Abstract
How the spike output of the retina enables human visual perception is not fully understood. Here, we address this at the sensitivity limit of vision by correlating human visual perception with the spike outputs of primate ON and OFF parasol (magnocellular) retinal ganglion cells in tightly matching stimulus conditions. We show that human vision at its ultimate sensitivity limit depends on the spike output of the ON but not the OFF retinal pathway. Consequently, nonlinear signal processing in the retinal ON pathway precludes perceptual detection of single photons in darkness but enables quantal-resolution discrimination of differences in light intensity.
Subject terms: Retina, Sensory processing
The neural mechanisms setting the lower limit of conscious visual perception in humans is not fully understood. Here the authors show by correlating human vision experiments and non-human primate retina recordings that primates rely on the retinal ON pathway in perception of the dimmest light increments, and that nonlinear thresholding in this pathway eliminates single photons and neural noise thereby allowing perception of minute differences in light intensity.
Introduction
Vision at absolute threshold provides a remarkable example of how sensory performance can approach the fundamental limits of physics. Classical psychophysical experiments showed that dark-adapted humans can detect less than a dozen light quanta1–3. Despite decades of work, the neural mechanisms that define this remarkable performance remain poorly understood. A key unresolved question is whether there is a neural threshold (nonlinearity) somewhere in the visual pathway that eliminates noise but requires that a minimum number of quanta are absorbed for a stimulus to be detected perceptually. The current theory for human visual perception (“classical model”, see Fig. 1a) proposes that signals initiated by each photon absorption propagate linearly or near-linearly through visual circuits in the retina before encountering a perceptual decision criterion or threshold in the brain. In this model, perception, with an appropriate criterion, can access signals produced by each individual photon4,5. Evidence from past psychophysics experiments6–9 for this theory, however, is circumstantial and does not account for recent mechanistic findings on retinal circuit function at the lowest light levels10–12.
Signals originating from single-photon absorptions traverse the mammalian retina through the conserved rod bipolar pathway across light levels ranging from absolute threshold to background lights that produce a few photon absorptions per rod per second7,13–15. In this pathway, rod signals are transmitted to ON and OFF retinal ganglion cells (RGCs) via rod bipolar cells and AII amacrine cells. A key feature of the rod bipolar pathway is the increasing rod convergence from its input to its outputs16. This convergence means that photon absorptions in a small fraction of the rods can noticeably modulate retinal outputs, but only if noise added in retinal circuits is avoided. Indeed, recent work shows that signals traversing the ON (but not the OFF) branch of the rod bipolar pathway in both primate and mouse are subject to a thresholding nonlinearity that discards noise and many single-photon responses while transmitting signals originating from coincidence of two or more photons in a pool of ~1000 rods10,11. Selective manipulation of the sensitivity of ON pathways in transgenic mice shows that behavioral detection of dim lights at the sensitivity limit relies on the responses of ON RGCs even when OFF RGC responses would allow higher sensitivity11.
These recent findings challenge the classical model for perception at the sensitivity limit. They suggest instead a new model in which perception relies on retinal outputs originating in the ON pathway, and those outputs are shaped by a thresholding nonlinearity that limits access to individual single-photon responses (see Fig. 1b). However, linking human visual perception to retinal ON and OFF pathway function requires a different approach than in mice, where genetic manipulations could be used to manipulate ON and OFF pathway sensitivities. Instead, we here test the classical model vs. the new model by comparing detection sensitivity – the ability to detect dim light flashes – with discrimination sensitivity – the ability to identify which of two flashes is brighter. Comparison of retinal and psychophysical performance in these two tasks in a two-alternative forced choice setting (2AFC) supports the new model in which the brain has access only to signals resulting from absorption of multiple photons. Further, our results suggest that behavioral detection of light increments in humans relies on the retinal ON pathway, which enables discrimination performance approaching the fundamental limits of physics.
Results
The “classical” and the “new” model perform differently on the discrimination task
Figure 1c–h illustrate predicted 2AFC task performance for linear and nonlinear models. Figure 1c shows the response (signal + noise) distributions for three flash strengths assuming linear processing. Figure 1d applies a threshold to these distributions. For a linear system, detection of a flash in darkness (blue trace, Fig. 1e) is always easier than discrimination between two flashes with a corresponding difference in flash strength (red trace, Fig. 1e). This is because Poisson distributions arising from the photon statistics of light get wider as the flash strength increases, causing their overlap to increase for a fixed difference in flash strength (Fig. 1c). Elimination of small responses by nonlinear processing, however, can lower sensitivity in the detection task without affecting the ability to discriminate between supra-threshold responses. This can permit discrimination of a difference in strength between two dim flashes that is smaller than the detection threshold (Fig. 1f). This phenomenon creates a dip when the just noticeable intensity difference (ΔIJND), i.e., the increment in flash strength (Iprobe – Iref) corresponding to 75% correct, is plotted as a function of the intensity of the reference flash (Fig. 1h). Such dips have been reported in high-light-level contrast discrimination tasks for retinal ganglion cells17, and their mechanistic origin could be understood in the context of nonlinear subunits in their receptive fields18–22. Similarly, dips have been previously reported for human psychophysics in various stimulus conditions23–25. This dip does not occur for a linear system with additive or multiplicative noise, where the function instead exhibits a monotonic increase (see Fig. 1g and Supplementary Fig. 3). The dip therefore is an identifying feature of a nonlinear system.
ON parasols perform better than OFF parasols on the discrimination task
We start by describing the detection and discrimination performance of primate ON and OFF parasol (magnocellular-projecting) RGCs. ON and OFF parasol RGCs are likely of direct relevance for human psychophysics at its sensitivity limit due to their high contrast sensitivity and abundant rod input (see Ala-Laurila and Rieke10). Primate ON and OFF parasol RGCs are also the closest functional15 and genetic26 homologs of ON and OFF alpha RGCs in mice: primate parasols being the closest genetic homologs of the transient alpha RGCs in mice and primate midget RGCs being the closest genetic homologs of the sustained alpha RGCs in mice. ON and OFF alpha RGCs are also the most sensitive RGCs among all types in the mouse retina11. ON and OFF parasols are thereby good proxies among primate RGC types for highly sensitive readouts of the rod bipolar pathway10,15.
We recorded spiking activity (cell-attached) of dark-adapted ON and OFF parasol RGCs (Fig. 2a inset) in response to sequences of dim flashes (Fig. 2a, b). We then used an ideal observer analysis to quantify detection (Fig. 2c, d; blue symbols) and discrimination performance (Fig. 2c, d, red symbols). As in Fig. 1, we defined performance as the light intensity difference (ΔIJND) needed to distinguish the brighter probe flash from the dimmer reference flash (Iref, which was 0, i.e. darkness, in the detection task) in 75% of the trials.
ON and OFF parasol RGC performance differed fundamentally. OFF parasol RGCs had better detection performance than discrimination performance, consistent with linear processing (Fig. 2c). ON parasol RGCs, however, had better discrimination performance than detection performance (Fig. 2d), consistent with nonlinear processing. Further, the ΔIJND as a function of the reference flash intensity rose monotonically for OFF parasol responses and exhibited a clear dip for ON parasol responses (Fig. 2e, f). The ON parasol performance approached the theoretical limit set by Poisson fluctuations of flash-induced visual pigment activations in the discrimination task but not the detection task (dashed black lines in Fig. 2e, f). OFF parasol performance was worse than that of ON parasol cells for discrimination (P = 0.0007, Welch’s t-test, Cohen’s d = 2.55) but similar for detection (P = 0.15, Welch’s t-test; Cohen’s d = 0.41; blue symbols in Fig. 2e, f, see also Ala-Laurila and Rieke10).
Human perception follows the nonlinear ON parasol output with a post-retinal nonlinearity
How do the differences in detection vs discrimination found in responses of ON and OFF parasol RGCs relate to perception? To answer this question, we evaluated the psychophysical performance of dark-adapted human observers using the same 2AFC tasks. In the detection task, the observer reported whether a dim flash was present in the first or the second of two stimulus intervals (see Detection in Fig. 3a). In the discrimination task, the observer reported which of the two intervals contained a brighter flash (see Discrimination in Fig. 3a). Discrimination performance exceeded detection performance (Fig. 3b, P < 0.01, Student’s t-test, Cohen’s d > 2, for all observers, see “Methods” section), leading to a clear dip in the ΔIJND function (Fig. 3c). This was the case for all five observers, and the average discrimination performance came close to the theoretical limit (black dashed line in Fig. 3c), while the detection performance was far from it. Human performance on the discrimination task was thus superior to that of OFF parasol RGCs but slightly inferior to that of ON parasol RGCs (Fig. 3d), supporting the dependence of behavior on responses of ON rather than OFF parasol cells under these conditions and in line with earlier findings in mice11.
Although both human behavior and ON parasol RGCs exhibit nonlinear processing, the dip in the ΔIJND function for human behavior is located at higher intensities than that for ON parasol RGCs. This change in the location of the dip suggests additional thresholding beyond the retina. We tested this hypothesis by comparing two distinct models: one with a post-retinal nonlinearity in the brain (Fig. 3e, M2) and one without it (M1). These models were based on ON RGCs because their discrimination performance was better than that of OFF RGCs. The aim was to test whether a simple model could link human psychophysics at the sensitivity limit of vision to retinal RGC outputs. The retinal components of both models are identical, with single-photon signals and noise (additive and multiplicative) being summed and thresholded by retinal subunits corresponding to ON-cone bipolar cells, as in Ala-Laurila and Rieke10. The retinal nonlinearity is followed by a second nonlinearity in M2, whereas M1 has a linear readout of the retina. For both models, we first fitted (least squares) the number of subunits (n), the additive and multiplicative noise (N), and the threshold (θ1) to describe the average performance of the ON parasols (Fig. 3e; M1), leading to n = 3 and θ1 = 2, close to earlier estimates10. This shows that a coincidence of a minimum of two single-photon responses in a subunit of ~2000 rods is required to create the response properties of ON parasol ganglion cells (see “Methods” section). Next, we tested if access to more than a single RGC could explain the difference between behavior and ON parasol responses, since the retinal stimulus size in psychophysics corresponds to ~2 times the RGC receptive field. Increasing the number of subunits, as demonstrated by doubling their number from 3 to 6 (M1, n = 6, see Fig. 3e) only shifted the curve upward in M1 but did not increase the dip. However, adding the second nonlinearity as in M2, resulted in a good fit to the human behavioral data (Fig. 3e; M2, n = 6, θ1 = 2, θ2 = 2; for model robustness, see Supplementary Fig. 4). This result is consistent with additional downstream thresholding of ON parasol signals in the brain at the sensitivity limit of vision.
Discussion
Our results provide three key insights related to the mechanistic and functional underpinnings of human vision at the lowest light levels. First, similar to previous results in mice11, our results support the hypothesis that human behavior at absolute threshold relies on retinal output signals provided by ON RGCs. This is true even when the sensitivity of OFF RGC responses is comparable to that of ON RGC responses. This suggests that, at least at visual threshold, increases in retinal firing rates (e.g., ON RGC responses to light increments) are more effective than decreases (OFF RGC responses to increments) in eliciting cortical responses and behavior. Indeed, earlier results on primates at photopic light levels suggest that cortical responses to light increments originate in ON RGCs and those to light decrements originate in OFF RGCs27. This distribution of labor between the retinal ON and OFF pathways in driving cortical responses will be important to consider in attempts to restore visual signals in visually impaired patients. However, it still remains to be seen if behavioral detection of the weakest light decrements in humans relies on the retinal OFF pathway, as our recent results on mice suggest12. Such a study on humans will have to consider the fundamental limit set by the intensity of background light for generating quantal shadow stimuli (the dimmest light decrements), as shown previously12.
Second, our results answer the decades-old question about whether humans can perceive a single photon. A recent study5 using a single-photon light source reported that humans can detect even a single photon. Two issues make this conclusion uncertain: (1) The probability of detection was very low, and only marginally statistically significant, (2) the paper does not provide a model that accounts for performance in both the single-photon detection task and the classical psychophysics task, which spans flash strengths in which detection performance ranges from chance to the near-perfect level. We have repeated near-identical experiments in an ongoing study (Tiihonen et al. in preparation) using both single-photon and conventional light sources; we used more trials and more near-threshold flash strengths than the previous study so that we could resolve small differences from chance performance. Human performance in these experiments did not exceed chance levels in the single-photon detection task and the full set of results were well described by the new model presented here (Fig. 1b).
Our results here indicate that responses to individual photons in the retina sum nonlinearly, providing evidence for the perceptual relevance of detecting coincident photons3. Thus, the biological circuit design appears to be optimized for extraordinary discrimination of small light intensity differences as evidenced by the nonlinear ON pathway as compared to the linear OFF pathway. Even if this nonlinear processing strategy leads to a loss of single-photon responses, this loss is almost fully compensated by the elimination of neural noise. This is evident from the observation that the nonlinear ON pathway and the linear OFF pathway perform almost equally well on a light detection task despite extra single-photon losses in the ON pathway (see also Ala-Laurila and Rieke10). Thus, visual perception of single photons has likely not been the central optimization goal during evolution. Instead, human vision approaches the theoretical limit of physics in discriminating dim flash intensities by utilizing a strategy where neural noise is eliminated at the expense of single-photon responses to optimize contrast coding in the retinal ON pathway.
Third, single-photon responses are separated from noise by thresholding nonlinearities in retinal circuits. The first synapse of the rod bipolar pathway thresholds rod signals to eliminate neural noise and many single-photon responses28. Since individual rods very rarely absorb overlapping photons for just-detectable inputs, this first nonlinearity functions as a linear loss mechanism for single-photon responses that is shared for ON and OFF pathways. The last synapse along the ON pathway, on the other hand, requires a coincidence of two or more single-photon responses within a collection of several thousand rods10, and this nonlinearity shapes retinal output signals. We add to this picture here by identifying an additional nonlinearity operating in a similar manner downstream of the retina. Such a post-retinal thresholding mechanism in the brain is similar to previous findings of nonlinear signal processing in thalamic neurons at higher light levels29–31. Together, these results show that a key neural strategy of high-fidelity coding of sparse signals is implemented by a combination of thresholding mechanisms operating at different levels of convergence of the neural circuits together with a large amount of pooling. It is likely that this general strategy and architecture are in use across other circuits and sensory modalities requiring sensitivity that approaches the limits of physics.
Methods
Data collection: retinal ganglion cells
Primate retinas were used as a proxy for human retinas since harvesting their dark-adapted ex vivo retinas at the uttermost sensitive conditions is more feasible than human retinas. It has also been recently shown that the spatiotemporal response properties of human and non-human primate ON and OFF parasol cells are very similar32. All recordings were from ON or OFF parasol retinal ganglion cells in the primate Macaca nemestrina (n = 11; 10 females and 1 male) and Macaca fascicularis (n = 7; 2 females and 5 males) retina (eccentricity > 30°) at the age of 4–21 years. Sex was considered in the study design to the extent that tissue was acquired from both female and male animals. Data were acquired with the Symphony data acquisitions system (https://symphony-das.github.io/) using a Multiclamp 700B amplifier and ITC-18 A/D board. The stimuli consisted of 10–20 ms flashes (5–12 different intensities and 20–140 repetitions per intensity) of a uniform circular light spot (diameter 560 μm), delivered from blue and/or green light-emitting diodes (LEDs; peak wavelength at 460 nm and 510 nm). Visual stimuli were generated using the Stage software package (https://stage-vss.github.io/). The procedures have previously been reported10. All experiments were done in accordance with the guidelines for the care and use of animals at the University of Washington and all procedures were approved by the University of Washington Animal Care and Use Committee.
Data collection: psychophysics
Observers
Five observers (three males (O1, O3 & O4), and two females (O2 & O5), aged 19–29 years) participated in the study. Sex was considered in the study design to the extent that both (self-reported) female and male participants were recruited. Observer O1 was one of the authors; the rest were naïve to the purposes of the study and received a small monetary compensation. All observers had normal uncorrected vision. This was validated by an ophthalmologist testing all observers for visual acuity and visual field sensitivity. All observers had normal monocular visual acuity (range 20/25–20/16) and no significant visual field defects within the eccentricity (<20°) were observed in a perimetry visual field test (Octopus 900, Haag-Streit Diagnostics, Switzerland). The study was conducted in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki and the guidelines of the University of Helsinki ethical review board, which also approved the study. The participants signed a written informed consent.
Stimuli
The stimulus was a homogeneous circular light spot (diameter 1.17°≈315 μm on the retina; = 500 nm) presented for 20 ms at 18° eccentricity in the lower visual field (superior retina). The fixation stimulus was a dim red ( = 680 nm) cross-hair with a diameter of 0.59° (157.5 μm) and a bar width of 0.098° (26.2 μm). The viewing distance was 58.5 cm, which is close to the expected dark focus distance for young adults33.
Apparatus
The stimulus was produced by a combination of an LED (AND520HB, = 466 nm), an interference filter (Edmund Optics, = 500 nm, FWHM 10 nm), ND filters, a 0.8 mm aperture (to produce a more point-like source), a diffuser, and a circular aperture. The intensity of the LED was adjusted at the beginning of each experimental session with ND filters to produce a suitable stimulus range. During the experimental sessions, the light intensity and the flash duration were set using a National Instruments USB-6343 (National Instruments, Austin, TX, USA) DAQ and a custom LED controller. Nonlinearities in the stimulation system were corrected for by measuring the system’s input-output function with a UDT Instruments S471 optometer and a UDT 268 R Photodiode (Gamma Scientific, San Diego, CA, USA), and by applying the inverse function to the intensity range. The fixation stimulus was produced by a combination of an LED (AND180HRP), an interference filter ( = 680 nm, FWHM 10 nm), and a cross-hair-shaped aperture. Data acquisition and stimulus delivery were controlled using MATLAB (version: R2013a) and the Data Acquisition Toolbox.
Procedure
The threshold for discriminating between two weak flashes or a flash and darkness (detection) was measured with a 2AFC method of constant stimuli procedure. Each trial proceeded as follows (see Fig. 3a): The fixation stimulus blinked (i.e., turned off for 100 ms) four times, with a 500 ms period. A flash was presented at 18° eccentricity on the third and fourth fixation blinks, and the observer’s task was to indicate during which blink the flash was stronger. For each run of trials, one of the flashes always had the same reference intensity (), whereas the other flashes had intensities within a predetermined range (7 steps), with the lowest intensity always being identical to the reference intensity. Consequently, when the reference intensity was 0, one of the flashes was always blank. The purpose of the four fixation blinks was to reduce temporal uncertainty.
One measurement session lasted approximately 1.5–2 h. In the beginning, the observer adapted to total darkness for 30 min. Then the observer performed about 500 trials at their own pace. The length of the session and the precise number of trials depended on the observer’s pace and the level of subjectively perceived fatigue. Each data point in psychometric functions (such as shown in Fig. 3b) represents 144–228 trials for = 0 and 74–140 trials for > 0. The condition = 0 was recorded twice for each observer, both at the beginning and at the end of the study, to reveal possible learning effects. The largest difference between the thresholds from the two measurements was 0.04 log units, which is negligible in comparison to the effect of reference intensity. The data from the two detection measurements has thus been pooled.
Light intensity conversions
In order to compare stimulus intensities between RGC measurements and psychophysics, stimulus intensities were first converted into photoisomerizations per rod per second (R*/rod/s) and later to photoisomerizations per RGC (R*/RGC) or per retina (R*).
Psychophysics
For psychophysics data, the initial conversion to R*/rod/s was done as follows. Firstly, the stimulus power (), measured with a photodiode, was converted to a corneal photon flux density () as:
1 |
where is the wavelength of the stimulus light (500 nm), is Planck’s constant, is the speed of light, and is the area of the photodiode (1 cm2). Secondly, the corneal photon flux density was converted to a retinal photon flux density () by:
2 |
where is the area of the pupil, the area of the projected stimulus on the retina (0.077 mm2), and is the ocular media factor (about 45%9). was measured from video recordings of the dark-adapted observers (44.6, 56.4, 32.5, 53.2, and 49.5 mm2 for O1, O2, O3, O4, and O5, respectively) while they fixated on the fixation stimulus in darkness (infrared illumination), whereas was computed from the stimulus size in visual angles (diameter 1.17°) using the conversion factor from visual angle to retinal subtense at 18° eccentricity (268 μm per °)34. Lastly, the photoisomerization rate per rod () for human observers was obtained as:
3 |
where is the collecting area of rods, obtained from:
4 |
where is the diameter of the rod outer segments (2.27 μm at 18°)35, is the length of the rod outer segment (42 μm)36, is the specific absorbance (0.019 μm−1)37, and is the quantum efficiency of rhodopsin (0.67)38. The values above resulted in a collecting area for human rods of 2.28 μm2.
Stimulus intensity in R*
Flash intensities (I) for the psychophysics data are given in R* (per retina). These values were obtained by multiplying the photoisomerization rate per rod () by the stimulus duration (20 ms) and the total number of rods (10,325) beneath the area covered by the stimulus (rod density of 134,000 rods/mm2 at 18°)35.
Ganglion cell recordings
The intensities used in ganglion cell recordings were converted to photoisomerization rates per rod based on the spectral output of the LEDs, the spectral sensitivity of rods, and a collecting area () of 1.40 μm2 ( = 2 μm and = 25 μm)38.
Stimulus intensity in R* per RGC
Flash intensities () for RGCs are given in photoisomerizations per ganglion cell (R*/RGC). These values were determined from the photoisomerization rates () as:
5 |
where is the number of rods converging on the ganglion cell and is the stimulus duration (20 ms). was estimated from the size of the spatial receptive field (RF) by multiplying the effective area (integral over the areas of Gaussian weighted annuli up to the size of the stimulus) with the rod density. The size of the spatial RF, in turn, was quantified by presenting dim spots of various sizes for 250 ms and by fitting (least squares) a Gaussian-shaped RF to map the spot size to observed responses (i.e., assuming a Gaussian weighted linear spatial summation). The size of the spatial RF (, where = standard deviation of the Gaussian) was always found to extend beyond the imaged dendritic tree (see Supplementary Fig. 1). This observation is in line with the notion of a spatial spread of rod signals in the rod bipolar pathway as well as with previous literature showing ~2–3 times larger physiological RFs as compared to the morphological RFs at scotopic light levels39. We quantified the scaling factor between the size of the spatial RF and the dendritic tree by fitting an ellipse to the edge of the dendritic tree and by comparing the mean axis length of the ellipse to the size of the Gaussian-shaped RF. The scaling factor was not significantly different between ON and OFF parasols (P = 0.36, Welch’s t-test; Cohen’s d = 0.61), and we pooled all data to get a joint mean scaling factor of 2.1. The final estimate of was thus obtained by combining the scaling factor, the size of the dendritic tree (diameter: 190 μm at 30°)40, the rod density (110,000 rods/mm2 at 30°)41, and the stimuli size (560 μm). This resulted in a final estimate of 6880 rods per RGC. The given values at 30° are not representative for all recorded RGCs, but the estimate of is: the convergence remains fixed as the rod density and dendritic tree size change with increasing eccentricity42.
Data analysis
We quantified discrimination performance in the retina using an ideal observer model that considered both the light response and the tonic firing rate when performing a 2AFC task10,43. The recorded spike response was condensed into a single number (r) for each epoch by computing the inner product of the binned (10 ms) spike counts with a discriminator over a 500 ms long interval. The discriminator was defined as the mean response difference to the reference intensity () and all higher intensities. The ideal observer evaluated the discriminability as a function of flash intensity by comparing the response distribution (the r values from all epochs) to a reference intensity () with those obtained at higher intensities (). Performance on the detection task thus corresponded to the special case where equaled zero, and for this special case, the responses were calculated from spontaneous activity (ON RGCs: 1.0 ± 0.3 Hz; OFF RGCs: 22.6 ± 3.6 Hz). The fraction of correct choices by the ideal observer was evaluated as:
6 |
where r denotes the response. The just noticeable difference was taken as the increase in flash intensity () that corresponded to . This value was obtained by fitting (least squares) a modified sigmoidal Hill function to the data and by determining the intensity where the fit reached 75% (see Fig. 2c, d). The Hill function was defined as:
7 |
where is the chance level (=½), is the best possible performance (≤1.0), I is the stimulus intensity, is the intensity at , and is the slope. For the ganglion cell data, the number of available data points decreased for each consecutively higher reference intensity, as responses were only available from a fixed number of flash intensities. We solved this by modeling additional response distributions for intermediate flash intensities using normal distributions with the mean and variance fixed to interpolated mean and variance values from the real data (see Supplementary Fig. 2). The modeled response distributions were always interpolated between the reference intensity and the subsequent intensity, and the number of modeled distributions was determined so that the Hill functions could be fit to at least five data points. The psychophysics data was analyzed in an equivalent manner, with the Hill functions fit to the response probabilities directly (see Fig. 3b). Ganglion cells were included in the main analyses if they fulfilled similar sensitivity criteria as established earlier10: RGCs had to generate an average spike difference of four to five spikes in response to a brief flash producing 0.001–0.002 R∗/rod as compared to the baseline firing rate.
Modeling
RGC and psychophysics dippers (Fig. 2f and Fig. 3c) were modeled (Fig. 3e) by mapping discrete input events (isomerizations) to discrete outputs (responses) using a subunit model. Thus, this model is not meant to capture the time course of responses but instead focuses on the impact of thresholding nonlinearities. The model considers a single set of excitatory subunits, as we have shown previously that the excitatory synaptic inputs and the spike outputs of ON parasol RGCs share similar sensitivity and thresholding nonlinearities10. The stimulus (signal) was distributed uniformly over all subunits and the responses were analyzed in the same way as the recorded data, the only difference being that the values could be calculated from the modeled output distributions directly. These output distributions were obtained by (1) pooling (adding) Poisson distributed signals and noise within each subunit, (2) passing the result through a rectifying nonlinearity, (3) summing up the contribution from each subunit, (4) including multiplicative noise, and (5) by thresholding the sum. Summation of the outputs from each subunit was implemented via convolutions, and the multiplicative noise was included by letting the summed subunit output set the mean response for a sequential Poisson process via a gain term (see Field et al.7). In total, the model thus had five parameters: the magnitude of the additive noise, the subunit nonlinearity’s threshold, the number of subunits, the gain parameter for the multiplicative noise, and finally, the threshold for the post-retinal nonlinearity. In all cases, the optimal values for free parameters were found by minimizing the mean squared error between the model’s dipper function and the target dipper function (from RGC or psychophysics data). The effects of varying each parameter separately are shown in Fig. 3e (number of subunits) and in Supplementary Fig. 3 (remaining parameters). Supplementary Fig. 4 shows the model robustness when changing each key model parameter either by doubling or halving their values. The main conclusions of the paper – namely that the RGC dataset and psychophysical datasets can be bridged within the “new model” (M2) and not within the “classical” model (linear retina model) – are robust across any reasonable parameter space.
Theoretical limit
The theoretical limit (dotted line in figures) corresponds to the modeled output when no noise or thresholds are included. That is, it corresponds to linear processing in the absence of noise and thus reflects ΔIJND for detection and discrimination due to inherent Poisson fluctuations in the stimulus.
Statistical analysis
Reported values are mean ± SEM. MATLAB (version: R2017b) was used for all statistical analyses, and all reported tests were two-tailed. The statistical significance and effect size of the ON-OFF detection and discrimination threshold difference were determined by conducting a Welch’s t-test (and Cohen’s d) on the cells’ average detection and discrimination thresholds (averaged over all Iref values). The statistical significance and effect size of the psychophysics dips were determined separately for each observer by conducting a Student’s t-test (with Bonferroni correction) and Cohen’s d on the K parameters (75% correct) of the Hill functions fitted to the detection and discrimination data (solid lines in Fig. 3b). For observers O1, O2, O3, O4, and O5, respectively, the most significant t-score was 13.72, 17.98, 4.21, 12.12, 11.19; P-values <0.001, <0.001, 0.007, <0.001, <0.001; Cohen’s d -values 7.33, 9.61, 2.25, 6.48, 5.98. Pooled SD was used in calculating Cohen’s d.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Supplementary information
Source data
Acknowledgements
We thank Drs. Kristian Donner, Greg Field, Kai Kaila, Gabriel Peinado Allina, Greg Schwartz, and Mark Georgeson for excellent comments on the manuscript; Matthew Dunkerley, Sathish Narayanan, and Mark Cafaro for the design of the data acquisition software. Support was provided by the Academy of Finland (296269 to P.A.-L.); the Aalto Brain Centre (J.W.); Svenska kulturfonden (J.W.); The Finnish Society of Sciences and Letters (J.W.); The Ella and Georg Ehrnrooth Foundation (M.K.); Aalto Centre for Quantum Engineering (CQE) grant (J.T.); University of Helsinki Brain & Mind Doctoral Programme funded position (J.T.) and the NIH (EY 028111 to F.R.). Human silhouette in Fig. 3 and Supplementary Fig. 2 retrieved from Vecteezy.com. We thank Juha Haapala for preparing the monkey silhouette used in Figs. 2, and 3.
Author contributions
F.R., J.W., M.K., P.A.-L. designed experiments, F.R. and P.A.-L. collected RGC data, M.K., A.L., J.T. collected psychophysics data, F.R., P.A.-L., J.W., M.K., D.T., J.T. analyzed data, F.R., J.W., M.K. & P.A.-L. wrote the paper.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Communications thanks Rebecca Holmes, Suva Roy, and the other, anonymous, reviewer(s) for their contribution to the peer review of this work. A peer review file is available.
Data availability
All data generated and analyzed in this study are available in the Figshare database: 10.6084/m9.figshare.25737099. Source data are provided with this paper.
Code availability
For the code, see the Zenodo repository: 10.5281/zenodo.10459936.
Competing interests
M.K., J.W., J.T., A.L., D.T., F.R. declare no competing interests. P.A.-L. is a founder and shareholder of Quantal Vision Technologies.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41467-024-48750-y.
References
- 1.de Vries HL. The quantum character of light and its bearing upon threshold of vision, the differential sensitivity and visual acuity of the eye. Physica. 1943;10:553–564. doi: 10.1016/S0031-8914(43)90575-0. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Hecht S, Shlaer S, Pirenne MH. Energy, quanta, and vision. J. Gen. Physiol. 1942;25:819–840. doi: 10.1085/jgp.25.6.819. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Van der Velden HA. Over het aantal lichtquanta dat nodig is voor een lichtprikkel bij het menselijk oog. Physica. 1944;11:179–189. doi: 10.1016/S0031-8914(44)80012-X. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Sakitt B. Counting every quantum. J. Physiol. 1972;223:131–150. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1972.sp009838. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Tinsley JN, et al. Direct detection of a single photon by humans. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:12172. doi: 10.1038/ncomms12172. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Donner K. Noise and the absolute thresholds of cone and rod vision. Vis. Res. 1992;32:853–866. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(92)90028-H. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Field GD, Sampath AP, Rieke F. Retinal processing near absolute threshold: from behavior to mechanism. Ann. Rev. Physiol. 2005;67:491–514. doi: 10.1146/annurev.physiol.67.031103.151256. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Kiani, R., Ala-Laurila, P. & Rieke, F. in The Senses: A Comprehensive Reference 293–308 (Elsevier, 2020).
- 9.Nelson PC. Old and new results about single-photon sensitivity in human vision. Phys. Biol. 2016;13:025001. doi: 10.1088/1478-3975/13/2/025001. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Ala-Laurila P, Rieke F. Coincidence detection of single-photon responses in the inner retina at the sensitivity limit of vision. Curr. Biol. 2014;24:2888–2898. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2014.10.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Smeds L, et al. Paradoxical rules of spike train decoding revealed at the sensitivity limit of vision. Neuron. 2019;104:576–587.e11. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.08.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Westö J, et al. Retinal OFF ganglion cells allow detection of quantal shadows at starlight. Curr. Biol. 2022;32:2848–2857.e6. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2022.04.092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Bloomfield SA, Dacheux RF. Rod vision: pathways and processing in the mammalian retina. Prog. Retin. Eye. Res. 2001;20:351–384. doi: 10.1016/S1350-9462(00)00031-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Grimes WN, Baudin J, Azevedo AW, Rieke F. Range, routing and kinetics of rod signaling in primate retina. Elife. 2018;7:e38281. doi: 10.7554/eLife.38281. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Takeshita D, Smeds L, Ala-Laurila P. Processing of single-photon responses in the mammalian On and Off retinal pathways at the sensitivity limit of vision. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. B. 2017;372:20160073. doi: 10.1098/rstb.2016.0073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Sterling P, Freed MA, Smith RG. Architecture of rod and cone circuits to the on-beta ganglion cell. J. Neurosci. 1988;8:623–642. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.08-02-00623.1988. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Dhingra NK, Kao YH, Sterling P, Smith RG. Contrast threshold of a brisk-transient ganglion cell in vitro. J. Neurophysiol. 2003;89:2360–2369. doi: 10.1152/jn.01042.2002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Crook JD, et al. Y-cell receptive field and collicular projection of parasol ganglion cells in macaque monkey retina. J. Neurosci. 2008;28:11277–11291. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2982-08.2008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Demb JB, Zaghloul K, Haarsma L, Sterling P. Bipolar cells contribute to nonlinear spatial summation in the brisk-transient (Y) ganglion cell in mammalian retina. J. Neurosci. 2001;21:7447–7454. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.21-19-07447.2001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Hochstein S, Shapley RM. Linear and nonlinear spatial subunits in Y cat retinal ganglion cells. J. Physiol. 1976;262:265–284. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011595. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Schwartz GW, et al. The spatial structure of a nonlinear receptive field. Nat. Neurosci. 2012;15:1572–1580. doi: 10.1038/nn.3225. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Shapley RM, Victor JD. Nonlinear spatial summation and the contrast gain control of cat retinal ganglion cells. J. Physiol. 1979;290:141–161. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1979.sp012765. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Cornsweet TN, Pinsker HM. Luminance discrimination of brief flashes under various conditions of adaptation. J. Physiol. 1965;176:294–310. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1965.sp007551. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Legge GE, Foley JM. Contrast masking in human vision. J. Opt. Soc. Am. 1980;70:1458–1471. doi: 10.1364/JOSA.70.001458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Nachmias J, Sansbury RV. Letter: grating contrast: discrimination may be better than detection. Vis. Res. 1974;14:1039–1042. doi: 10.1016/0042-6989(74)90175-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hahn J, et al. Evolution of neuronal cell classes and types in the vertebrate retina. Nature. 2023;624:415–424. doi: 10.1038/s41586-023-06638-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Schiller PH, Sandell JH, Maunsell JH. Functions of the ON and OFF channels of the visual system. Nature. 1986;322:824–825. doi: 10.1038/322824a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Field GD, Rieke F. Nonlinear signal transfer from mouse rods to bipolar cells and implications for visual sensitivity. Neuron. 2002;34:773–785. doi: 10.1016/S0896-6273(02)00700-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Carandini M, Horton JC, Sincich LC. Thalamic filtering of retinal spike trains by postsynaptic summation. J. Vis. 2007;7:20. doi: 10.1167/7.14.20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Kremkow J, et al. Neuronal nonlinearity explains greater visual spatial resolution for darks than lights. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. Biol. 2014;111:3170–3175. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1310442111. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Lee BB, Virsu V, Creutzfeldt OD. Linear signal transmission from prepotentials to cells in the macaque lateral geniculate nucleus. Exp. Brain Res. 1983;52:50–56. doi: 10.1007/BF00237148. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Soto F, et al. Efficient coding by midget and parasol ganglion cells in the human retina. Neuron. 2020;107:656–666.e5. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2020.05.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Leibowitz HW, Owens DA. New evidence for the intermediate position of relaxed accommodation. Doc. Ophthalmol. 1978;46:133–147. doi: 10.1007/BF00174103. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Watson AB. A formula for human retinal ganglion cell receptive field density as a function of visual field location. J. Vis. 2014;14:15. doi: 10.1167/14.7.15. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Curcio CA, Millican CL, Allen KA, Kalina RE. Aging of the human photoreceptor mosaic: evidence for selective vulnerability of rods in central retina. Invest. Ophthalmol. Vis. Sci. 1993;34:3278–3296. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hendrickson A, Drucker D. The development of parafoveal and mid-peripheral human retina. Behav. Brain Res. 1992;49:21–31. doi: 10.1016/S0166-4328(05)80191-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Bowmaker JK, Dartnall HJ, Lythgoe JN, Mollon JD. The visual pigments of rods and cones in the rhesus monkey, Macaca mulatta. J. Physiol. 1978;274:329–348. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1978.sp012151. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Baylor DA, Nunn BJ, Schnapf JL. The photocurrent, noise and spectral sensitivity of rods of the monkey Macaca fascicularis. J. Physiol. 1984;357:575–607. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1984.sp015518. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Dacey D, et al. Center surround receptive field structure of cone bipolar cells in primate retina. Vis. Res. 2000;40:1801–1811. doi: 10.1016/S0042-6989(00)00039-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Watanabe M, Rodieck RW. Parasol and midget ganglion cells of the primate retina. J. Comp. Neurol. 1989;289:434–454. doi: 10.1002/cne.902890308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Packer O, Hendrickson AE, Curcio CA. Photoreceptor topography of the retina in the adult pigtail macaque (Macaca nemestrina) J. Comp. Neurol. 1989;288:165–183. doi: 10.1002/cne.902880113. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Goodchild AK, Ghosh KK, Martin PR. Comparison of photoreceptor spatial density and ganglion cell morphology in the retina of human, macaque monkey, cat, and the marmoset Callithrix jacchus. J. Comp. Neurol. 1996;366:55–75. doi: 10.1002/(SICI)1096-9861(19960226)366:1<55::AID-CNE5>3.0.CO;2-J. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Chichilnisky EJ, Rieke F. Detection sensitivity and temporal resolution of visual signals near absolute threshold in the salamander retina. J. Neurosci. 2005;25:318–330. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2339-04.2005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All data generated and analyzed in this study are available in the Figshare database: 10.6084/m9.figshare.25737099. Source data are provided with this paper.
For the code, see the Zenodo repository: 10.5281/zenodo.10459936.