Skip to main content
Continuity in Education logoLink to Continuity in Education
. 2020 Apr 27;1(1):50–63. doi: 10.5334/cie.5

Adolescents With Chronic Medical Conditions and High School Completion: The Importance of Perceived School Belonging

Kathryn M Kirkpatrick 1
PMCID: PMC11104302  PMID: 38774529

Abstract

Students with chronic medical conditions often experience barriers to academic progress, including impact of disease and treatment, increased school absence, and altered expectations of teachers and parents. School belonging is an important element of academic success and can be promoted by positive relationships, structure, and support in the school environment. One aim of this study was to explore group differences in perceived school belonging and rate of on-time high school completion for students with chronic medical conditions as compared to their healthy peers. The second goal was to analyze relations between belonging, health status, and on-time completion of high school. Restricted data from Add Health was used to answer the study questions.

Results showed that students with chronic medical conditions reported lower levels of perceived school belonging than their healthy peers (t(1056) = 3.69, p < .001, d = 0.23). Students with chronic medical conditions also attained lower levels of on-time high school graduation than their healthy peers (t(1056) = 2.60, p = .005, d = 0.16). Perceived school belonging had a different impact for students with chronic medical conditions than for those who had no health concerns. Each unit increase in belonging for students with health impairment was related to a 63% increased likelihood of on-time high school graduation (OR = 1.629, p = .003). School belonging is especially important for students living with chronic medical conditions. Finding ways to facilitate a stronger sense of school belonging may be a way to support desired academic outcomes.

Keywords: adolescents, school belonging, chronic medical conditions, high school completion


As many as 20% of the school-age youth in the United States live with a chronic medical condition and about one third of those students experience sequelae severe enough to interfere with school participation on a regular basis (Kaffenberger, 2006; Shaw, Glaser, Stern, Sferdenschi, & McCabe, 2010; Thies, 1999). These numbers mean that, across their careers, any classroom teacher or school administrator is likely to encounter multiple children managing a chronic medical condition, generally defined as a “medical condition, lasting for more than 3 months, which requires medical attention and interferes with a person’s daily living” (Barraclough & Machek, 2010, p. 132). Chronic medical conditions impact both physical and psychological functioning for a child in domains that include family, school, and peer relationships (Brown & Anderson, 1999; Power, 2006; Shaw et al., 2010).

Some researchers have suggested the use of a non-categorical approach when studying the social or educational impact of illness for young people (Perrin et al., 1993; Stein, Bauman, Westbrook, Coupey, & Ireys, 1993), meaning that all students have many common life experiences, despite the actual diagnosis involved (Davis & Brosco, 2007; Perrin et al., 1993; Stein et al., 1993; Stein & Jessop, 1989). Thus, different disease processes may have different mechanisms of impact, but the resulting challenges in a school setting look similar across populations. For example, educators can accommodate slowed cognitive processing, fatigue, pain, or inattention with appropriate intervention strategies regardless of the origin of the difficulty.

Evidence suggests that children and adolescents who experience neurological or cognitive impairment as a part of their condition have more difficulty than those who do not have such sequelae (Howe, Feinstein, Reiss, Molock, & Berger, 1993; Northam, 1997). However, Howe and colleagues (1993) concluded that though children with brain-based limits have different risks and needs than children with other consequences of their medical condition, even children with those more general impacts fare more poorly overall than their healthy peers.

Impact of Chronic Medical Conditions at School

When young people must balance the demands of managing a chronic medical condition along with the school demands all adolescents experience, a new dimension is added to the quest for academic and social success. There is no reason to believe that adolescents with chronic medical conditions would have any less desire than their healthy peers to meet their basic psychological and developmental needs (Schwartz & Drotar, 2009; Shaw & McCabe, 2008; Svavarsdottir, 2008). However, depending on the level of impact a given condition has on the physical, emotional, or cognitive aspects of the student’s life, he or she may experience interruptions to typical developmental and academic progress (Fowler, Johnson, & Atkinson, 1985; Northam, 1997; Schwartz & Drotar, 2009; Shaw et al., 2010).

Effects of health on academic performance may be directly related to the cognitive impact of disease and treatment or a result of indirect factors (Haas, 2006; Lum et al., 2017; Madan-Swain, Fredrick, & Wallander, 1999). Factors that can directly affect academic outcomes include fatigue, pain, and cognitive changes resulting from the disease and treatment. Indirect effects arise via school attendance patterns and related missed instructional time, changes in teacher and parental expectations for academic achievement, or other psychosocial adjustment issues such as the development of school phobia or separation anxiety (Clay, Cortina, Harper, Cocco, & Drotar, 2004; Haas, 2006; Jackson, 2009; Maslow, Haydon, Ford, & Halpern, 2011; Sexson & Madan-Swain, 1995).

Academic struggles may occur acutely as a result of disease symptoms and treatment side effects or become evident later, when the child reaches an age at which the affected skills would be expected to emerge developmentally (Armstrong, 2006; Armstrong & Briery, 2004). Acute interference with academic progress is often a result of pain, fatigue, lethargy, general malaise, or medication side effects (Madan-Swain et al., 1999; Meijer, Sinnema, Bijstra, Mellenbergh, & Wolters, 2000; Thies, 1999). When cognitive effects appear over time, they do not represent deterioration of previously accomplished milestones, but are the result of impact on the rate of brain growth and development of complex structures within the brain (Armstrong & Briery, 2004; Daly, Kral, & Brown, 2008; Moore, 2005, Oeffinger, Nathan, & Dremer, 2008). Neurocognitive sequelae can include impact on cognitive ability, attention, processing speed, memory, visual-motor integration, school performance, social interactions, and adaptive behaviors (Armstrong & Briery, 2004; Brown & Anderson, 1999; Burke & Elliott, 1999; Martinez & Ercikan, 2008; Meijer et al., 2000; Power, 2006). These neurocognitive deficits, in turn, may interfere not only with academic progress but also with the development of competence and social skills (Power, 2006). They may be observed by parents and by teachers in the classroom as failure to complete work, slowness with approaching and accomplishing work, periods of inattention, fine-motor deficits, or difficulty with developing and maintaining peer relationships (Armstrong & Briery, 2004; Madan-Swain, Katz, & LaGory, 2004).

Academic Attainment

Children with a variety of chronic medical conditions have demonstrated lower levels of academic attainment, even when they appear to have typical cognitive functioning or global IQ scores similar to those of healthy peers (Berg & Linton, 2009; Case, Fertig, & Paxson, 2005; Fowler et al., 1985; Haas & Fosse, 2008; Sexson & Madan-Swain, 1993, 1995). Evidence suggests that children and adolescents with chronic medical conditions reach developmental milestones later than their healthy peers (Barraclough & Machek, 2010; Maslow et al., 2011; Suris, Michaud, & Viner, 2004) and are less likely to complete high school on time (Champaloux & Young, 2015; Haas & Fosse, 2008; Maslow, Haydon, McRee, & Halpern, 2012). These lower levels of academic attainment have consequences for vocational achievement and psychosocial outcomes in adulthood (Case et al., 2005; Joe, Joe, & Rowley, 2009; Maslow et al., 2012; Needham, Crosnoe, & Muller, 2004).

School Belonging and Connectedness

Belonging has been defined as a basic human need, important for optimal functioning (Baumeister & Leary, 1995; Goodenow, 1993; Libbey, 2004). Humans need an integrated and reciprocal relationship with others in the community. For children and adolescents, school is the primary community outside the family. School environments with characteristics such as high academic standards, high levels of teacher support, a community where relationships between students and adults are caring and respectful, and safety have been identified as promoting school connectedness for students (Blum, 2005; Libbey, 2004; Wingspread Conference, 2004) and as being important for students of all ages (Anderman, 2003; Anderman & Freeman, 2004; Birch & Ladd, 1997). Many benefits of school belonging for student success have been noted, including social, behavioral, and academic (Anderman & Anderman, 1999; Blum, 2005; Goodenow, 1993; Libbey, 2004; Wingspread Conference, 2004).

Not surprisingly, school connectedness has been identified as a protective factor for adolescents (McNeely, Nonnemaker, & Blum, 2002; Resnick et al., 1997), and it may be especially related to academic attainment for students with chronic medical conditions (Lum et al., 2017; Maslow et al., 2011). Social isolation can be a greater concern for students with medical conditions, and it may be that lack of connectedness is related to lost opportunities for development of social relationships. Peer interaction and teacher-student relationships may be especially important for students with chronic medical conditions, as those relationships can foster a sense of school belonging just as they do for healthy, typical students (Capurso & Dennis, 2017; LaGreca, Bearman, & Moore, 2004; McMahon, Parnes, Keys, & Viola, 2008; Shiu, 2001). When youngsters feel they belong, they may be able to pull from a stronger set of inner resources. Conversely, they may suffer when they anticipate isolation from experiencing decrements in reasoning and thought processing (Baumeister, Twenge, & Nuss, 2002).

A sense of school belonging seems to have long-term implications for adolescents (Eccles et al., 1993; Finn, 1989; Patrick, Anderman, & Ryan, 2002). That is, when students experience a sense of belonging or connectedness to their school and its values, they are likely to have positive school functioning and are less likely to leave school before graduation (Finn, 1989; Murdock, 1999). Young adolescents need an opportunity to interact with peers, teachers, and a mastery-oriented environment so they can become invested and motivated for academic tasks (Anderman & Freeman, 2004).

Students with chronic health conditions have consistently described lower levels of motivation, engagement, school satisfaction, feelings of safety, and academic achievement along with higher levels of loneliness and isolation compared to healthy peers (Forrest Bevans, Riley, Crespo, & Louis, 2011; Hogan, McLellan, & Bauman, 2000; McDougall, DeWit, King, Miller, & Killup, 2004; Svavarsdottir, 2008). When adolescents with chronic medical conditions are absent from school frequently or have low levels of energy, they are likely to have decreased levels of both instructional and social participation in the community and can develop a sense of isolation or experience academic struggle (Needham et al., 2004; Phelps, 2006; Thies, 1999). High-risk students may need to be offered specific opportunities for positive teacher and peer relationships in an effort to encourage a sense of school belonging.

Present Study

The primary aim of the current study was to examine associations between an adolescent’s health status, perceptions of school belonging, and the likelihood of high school completion by 19 years of age. The following four questions guided the study:

  1. Does health status predict level of perceived school belonging in adolescents?

    It was hypothesized that poorer health status (as measured by health reports and a symptoms score) would be associated with lower student perceptions of school belonging.

  2. Are there mean differences in the levels of perceived school belonging between adolescents who have had a chronic medical condition since childhood and healthy peers?

    It was hypothesized that students with chronic medical conditions would report lower levels of perceived belonging.

  3. Does perceived school belonging predict the odds of high school completion by the age of 19 years?

    It was hypothesized that higher levels of perceived school belonging would be related to higher high school completion rates.

  4. Is the relation between perceived school belonging and high school completion the same for adolescents with chronic medical conditions since childhood and their peers who report no health concerns?

    It was hypothesized that health status would moderate the association between perceived belonging and high school completion.

Methods

The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health (Add Health; Harris et al., 2009) is a long-term study using a nationally representative sample of U.S. adolescents who were in grades 7–12 during the 1994–95 school year. A stratified, complex sample was used, and five waves of data have been collected to date. The purpose of the Add Health project has been to explore the social, economic, psychological, and physical outcomes for the participants in a variety of contexts as they moved from adolescence into adulthood (Carolina Population Center, n.d.).

Wave I data were collected via in-home interviews of both parents and students in the 1994–95 school year. Wave III data were collected in 2001–02, when the participants were 18 to 26 years old. Wave IV data were collected in 2007–08 when participants were 24 to 32 years old Waves III and IV included in-home interviews of all participants from Wave I who could be located.

Wave II of Add Health used a different set of participants, and is not included in this study. Wave V data have not yet been released. Waves I, III, and IV were each used to gather particular pieces of data for the current study. Table 1 provides a description of the Add Health information used in this study. Waves III and IV were used when the variable of interest was not available in an earlier wave of data.

Table 1.

Participants and Information Included in Add Health Waves I, III, and IV.

Students Parents Information Used in Current Study

Wave Core Sample Oversamples

Wave I In-Home Interview 1994–95 Random selection from stratum of US students in grades 7–12 (N = 12,105) Purposeful selection for oversamples plus saturation samples of 16 schools (N = 8,640) Parents of participating students (N = 17,670) * Demographic variables
* Health report
* Items for measurement of perceived belonging
* Grades
* Report of symptoms
Total in-home interview sample of grade 7–12 students at Wave I (N = 20,745)
Wave III In-Home Interview 2001–02 All Wave I respondents (now aged 18–26 years) who could be located and re-interviewed (N = 15,197) * Information about on-time high school completion
Wave IV In-Home Interview 2007–08 All Wave I respondents (now aged 24–32 years) who could be located and re-interviewed (N = 15,701) * Information related to diagnosis of chronic medical conditions in childhood
* Student level weights

SPSS 20 (IBM Corp, 2011) software was used for initial data cleaning and recoding. Stata 12 (StataCorp, 2011a) software was used to compute the results of all further analyses. The Stata 12 survey set commands address the stratified, clustered and weighted nature of the data (StataCorp, 2011b) and allow for accurate parameter and variance estimates. Design-based features, including stratum, cluster, and weights, were included in analyses, as appropriate (Chantala, 2006).

Participants

Participants from Wave I of the Add Health study who had complete data for the items of interest (N = 20,180) were included for variable development. Wave III data were used to determine which participants from Wave I had completed high school by the age of 19. Wave IV data were used to determine which Wave I participants identified the diagnosis of a chronic medical condition prior to the age of 12 years. Wave IV data were also used for a logistic regression model to assess the likelihood of on-time high school completion based on health status and levels of perceived school belonging.

Subsamples were identified from the larger Add Health sample for the purpose of assessing mean levels of perceived school belonging and rate of high school completion by age 19 based on health history. Two groups were chosen based on self-reported information about diagnosis of a chronic medical condition collected at the Wave IV data time point.

The first group included participants who reported diagnosis of a chronic medical condition at the age of 12 years or younger (N = 629). Diagnoses in the chronic medical condition group included cancer (n = 27), diabetes (n = 40), heart disease (n = 44), epilepsy (n = 107), and migraine (n = 436). These conditions have been noted to have similar impact on school (Barrett & Sachs, 2006; Berg & Linton, 2009; Brown, 2004; Clay, 2004; Delamater, Brady, & Blumberg, 2004; Phelps, 2006; Powers, Patton, Hommel, & Hershey, 2003; Robinson et al., 2010; Rovet & Fernandes, 2004; Taras & Potts-Datema, 2005) and multiple previous studies have used a similar list of diagnoses with the non-categorical approach for research (Cadman, Boyle, Szaltmari, & Offord, 1987; Fowler et al., 1985; Maslow et al., 2011; Meijer et al., 2000; Thies, 1999). The healthy group (N = 897) was randomly chosen from all who identified as having no history of health concerns at the Wave IV data collection point. Table 2 provides information about the characteristics of each of the groups in the study.

Table 2.

Characteristics of Participants in the Study.

Variable Range WAVE I Full Data Set (N = 20,180) Subsamples Wave IV Full Data Set (N = 10,813)

Design-Based M (se) Unweighted M (SD) Healthy (N = 897) M (SD) Chronic Ill (N = 629) M (SD) Design-Based M (se)

Age 12–19 15.85 (.12) 16.11 (1.70) 16.07 (1.73) 16.0 (1.69) 15.82 (.12)
Gender %female 50.5% 50.5% 47% 63% 52%
Race %minority 27.9% 42.3% 42% 32% 27%
Parent Educ 1–5 2.67 (.05) 2.68 (1.14) 2.71 (1.14) 2.59 (1.07) 2.76 (.05)
Grade Avg 1–4 2.85 (.02) 2.75 (.77) 2.79 (.77) 2.72 (.76) 2.90 (.02)
Belong Scale 1–5 3.71(.02) 3.68 (.75) 3.74 (.71) 3.59 (.82) 3.74 (.02)
Health Report 1–5 4.01 (.01) 3.97 (.76) 4.06 (.73) 3.74 (.82) 4.03 (.02)
Symptoms Score 0–31 0.78 (.01) 0.76 (.45) 7.70 (4.50) 10.60 (5.78) 0.77 (.01)
Complete HS %Yes 92.7% 92.3% 94.4% 90.1% 93.7%

Note: Gender: 0 = male, 1 = female; race: 0 = white, 1 = minority; HS completion: 0 = No, 1 = Yes.

Measurement

Control variables were developed using the Wave I Add Health data. Age, gender, and race were all available in the Wave I data. Grade average was computed by using the mean of the most recent grades for English, math, science, and history reported by the participant. The parent education variable was created using the mean of mother and father education level when both were reported. The completion level of one parent was used when only one was identified.

Health report at Wave 1

Students and their parents were each asked to assess the student’s general health at Wave 1 (“In general, how is your/student’s health?”; scale of 1 = poor to 5 = excellent). The health report value was created using the mean of the two scores reporting health status. Many chronic medical conditions are not identified in the Add Health data, so the health report at Wave 1 was added as a more general health variable for the assessment of outcomes.

Symptom score

Students reported the frequency of a variety of physical symptoms for the previous 12 months. Responses ranged from 0 (never) to 4 (almost every day) for each symptom. A mean score indicating the self-reported frequency of 11 symptoms was developed from Wave1 responses. The items describe a variety of student complaints, some that would be considered more physical (i.e., headaches, feeling weak) and others that might be categorized as more emotional (i.e., moodiness, unable to relax). Physical and emotional symptoms were both included as a way of addressing the fuzziness that exists between the two, and because the symptoms, per se, were not the focus of this study.

Principal-components analysis with an oblique rotation was used to assess the items. The symptoms loaded on one factor with an eigenvalue of 3.59. The 11 items had an alpha coefficient of 0.79. The variable was created by using a composite symptoms mean score (range 0–31; M = 0.78, linearized se = .01). This variable was also used as a general measure of student health for the initial analyses.

Perceived school belonging

A scaled score was created using five items from the Wave I student survey: You feel close to people at your school; You feel like you are part of your school; You are happy to be at your school; The teachers at your school treat students fairly; and You feel safe in your school. The items were measured using a 5-point Likert scale, ranging from 1 = strongly disagree to 5 = strongly agree, and the scale was created using the average of responses for each of the items (M = 3.71, linearized se = .02). Previous researchers have used the same items to assess the construct of school belonging (Anderman, 2002; Maslow et al., 2012; McNeely et al., 2002; Resnick et al., 1997). The 5-item scale had an acceptable level of reliability for the current sample (α = .76).

High school completion

Age of completion was calculated using participant date of birth and Wave III responses about high school graduation or GED completion dates. This outcome variable was a binary measure (yes or no). Completion by the age of 19 years was considered “on-time” completion for the purposes of this study.

Results

Prediction of Perceived Belonging

Linear regression modeling with Wave I data was used to explore the relation between levels of perceived belonging and health variables. Gender, race, and parent education were not significant predictors of belonging in the final regression model. Age was inversely related and grade average was positively related, and both were significant predictors of perceived belonging. Report of health status had a positive relationship with belonging, while symptom scores had an inverse relation with belonging. Health report and symptoms score both provided significant predictive utility for perceived belonging. With demographic variables controlled, the two health variables together explained an additional 7.2% variance for perceived belonging. Final regression model values are found in Table 3.

Table 3.

Final Regression Model for Perceived School Belonging Using Design-Based Wave I Data.

Stand. Coefficient Linearized se p

Age –.029 .008 <.001
Gender .001 .020 .947
Race –.026 .025 .307
Parent Education –.009 .009 .344
Grade Average .197 .014 <.001
Initial Model R2 .067
Wave I Health Report .120 .015 <.001
Symptoms Mean Score –.377 .027 <.001
Model R2 .072
Total Model R2 .139

Note: Gender coded male = 0, female = 1; race coded white = 0, minority =1; bold = variance explained by control variables, additional variance explained by health variables and total variance explained.

Group Differences

T-tests were used to examine mean differences for the demographic variables between the group of students with chronic medical conditions and the group of healthy peers (total N = 1,526). Hedges g was used for effect size estimation (Fritz, Morris, & Richler, 2012).

Based on diagnostic status (yes vs. no), no differences were found for age or prior grades. The reported parent level of education was higher for the healthy group of participants (t(1464) = 2.10, p = .014, g = 0.11). The chronic medical condition group included more females (t(1552) = –6.32, p < .001, g = 0.33). The healthy group included more students of minority status (t(1549) = 4.17, p < .001, g = 0.22).

Results of previous research have indicated that gender classification may lead to differing levels of chronic medical condition incidence and consequences of the disease and treatment (Berg & Linton, 2009; Case et al., 2005; Madan-Swain et al., 2004). The differences in minority membership may be related to the oversampling for the Wave I data collection. All of the differences had a small effect size.

A structured mean differences model was developed to assess participants’ understanding of the belonging construct across the two groups. Structured mean differences models are used to determine whether the latent (unobservable) construct is perceived similarly by individuals in both groups. The latent construct (belonging) is measured using observable variables (items from survey). Model fit is the match between the covariance matrix of the groups and the estimated covariance matrix of the population. In a good fit, the two groups would both match the population matrix as closely as possible (Acock, 2013; Schumacker & Lomax, 2010).

The groups were assessed for invariance, and the final model indicated that the two groups perceived the concept of belonging in a similar fashion. Model fit statistics are presented in Table 4. Goodness-of-fit parameters may be found in Table 5.

Table 4.

Model Comparisons for Healthy and Chronically Ill Student Groups.

Model X 2 df p X2
difference**
df
difference
Significance
(difference)
RMSEA CFI SRMR

1. Same Form 301.71 98 <.001 .052 .974 .028
2. Same Loadings 302.07 106 <.001 .36(1v2) 8 ns .049 .975 .032
3. Same Loadings and Err Variances 407.17 133 <.001 105.10(2v3) 27 Sig. .052 .965 .051
4. Same Loadings and Intercepts 341.29 119 <.001 39.22(2v4) 13 Sig. .049 .972 .039

Note: Target parameters for good model fit are RMSEA < .05, CFI > .95, and SRMR < .05; RMSEA = root mean square error of approximation, CFI = comparative fit index, SRMR = standardized root mean-squared residual. The goal for X2 is non-significance; however, large sample size may lead to a significant X2 result.

** Model elements are constrained to compare between the groups, 1 v. 2 = comparison between fully unconstrained model and model with constrained factor loadings, 2 v. 3 = comparison between constrained loadings only and constrained loadings plus error variances, 2. v. 4 = comparison between constrained loadings only and constrained loadings and intercepts.

Table 5.

Group Goodness-of-Fit Parameters (SRMR) for Comparison Models.

Observations 1. Same Form 2. Same Loadings 3. Same Loadings and Error Variances 4. Same Loadings and Intercepts

Healthy 924 .024 .027 .046 .034
Chronically Ill 627 .032 .037 .055 .044

Note: SRMR = standardized root mean-squared residual, target < .05.

Perceived belonging

Group mean levels of perceived school belonging were compared using a t-test (healthy vs. chronically ill). The healthy group had a mean of 3.78 (SD = 0.68), whereas the group with chronic health conditions had a mean of 3.61 (SD = 0.80). The mean difference was significantly different than zero (t(1056) = 3.687, p < .001), with an effect size in the small range (d = 0.23; Cohen, 1988). As a result, the hypothesis stating that healthy students would report higher levels of belonging than those with chronic medical conditions was supported.

High school completion

Group differences in the rate of high school graduation by age 19 were significantly different than zero (t(1056) = 2.60, p = .005) with a small effect size (d = 0.16; Cohen, 1988). Healthy students had an on-time high school completion rate of 94.4% (M = 0.9439, SD = 0.230), whereas students with chronic medical conditions had on-time high completion rates of 90.1% (M = 0.9014, SD = 0.298). This finding supports the hypothesis that healthy students would have higher high school completion rates than their chronically ill peers.

Belonging and High School Completion

After the differences in mean levels of perceived belonging and high school completion were established, logistic regression was used to determine the relation of belonging and diagnostic status to the likelihood of on-time high school completion. For the logistic regression model, the full Add Health data set was used with Wave IV weights applied (Chantala, 2006). Age, gender, race, parent education, and prior grades were all significant predictors of high school completion by the age of 19. Level of perceived belonging was a significant predictor of on-time high school completion as well. Diagnostic status did not have a significant main effect on the outcome, but it did serve as a significant moderator for belonging. Holding all demographic variables constant, perceived belonging had a different relation with likelihood of on-time high school completion for a student with a chronic health condition than for a peer without an identified health condition.

While belonging is important for all students, those with identified medical conditions experienced more benefits from increased levels of perceived belonging than their healthy peers. The odds ratios for each of the variables are included in Table 6. As illustrated, the odds ratio for the moderation of diagnosis status on belonging was 1.629 (p = .003), indicating that an increase of one unit in perceived belonging led to a 63% increase in the odds of completing high school on time for a student with a chronic medical condition. An odds ratio of this magnitude is considered to have a small effect size (Sullivan & Feinn, 2012).

Table 6.

Design-Based Coefficients and Odds Ratios for Final Logistic Regression Model to Predict High School Completion by the Age of 19 Years (N = 10,253).

Variable b (log odds)
(Linearized se)
p OR
(Linearized se)
95% CI for OR

Age –.3252 (.034) <.001 .7223 (.024) .6759, .7719
Gender .3185 (.123) .011 1.3751 (.169) 1.0786, 1.7530
Race –.4326 (.146) .004 .6488 (.095) .4861, .8659
Parent Education .5008 (.071) <.001 1.6500 (.117) 1.4342, 1.8983
Grade Average .9250 (.076) <.001 2.5219 (.192) 2.1697, 2.9313
W1 Health Report .1703 (.078) .031 1.1857 (.093) 1.0158, 1.3840
Diagnosis Status –.5055 (.751) .502 .6032 (.453) 1.1365, 2.6651
School Belonging –.3373 (.130) .010 .7137 (.092) .5523, .9222
Diag X Belonging .4882 (.158) .003 1.6293 (.258) 1.1911, 2.2287

Note: High school completion coded no = 0, yes = 1; Gender coded 0 = male, 1 = female; Race coded 0 = white, 1 = minority; diagnosis coded 0 = no, 1 = yes

Discussion

The findings of this study contribute to the fields of education and psychology in several ways. Specifically, helping school staff (teachers, administrators, coaches, etc.) understand the importance of perceived belonging for all students under their charge may be a way to promote academic attainment. Students need to reach the first level of academic attainment – high school completion – before they can move on to higher levels of attainment.

It is critical for school personnel to understand that belonging is important for all students, but especially for students with chronic medical conditions or other or identified health concerns. If health status and belonging interact to provide some explanation of variance for academic attainment, it seems prudent for academic leaders to find ways to promote perceived belonging for students with chronic medical conditions/health concerns and to create a supportive environment for high school completion.

There are several ways the findings reported here can be addressed in the school setting. For example, acknowledging the extra struggle that a student with a chronic medical condition may experience and communicating empathy can promote perceived belonging. Further, school personnel can strive to create a safe environment for all students, particularly those who sometimes feel different because of frequent absence or visible signs of a medical condition. In addition, expressing awareness of the issues experienced by students with chronic medical conditions that might not be obvious (e.g., fatigue, processing speed delays) could support a positive relationship with the students.

Relationships

Teacher-student relationships have been shown to have a clear positive impact on students’ sense of school belonging (Goodenow, 1993; Klem & Connell, 2004; Libbey, 2004; McNeely & Falci, 2004; Murray & Greenberg, 2000; Patrick et al., 2002; Rosenfeld, Richman, & Bowen, 2000; Wentzel, 1998). Teachers can impact their students through caring, treating the them fairly, and actively engaging them in learning. For example, Libbey (2004) noted that “student relationships with their school often were operationalized as their relationship with their teachers” (p. 281). Similarly, Goodenow (1993) found that teacher support explained more than one third of students’ assessment of value and interest related to their academic work.

Pedagogical caring (Klem & Connell, 2004; Noddings, 1992; Wentzel, 1997) is a concept that involves the ways that students perceive care from their teachers. Teacher behaviors that suggest pedagogical caring include modeling caring behaviors, democratic communication style, treating students as individuals, structure and expectations, and a nurturing manner. When students feel cared for by their teachers, they are more likely to be academically motivated and pursue prosocial goals (Klem & Connell, 2004; Wentzel, 1997). Capurso and Dennis (2017) identified relationships (including authentic social interactions with peers and teachers) as one of six Key Educational Factors (KEF) necessary for students with chronic medical conditions to have a successful school experience.

There is research available to provide guidance to school administrators and teachers about how to provide belonging support for students. The Wingspread Declaration (Wingspread, 2004) describes school connection as “the belief by students that adults in the school care about their learning as well as about them as individuals” (p. 233). Three critical school-level components of belonging are identified as predictors of positive academic outcomes for students: (a) high academic expectations coupled with support for learning, (b) positive and respectful student-adult relationships, and (c) physical and emotional safety of the school environment (Blum, 2005; Klem & Connell, 2004; Wingspread, 2004).

Conclusion

The findings of this study support prior reports that students with chronic medical conditions experience more difficulty in the school environment than their healthy peers. The hypothesis that students with chronic medical conditions would be less likely to complete high school on time was supported. The hypothesis that students with chronic medical conditions would experience lower levels of perceived school belonging was also supported. The important finding that perceived school belonging is necessary for students, especially those with chronic medical needs, should be kept in mind as school administrators and teachers look for ways to promote high rates of high school completion for all students.

Limits of the Study

Limitations come with any study using secondary data. Some of the concerns related to this study include the abbreviated list of diagnoses identified in the Add Health data set, small numbers for any one specific disease group, and the lack of information about the intensity of disease for the participants. It is also important to note that the diagnosis of a chronic medical condition before the age of 12 was self-reported by the participants when they were between 24 and 32 years old. Many of the more common pediatric chronic conditions were excluded from the list, and those exclusions may have made the division between healthy and chronically ill students blurry and difficult to account. For example, students with sickle cell disease or inflammatory bowel disease are not identified in the Add Health sample but are among the students with significant care needs that can interfere with school (Brown, 2004; Clay, 2004; Daly et al., 2008; Phelps, 2006). Large data sets in the United States are typically focused on health or education factors, and both domains are rarely included in the same study. Therefore, some of the detail of either domain may be lost when the focus is on the other.

Further, there are pros and cons of using a non-categorical approach to the study of students with chronic medical conditions. Hundreds of pediatric diseases could be considered, so the purpose of the study is important to keep in mind. While a non-categorical approach does not allow for fine-grained detail related to any one specific diagnosis, it can move the understanding of a students with a broader range of diagnoses forward and allow for more general guidance around school policies and practices.

Implications for Further Research

Further study of a larger and more heterogeneous group of students with chronic medical conditions will be useful in understanding ways to address student need for belonging. Any study that looks at the impact of chronic medical conditions at school should include not only identification of diagnosis but also an indicator of disease or symptom severity. Perhaps severity of impact (pain, fatigue, school absence, social isolation) is more important for school personnel to understand than a focus on specific diagnosis.

Acknowledgements

This research used data from Add Health, a program project directed by Kathleen Mullan Harris and designed by J. Richard Udry, Peter S. Bearman, and Kathleen Mullan Harris at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, and funded by grant P01-HD31921 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, with cooperative funding from 23 other federal agencies and foundations. Information on how to obtain the Add Health data files is available on the Add Health website (http://www.cpc.unc.edu/addhealth). No direct support was received from grant P01-HD31921 for this analysis.

The editorial team of Continuity in Education would like to express their gratitude to the reviewers who generously gave their time and expertise to improve this article: Steven Shaw and others who wish to remain anonymous.

The editorial processing of this article was managed by Chief Editor Michele Capurso while the copyediting was carried out by Kirsten McBride.

Ethics and Consent

Approval was obtained for use of restricted data from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent to Adult Health (Add Health; Harris et al., 2009). The Institutional Review Board approved use of the data for the study (protocol #2008B0310). Data access was also approved by the Carolina Population Center.

Competing Interests

The author has no competing interests to declare.

References

  • 1.Acock, A. C. (2013). Discovering structural equation modeling using Stata. College Station, TX: StataCorp LP. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Anderman, E. M. (2002). School effects on psychological outcomes during adolescence. Journal of Educational Psychology, 94(4), 795–809. DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.94.4.795 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Anderman, L. H. (2003). Academic and social perceptions as predictors of change in middle school students’ sense of school belonging. The Journal of Experimental Education, 72(1), 5–22. DOI: 10.1080/00220970309600877 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Anderman, L. H., & Anderman, E. M. (1999). Social predictors of changes in students’ achievement goal orientations. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 25, 21–37. DOI: 10.1006/ceps.1998.0978 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Anderman, L. H., & Freeman, T. M. (2004). Students’ sense of belonging in school. In Pintrich P. R. & Maehr M. L. (Eds.), Advances in motivation and achievement, Volume 13. Motivating students, improving schools: The legacy of Carol Midgley (pp. 27–63). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier JAI. DOI: 10.1016/S0749-7423(03)13002-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Armstrong, F. D. (2006). Neurodevelopment and chronic illness: Mechanisms of disease and treatment. Mental Retardation and Developmental Disabilities Research Reviews, 12, 168–173. DOI: 10.1002/mrdd.20114 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Armstrong, F. D., & Briery, B. G. (2004). Childhood cancer and the school. In Brown R. T. (Ed.), Handbook of pediatric psychology in school settings (pp. 263–281). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Barraclough, D., & Machek, G. (2010). School psychologists’ role concerning children with chronic illnesses in schools. Journal of Applied School Psychology, 26, 132–148. DOI: 10.1080/15377901003712694 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Barrett, R. P., & Sachs, H. T. (2006). Epilepsy and seizures. In Phelps L. (Ed.), Chronic health-related disorders in children (pp. 91–110). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. DOI: 10.1037/11435-006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Baumeister, R. F., & Leary, M. R. (1995). The need to belong: Desire for interpersonal attachments as a fundamental human motivation. Psychological Bulletin, 117(3), 497–529. DOI: 10.1037/0033-2909.117.3.497 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Baumeister, R. F., Twenge, J. M., & Nuss, C. K. (2002). Effects of social exclusion on cognitive processes: Anticipated aloneness reduces intelligent thought. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 83(4), 817–827. DOI: 10.1037/0022-3514.83.4.817 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Berg, R. A., & Linton, J. C. (2009). Neuropsychological sequelae of chronic medical disorders in children and youth. In Reynolds C. R. & Fletcher-Janzen E. (Eds.), Handbook of clinical child neuropsychology (pp. 671–702). New York, NY: Springer Science+Business Media. DOI: 10.1007/978-0-387-78867-8_27 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Birch, S. H., & Ladd, G. W. (1997). Children’s interpersonal behaviors and the teacher-child relationship. Developmental Psychology, 34(5), 934–946. DOI: 10.1037/0012-1649.34.5.934 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Blum, R. W. (2005). A case for school connectedness. Educational Leadership, 62(April), 16–20. [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Brown, R. T. (Ed.). (2004). Cognitive aspects of chronic illness in children. New York, NY: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Brown, R. T., & Anderson, D. L. (1999). Cognition in chronically ill children: A collaborative endeavor of pediatrics and psychology. In Brown R. T. (Ed.), Cognitive aspects of chronic illness in children (pp. 1–11). New York, NY: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Burke, P., & Elliott, M. (1999). Depression in pediatric chronic illness. Psychosomatics, 40(1), 5–17. DOI: 10.1016/S0033-3182(99)71266-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Cadman, D., Boyle, M., Szaltmari, P., & Offord, D. R. (1987). Chronic illness, disability, and mental and social well-being: Findings of the Ontario Child Health Study. Pediatrics, 79(5), 805–813. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Capurso, M., & Dennis, J. L. (2017). Key educational factors in the education of students with a medical condition. Support for Learning, 32(2), 158–179. DOI: 10.1111/1467-9604.12156 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Carolina Population Center, University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill. (n.d.). http://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth
  • 21.Case, A., Fertig, A., & Paxson, C. (2005). The lasting impact of childhood health and circumstance. Journal of Health Economics, 24, 365–389. DOI: 10.1016/j.jhealeco.2004.09.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Champaloux, S. W., & Young, D. R. (2015). Childhood chronic health conditions and educational attainment: A social ecological approach. Journal of Adolescent Health, 56, 98–105. DOI: 10.1016/j.jadohealth.2014.07.016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Chantala, K. (2006). Guidelines for analyzing Add Health data. Chapel Hill, NC: University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, Carolina Population Center. www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth/data/guides [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Clay, D. L. (2004). Helping schoolchildren with chronic health conditions. New York, NY: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Clay, D. L., Cortina, S., Harper, D. C., Cocco, K. M., & Drotar, D. (2004). Schoolteachers’ experience with childhood chronic illness. Children’s Health Care, 33(3), 227–239. DOI: 10.1207/s15326888chc3303_5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Daly, B. P., Kral, M. C., & Brown, R. T. (2008). Cognitive and academic problems associated with childhood cancers and sickle cell disease. School Psychology Quarterly, 23(2), 230–242. DOI: 10.1037/1045-3830.23.2.230 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Davis, M. M., & Brosco, J. P. (2007). Being specific about being special. Archives in Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, 161(10), 1003–1005. DOI: 10.1001/archpedi.161.10.1003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Delamater, A. M., Brady, E. U., & Blumberg, M. J. (2004). Cardiac conditions. In Brown R. T. (Ed.), Cognitive aspects of chronic illness in children (pp. 190–219). New York, NY: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Eccles, J. S., Midgley, C., Wigfield, A., Buchanan, C. M., Reuman, D., Flanagan, C., & MacIver, D. (1993). Development during adolescence: The impact of stage-environment fit on young adolescents; experiences in schools and in families. American Psychologist, 48(2), 90–101. DOI: 10.1037/0003-066X.48.2.90 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Finn, J. D. (1989). Withdrawing from school. Review of Educational Research, 59(2), 117–142. DOI: 10.3102/00346543059002117 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Forrest, C. B., Bevans, K. B., Riley, A. W., Crespo, R., & Louis, T. A. (2011). School outcomes of children with special health care needs. Pediatrics, 128(2), 303–312. DOI: 10.1542/peds.2010-3347 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Fowler, M. G., Johnson, M. P., & Atkinson, S. S. (1985). School achievement and absence in children with chronic health conditions. The Journal of Pediatrics, 106(4), 683–687. DOI: 10.1016/S0022-3476(85)80103-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Fritz, C. O., Morris, P. E., & Richler, J. J. (2012). Effect size estimates: Current use, calculations, and interpretation. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 141(1), 2–18. DOI: 10.1037/a0024338 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Goodenow, C. (1993). Classroom belonging among early adolescent students: Relationships to motivation and achievement. Journal of Early Adolescence, 13(1), 21–43. DOI: 10.1177/0272431693013001002 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Haas, S. A. (2006, December). Health selection and the process of social stratification: The effect of childhood health on socioeconomic attainment. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 47, 339–354. DOI: 10.1177/002214650604700403 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Haas, S. A., & Fosse, N. E. (2008). Health and the educational attainment of adolescents: Evidence from the NLSY97. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 49, 178–192. DOI: 10.1177/002214650804900205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Harris, K. M., Halpern, C. T., Whitsel, E., Hussey, J., Tabor, J., Entzel, P., & Udry, J. R. (2009). The National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health: Research design. Retrieved from http://www.cpc.unc.edu/projects/addhealth/design
  • 39.Hogan, A., McLellan, L., & Bauman, A. (2000). Health promotion of young people with disabilities: A population study. Disability and Rehabilitation, 22(8), 352–357. DOI: 10.1080/096382800296593 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Howe, G. W., Feinstein, C., Reiss, D., Molock, S., & Berger, K. (1993). Adolescent adjustment to chronic physical disorders – I. Comparing neurological and non-neurological conditions. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 34(7), 1153–1171. DOI: 10.1111/j.1469-7610.1993.tb01780.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.IBM Corp. (2011). IBM SPSS statistics for windows, Version 20.0. Armonk, NY: Author. [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Jackson, M. I. (2009). Understanding links between adolescent health and educational attainment. Demography, 46(4), 671–694. DOI: 10.1353/dem.0.0078 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Joe, S., Joe, E., & Rowley, L. L. (2009). Consequences of physical health and mental illness risks for academic achievement in grades K-12. Review of Research in Education, 33, 283–309. DOI: 10.3102/0091732X08327355 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Kaffenberger, C. (2006). School reentry for students with a chronic illness: A role for professional school counselors. Professional School Counseling, 9(3), 223–230. DOI: 10.5330/prsc.9.3.xr27748161346325 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Klem, A. M., & Connell, J. P. (2004). Relationships matter: Linking teacher support to student engagement and achievement. Journal of School Health, 74(7), 262–273. DOI: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2004.tb08283.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.LaGreca, A. M., Bearman, K. J., & Moore, H. (2004). Peer relations. In Brown R. T. (Ed.), Handbook of pediatric psychology in school settings (pp. 657–678). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Libbey, H. P. (2004). Measuring student relationships to school: Attachment, bonding, connectedness, and engagement. Journal of School Health, 74(7), 274–283. DOI: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2004.tb08284.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Lum, A., Wakefield, C. E., Donnan, B., Burns, M. A., Fardell, J. E., & Marshall, G. M. (2017). Understanding the school experiences of children and adolescents with serious chronic illness: a systematic meta-review. Child: Care, Health, and Development, 43(5), 645–662. DOI: 10.1111/cch.12475 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Madan-Swain, A., Fredrick, L. D., & Wallander, J. L. (1999). Returning to school after serious illness or injury. In Brown R. T. (Ed.), Cognitive aspects of chronic illness in children (pp. 312–332). New York, NY: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Madan-Swain, A., Katz, E. R., & LaGory, J. (2004). School and social reintegration after a serious illness or injury. In Brown R. T. (Ed.), Handbook of pediatric psychology in school settings (pp. 637–655). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Martinez, Y. J., & Ercikan, K. (2008). Chronic illness in Canadian children: What is the effect of illness on academic achievement, and anxiety and emotional disorders? Child: Care, Health and Development, 35(3), 391–401. DOI: 10.1111/j.1365-2214.2008.00916.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Maslow, G. R., Haydon, A. A., Ford, C. A., & Halpern, C. T. (2011). Young adult outcomes of children growing up with chronic illness. Archives of Pediatric and Adolescent Medicine, 165(3), 256–261. DOI: 10.1001/archpediatrics.2010.287 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Maslow, G. R., Haydon, A. A., McRee, A. L., & Halpern, C. T. (2012). Protective connections and educational attainment among young adults with childhood-onset chronic illness. Journal of School Health, 82(8), 364–370. DOI: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2012.00710.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.McDougall, J., DeWit, D. J., King, G., Miller, L. T., & Killup, S. (2004). High school-aged youths’ attitudes toward their peers with disabilities: The role of school and student interpersonal factors. International Journal of Disability, Development, and Education, 51(3), 287–313. DOI: 10.1080/1034912042000259242 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.McMahon, S. D., Parnes, A. L., Keys, C. B., & Viola, J. J. (2008). School belonging among low-income urban youth with disabilities: Testing a theoretical model. Psychology in the Schools, 45(5), 387–401. DOI: 10.1002/pits.20304 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.McNeely, C., & Falci, C. (2004). School connectedness and the transition into and out of health-risk behavior among adolescents: A comparison of social belonging and teacher support. Journal of School Health, 74(7), 284–292. DOI: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2004.tb08285.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.McNeely, C. A., Nonnemaker, J. M., & Blum, R. W. (2002). Promoting school connectedness: Evidence from the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health. Journal of School Health, 72(4), 138–146. DOI: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2002.tb06533.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Meijer, S. A., Sinnema, G., Bijstra, J. O., Mellenbergh, G. J., & Wolters, W. H. G. (2000). Social functioning in children with a chronic illness. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 41(3), 309–317. DOI: 10.1111/1469-7610.00615 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Moore, B. D. (2005). Neurocognitive outcomes in survivors of childhood cancer. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 30(1), 51–63. DOI: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsi016 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Murdock, T. (1999). The social context of risk: Status and motivational predictors of alienation in middle school. Journal of Educational Psychology, 91(1), 62–75. DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.91.1.62 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Murray, C., & Greenberg, M. T. (2000). Children’s relationship with teachers and bonds with school: An investigation of patterns and correlates in middle childhood. Journal of School Psychology, 38(5), 423–445. DOI: 10.1016/S0022-4405(00)00034-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Needham, B. L., Crosnoe, R., & Muller C. (2004). Academic failure in secondary school: The inter-related role of health problems and educational context. Social Problems, 51, 569–586. DOI: 10.1525/sp.2004.51.4.569 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Noddings, N. (1992). The challenge to care in schools: An alternative approach to education. New York, NY: Teachers College Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Northam, E. A. (1997). Psychosocial impact of chronic illness in children. Journal of Pediatrics and Child Health, 33, 369–372. DOI: 10.1111/j.1440-1754.1997.tb01622.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Oeffinger, K. C., Nathan, P. C., & Dremer, L. C. M. (2008). Challenges after curative treatment for childhood cancer and long-term follow up of survivors. Pediatric Clinics of North America, 55, 251–273. DOI: 10.1016/j.pcl.2007.10.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Patrick, H., Anderman, L. H., & Ryan, A. M. (2002). Social motivation and the classroom social environment. In Midgley C. (Ed.), Goals, goal structures, and patterns of adaptive learning (pp. 85–108). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Perrin, E. C., Newacheck, P., Pless, I. B., Drotar, D., Gortmaker, S. L., Leventhal, J., Weitzman, M., et al. (1993). Issues involved in the definition and classification of chronic health conditions. Pediatrics, 91(4), 787–793. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Phelps, A. (Ed.). (2006). Chronic health-related disorders in children. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. DOI: 10.1037/11435-000 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Power, T. J. (2006). Collaborative practices for managing children’s chronic health needs. In Phelps L. (Ed.), Chronic health-related disorders in children (pp. 7–23). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association. DOI: 10.1037/11435-001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Powers, S. W., Patton, S. R., Hommel, K. A., & Hershey, A. D. (2003). Quality of life in childhood migraines: Clinical impact and comparison to other chronic illnesses. Pediatrics, 112(1), e1–e5. http://www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/112/1/e1. DOI: 10.1542/peds.112.1.e1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Resnick, M. D., Bearman, P. S., Blum, R. W., Bauman, K. E., Harris, K. M., Jones, J., Udry, J. R., et al. (1997). Protecting adolescents from harm: Findings from the National Longitudinal Study on Adolescent Health. JAMA, 278(10), 823–832. DOI: 10.1001/jama.1997.03550100049038 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Robinson, K. E., Kuttesch, J. F., Champion, J. E., Andreotti, C. F., Hipp, D. W., Bettis, A., Barnwell, A., & Compas, B. E. (2010). A quantitative meta-analysis of neurocognitive sequelae in survivors of pediatric brain tumors. Pediatric Blood Cancer, 55, 525–531. DOI: 10.1002/pbc.22568 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Rosenfeld, L. B., Richman, J. M., & Bowen, G. L. (2000). Social support networks and school outcomes: The centrality of the teacher. Child and Adolescent Social Work Journal, 17(3), 205–226. DOI: 10.1023/A:1007535930286 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Rovet, J., & Fernandes, C. (2004). Insulin-dependent diabetes mellitus. In Brown R. T. (Ed)., Cognitive aspects of chronic illness in children (pp. 142–171). New York, NY: Guilford. [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Schumacker, R. E., & Lomax, R. G. (2010). A beginner’s guide to structural equation modeling, 3rd ed. New York, NY: Routledge. [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Schwartz, L. A., & Drotar, D. (2009). Health-related hindrance of personal goal pursuit and well-being of young adults with cystic fibrosis, pediatric cancer survivors, and peers without a history of chronic illness. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 34(9), 954–965. DOI: 10.1093/jpepsy/jsn144 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Sexson, S. B., & Madan-Swain, A. (1993). School reentry for the child with chronic illness. Journal of Learning Disabilities, 26(2), 115–125, 137. DOI: 10.1177/002221949302600204 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Sexson, S. B., & Madan-Swain, A. (1995). The chronically ill child in the school. School Psychology Quarterly, 10(4), 359–368. DOI: 10.1037/h0088311 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Shaw, S. R., Glaser, S. E., Stern, M., Sferdenschi, C., & McCabe, P. C. (2010, March). Responding to students’ chronic illness. Principal Leadership (pp. 12–16). [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Shaw, S. R., & McCabe, P. C. (2008). Hospital-to-school transition for children with chronic illness: Meeting the new challenges of an evolving health care system. Psychology in the Schools, 45(1), 74–87. DOI: 10.1002/pits.20280 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Shiu, S. (2001). Issues in the education of students with chronic illness. International Journal of Disability, Development, and Education, 48(3), 269–281. DOI: 10.1080/10349120120073412 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.StataCorp. (2011a). Stata statistical software: Release 12. College Station, TX: Author. [Google Scholar]
  • 83.StataCorp. (2011b). Stata survey data reference manual: Release 12. College Station, TX: Author. [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Stein, R. E. K., Bauman, L. J., Westbrook, L. E., Coupey, S. M., & Ireys, H. T. (1993). Framework for identifying children who have chronic conditions: The case for a new definition. The Journal of Pediatrics, 122(3), 342–347. DOI: 10.1016/S0022-3476(05)83414-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Stein, R. E. K., & Jessop, D. J. (1982). A noncategorical approach to chronic childhood illness. Public Health Reports, 97(4), 354–362. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Sullivan, G. M., & Feinn, R. (2012, September). Using effect size – or why the p value is not enough. Journal of Graduate Medical Education (pp. 279–282). DOI: 10.4300/JGME-D-12-00156.1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Suris, J-C., Michaud, P-A., & Viner, R. (2004). The adolescent with a chronic condition. Part I: Developmental issues. Archives of Disability in Childhood, 89, 938–942. DOI: 10.1136/adc.2003.045369 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Svavarsdottir, E. K. (2008). Connectedness, belonging and feelings about school among health and chronically ill Icelandic schoolchildren. Scandinavian Journal of Caring Sciences, 22(3), 463–471. DOI: 10.1111/j.1471-6712.2007.00553.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Taras, H., & Potts-Datema, W. (2005). Chronic health conditions and student performance at school. Journal of School Health, 75(7), 255–266. DOI: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2005.00034.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Thies, K. M. (1999). Identifying the educational implications of chronic illness in school children. Journal of School Health, 69(10), 392–397. DOI: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.1999.tb06354.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Wentzel, K. R. (1997). Student motivation in middle school: The role of perceived pedagogical caring. Journal of Educational Psychology, 89(3), 411–419. DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.89.3.411 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Wentzel, K. R. (1998). Social relationships and motivation in middle school: The role of parents, teachers, and peers. Journal of Educational Psychology, 90(2), 202–209. DOI: 10.1037/0022-0663.90.2.202 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Wingspread Conference. (2004). Wingspread declaration of school connections. Journal of School Health, 74(7), 233–234. DOI: 10.1111/j.1746-1561.2004.tb08279.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from Continuity in Education are provided here courtesy of Ubiquity Press

RESOURCES