Skip to main content
BJA: British Journal of Anaesthesia logoLink to BJA: British Journal of Anaesthesia
. 2024 Feb 9;132(5):911–917. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2023.12.033

Effects of colour-coded compartmentalised syringe trays on anaesthetic drug error detection under cognitive load

Victoria Laxton 1, Frances A Maratos 2, David W Hewson 3,4, Andrew Baird 2, Stephanie Archer 5,6,7, Edward JN Stupple 2,
PMCID: PMC11103169  PMID: 38336517

Abstract

Background

Anaesthetic drug administration is complex, and typical clinical environments can entail significant cognitive load. Colour-coded anaesthetic drug trays have shown promising results for error identification and reducing cognitive load.

Methods

We used experimental psychology methods to test the potential benefits of colour-coded compartmentalised trays compared with conventional trays in a simulated visual search task. Effects of cognitive load were also explored through an accompanying working memory-based task. We hypothesised that colour-coded compartmentalised trays would improve drug-detection error, reduce search time, and reduce cognitive load. This comprised a cognitive load memory task presented alongside a visual search task to detect drug errors.

Results

All 53 participants completed 36 trials, which were counterbalanced across the two tray types and 18 different vignettes. There were 16 error-present and 20 error-absent trials, with 18 trials presented for each preloaded tray type. Syringe errors were detected more often in the colour-coded trays than in the conventional trays (91% vs 83%, respectively; P=0.006). In signal detection analysis, colour-coded trays resulted in more sensitivity to the error signal (2.28 vs 1.50, respectively; P<0.001). Confidence in response accuracy correlated more strongly with task performance for the colour-coded tray condition, indicating improved metacognitive sensitivity to task performance (r=0.696 vs r=0.447).

Conclusions

Colour coding and compartmentalisation enhanced visual search efficacy of drug trays. This is further evidence that introducing standardised colour-coded trays into operating theatres and procedural suites would add an additional layer of safety for anaesthetic procedures.

Keywords: cognitive load, colour coding, injectable, medication error, syringe trays, visual search


Editor's key points.

  • Medication errors are common in anaesthesia such that systems approaches to mitigation can have significant impact on patient safety.

  • Colour-coded anaesthetic drug trays could increase error identification and reduce cognitive load.

  • This simulation study in volunteer anaesthesia practitioners examined the impact of colour-coded drug compartment trays on drug-detection error and search time under cognitive load.

  • Colour coding and compartmentalisation enhanced visual search efficacy of drug trays, which could add an additional layer of safety in anaesthesia procedures.

Drug administration during anaesthesia is complex and commonly occurs concurrent with fatigue, stress, and high cognitive load. Anaesthetists frequently work in noisy, cluttered, and time-pressured environments. Multitasking (e.g. conversations during drug preparation) disrupts working memory,1, 2, 3, 4 deflects attention from the primary task, and results in action slips.5, 6, 7

Drug errors have been reported to occur in at least one in 133 procedures,8,9 with syringe swaps (a syringe mistakenly administered in place of another10,11) accounting for 81% of mistakes.12 As these errors often involve drugs with powerful cardiorespiratory and neuromuscular effects,13,14 mitigation strategies could improve perioperative patient safety. Physical grouping of information (compartments) and use of visual cues (colour coding) can reduce cognitive load,15, 16, 17 and has been applied to support airway management.18 An eye-tracking study showed the benefits of a colour-coded tray with standardised drug locations, improving visual search efficacy and enabling rapid error identification compared with conventional trays.19

Situational awareness is essential for task performance.20 Given that perceptual errors are common, and errors of situational comprehension can follow from perceptual errors, simplifying perceptual tasks to ameliorate information processing is desirable in safety critical environments.20 If colour-coded compartmentalised trays enable faster visual search while preserving or enhancing error detection, this would facilitate perceptual situational awareness. Simplifying visual search might also enable the wider theatre team to identify drug and syringe swap errors.

The aim of this study was to test the visual search efficacy of colour-coded compartmentalised trays to reduce anaesthetic drug administration errors compared with conventional trays under cognitive load. This was manipulated through a visuospatial working memory task in an online experiment. The experiment also tested whether the colour-coded trays influenced the relationship between participant confidence in responding and their response accuracy.21 We hypothesised that colour-coded trays elicit more accurate and faster error detection than conventional trays. Effects of colour-coded trays on confidence in responses were also tested.

Methods

This study received ethical approval from the University of Derby (Reference ETH2021-4512; granted July 30, 2021) and Health Research Authority approval (Reference 21/HRA/1087; granted May 21, 2021). The experiment was accessed via weblink hosted by Psychopy Pavlovia (Psychopy, Nottingham, UK). Consent was obtained from all participants using an online form.

Participants

We recruited clinical anaesthetists and operating department practitioners (ODPs) in any stage of training or practice through the National Institute for Health and Care Research (NIHR) Clinical Research Network (CRN) supported by the East Midlands NIHR CRN. Further participants were recruited using targeted online adverts (e.g. Twitter/X and LinkedIn). All participants had experience in the operating theatre and anaesthetic environment, and were aware of the specific drugs used in anaesthetic procedures and typical errors that might occur.

G-Power analyses22 revealed a minimum of 47 participants were required to detect a medium effect size (d=0.6) for drug detection errors (error-present vs error-absent) as a function of tray type (colour-coded vs conventional) with a power of 0.8, β=0.95 and α=0.05.

Cognitive load error detection task

This was a 2×2 repeated-measures experimental design with tray type (colour-coded vs conventional trays) and drug error (error-present vs error-absent) as the independent variables. There were 16 error-present trials, eight for each tray condition (colour-coded, conventional). Errors included missing syringes, additional drugs, allergy risks, and syringes placed in an incorrect compartment/tray. All errors were matched across the tray conditions. There were also 20 error-absent trials, 10 for each tray condition. Trials were presented in a random order.

The trial process flow is displayed in Figure 1. Each trial included a different clinical vignette (designed by DH) describing routine clinical situations an anaesthetist would expect to encounter: for example, an Achilles tendon repair for a 37-yr-old female, in general good health, healthy weight range, and who has known penicillin allergy. Vignettes were accompanied by a trial-specific list of perioperative drugs: for example, propofol, atracurium, fentanyl, ondansetron, teicoplanin, neostigmine; and emergency drugs: atropine, ephedrine, metaraminol. An image of a loaded tray was then presented, and participants were required to conduct a visual search and indicate whether there was a drug error present with a mouse click (in the present example, co-amoxiclav was present instead of teicoplanin). There were 16 different vignettes for error-present trials. These were split into two groups of eight and counterbalanced between participants, such that half the participants completed error-present trials from Group 1 and the other half completed error-present trials from Group 2. There were 10 vignettes for error-absent trials, which were presented to all participants once for each tray type. Thus, all participants saw 18 different vignettes, which were counterbalanced across the two trays (36 trials in total).

Fig 1.

Fig 1

Schematic display of the experimental protocol demonstrating the wrap around cognitive load task [1,5]; vignette [2]; error detection task for the colour-coded tray [3]; and confidence rating [4]. The text in panel 3 provides a reminder of the relevant drugs list. The text in panel 5 states "Please enter the order in which you think the dots appeared using the corresponding arrow keys".

To increase difficulty for the primary drug search task, a working memory-based cognitive load task was presented to all participants. This was used because in theatre settings visual search entails high cognitive demand and multitasking. The working memory task was presented in a wrap-around design, where participants were first asked to learn a sequence presented prior to the vignette and visual search task and then asked to recall the memory sequence after the search task and a confidence rating task (described below). The memory task increased the cognitive demand on the participants by requiring working memory capacity and therefore, to some extent, mimics the demands an anaesthetist might face when preparing and administering drugs.16,17

Outcome measures

Accuracy, response times, and participant confidence scores for the error detection task were recorded, and accuracy scores were recorded for the cognitive load task. Confidence scores were used to calculate metacognitive sensitivity: the correlation between trial accuracy and trial confidence. Trials were marked correct if a drug error was correctly identified on error trials, and no drug error correctly identified on error-absent trials. The cognitive load task was given a recall accuracy score between 0 and 4 for each trial.

Error detection under cognitive load task

The cognitive load task consisted of four black circles appearing sequentially in one of four cardinal locations randomised across all trials. Each circle was presented for 1 s. Once the sequence of four cardinal locations was complete, the screen moved automatically to display a surgical scenario and drugs list vignette. To allow the participant time to read at their own speed, this vignette screen was moved on by pressing the spacebar. The clinical vignette described a surgical procedure including key patient characteristics and a drugs list. Surgery type, patient characteristics, and error type were counterbalanced.

The next phase was the presentation of the drug tray image where participants were required to detect if an error was present, again triggered with a press of the spacebar to move from the vignette to the error detection task. Here, an image of either a conventional single-compartmented, grey, multipurpose paper mulch tray or loaded colour-coded tray was presented with the drugs list (Fig. 2). The colour-coded tray was organised by drug class matching the ISO26825:2020 international colour-coded labelling system (Rainbow Tray™; Uvamed, Loughborough, UK). For all trials, the relevant drugs list was presented at the top of the screen. Participants pressed the keyboard left arrow key if an error was present or the keyboard right arrow key if there was no error.

Fig 2.

Fig 2

Snapshot of (a) the colour-coded tray and (b) the conventional tray.

The experiment progressed when a participant error-present or error-absent response was given. Participants then provided a confidence score, from 1 (not very confident) to 5 (very confident), for their error detection response. The cognitive load recall task was then presented, this was four circles and arrows corresponding to the direction of the circle presented. Here the participant used the arrow keys to recall the exact sequence order the circles had appeared in (left, right, down, up). Once four responses had been recorded, the trial ended.

Between each trial, a rest screen was presented to participants. This was manually moved on with the spacebar. Thus, there were 36 different trials for each participant.

Drug scenario images were taken on a Canon EOS camera, with 35 mm focal length, positioned on a tripod above the drug tray on a medical trolly.

The study was created using Psychopy (Nottingham, UK) and hosted on their online platform Pavlovia (Open Science Tools Limited, https://pavlovia.org/docs/home/about).

Procedure

Participants were sent a hyperlink to the online experiment. This opened a Qualtrics survey, where participants could read the study information sheet, sign an electronic consent form, and complete demographic questions. Participants were then presented with the experiment instructions and a link to the experiment on the online platform Pavlovia.

Statistical analysis plan

Trials with correct responses were subjected to a tray (colour-coded vs conventional) × drug-error presence (error-present vs error-absent) repeated-measures analysis of variance (anova). A log transformation corrected skewness in response times. Log transformed times for correct responses were analysed with a 2×2 repeated-measures anova.

Metacognitive sensitivity was calculated from accuracy of responses and confidence scores. This involved subtracting the mean confidence score from the mean accuracy rate. Negative scores indicate under-confidence, and positive scores indicate overconfidence.23 Metacognitive sensitivity scores were entered into a tray (colour-coded vs conventional) and drug-error (error-present vs error-absent) repeated-measures anova. The relationship between confidence ratings and response accuracy for each condition were tested with Pearson's correlation.

The design also allowed for signal detection measures. These included d′ (a measure of sensitivity to the error/signal) and c (the criterion bias to say ‘yes’ regardless of the drug error condition), as measures of drug error detection performance. A hit was recorded when participants correctly identified an error. A false alarm was recorded when participants identified an error on an error-absent trial. Signal detection analysis was conducted using the z-scores of hits (zHits) and the z-scores of False Alarms (zFalse Alarms). By comparing the values of zHits – zFalse Alarms for each tray type, sensitivity to the error signal was tested. The response bias towards indicating that there was an error irrespective of the stimuli was tested using the values derived from (zHits + zFalse Alarms)/2 for each tray type.

Results

Participants

There were 53 complete responses. Of these (mean [range] age: 40 [23–61] yr, 22 female), 32 were consultant anaesthetists (11 female, mean experience 16.7 [7.4] yr), six were trainee anaesthetists (three female, mean experience 2.8 [1.5] yr), four were ODPs (four female, mean experience 18.9 [7.9] yr), and 11 were trainee ODPs (six female, mean experience 1.1 [1.6] yr). One participant's data were more than three standard deviations below the mean; as an outlier their data were removed.

Drug error detection task

Accuracy

Trials were explored in a tray (colour-coded vs conventional) × drug-error (error-present vs error-absent) repeated-measures anova. The main effect of tray was significant (F[51]=8.17, MSe=0.013, P=0.006, ηp2 =0.14). Participants detected errors more often in colour-coded trays than in conventional trays. There was no significant effect of drug error (error vs no error) (F[51]=2.91, MSe=0.050, P=0.094, ηp2 =0.05). There was a significant interaction between tray type and drug-error (F[51]=4.38, MSe=0.010, P=0.041, ηp2 =0.08) (Fig. 3). Post hoc Bonferroni corrected t-tests (p<0.0125) revealed that error-absent trials had more accurate response rates in colour-coded trays compared with conventional trays (t[51]=4.12, P<0.001, Cohen's d=0.57). Responses in error-absent trials were not reliably more accurate than error-present trials in colour-coded trays (t[51]=–2.44, P=0.018, Cohen's d=0.36) (Table 1). There was also no difference between colour-coded trays and conventional trays for error-present trials (P=0.65), or between error-present and error-absent trials for conventional trays (P=0.78).

Fig 3.

Fig 3

Mean percentage of correct responses.

Table 1.

Mean percentage correct response and response times as a function of tray type.

Error type Tray type
Colour-coded tray correct response,% (sd) Conventional tray correct response,% (sd) Colour-coded tray response time, s (sd) Conventional tray response time, s (sd)
Error-present 83.0 (22.4) 81.3 (20.5) 12.1 (5.0) 12.0 (5.7)
Error-absent 91.1 (10.5) 83.8 (11.7) 17.4 (7.8) 18.7 (9.2)
Total mean 87.0 (18.0) 82.5 (16.7) 14.7 (7.0) 15.3 (8.3)

Response times

Trials were explored in a tray (colour-coded vs conventional) × drug-error (error-present vs error-absent) repeated-measures anova (Fig. 4). The main effect of tray on response times was not significant (F[51]=0 .015, MSe=0.010, P=903, ηp2 <0.01). Correct responses to error-present trials were faster than correct responses to error-absent trials (F[51]=78.56, MSe=0.028, P<0.001, ηp2 =0.61). There was also a tray by error interaction (F[51]=6.94, MSe=0.006, P=0.011, ηp2 =0.12), driven by faster responses to error-absent trials in the colour-coded tray condition compared with the conventional tray (Table 1), although comparisons did not pass the Bonferroni threshold (p<0.0125) for significance (t[51]=–2.131, p=0.038, Cohen's d=0.32). There were, however, significant differences between error-present and error-absent trials in colour-coded trays (t[51]=–6.21, P<0.001, Cohen's d=–0.85) and between the two error categories in the conventional tray (t[51]=–7.50, P<0.001, Cohen's d=–0.77). The difference for error-present trials between colour-coded trays and conventional trays was not significant (P=0.67).

Fig 4.

Fig 4

Mean response time in seconds for correctly identified trials.

Confidence ratings

The main effect of tray on confidence rating was not significant (F[51]=2.83, MSe=127.57, P=0.098, ηp2 =0.05). However, the main effect of drug-error presence (F[51]=10.77, MSe=504.50, P=0.002, ηp2 =0.17) demonstrated that participants were under-confident when responding to error-present trials across both tray types (–9.76 [sd 17.34]) compared with error-absent trials (0.458 [sd 16.73]). The interaction between tray and error presence was not significant (F[51]=1.91, MSe=114.45, P=0.173, ηp2 =0.04).

There was a strong positive relationship between accuracy of responses and confidence scores for colour-coded trays (r[52]=0.696, P<0.001) and a moderate positive relationship for conventional trays (r[52]=0.447, P<0.001). This indicated a stronger correlation between confidence and task performance for colour-coded compartmentalised trays, indicative of good metacognitive sensitivity and accurate perception of performance.

Signal detection analysis

Signal detection analysis tests participants' ability to detect errors and avoid response bias (to check whether participants responded that there was an error present irrespective of trial content). There was a significant difference between d’ scores (a measure of sensitivity to the signal; zHits – zFalse Alarms) of the two trays, with better sensitivity to the error signal for colour-coded trays (2.28 [sd 0.90]) compared with conventional trays (1.50 [sd 1.15]) (t[51]=4.80, P<0.001, Cohen's d=0.67). There was also a significant difference between c ratings (the criterion bias to say ‘yes’ regardless of the information); (zHits + zFalse Alarms)/2), with colour-coded trays (–1.68 [sd 0.61]) scoring lower than conventional trays (–1.25 [sd 0.55]) (t[51]=–4.38, P<0.001, Cohen's d=0.61), indicating reduced response bias for colour-coded trays.

Discussion

We compared visual search efficacy for colour-coded trays and conventional trays under cognitive load. Colour-coded trays were superior for the accuracy of visual search of loaded drug trays, and elicited more accurate responses to drug errors than conventional trays. Signal detection measures indicated more sensitivity to signals in colour-coded trays and a reduced criterion to respond that there was an error irrespective of presented information.

The superiority of responses using colour-coded trays was consistent with other visual search research where colour coding and standardisation improve outcomes.24,25 Our findings support recent national recommendations made on the handling of injectable medicines that colour-coded trays are a short-term technological solution to improve medicines safety.26 The results support existing research into colour-coded templates and trays organised by colour and location, indicating that colour coding can facilitate performance.27, 28, 29, 30 The trays used in previous studies have shown advantages for mitigating syringe-type drug errors. With improved sensitivity to errors in colour-coded compartmentalised trays, introducing these trays into anaesthetising locations could mitigate some drug-related errors in anaesthetic drug administration (e.g. syringe swaps or misidentifications).

The safety benefits arising from colour coding of drug trays is consistent with the literature on colour-coded labelling of individual drug syringes. Correct recognition of colour-labelled drug syringes is faster than when monochrome labels or drug ampules are used.31 Similarly, increasing colour-related cues during drug administration as a component of a systems approach to improving medication handling has been shown in pragmatic clinical trials to reduce drug-swap errors.32

Our study also demonstrates that colour-coded trays helped participants show greater metacognitive sensitivity, such that they were more confident about their correct responses than their errors. Colour acts as a guide during visual search, with early visual features (e.g. colour) guiding visual attention,33,34 but relies on standardised colour coding by manufacturers. These findings further support standardised colour coding for vial tops to mitigate medication error.35,36

The search benefit identified in this study is primarily driven by responses to error-absent trials. Error-absent trials for conventional trays were identified more slowly and less often, with more ‘false-positive’ responses (reporting an error when there was none). In visual search, trials without a target can be problematic. The decision to terminate a search when a target has been found is clear, whereas the decision to terminate a search when a target is absent is more ambiguous.37,38 Ending a search early can result in targets being missed, whereas prolonging search for non-existent errors can create issues with speed and efficiency.39,40

Challenges and future directions

To ensure experimental controls and a fair comparison, syringes in the conventional trays were neatly laid out side-by-side within the tray, with all labels visible, a more favourable scenario than would typically occur in the real world. It is possible that positive results for colour-coded trays might have been underestimated because of the artificial neatness of the syringe presentation in conventional trays. It should be noted that this was a screen-based task rather than a clinical task. The findings compliment those of an eye-tracking experiment testing colour-coded trays,19 and corroborate subjective reports from anaesthetists about the same tray design.30 Future research should test a more realistic layout of syringes in conventional trays compared with colour-coded trays to gain an understanding of the effect of tray organisation. This could be achieved in an eye-tracking study during simulated anaesthetic environments with anaesthetists using different tray types.

Conclusions

Our results in a simulated environment favoured colour-coded trays, which offered search advantages over conventional trays in error detection and identification of correctly loaded trays. This supports previous findings that colour-coded trays improve visual search efficiency, providing further evidence that introducing colour-coded trays can provide safety mitigations in anaesthesia.

Authors’ contributions

Study conception and design: all authors

Development of materials for the study: EJNS, VL, FAM, DWH

Data collection and analysis: VL

Writing of the manuscript: all authors

All authors revised the manuscript for significant intellectual content, gave approval for the version to be published, and agreed to be accountable for the work in ensuring that questions related to the accuracy or integrity of any part of the work are appropriately investigated and resolved.

Acknowledgements

With thanks to Ann Bates, Amy Cauldwell, University of Derby ODP students, Sophie Rutherford, John Shacklock, and the East Midlands Clinical Research Network.

Declarations of interest

DWH accepts fees for advising in civil, criminal, and coronial medicolegal cases and is a member of the associate editorial board of the British Journal of Anaesthesia. The remaining authors declare that they have no competing interests. None of the authors have any financial interest in sales of the company selling the trays (Uvamed, Leicester, UK), which provided free samples of the trays during the design of the study. Rainbow Trays™ are available commercially and were provided by the company using grant funding. The funder and Uvamed had no involvement in the design, conduct, analysis, or interpretation of this study.

Funding

Innovate UK by a grant (no. 44612) shared between University of Derby and Uvamed, UK.

Handling Editor: Hugh C Hemmings Jr

Footnotes

This article is accompanied by an editorial: Psychology in the operating theatre: the importance of colour and cognition in the redesign of clinical systems for medication safety by Craig S. Webster, Br J Anaesth 2024:132: 837–839, doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2024.02.003

References

  • 1.van Harten A., Gooszen H.G., Koksma J.J., Niessen T.J.H., Abma T.A. An observational study of distractions in the operating theatre. Anaesthesia. 2021;76:346–356. doi: 10.1111/anae.15217. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Chun M.M. Visual working memory as visual attention sustained internally over time. Neuropsychologia. 2011;49:1407–1409. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2011.01.029. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Fougnie D., Marois R. Attentive tracking disrupts feature binding in visual working memory. Vis Cogn. 2009;17:48–66. doi: 10.1080/13506280802281337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Smyth M.M., Scholey K.A. Interference in immediate spatial memory. Mem Cognit. 1994;22:1–13. doi: 10.3758/bf03202756. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Heckhausen H., Beckmann J. Intentional action and action slips. Psychol Rev. 1990;97:36. [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Mylopoulos M. Oops! I did it again: the psychology of everyday action slips. Top Cogn Sci. 2022;14:282–294. doi: 10.1111/tops.12552. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Reason J. Little slips and, big disasters. Interdis Sci Rev. 1984;9:179–189. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Webster C.S., Merry A.F., Larsson L., McGrath K.A., Weller J. The frequency and nature of drug administration error during anaesthesia. Anaesth Intensive Care. 2001;29:494–500. doi: 10.1177/0310057X0102900508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bratch R., Pandit J.J. An integrative review of method types used in the study of medication error during anaesthesia: implications for estimating incidence. Br J Anaesth. 2021;127:458–469. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2021.05.023. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Charuluxananan S., Sriraj W., Lapisatepun W., Kusumaphanyo C., Ittichaikulthol W., Suratsunya T. Drug errors from the Thai anesthesia incidents monitoring study: analysis of 1,996 incident reports. Asian Biomed. 2012;6:541–547. [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hew C.M., Cyna A.M., Simmons S.W. Avoiding inadvertent epidural injection of drugs intended for non-epidural use. Anaesth Intensive Care. 2003;31:44–49. doi: 10.1177/0310057X0303100108. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Currie M., Mackay P., Morgan C., et al. The Australian Incident Monitoring Study. The “wrong drug” problem in anaesthesia: an analysis of 2000 incident reports. Anaesth Intensive Care. 1993;21:596–601. doi: 10.1177/0310057X9302100517. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Gordon P.C., Llewellyn R.L., James M.F.M. Drug administration errors by South African anaesthetists-a survey. S Afr Med J. 2006;96:630–632. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Khan F.A., Hoda M.Q. Drug related critical incidents. Anaesthesia. 2005;60:48–52. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2044.2004.04014.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Lee Y.C., Lee J.D., Ng Boyle L. Visual attention in driving: the effects of cognitive load and visual disruption. Hum Factors. 2007;49:721–733. doi: 10.1518/001872007X215791. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Oh S.H., Kim M.S. The role of spatial working memory in visual search efficiency. Psychon Bull Rev. 2004;11:275–281. doi: 10.3758/bf03196570. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Young J.Q., Van Merrienboer J., Durning S., Ten Cate O. Cognitive load theory: implications for medical education: AMEE Guide No. 86. Med Teach. 2014;36:371–384. doi: 10.3109/0142159X.2014.889290. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Fuchs A., Haller M., Riva T., Nabecker S., Greif R., Berger-Estilita J. Translation and application of guidelines into clinical practice: a colour-coded difficult airway trolley. Trends Anaesth Crit Care. 2021;40:46–54. [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Laxton V., Maratos F.A., Hewson D.W., Baird A., Stupple E.J.N. Standardised colour-coded compartmentalised syringe trays improve anaesthetic medication visual search and mitigate cognitive load. Br J Anaesth. 2023;130:343–350. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2022.11.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Stanton N.A., Chambers P.R., Piggott J. Situational awareness and safety. Saf Sci. 2001;39:189–204. [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rahnev D. Visual metacognition: measures, models, and neural correlates. Am Psychol. 2021;76:1445–1453. doi: 10.1037/amp0000937. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Faul F., Erdfelder E., Lang A.G., Buchner A. G∗ Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behav Res Methods. 2007;39:175–191. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Metcalfe J., Shimamura A.P. MIT Press; Cambridge, MA: 1996. Metacognition: knowing about knowing. [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Fasting S., Gisvold S.E. Adverse drug errors in anesthesia, and the impact of coloured syringe labels. Can J Anaesth. 2000;47:1060–1067. doi: 10.1007/BF03027956. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Shannon J., O’Riain S. Introduction of “international syringe labelling” in the Republic of Ireland. Ir J Med Sci. 2009;178:291–296. doi: 10.1007/s11845-009-0342-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Kinsella S.M., Boaden B., El-Ghazali S., et al. Handling injectable medications in anaesthesia: guidelines from the Association of Anaesthetists. Anaesthesia. 2023;78:1285–1294. doi: 10.1111/anae.16095. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Maximous R., Wong J., Chung F., Abrishami A. Interventions to reduce medication errors in anesthesia: a systematic review. Can J Anaesth. 2021;68:880–893. doi: 10.1007/s12630-021-01959-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Grigg E.B., Martin L.D., Ross F.J., et al. Assessing the impact of the anesthesia medication template on medication errors during anesthesia: a prospective study. Anesth Analg. 2017;124:1617–1625. doi: 10.1213/ANE.0000000000001823. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Roesler A., Grigg E.B., Martin L.D., et al. Practice-centred design of a medication template to reduce medication handling errors in the operating room. Int J Des. 2019;13:53–68. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Almghairbi D.S., Sharp L., Griffiths R., Evley R., Gupta S., Moppett I.K. An observational feasibility study of new anaesthesia drug storage tray. Anaesthesia. 2018;73:356–364. doi: 10.1111/anae.14187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Cheeseman J.F., Webster C.S., Pawley M.D., Francis M.A., Warman G.R., Merry A.F. Use of a new task-relevant test to assess the effects of shift work and drug labelling formats on anesthesia trainees’ drug recognition and confirmation. Can J Anaesth. 2011;1:38–47. doi: 10.1007/s12630-010-9404-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Webster C.S., Larsson L., Frampton C.M., et al. Clinical assessment of a new anaesthetic drug administration system: a prospective, controlled, longitudinal incident monitoring study. Anaesthesia. 2010;65:490–499. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2044.2010.06325.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Wolfe J.M., Horowitz T.S. What attributes guide the deployment of visual attention and how do they do it? Nat Rev Neurosci. 2004;5:495–501. doi: 10.1038/nrn1411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Bauer B., McFadden S. Linear separability and redundant colour coding in visual search displays. Displays. 1997;18:21–28. [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Ankam A., Madhabushi P., Hsu M. Standardising drug vial top colours to minimise medication errors in anaesthesia. Br J Anaesth. 2022;128:e338–e349. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2022.02.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Skelly J.R., O'Reilly D., Ward V. Non-standardised colour coding of anaesthetic ampoule labelling: a dangerous practice? Br J Anaesth. 2021;128:e10–e12. doi: 10.1016/j.bja.2021.09.030. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Treisman A., Gelade G. A feature-integration theory of attention. Cogn Psychol. 1980;12:97–136. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Wolfe J.M. In: Gray W., editor. Oxford; New York: 2007. Guided search 4.0: current progress with a model of visual search; pp. 99–119. (Integrated Models of Cognitive Systems). [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wolfe J.M. When do I quit? The search termination problem in visual search. Nebr Symp Motiv. 2012;59:183–208. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-4794-8_8. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Wolfe J.M., Horowitz T.S., Kenner N.M. Rare items often missed in visual searches. Nature. 2005;435:439–440. doi: 10.1038/435439a. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from BJA: British Journal of Anaesthesia are provided here courtesy of Elsevier

RESOURCES