We thank Darlow and Gray (henceforth D&G) (1) for raising possible misconceptions regarding our paper on scientific censorship (2). First, D&G conflate explanation with blame; to explain is not to blame, and the concept of blame is irrelevant to our paper. Next, D&G write, “the authors suggest that increased participation of women and people from diverse backgrounds in academia increases censorship….” While we proposed harm-aversion as one explanation for women supporting censorship more than men, we made no claims about people from diverse backgrounds, nor are we aware of relevant data. D&G then combine ethical concerns regarding the treatment of participants in research with harm concerns regarding the dissemination of science. Censorship pertains only to the latter.
D&G note that one study we cited (3) had a very low response rate, which led Gray (the second author of D&G) (1) to file an official complaint regarding the credibility of the findings. We agree that with such a low response rate, the report may over- or underrepresent true rates of self-censorship among New Zealand academics and that more research is needed.
D&G state, “None of the solutions proposed address underlying problems that give rise to prosocial concerns.” To the contrary, greater transparency and academic audits can help identify any nonscientific basis for editorial decisions or double standards, including discrimination. Audit studies are a rigorous method for identifying discrimination (4) and safeguards against censorship protect minority perspectives.
D&G suggest that censorship is necessary to prevent harm but that it would be unnecessary if vulnerable groups were involved in scholarship affecting their communities. As noted in our paper, there are no data on the costs and benefits of censorship (or alternative harm-prevention solutions) upon which to base such judgments. Additionally, involving community members in research may not eliminate harm-avoidant censorship. Roland Fryer, a Black scholar from an impoverished community, overcame soft-censorship to publish his findings regarding (a lack of) racial differences in the victims of police officer shootings, (5) reporting that he “had colleagues take me to the side and say, ‘Don’t publish this. You’ll ruin your career.’”
D&G argue that “All research pertaining to specific communities should involve active participation of those communities.” Assuming D&G refer to involvement as coinvestigators rather than as research participants; this demand is impractical and unnecessarily restrictive. Just as insiders have provided insights, so have outsiders (e.g., when left-leaning scholars study right-leaning ideologies).
D&G appear to agree with our main point that scientific censorship is often driven by prosocially motivated scientists. They suggest this censorship is warranted, whereas we call for caution. As described in our paper, many sources of data could enable metascientific investigation of the frequency, costs, and benefits of this form of censorship, but in the absence of such investigation, we are “left to quarrel based on competing values, assumptions, and intuitions.” D&G’s letter exemplifies just such an outcome.
Acknowledgments
Author contributions
C.J.C., M.a.-G., R.F.B., A.B.-R., D.B., S.C., J.F., K.F., D.C.G., G.G., M.D.G., L.S.J., A.I.K., C.M., G.M., P.P., C.S., S.S.-W., A.E.W., W.W., B.M.W., and W.v.H. wrote the paper.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interest.
References
- 1.Darlow B., Gray B., Censorship or inclusion? Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 121, e2321261121 (2024). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Clark C. J., et al. , Prosocial motives underlie scientific censorship by scientists: A perspective and research agenda. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S.A. 120, e2301642120 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Free Speech Union, “Free Speech Union: Academic freedom report 2023” (Auckland, NZ, 2023). [Google Scholar]
- 4.Schaerer M., et al. , On the trajectory of discrimination: A meta-analysis and forecasting survey capturing 44 years of field experiments on gender and hiring decisions. Organ. Behav. Hum. Decis. Processes 179, 104280 (2023). [Google Scholar]
- 5.Fryer R. G. Jr., An empirical analysis of racial differences in police use of force. J. Political Econ. 127, 1210–1261 (2019). [Google Scholar]