictors of change in ASB is needed. In addition, we found similar effect sizes across aggressive versus nonaggressive behaviors (Aim 3).

Importantly, all results should be interpreted with caution; although several of our results were nominally significant, associations did not remain significant after controlling for multiple testing. As such, we also would not necessarily expect significant differences in associations across sex or adoptive status. Thus, we cannot make definitive conclusions about the genetic versus environmental etiology of associations between predictors and ASB (Aim 1). We also cannot conclude confidently whether influence of SES or ND on ASB differs across sex.

Limitations

There are several limitations to consider when interpreting these results. First, it is often assumed correctly that adoption studies have a restricted range of environmental influences provided by adoptive parents. Potential adoptive parents are often precluded from adopting if they do not meet thresholds of financial security, mental health, and marital stability. Of those environmental factors most restricted in adoption studies, SES is among the top, with one study estimating an 18% reduction in variance [81]. Interestingly, the same study showed that ASB in adoptive parents was much less restricted than SES, with only a 7–8% reduction in variance for adoptive parents. Second, only paternal occupational status was examined since the adoption agencies required one parent be an at-home parent and few mothers worked outside of the home when the study was initiated, but SES estimates may be low for families with mothers working outside of the home. Third, measures used for SES and ND were obtained in general when subjects were less than a year old. Changes in familial SES or ND may have occurred over the course of the study and our estimates do not capture how those changes or SES and ND in later childhood or adolescence influence ASB, although changes in SES and ND may play a role in ASB change over time. Children may be more likely to engage in delinquent behaviors during years when their parents’ SES is lower [52] and ASB may decrease if children are they are relocated from high to low ND environments [52, 53]. Research addressing this issue by assessing familial SES and ND along with ASB prospectively and regularly is needed.

In addition, other measures of ND may better capture factors influencing child ASB, as prior research has demonstrated that the magnitude of the associations between ND and child health [82] and externalizing outcomes [83] depend on the ND metric examined and covariates included, such as family level SES. Lastly, in adoptive families, the influence of adoptive familial SES [58], but likely not ND [84], on child ASB may be moderated by biological parent ASB. However, we were unable to test for gene-environment interaction in the present study, due to lack of a standard ASB assessment in biological parents of adoptees.

Strengths

The CAP is an ideal adoption study to examine putative environmental effects because it addresses potential limitations of adoption designs [85]. In the CAP, selective placement (e.g., resemblance between adoptive and biological parents that inflates estimates of genetic and environmental influences; [86, 87] is negligible for measures of education and SES [60]. Adoptees in the study were placed in adoptive homes within 29 days of birth, mitigating confounded genetic and environmental influences.

Conclusions

Despite limitations, the present study provides a unique contribution to the literature on the mechanisms involved in associations between SES, ND, and ASB. We found nominally significant negative associations between ASB and SES in nonadoptive families, although associations were no longer significant after correcting for multiple testing. Associations between ND and ASB were nonsignificant and inconsistent in direction. In this study, ND at an early age was not a reliable predictor of ASB. We found no evidence for differences in associations across sex, and the effects sizes of the associations between SES/ND and the aggression and delinquency scales were similar. Although our study’s findings were inconclusive, in nonadoptees, results align in effect size and direction with prior research indicating that individuals with lower SES are at higher risk for ASB. Results provide support for the potential impact of familial poverty on child psychological health and as income inequality rises globally [88], supporting families via effective social programs may mitigate poverty’s impact on negative child outcomes [21, 89].

Supporting information

S1 Table. Frequencies for parent and teacher reported externalizing scales.

(DOCX)

pone.0301765.s001.docx (19.6KB, docx)
S2 Table. Descriptive statistics for parent and teacher reported aggression and delinquency scales.

(DOCX)

pone.0301765.s002.docx (16.6KB, docx)
S3 Table. Correlations between SES variables and ND in adoptees.

(DOCX)

pone.0301765.s003.docx (15.2KB, docx)
S4 Table. Correlations between SES variables and ND in nonadoptees.

(DOCX)

pone.0301765.s004.docx (14.3KB, docx)
S5 Table. Parent reported ASB slope, intercept, and correlations between slope and intercept.

(DOCX)

pone.0301765.s005.docx (14.8KB, docx)
S6 Table. Correlations between parent reported ASB intercept and slope, ND, and biological and adoptive parent SES in adoptees.

(DOCX)

pone.0301765.s006.docx (15.2KB, docx)
S7 Table. Correlations between parent reported ASB intercept, slope, and biological Parent SES in nonadoptees.

(DOCX)

pone.0301765.s007.docx (14.8KB, docx)
S8 Table. ASB hierarchical factor regressed on adoptive and biological parent SES and ND in adoptees: Individuals with ND data only.

(DOCX)

pone.0301765.s008.docx (15.2KB, docx)
S9 Table. ASB hierarchical factor regressed on biological parent SES and ND in nonadoptees: Individuals with ND data only (N = 365).

(DOCX)

pone.0301765.s009.docx (14.7KB, docx)
S10 Table. Parent reported ASB slope, intercept, and correlations between slope and intercept: Individuals with ND data only (N = 428).

(DOCX)

pone.0301765.s010.docx (14.9KB, docx)
S11 Table. Parent reported ASB intercept regressed on biological and adoptive parent SES and ND in adoptees: Individuals with ND data only.

(DOCX)

pone.0301765.s011.docx (15.3KB, docx)
S12 Table. Parent reported ASB slope regressed on biological and adoptive parent SES and ND in adoptees: Individuals with ND data only.

(DOCX)

pone.0301765.s012.docx (15.3KB, docx)
S13 Table. Parent reported ASB intercept regressed on biological parent SES and ND in nonadoptees: Individuals with ND data only (N = 365).

(DOCX)

pone.0301765.s013.docx (14.8KB, docx)
S14 Table. Parent reported ASB slope regressed on biological parent SES and ND in nonadoptees: Individuals with ND data only (N = 365).

(DOCX)

pone.0301765.s014.docx (14.8KB, docx)

Acknowledgments

The authors would like to acknowledge the individuals who participated in this study.

Data Availability

Given that the data contain potentially identifying and sensitive patient information, data cannot be shared publicly because of restrictions put in place by the University of Colorado Boulder Institutional Review Board. Unidentifiable patient data are in the process of being added to the Harvard Dataverse repository. A portion of this data is currently available through the Harvard Dataverse at https://doi.org/doi:10.7910/DVN/BCDSEU (Plomin et al., 2022) and access can be requested by contacting the Manager of Operations for the Henry A. Murray Research Archive, Institute for Quantitative Social Sciences, 1737 Cambridge St, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA., mra@help.hmdc.harvard.edu. Researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data may apply for access to data not yet added to the Harvard Dataverse by contacting the corresponding author (shelley.gresko@colorado.edu) and the University of Colorado Boulder Institutional Review Board (irbadmin@colorado.edu). Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (2022). Colorado Adoption Project, 1976-1989 Version V1) [longitudinal, field study, hereditary]. Harvard Dataverse. https://doi.org/doi:10.7910/DVN/BCDSEU.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by National Institute on Drug Abuse grants: DA054087, DA011015, DA017637; National Institute on Aging grant: AG046938; and Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development grant: HD010333 SHR received grant #DA054087 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse, URL: https://nida.nih.gov/ John K. Hewitt received grant #DA011015 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse, URL: https://nida.nih.gov/ Jerry A. Stitzel received grant #DA017637 from the National Institute on Drug Abuse, URL: https://nida.nih.gov/ CAR and Sally J. Wadsworth received grant #AG046938 from the National Institute on Aging, URL: https://www.nia.nih.gov/ Sally J. Wadsworth received grant #HD010333 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, URL: https://www.nichd.nih.gov/ The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Morgan RE, Thompson A. Criminal Victimization, 2020 [Internet]. U.S. Department of Justice, Bureau of Justice Statistics; 2021. Report No.: 301775. https://bjs.ojp.gov/sites/g/files/xyckuh236/files/media/document/cv20.pdf
  • 2.Breslau J, Miller E, Joanie Chung WJ, Schweitzer JB. Childhood and adolescent onset psychiatric disorders, substance use, and failure to graduate high school on time. J Psychiatr Res. 2011. Mar;45(3):295–301. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2010.06.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Zeitlin H. Psychiatric comorbidity with substance misuse in children and teenagers. Drug Alcohol Depend. 1999. Jul 1;55(3):225–34. doi: 10.1016/s0376-8716(99)00018-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Ridder EM. Show me the child at seven: the consequences of conduct problems in childhood for psychosocial functioning in adulthood. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 2005;46(8):837–49. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.00387.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kim-Cohen J, Caspi A, Moffitt TE, Harrington H, Milne BJ, Poulton R. Prior Juvenile Diagnoses in Adults With Mental Disorder: Developmental Follow-Back of a Prospective-Longitudinal Cohort. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003. Jul 1;60(7):709–17. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.7.709 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Border R, Corley RP, Brown SA, Hewitt JK, Hopfer CJ, Stallings MC, et al. Predictors of adult outcomes in clinically- and legally-ascertained youth with externalizing problems. PLoS ONE [Internet]. 2018. Nov 1 [cited 2020 Apr 30];13(11). Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC6211688/ doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0206442 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Laub JH, Vaillant GE. Delinquency and mortality: A 50-year follow-up study of 1,000 delinquent and nondelinquent boys. Am J Psychiatry. 2000;157:96–102. doi: 10.1176/ajp.157.1.96 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kendler KS, Prescott CA, Myers J, Neale MC. The Structure of Genetic and Environmental Risk Factors for Common Psychiatric and Substance Use Disorders in Men and Women. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2003. Sep 1;60(9):929–37. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.60.9.929 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Polderman TJC, Benyamin B, de Leeuw CA, Sullivan PF, van Bochoven A, Visscher PM, et al. Meta-analysis of the heritability of human traits based on fifty years of twin studies. Nat Genet. 2015. Jul;47(7):702–9. doi: 10.1038/ng.3285 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Moffitt TE. Genetic and Environmental Influences on Antisocial Behaviors: Evidence from Behavioral–Genetic Research. In: Advances in Genetics [Internet]. Academic Press; 2005. [cited 2020 May 28]. p. 41–104. http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S006526600555003X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Rhee SH, Waldman ID. Genetic and environmental influences on antisocial behavior: A meta-analysis of twin and adoption studies. Psychol Bull. 2002. May;128(3):490–529. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Piotrowska PJ, Stride CB, Croft SE, Rowe R. Socioeconomic status and antisocial behaviour among children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2015. Feb 1;35:47–55. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2014.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Peverill M, Dirks MA, Narvaja T, Herts KL, Comer JS, McLaughlin KA. Socioeconomic status and child psychopathology in the United States: A meta-analysis of population-based studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2021. Feb 1;83:101933. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2020.101933 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Chang LY, Wang MY, Tsai PS. Neighborhood disadvantage and physical aggression in children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis of multilevel studies. Aggress Behav. 2016;42(5):441–54. doi: 10.1002/ab.21641 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Buu A, DiPiazza C, Wang J, Puttler LI, Fitzgerald HE, Zucker RA. Parent, Family, and Neighborhood Effects on the Development of Child Substance Use and Other Psychopathology From Preschool to the Start of Adulthood. J Stud Alcohol Drugs. 2009. Jul 1;70(4):489–98. doi: 10.15288/jsad.2009.70.489 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Duncan GJ, Brooks-Gunn J, Klebanov PK. Economic Deprivation and Early Childhood Development. Child Dev. 1994;65(2):296–318. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Fite PJ, Wynn P, Lochman JE, Wells KC. The effect of neighborhood disadvantage on proactive and reactive aggression. J Community Psychol. 2009. May 1;37(4):542–6. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Flouri E, Sarmadi Z. Prosocial behavior and childhood trajectories of internalizing and externalizing problems: The role of neighborhood and school contexts. Dev Psychol. 2016. Feb;52(2):253–8. doi: 10.1037/dev0000076 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Leventhal T, Brooks-Gunn J. The neighborhoods they live in: the effects of neighborhood residence on child and adolescent outcomes. Psychol Bull. 2000. Mar;126(2):309–37. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.126.2.309 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Martin-Storey A, Bizier-Lacroix R, Temcheff C, Déry M. Understanding Youth Perceptions of Neighborhood Disorder: The Role of Conduct Problems. J Youth Adolesc. 2021. May;50(5):952–64. doi: 10.1007/s10964-021-01418-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Odgers CL, Caspi A, Russell MA, Sampson RJ, Arseneault L, Moffitt TE. Supportive parenting mediates neighborhood socioeconomic disparities in children’s antisocial behavior from ages 5 to 12. Dev Psychopathol. 2012. Aug;24(3):705–21. doi: 10.1017/S0954579412000326 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Santiago CD, Wadsworth ME, Stump J. Socioeconomic status, neighborhood disadvantage, and poverty-related stress: Prospective effects on psychological syndromes among diverse low-income families. J Econ Psychol. 2011. Mar 1;32(2):218–30. [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Singh GK, Ghandour RM. Impact of Neighborhood Social Conditions and Household Socioeconomic Status on Behavioral Problems Among US Children. Matern Child Health J. 2012. Apr 1;16(1):158–69. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Sundquist J, Li X, Ohlsson H, Råstam M, Winkleby M, Sundquist K, et al. Familial and neighborhood effects on psychiatric disorders in childhood and adolescence. J Psychiatr Res. 2015. Jul 1;66–67:7–15. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2015.03.019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Winslow EB, Shaw DS. Impact of neighborhood disadvantage on overt behavior problems during early childhood. Aggress Behav. 2007;33(3):207–19. doi: 10.1002/ab.20178 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Sampson RJ, Groves WB. Community Structure and Crime: Testing Social-Disorganization Theory. Am J Sociol. 1989. Jan 1;94(4):774–802. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Heberle AE, Thomas YM, Wagmiller RL, Briggs-Gowan MJ, Carter AS. The impact of neighborhood, family, and individual risk factors on toddlers’ disruptive behavior. Child Dev. 2014. Oct;85(5):2046–61. doi: 10.1111/cdev.12251 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Schneiders J, Drukker M, van der Ende J, Verhulst F, van Os J, Nicolson N. Neighbourhood socioeconomic disadvantage and behavioural problems from late childhood into early adolescence. J Epidemiol Community Health. 2003. Sep;57(9):699–703. doi: 10.1136/jech.57.9.699 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Choi JK, Kelley MS, Wang D. Neighborhood Characteristics, Maternal Parenting, and Health and Development of Children from Socioeconomically Disadvantaged Families. Am J Community Psychol. 2018;62(3–4):476–91. doi: 10.1002/ajcp.12276 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Ingoldsby EM, Shaw DS, Winslow E, Schonberg M, Gilliom M, Criss MM. Neighborhood disadvantage, parent-child conflict, neighborhood peer relationships, and early antisocial behavior problem trajectories. J Abnorm Child Psychol. 2006. Jun;34(3):303–19. doi: 10.1007/s10802-006-9026-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Kohen DE, Leventhal T, Dahinten VS, McIntosh CN. Neighborhood disadvantage: pathways of effects for young children. Child Dev. 2008. Feb;79(1):156–69. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-8624.2007.01117.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Brooks-Gunn J, Duncan GJ, Klebanov PK, Sealand N. Do Neighborhoods Influence Child and Adolescent Development? Am J Sociol. 1993. Sep 1;99(2):353–95. [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Latham RM, Arseneault L, Alexandrescu B, Baldoza S, Carter A, Moffitt TE, et al. Violent experiences and neighbourhoods during adolescence: understanding and mitigating the association with mental health at the transition to adulthood in a longitudinal cohort study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2022. Dec 1;57(12):2379–91. doi: 10.1007/s00127-022-02343-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Center on the Developing Child at Harvard University. From Best Practices to Breakthrough Impacts. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University; 2016. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Garner AS. Home Visiting and the Biology of Toxic Stress: Opportunities to Address Early Childhood Adversity. Pediatrics. 2013. Nov 1;132(Supplement_2):S65–73. doi: 10.1542/peds.2013-1021D [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.McEwen BS, Gianaros PJ. Stress- and allostasis-induced brain plasticity. Annu Rev Med. 2011;62:431–45. doi: 10.1146/annurev-med-052209-100430 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Ha T, Granger DA. Family Relations, Stress, and Vulnerability: Biobehavioral Implications for Prevention and Practice. Fam Relat. 2016;65(1):9–23. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Murray DW, Rosanbalm K, Christopoulos C, Hamoudi A. Self-Regulation and Toxic Stress: Foundations for Understanding Self-Regulation from an Applied Developmental Perspective. Office of Planning, Research, and Evaluation; 2015. Jan. Report No.: 2015–2. [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Ehrensaft MK. Interpersonal Relationships and Sex Differences in the Development of Conduct Problems. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev. 2005. Mar 1;8(1):39. doi: 10.1007/s10567-005-2341-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Moffitt TE. Sex differences in antisocial behaviour: conduct disorder, delinquency, and violence in the Dunedin longitudinal study. Cambridge, UK; New York, NY: Cambridge University Press; 2001. [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Burt SA. The Genetic, Environmental, and Cultural Forces Influencing Youth Antisocial Behavior Are Tightly Intertwined. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2022;18(1):155–78. doi: 10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-072220-015507 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Cleveland HH. Disadvantaged Neighborhoods and Adolescent Aggression: Behavioral Genetic Evidence of Contextual Effects. J Res Adolesc. 2003;13(2):211–38. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Burt SA, Klump KL, Kashy DA, Gorman-Smith D, Neiderhiser JM. Neighborhood as a predictor of non-aggressive, but not aggressive, antisocial behaviors in adulthood. Psychol Med. 2015. Oct;45(13):2897–907. doi: 10.1017/S0033291715000975 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Dohrenwend BP, Levav I, Shrout PE, Schwartz S, Naveh G, Link BG, et al. Socioeconomic status and psychiatric disorders: the causation-selection issue. Science. 1992. Feb 21;255(5047):946–52. doi: 10.1126/science.1546291 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Goldberg EM, Morrison SL. Schizophrenia and Social Class. Br J Psychiatry. 1963. Nov;109(463):785–802. doi: 10.1192/bjp.109.463.785 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Knopik VS, Neiderhiser JM, DeFries JC, Plomin R. Behavioral Genetics. 7th ed. United Kingdom: Worth Publishers; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Plomin R, DeFries JC, Loehlin JC. Genotype-environment interaction and correlation in the analysis of human behavior. Psychol Bull. 1977;84(2):309–22. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Scarr S, McCartney K. How People Make Their Own Environments: A Theory of Genotype → Environment Effects. Child Dev. 1983;54(2):424–35. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Costello EJ, Compton SN, Keeler G, Angold A. Relationships between poverty and psychopathology: a natural experiment. JAMA J Am Med Assoc. 2003;290(15):2023–9. doi: 10.1001/jama.290.15.2023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Gennetian LA, Miller C. Children and Welfare Reform: A View from an Experimental Welfare Program in Minnesota. Child Dev. 2002;73(2):601–20. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00426 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Milligan K, Stabile M. Do Child Tax Benefits Affect the Well-Being of Children? Evidence from Canadian Child Benefit Expansions. Am Econ J Econ Policy. 2011. Aug;3(3):175–205. [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Rekker R, Pardini D, Keijsers L, Branje S, Loeber R, Meeus W. Moving in and out of Poverty: The Within-Individual Association between Socioeconomic Status and Juvenile Delinquency. PLoS ONE [Internet]. 2015. Nov 17 [cited 2020 May 28];10(11). Available from: https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC4648521/ doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0136461 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ludwig J, Duncan GJ, Hirschfield P. Urban Poverty and Juvenile Crime: Evidence from a Randomized Housing-Mobility Experiment. Q J Econ. 2001. May 1;116(2):655–79. [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Rhea SA, Bricker JB, Wadsworth SJ, Corley RP. The Colorado Adoption Project. Twin Res Hum Genet. 2013. Feb;16(1):358–65. doi: 10.1017/thg.2012.109 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Shonkoff JP, Garner AS, THE COMMITTEE ON PSYCHOSOCIAL ASPECTS OF CHILD AND FAMILY HEALTH COEC ADOPTION, AND DEPENDENT CARE, AND SECTION ON DEVELOPMENTAL AND BEHAVIORAL PEDIATRICS, Siegel BS, Dobbins MI, Earls MF, et al. The Lifelong Effects of Early Childhood Adversity and Toxic Stress. Pediatrics. 2012. Jan 1;129(1):e232–46. doi: 10.1542/peds.2011-2663 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.McEwen CA, McEwen BS. Social Structure, Adversity, Toxic Stress, and Intergenerational Poverty: An Early Childhood Model. Annu Rev Sociol. 2017;43(1):445–72. [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Tremblay RE, Nagin DS, Séguin JR, Zoccolillo M, Zelazo PD, Boivin M, et al. Physical Aggression During Early Childhood: Trajectories and Predictors. Can Child Adolesc Psychiatry Rev. 2005. Feb;14(1):3–9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Cadoret RJ, Troughton E, Bagford J, Woodworth G. Genetic and environmental factors in adoptee antisocial personality. Eur Arch Psychiatry Neurol Sci. 1990;239(4):231–40. doi: 10.1007/BF01738577 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Eley TC, Lichtenstein P, Stevenson J. Sex Differences in the Etiology of Aggressive and Nonaggressive Antisocial Behavior: Results from Two Twin Studies. Child Dev. 1999;70(1):155–68. doi: 10.1111/1467-8624.00012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Plomin R, DeFries JC. Origins of individual differences in infancy: the Colorado Adoption Project. Orlando, FL: Academic Press; 1985. [Google Scholar]
  • 61.General Social Survey. OCCUPATIONAL CLASSIFICATION DISTRIBUTIONS: Appendix F [Internet]. [cited 2023 Sep 2]. https://gss.norc.org/documents/codebook/GSS_Codebook_AppendixF.pdf
  • 62.Logan JR, Xu Z, Stults BJ. Interpolating U.S. Decennial Census Tract Data from as Early as 1970 to 2010: A Longitudinal Tract Database. Prof Geogr. 2014. Jul 3;66(3):412–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Achenbach T. Manual for the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18 and 1991 profile. University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; 1991.
  • 64.Achenbach TM. Manual for the Teacher’s Report Form and 1991 profile. University of Vermont Department of Psychiatry; 1991.
  • 65.Derks EM, Dolan CV, Boomsma DI. Effects of censoring on parameter estimates and power in genetic modeling. Twin Res Off J Int Soc Twin Stud. 2004. Dec;7(6):659–69. doi: 10.1375/1369052042663832 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Muthén LK, Muthén BO. Mplus: Statistical Analysis with Latent Variables: User’s Guide (Version 8). Los Angeles, CA: Authors; 2017. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Browne MW, Cudeck R. Alternative Ways of Assessing Model Fit. Sociol Methods Res. 1992;21(2):230–58. [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Bentler PM. Comparative fit indexes in structural models. Psychol Bull. 1990;107(2):238–46. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.107.2.238 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Hu LT, Bentler PM. Fit indices in covariance structure modeling: Sensitivity to underparameterized model misspecification. Psychol Methods. 1998;3(4):424–53. [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Fergusson DM, Horwood LJ, Lynskey MT. Structure of DSM-III-R Criteria for Disruptive Childhood Behaviors: Confirmatory Factor Models. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 1994. Oct 1;33(8):1145–57. doi: 10.1097/00004583-199410000-00010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Greenbaum PE, Dedrick RF. Hierarchical confirmatory factor analysis of the Child Behavior Checklist/4–18. Psychol Assess. 1998. Jun;10(2):149–55. [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Benjamini Y, Hochberg Y. Controlling the False Discovery Rate: A Practical and Powerful Approach to Multiple Testing. J R Stat Soc Ser B Methodol. 1995;57(1):289–300. [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Radua J, Albajes-Eizagirre A. FDR online calculator [Internet]. Seed-based d Mapping (formerly Signed Differential Mapping). [cited 2022 Nov 17]. https://www.sdmproject.com/utilities/?show=FDR
  • 74.Ram N, Grimm K. Using simple and complex growth models to articulate developmental change: Matching theory to method. Int J Behav Dev. 2007. Jul 1;31(4):303–16. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Bollen KA, Curran PJ. Latent curve models: a structural equation perspective. Hoboken, N.J: Wiley-Interscience; 2006. 285 p. (Wiley series in probability and statistics). [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Burt SA. Rethinking environmental contributions to child and adolescent psychopathology: A meta-analysis of shared environmental influences. Psychol Bull. 2009;135(4):608–37. doi: 10.1037/a0015702 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Burt SA. Are there meaningful etiological differences within antisocial behavior? Results of a meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Rev. 2009. Mar 1;29(2):163–78. doi: 10.1016/j.cpr.2008.12.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Van Dusen KT, Mednick SA, Gabrielli WFJ, Hutchings B. Social Class and Crime in an Adoption Cohort. J Crim Law Criminol. 1983;74:249. [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Tremblay RE, Nagin DS. The Developmental Origins of Physical Aggression in Humans. In: Developmental origins of aggression. New York, NY, US: The Guilford Press; 2005. p. 83–106. [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Stanger C, Achenbach TM, Verhulst FC. Accelerated longitudinal comparisons of aggressive versus delinquent syndromes. Dev Psychopathol. 1997. Mar;9(1):43–58. doi: 10.1017/s0954579497001053 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.McGue M, Keyes M, Sharma A, Elkins I, Legrand L, Johnson W, et al. The Environments of Adopted and Non-adopted Youth: Evidence on Range Restriction From the Sibling Interaction and Behavior Study (SIBS). Behav Genet. 2007. May 1;37(3):449–62. doi: 10.1007/s10519-007-9142-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Davis D, Jawad K, Feygin Y, Stevenson M, Wattles B, Porter J, et al. Measuring the impact of neighborhood disadvantage on child outcomes: All tools are not equal. In Salt Lake City, UT; 2023. [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Beyer L, Keen R, Ertel KA, Okuzono SS, Pintro K, Delaney S, et al. Comparing two measures of neighborhood quality and internalizing and externalizing behaviors in the adolescent brain cognitive development study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol [Internet]. 2024. Feb 2 [cited 2024 Feb 6]; Available from: doi: 10.1007/s00127-024-02614-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Burt SA, Klump KL, Gorman-Smith D, Neiderhiser JM. Neighborhood Disadvantage Alters the Origins of Children’s Nonaggressive Conduct Problems. Clin Psychol Sci. 2016. May 1;4(3):511–26. doi: 10.1177/2167702615618164 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Fulker DW, DeFries JC, Plomin R. Genetic influence on general mental ability increases between infancy and middle childhood. Nature. 1988. Dec;336(6201):767–9. doi: 10.1038/336767a0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.DeFries JC, Plomin R. Behavioral Genetics. Annu Rev Psychol. 1978;(29):473–515. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ps.29.020178.002353 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Hardy-Brown K, Plomin R, Greenhalgh J, Jax K. Selective Placement of Adopted Children: Prevalence and Effects. J Child Psychol Psychiatry. 1980;21(2):143–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1980.tb00026.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Blanchet T, Martínez-Toledano C. Wealth inequality dynamics in europe and the united states: Understanding the determinants. J Monet Econ. 2023. Jan 1;133:25–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 89.National Academies of Sciences E, Education D of B and SS and, Statistics C on N, Board on Children Y, Years C on B an A to R the N of C in P by H in 10, Menestrel SL, et al. Consequences of Child Poverty. In: A Roadmap to Reducing Child Poverty [Internet]. National Academies Press (US); 2019. [cited 2023 Jun 16]. https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/books/NBK547371/ [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Bárbara Oliván-Blázquez

26 Jan 2024

PONE-D-23-32462An examination of early socioeconomic status and neighborhood disadvantage as independent predictors of antisocial behavior: a longitudinal adoption studyPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Gresko,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

I have received the report of the reviewers. You can find below their coments. The manuscript needs a MAYOR REVISION.

I recommend that the authors carefully address the major and minor comments outlined above and provide a revised version of the manuscript. It would be helpful if the authors include a detailed response letter indicating how each comment has been addressed in the revised manuscript. Furthermore, to make sure that the manuscript adheres to the journal's guidelines and formatting requirements, please, check the “Instructions for the authors” document: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-financial-disclosure-statement

Please submit your revised manuscript by Mar 11 2024 11:59PM.  If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Bárbara Oliván-Blázquez, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

 Journal requirements: 1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf. 2. Note from Emily Chenette, Editor in Chief of PLOS ONE, and Iain Hrynaszkiewicz, Director of Open Research Solutions at PLOS: Did you know that depositing data in a repository is associated with up to a 25% citation advantage (https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230416)? If you’ve not already done so, consider depositing your raw data in a repository to ensure your work is read, appreciated and cited by the largest possible audience. You’ll also earn an Accessible Data icon on your published paper if you deposit your data in any participating repository (https://plos.org/open-science/open-data/#accessible-data). 3. We note that you have indicated that there are restrictions to data sharing for this study. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.  Before we proceed with your manuscript, please address the following prompts: a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially identifying or sensitive patient information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., a Research Ethics Committee or Institutional Review Board, etc.). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent. b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of recommended repositories, please seehttps://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/recommended-repositories. You also have the option of uploading the data as Supporting Information files, but we would recommend depositing data directly to a data repository if possible. We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide. 4. Please include your full ethics statement in the ‘Methods’ section of your manuscript file. In your statement, please include the full name of the IRB or ethics committee who approved or waived your study, as well as whether or not you obtained informed written or verbal consent. If consent was waived for your study, please include this information in your statement as well.  5. Please include a copy of Table S16 in Supporting Information which you refer to in your text on page 23.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This study shares important information about the potential impact of socioeconomic status on children's psychological health.

An extensive review of the topic has been carried out as a background, however, reviewing the bibliography used, I consider that it would be necessary to incorporate more current existing bibliography. It is true that it is a project started in 1975, but if the purpose is to give it scope and provide us with evidence of the present, it would be necessary to incorporate more recent information.

On the other hand, in Line 46, if "" is part of the objective of the study, I do not consider it so relevant to put this information at the beginning of the introduction without having given justifying arguments.

Methodologically, it is a study that uses very complex statistical analyses. However, I have doubts about this methodology:

--There is a lack of a contribution of ethical considerations in methodology, which specifies the ethical committee and how the data have been treated, which in this study precisely involving minors has to be very well argued.

-Line 160 indicates that they come from data from a longitudinal study. started in 1975 and recruited as indicated on line 171 until 1987.

Being a longitudinal study, it is not indicated if or when these people were recruited. Were they monitored during the recruitment period?

Reviewing the articles you cite (Plomin and DeFries (55) and Rhea et al. (49). ), they also do not specify when follow-up evaluations are done.

On the other hand, line 191 specifies that the data has been accessed in 2021.

I consider that it would be necessary to better explain what date the data presented in this manuscript corresponds to.

In measures, a description of the sample has been made in terms of the socioeconomic variables (From line 202 to 221, including table 1), of the Neighborhood disadvantage variables (Line 242-246). I consider this information provided to be not so relevant in methodology but rather as presentation and description of the sample at the beginning of the results section.

As in the introduction, an extensive search has been carried out in the discussion, but also outdated in terms of publication dates.

Reviewer #2: General Assessment:

The introduction effectively addresses various aspects related to Antisocial Behavior (ASB), focusing on potential environmental predictors such as parental socioeconomic status and neighborhood disadvantage. It explores the associations between socioeconomic status (SES), neighborhood disadvantage (ND), and ASB across different genders and types of antisocial behaviors. Additionally, the introduction highlights the use of adoption studies to differentiate between passive genetic influences (passive rGE) and environmental mediation in understanding ASB. The topic is highly relevant to the field, and the research question is well-defined. The study investigated the influence of early socioeconomic status (SES) and neighborhood disadvantage (ND) on antisocial behavior (ASB) using a longitudinal adoption design. The research, based on data from the Colorado Adoption Project, found that lower SES was a significant predictor of ASB in nonadoptive families, while ND did not show a significant association with ASB. The study did not provide conclusive evidence regarding the genetic versus environmental factors influencing the relationship between SES, ND, and ASB, as associations were not consistently significant across sex or adoptive status. However, in nonadopted individuals, lower SES was consistently associated with an increased risk of ASB, aligning with previous research findings.

First of all, to make sure that the manuscript adheres to the journal's guidelines and formatting requirements, please, check the “Instructions for the authors” document:

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-financial-disclosure-statement

Apart from that, please consider the below comments to strengthen the study as reported, which reflect my responses to general checklists:

Major Comments:

• Line 182. In terms of racial diversity, could you provide more information on the distribution within the five reported categories OR whether there were any notable differences in outcomes based on race?

• Line 200. The paragraph mentions that only paternal occupational status was examined due to limited mothers working outside the home at the study's initiation. Could you provide more context on this decision and its potential impact on the comprehensiveness of the SES assessment?

• Line 259. The decision to conduct confirmatory factor analyses (CFA) for examining aggression, delinquency, and overall Antisocial Behavior (ASB) is mentioned. Could you provide insights into the specific reasons or theoretical justifications for choosing CFA over exploratory factor analysis (EFA) in this study?

• Line 476. The mention of potential changes in SES and ND in later childhood or adolescence raises questions about the dynamics of these factors over time. Could you discuss how these potential changes might influence the study's results and interpretations, and suggest ways to address this limitation in future research?

Minor Comments:

• In line 260, page 13. There is a symbol that is not displayed well. Same in line 280, 299.

Overall, the manuscript has the potential to make a valuable contribution to the scientific community. With appropriate revisions, it could be considered suitable for publication in Plos One.

Recommendation:

I recommend that the authors carefully address the major and minor comments outlined above and provide a revised version of the manuscript. It would be helpful if the authors include a detailed response letter indicating how each comment has been addressed in the revised manuscript.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Fárima Méndez-López

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2024 Apr 29;19(4):e0301765. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0301765.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


29 Feb 2024

1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf.

We have ensured that our manuscript meets PLOS ONE’s style requirements, including those for file naming.

2. Note from Emily Chenette, Editor in Chief of PLOS ONE, and Iain Hrynaszkiewicz, Director of Open Research Solutions at PLOS: Did you know that depositing data in a repository is associated with up to a 25% citation advantage (https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0230416)? If you’ve not already done so, consider depositing your raw data in a repository to ensure your work is read, appreciated and cited by the largest possible audience. You’ll also earn an Accessible Data icon on your published paper if you deposit your data in any participating repository (https://plos.org/open-science/open-data/#accessible-data).

A portion of our data is currently available through the Harvard Dataverse at https://doi.org/doi:10.7910/DVN/BCDSEU (Plomin et al., 2022) and access can be requested by contacting the Manager of Operations for the Henry A. Murray Research Archive, Institute for Quantitative Social Sciences, 1737 Cambridge St, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA., mra@help.hmdc.harvard.edu. Researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data may apply for access to data not yet added to the Harvard Dataverse by contacting the corresponding author (shelley.gresko@colorado.edu) and the University of Colorado Boulder Institutional Review Board (irbadmin@colorado.edu).

3. We note that you have indicated that there are restrictions to data sharing for this study. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially identifying or sensitive patient information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., a Research Ethics Committee or Institutional Review Board, etc.). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

We have provided the following information on ethical restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set: “Given that the data contain potentially identifying and sensitive patient information, data cannot be shared publicly because of restrictions put in place by the University of Colorado Boulder Institutional Review Board. Unidentifiable patient data are in the process of being added to the Harvard Dataverse repository. A portion of this data is currently available through the Harvard Dataverse at https://doi.org/doi:10.7910/DVN/BCDSEU (Plomin et al., 2022) and access can be requested by contacting the Manager of Operations for the Henry A. Murray Research Archive, Institute for Quantitative Social Sciences, 1737 Cambridge St, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA., mra@help.hmdc.harvard.edu. Researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data may apply for access to data not yet added to the Harvard Dataverse by contacting the corresponding author (shelley.gresko@colorado.edu) and the University of Colorado Boulder Institutional Review Board (irbadmin@colorado.edu).”

Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (2022). Colorado Adoption Project, 1976-1989 Version V1) [longitudinal, field study, hereditary]. Harvard Dataverse. https://doi.org/doi:10.7910/DVN/BCDSEU

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

4. Please include your full ethics statement in the ‘Methods’ section of your manuscript file. In your statement, please include the full name of the IRB or ethics committee who approved or waived your study, as well as whether or not you obtained informed written or verbal consent. If consent was waived for your study, please include this information in your statement as well.

In our Methods section on page 9, lines 187-191, we include the following: “This project is approved by the University of Colorado Boulder Office of Research Integrity’s Institutional Review Board (protocol number 14-0421). Parents completed written consent and children aged seven and older provided written or verbal assent and children provided written consent at age 16. Only participants who provided consent for researchers to geocode first addresses were included in this study.

5. Please include a copy of Table S16 in Supporting Information which you refer to in your text on page 23.

Thank you for drawing attention to this discrepancy. The reference to Table S16 in Supporting Information was an error and has been deleted.

5. Review Comments to the Author

Reviewer #1: This study shares important information about the potential impact of socioeconomic status on children's psychological health.

An extensive review of the topic has been carried out as a background, however, reviewing the bibliography used, I consider that it would be necessary to incorporate more current existing bibliography. It is true that it is a project started in 1975, but if the purpose is to give it scope and provide us with evidence of the present, it would be necessary to incorporate more recent information.

We have added the following studies to include more contemporary references in the introduction section:

13. Peverill M, Dirks MA, Narvaja T, Herts KL, Comer JS, McLaughlin KA. Socioeconomic status and child psychopathology in the United States: A meta-analysis of population-based studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2021 Feb 1;83:101933.

14. Chang LY, Wang MY, Tsai PS. Neighborhood disadvantage and physical aggression in children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis of multilevel studies. Aggress Behav. 2016;42(5):441–54.

29. Choi JK, Kelley MS, Wang D. Neighborhood Characteristics, Maternal Parenting, and Health and Development of Children from Socioeconomically Disadvantaged Families. Am J Community Psychol. 2018;62(3–4):476–91.

33. Latham RM, Arseneault L, Alexandrescu B, Baldoza S, Carter A, Moffitt TE, et al. Violent experiences and neighbourhoods during adolescence: understanding and mitigating the association with mental health at the transition to adulthood in a longitudinal cohort study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2022 Dec 1;57(12):2379–91.

41. Burt SA. The Genetic, Environmental, and Cultural Forces Influencing Youth Antisocial Behavior Are Tightly Intertwined. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2022;18(1):155–78.

On the other hand, in Line 46, if "" is part of the objective of the study, I do not consider it so relevant to put this information at the beginning of the introduction without having given justifying arguments.

We have removed the following sentence “The present study aims to examine whether early childhood socioeconomic status measured in infancy and neighborhood disadvantage independently contribute to level and change of ASB and whether these influences are best explained by gene¬–environment correlation or environmental mediation, using a longitudinal adoption design.” in the first paragraph of the introduction per the reviewer’s suggestion.

Methodologically, it is a study that uses very complex statistical analyses. However, I have doubts about this methodology:

--There is a lack of a contribution of ethical considerations in methodology, which specifies the ethical committee and how the data have been treated, which in this study precisely involving minors has to be very well argued.

On page 9, lines 187-189, we clarify the following related to ethical considerations: “This project is approved by the University of Colorado Boulder Office of Research Integrity’s Institutional Review Board (protocol number 14-0421). Parents completed written consent and children aged seven and older provided written or verbal assent and children provided written consent at age 16.”

-Line 160 indicates that they come from data from a longitudinal study. started in 1975 and recruited as indicated on line 171 until 1987.

Being a longitudinal study, it is not indicated if or when these people were recruited. Were they monitored during the recruitment period?

Reviewing the articles you cite (Plomin and DeFries (55) and Rhea et al. (49). ), they also do not specify when follow-up evaluations are done.

On pages 8-9, lines 169-177, we clarify when parents and siblings were recruited and have added additional information: Prospective recruitment began on January 1st, 1976, and ended on September 30th, 1987. Biological mothers of adoptees were recruited from two large adoption agencies in Colorado. Their children were placed into their adoptive family homes within one year after birth. Social workers matched adoptees with adoptive families on non-proximity of location and similarity of height between adoptive and biological parents. No additional explicit selective placement practices were followed. Seventy-five percent of adoptive parents recruited for the study agreed to participate. Matched control families were recruited from local hospitals. Sibling enrollment and recruitment ended with the last longitudinally followed sibling.

On page 9, lines 177-180, we specify how often follow up evaluations were completed and have added information about age at assessment: “Biological and adoptive parents were assessed with a comprehensive battery of psychological measures when children were one year old or younger. Adoptees and nonadoptees were assessed approximately annually either through home or lab visits or telephone interviews, from age one year to 16 years.

On the other hand, line 191 specifies that the data has been accessed in 2021.

I consider that it would be necessary to better explain what date the data presented in this manuscript corresponds to.

On page 9, lines 193-196, we have clarified that the raw data collected prospectively beginning in 1976 and was accessed by the authors of the manuscript in 2021 and 2023: “Data were collected beginning in 1976. The socioeconomic status and antisocial behavior data were accessed by authors on April 27th, 2021, and the neighborhood disadvantage data were finalized and accessed on January 20th, 2023 (see Methods section for more details on the neighborhood disadvantage variable).”

In measures, a description of the sample has been made in terms of the socioeconomic variables (From line 202 to 221, including table 1), of the Neighborhood disadvantage variables (Line 242-246). I consider this information provided to be not so relevant in methodology but rather as presentation and description of the sample at the beginning of the results section.

We have moved the sample description for SES and ND variables into the results section, under the heading “Sample Description” (pages 14-15).

As in the introduction, an extensive search has been carried out in the discussion, but also outdated in terms of publication dates.

We have added the following studies to include more contemporary references in the discussion section:

13. Peverill M, Dirks MA, Narvaja T, Herts KL, Comer JS, McLaughlin KA. Socioeconomic status and child psychopathology in the United States: A meta-analysis of population-based studies. Clin Psychol Rev. 2021 Feb 1;83:101933.

14. Chang LY, Wang MY, Tsai PS. Neighborhood disadvantage and physical aggression in children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis of multilevel studies. Aggress Behav. 2016;42(5):441–54.

41. Burt SA. The Genetic, Environmental, and Cultural Forces Influencing Youth Antisocial Behavior Are Tightly Intertwined. Annu Rev Clin Psychol. 2022;18(1):155–78.

85. Burt SA, Klump KL, Gorman-Smith D, Neiderhiser JM. Neighborhood Disadvantage Alters the Origins of Children’s Nonaggressive Conduct Problems. Clin Psychol Sci. 2016 May 1;4(3):511–26.

Reviewer #2: General Assessment:

The introduction effectively addresses various aspects related to Antisocial Behavior (ASB), focusing on potential environmental predictors such as parental socioeconomic status and neighborhood disadvantage. It explores the associations between socioeconomic status (SES), neighborhood disadvantage (ND), and ASB across different genders and types of antisocial behaviors. Additionally, the introduction highlights the use of adoption studies to differentiate between passive genetic influences (passive rGE) and environmental mediation in understanding ASB. The topic is highly relevant to the field, and the research question is well-defined. The study investigated the influence of early socioeconomic status (SES) and neighborhood disadvantage (ND) on antisocial behavior (ASB) using a longitudinal adoption design. The research, based on data from the Colorado Adoption Project, found that lower SES was a significant predictor of ASB in nonadoptive families, while ND did not show a significant association with ASB. The study did not provide conclusive evidence regarding the genetic versus environmental factors influencing the relationship between SES, ND, and ASB, as associations were not consistently significant across sex or adoptive status. However, in nonadopted individuals, lower SES was consistently associated with an increased risk of ASB, aligning with previous research findings.

First of all, to make sure that the manuscript adheres to the journal's guidelines and formatting requirements, please, check the “Instructions for the authors” document:

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-financial-disclosure-statement

Apart from that, please consider the below comments to strengthen the study as reported, which reflect my responses to general checklists:

Major Comments:

• Line 182. In terms of racial diversity, could you provide more information on the distribution within the five reported categories OR whether there were any notable differences in outcomes based on race?

We have included more specific demographic information regarding percentage of participants who belonged to each race on page 9, lines 180-184:

“The sample is over 90% White and is demographically representative of the Denver Metropolitan area at the time of recruitment (55). Participants self-reported their race as one of five categories: Alaskan Native/American Indigenous (1.4%), Asian (4.7), Black (0.6%), White (91.6%), and more than one race (0.9%), or unknown or not reported (0.9%).”

Given that the number of non-White subjects is very small, we do not have the power to detect any significant differences in outcomes based on race.

• Line 200. The paragraph mentions that only paternal occupational status was examined due to limited mothers working outside the home at the study's initiation. Could you provide more context on this decision and its potential impact on the comprehensiveness of the SES assessment?

On page 10, lines 205-209, we have provided additional context regarding the decision to include only paternal occupational status: “Only paternal occupational status was examined; given that the adoption agencies required that one parent be an at-home parent and given the limited number of mothers working outside of the home when the study was initiated, including occupational ratings for mothers would not have meaningfully added to the assessment of familial SES during infancy.”

We have also included this in the limitations section on page 30, lines 492-495: “Second, only paternal occupational status was examined since the adoption agencies required one parent be an at-home parent and few mothers worked outside of the home when the study was initiated, but SES estimates may be low for families with mothers working outside of the home.”

• Line 259. The decision to conduct confirmatory factor analyses (

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

pone.0301765.s015.docx (37.1KB, docx)

Decision Letter 1

Bárbara Oliván-Blázquez

24 Mar 2024

An examination of early socioeconomic status and neighborhood disadvantage as independent predictors of antisocial behavior: a longitudinal adoption study

PONE-D-23-32462R1

Dear Dr. Gresko,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice will be generated when your article is formally accepted. Please note, if your institution has a publishing partnership with PLOS and your article meets the relevant criteria, all or part of your publication costs will be covered. Please make sure your user information is up-to-date by logging into Editorial Manager at Editorial Manager® and clicking the ‘Update My Information' link at the top of the page. If you have any questions relating to publication charges, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Bárbara Oliván-Blázquez, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Dear Shelley Gresko,

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been accepted for being published in Pthe journal Plos One.

The authors have the responded to the reviewers' comments, and the manuscript has the clarity, quality, and innovation to be published in Plos One.

congratulations.

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The author has addressed all the requested comments, allowing a better understanding of the manuscript for reading by any public interested in the topic.

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Fátima Méndez-López

Reviewer #2: Yes: Alejandra Aguilar-Latorre

**********

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Table. Frequencies for parent and teacher reported externalizing scales.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0301765.s001.docx (19.6KB, docx)
    S2 Table. Descriptive statistics for parent and teacher reported aggression and delinquency scales.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0301765.s002.docx (16.6KB, docx)
    S3 Table. Correlations between SES variables and ND in adoptees.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0301765.s003.docx (15.2KB, docx)
    S4 Table. Correlations between SES variables and ND in nonadoptees.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0301765.s004.docx (14.3KB, docx)
    S5 Table. Parent reported ASB slope, intercept, and correlations between slope and intercept.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0301765.s005.docx (14.8KB, docx)
    S6 Table. Correlations between parent reported ASB intercept and slope, ND, and biological and adoptive parent SES in adoptees.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0301765.s006.docx (15.2KB, docx)
    S7 Table. Correlations between parent reported ASB intercept, slope, and biological Parent SES in nonadoptees.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0301765.s007.docx (14.8KB, docx)
    S8 Table. ASB hierarchical factor regressed on adoptive and biological parent SES and ND in adoptees: Individuals with ND data only.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0301765.s008.docx (15.2KB, docx)
    S9 Table. ASB hierarchical factor regressed on biological parent SES and ND in nonadoptees: Individuals with ND data only (N = 365).

    (DOCX)

    pone.0301765.s009.docx (14.7KB, docx)
    S10 Table. Parent reported ASB slope, intercept, and correlations between slope and intercept: Individuals with ND data only (N = 428).

    (DOCX)

    pone.0301765.s010.docx (14.9KB, docx)
    S11 Table. Parent reported ASB intercept regressed on biological and adoptive parent SES and ND in adoptees: Individuals with ND data only.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0301765.s011.docx (15.3KB, docx)
    S12 Table. Parent reported ASB slope regressed on biological and adoptive parent SES and ND in adoptees: Individuals with ND data only.

    (DOCX)

    pone.0301765.s012.docx (15.3KB, docx)
    S13 Table. Parent reported ASB intercept regressed on biological parent SES and ND in nonadoptees: Individuals with ND data only (N = 365).

    (DOCX)

    pone.0301765.s013.docx (14.8KB, docx)
    S14 Table. Parent reported ASB slope regressed on biological parent SES and ND in nonadoptees: Individuals with ND data only (N = 365).

    (DOCX)

    pone.0301765.s014.docx (14.8KB, docx)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    pone.0301765.s015.docx (37.1KB, docx)

    Data Availability Statement

    Given that the data contain potentially identifying and sensitive patient information, data cannot be shared publicly because of restrictions put in place by the University of Colorado Boulder Institutional Review Board. Unidentifiable patient data are in the process of being added to the Harvard Dataverse repository. A portion of this data is currently available through the Harvard Dataverse at https://doi.org/doi:10.7910/DVN/BCDSEU (Plomin et al., 2022) and access can be requested by contacting the Manager of Operations for the Henry A. Murray Research Archive, Institute for Quantitative Social Sciences, 1737 Cambridge St, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA., mra@help.hmdc.harvard.edu. Researchers who meet the criteria for access to confidential data may apply for access to data not yet added to the Harvard Dataverse by contacting the corresponding author (shelley.gresko@colorado.edu) and the University of Colorado Boulder Institutional Review Board (irbadmin@colorado.edu). Plomin, R., DeFries, J. C., & Fulker, D. W. (2022). Colorado Adoption Project, 1976-1989 Version V1) [longitudinal, field study, hereditary]. Harvard Dataverse. https://doi.org/doi:10.7910/DVN/BCDSEU.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES