Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2024 May 29;19(5):e0303562. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0303562

Investigation of preference for local and global processing of Capuchin-monkeys (Sapajus spp.) in shape discrimination of mosaic arrangements

Fernanda Mendes 1,2,*, Ana Leda de Faria Brino 1, Paulo Roney Kilpp Goulart 1, Olavo de Faria Galvão 1, Dora Selma Fix Ventura 3, Letícia Miquilini 1, Felipe André da Costa Brito 4, Givago Silva Souza 2,4
Editor: Carlos Tomaz5
PMCID: PMC11135723  PMID: 38809944

Abstract

Classical experiments using hierarchical stimuli to investigate the ability of capuchin monkeys to integrate visual information based on global or local clues reported findings suggesting a behavioral preference for local information of the image. Many experiments using mosaics have been conducted with capuchin monkeys to identify some of their perceptual phenotypes. As the identification of an image in a mosaic demands the integration of elements that share some visual features, we evaluated the discrimination of shapes presented in solid and mosaic stimuli in capuchin monkeys. Shape discrimination performance was tested in 2 male adult capuchin monkeys in an experimental chamber with a touchscreen video monitor, in three experiments: (i) evaluation of global and local processing using hierarchical stimuli; (ii) evaluation of target detection using simple discrimination procedures; (iii) evaluation of shape discrimination using simple discrimination and delayed matching-to-sample procedures. We observed that both monkeys had preferences for local processing when tested by hierarchical stimuli. Additionally, detection performance for solid and mosaic targets was highly significant, but for shape discrimination tasks we found significant performance when using solid figures, non-significant performance when using circle and square shapes in mosaic stimuli, and significant performance when using Letter X and Number 8 shapes in mosaic stimuli. Our results are suggestive that the monkeys respond to local contrast and partly to global contrast in mosaic stimuli.

Introduction

Research on perceptual grouping has extensively examined two levels of hierarchical processing, centering on the relative primacy of global features versus local features within a scene [16]. Global processing involves integrating various elements into a coherent whole, while local processing concentrates on the finer details of a prominent object or situation [7, 8].

In his research, Navon8 examined the role of global and local cues in the perceptual organization of images in humans, utilizing hierarchical stimuli where larger global shapes, often letters, were formed by smaller local elements. These global and local shapes could either be identical (consistent stimulus) or differ (inconsistent stimulus). His findings demonstrated that human perception exhibited a global advantage or precedence, meaning that individuals tended to perceive the overall global structure before discerning the specific local elements constituting the scene. Examples of hierarchical stimuli are shown in the Fig 1.

Fig 1. Hierarchical and mosaic stimulus.

Fig 1

Hierarchical stimuli have identical and different global and local clues that can be used for visual processing. Mosaic stimuli is composed of non-overlapping elements and a subgroup of the mosaic elements shares some feature (in the case of the figure the shared feature is the chromaticity) that creates an illusory target over a background.

Numerous studies exploring perceptual grouping processes with visual stimuli reveal variations in the precedence of global or local cues across different species, including humans [911], chimpanzees [3, 11, 12], baboons [10], rhesus monkeys [12], cotton top tamarins [13], and capuchin monkeys [6, 9, 14]. In the context of platyrrhine monkeys, investigations into global and local processing have predominantly focused on capuchin monkeys [1, 5, 6, 9, 1419]. Several of these experiments utilized matching-to-sample (MTS) procedures, and their outcomes consistently demonstrated an advantage for local level of hierarchical processing [1, 14, 16, 18, 19].

In recent years, researchers have conducted studies to investigate the chromatic discrimination abilities of platyrrhine monkeys using a stimulus named pseudoisochromatic [20, 21], which is formed by a set of non-overlapped individual elements arranged as a mosaic of circular patches. A subgroup of these elements shared the same color shaping a visual target over the remained elements that had different color compared to the target in region of the stimulus named background. Although there is no overlapping in the different elements from target of the mosaic, there is a construction of illusory object because the similarity of colors they share (Fig 1). For instance, studies have been conducted with squirrel monkeys (Saimiri spp.: [22]), capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.: [20,21]), and howler monkeys (Alouatta spp.: [23]) using mosaics which the target is an illusory square. In these experiments, the subjects’ successful performance necessitates identifying the chromatic target irrespective of its shape. It remains unclear whether non-human primates select the target by its identification as global object or by detecting a region of the mosaic with different color from the other parts of the mosaic.

The present study aimed to extend the question of preference for global versus local processing in capuchin monkeys by using mosaic stimuli. We assumed that the perception of the target in mosaic stimuli would represent the end product of global processing and that shape discrimination would be an appropriate method to investigate the emergence of the illusory target from mosaics. The absence of shape discrimination favored the hypothesis of a preference for local processing in these monkeys. For that, we examined both local and global processing through hierarchical stimuli as a control experiment, as well as target detection involving solid and mosaic stimuli.

Methods

Subjects

Two adult male capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.), namely Tico and Raul, were involved in the study (Fig 2A and 2B). Both monkeys possessed significant prior experience with simple discrimination and matching-to-sample procedures, like those utilized in the present research [20, 2428]. Moreover, Tico had previously participated in experiments focused on tool use [29]. For the record, the subjects were genetically and behaviorally identified as dichromat deuteranopes, as established by previous research [20].

Fig 2.

Fig 2

Subjects Tico (A) and Raul (B), and experimental chamber (C).

They were housed in nursery cages along with one or two other Sapajus in an open area at the Experimental School for Primates, which is located at the Federal University of Pará. Tico shared his living space with another male monkey in a glass-enclosed housing measuring 45.09 m2. This glass enclosure lacked visual barriers and a roof, and it was situated next to an identical enclosure that housed two other monkeys. The two enclosures were separated by a glass wall and had partial coverage to protect the primates from direct sunlight. Raul resided with two other monkeys in a metal nursery that measured 3.00 m x 2.50 m x 2.50 m (height x width x length). The nursery rested on a cement floor and provided partial coverage against direct sunlight. All enclosures were equipped with drinking water fountains featuring automatic spouts. The primates’ diet consisted of a special ration for primates from Quimtia (Nuvilab neotropical primates, extruded food), which was distributed twice a day, once in the early morning and once in the late afternoon. Additionally, the animals received a meal at 3:00 pm daily, consisting of fruits, eggs (three times a week), and vegetables. Before the experiments the subject were kept at a certain percentage of free-feeding weight. The Experimental School for Primates holds an operating license (LO N° 12493/2021) that categorizes it as a scientific breeding ground for research purposes. This license was issued by the local environmental agency (SEMAS—Secretaria Estadual de Meio Ambiente e Sustentabilidade [State Secretariat for the Environment and Sustainability]), which is responsible for inspecting environmental management activities. Furthermore, the experimental procedures conducted on the primates received approval from the Ethics Committee on Animal Use of the Federal University of Pará (CEUA/UFPA), with protocol number #4629251121.

Apparatus

For the purpose of data collection, we utilized an experimental chamber measuring 0.60 m x 0.60 m x 0.60 m. This chamber was equipped with a 15-inch color LCD monitor (Elo Systems®, 18 inches) with a surface acoustic wave touchscreen-type device that could be accessed through a rectangular opening (0.26 m long x 0.20 m high) in the front wall of the chamber (refer to Fig 2C). The control of the experimental sessions, the pellet dispenser, and the recording of the monkey’s responses were managed by a home-made software programmed in C++. The pellet dispenser was triggered upon the occurrence of correct choices, releasing pellets into a tray positioned on the opposite side of the screen. Positioned just above the corners of the touchscreen, two small red lamps would illuminate upon a correct response. Additionally, it facilitated the generation of scheduled sessions, recorded response data, and generated a comprehensive report encompassing details such as the date, time, subject, and task performance.

Experimental procedures

To explore the impact of perceptual global and local processing on the shape discrimination capabilities of capuchin monkeys using a mosaic design, we carried out a series of three experiments: (i) evaluating the capuchins’ proficiency in processing the global and local attributes of hierarchical visual stimuli as outlined in Spinozzi et al. [18] (ii) appraising target detection through both solid and mosaic stimuli; and (iii) discerning shape discrimination utilizing stimuli presented in mosaic design as well as solid stimuli. Each subject performed one session (48 trials) per day.

We wrote a routine in Python language to calculate the statistical significance of the subject’s performance in each experiment by calculating the binomial probability, which was the probability to reach x or more successful decisions in n repeated trials in an experiment with z possible decisions and success probability in a single trial of p equal to 1/z (Eqs 1 and 2). We considered as significant number of hits in the experiment when the binomial probability was smaller than 0.05.

bp= nCx×px×(1p)nx (Eq 1)
 nCx=(n!)x!×(nx)! (Eq 2)

To facilitate the comprehension, we chose to describe the methods of each experiment followed by its results.

Experiment 1. Global and local processing evaluation

Stimuli

In the context of a delayed matching-to-sample task, we employed four hierarchical stimuli reminiscent of those utilized by Spinozzi et al. [18]. These stimuli comprised configurations spanning 4.9° of visual angle, each encompassing 8 individual circles or squares (each with a diameter or side measuring 0.95°), as illustrated in Fig 3. Depending on the interplay between shapes at the global and local levels, a stimulus was designated as either "Consistent" (characterized by congruent global and local shapes) or "Inconsistent" (marked by incongruent global and local shapes).

Fig 3. Global and local conditions with hierarchical stimuli.

Fig 3

Hierarchical stimuli were made up of eight local elements. Local elements (circles or squares) could be consistent as in A and B or inconsistent as in C and D with the global shape of the stimulus. The experiment comprised a delayed matching-to-sample test using four different configurations: global condition and consistent trial, global condition and inconsistent trial, local condition and consistent trial, and local condition and inconsistent trial are exemplified in the figure. Sample: model to search for in the next window; S+: correct choice; S-: incorrect choice.

Procedure

Four distinct configurations of delayed matching-to-sample training were executed, encompassing: (1) the global condition with consistent trials, (2) the global condition with inconsistent trials, (3) the local condition with consistent trials, and (4) the local condition with inconsistent trials, all of which are depicted in Fig 3. The animals were exposed to five sessions with global configuration trials and another five sessions with local configuration trials. The trials were repeated and there were differential consequences for success and error. Five sessions with 48 trials had global configurations, in a total of 240 trials, 120 of global consistent and 120 of global inconsistent; the same occurred for the five sessions of 48 trials having local configuration, 240 total trials, 120 local consistent and 120 local inconsistent.

The two-choice 1-second delayed matching-to-sample procedure was applied. There were only two possible stimuli. At the initiation of each trial, a sample stimulus appeared on a dark screen, prompting the monkey to initiate the task by tapping twice on the sample. A correct response from the monkey prompted a 1-second delay, followed by the presentation of one S+ (equal to the sample) and one S- stimuli (different to the sample) on a dark screen. The trial concluded upon the monkey’s one tap on any stimulus (S+ or S-). If the monkey’s tap fell upon the S+, a banana pellet reward was dispensed. The intertrial interval persisted for 6 seconds, during which the monitor screen remained dark. Touches on the dark screen during this interval held no consequences.

The trial background and other features were identical in the global and local configurations. The experimenter defined the rule related to each configuration trial and rewarded the animal whenever it followed that rule. In this study, the global trials have different comparisons in terms of the global shape, having the same local shapes, so that to choose the correct comparison, the subject had to look at the global aspect. In trials called local, the comparisons had identical global shapes and distinct local shapes so that the subject could only choose the correct one if they were responding based on local shape. The order of stimulus presentation was randomly chosen by the software in each session. The monkeys were instructed on which global and local clues to follow using a reward system that favored each condition.

The subjects were exposed to the two conditions sequentially. Firstly, Raul underwent five sessions of the global condition, while simultaneously Tico engaged in five sessions of the local condition. Subsequently, Tico underwent five training sessions in the global condition, while Raul participated in the five training sessions of the local condition. The evaluation of subjects’ performance was based on the accuracy of responses across the five sessions.

Results of the Experiment 1

Results of the global and local processing evaluation

Table 1 presents the performance data, represented as the percentage of hits (correct responses out of total trials), exhibited by both subjects during the trials involving global and local processing. Notably, we observed that both subjects demonstrated nonsignificant performance levels in the experiments pertaining to global processing, encompassing both consistent and inconsistent trials. Conversely, in the experiments related to the local condition, encompassing both consistent and inconsistent trials, the subjects displayed statistically significant performance. These results suggest a preference for local processing in capuchin monkeys.

Table 1. Performance for global and local processing from both subjects.

The statistical significance was based on probability of success = 0.5, number of trials, and number of successes. The values represent the number of successes per number of trials.

Test condition Raul p-value Tico p-value
Global condition and consistent trial 69/120 0.06 66/120 0.15
Global condition and inconsistent trial 65/120 0.2 50/120 0.97
Local condition and consistent trial 104/120 <0.001* 107/120 <0.001*
Local condition and inconsistent trial 101/120 <0.001* 102/120 <0.001*

*Significant number of successes.

Experiments 2 and 3: Assessment of stimulus target detection and shape discrimination

Two sets of experiments were conducted to elucidate the subjects’ capacity for target detection (Experiment 2) and shape discrimination utilizing both mosaic and solid stimuli (Experiment 3). To achieve this, we employed an identical set of stimuli as delineated below.

Stimuli

Mosaic stimuli were meticulously crafted within the MATLAB programming environment (R2021a, Mathworks, CA). Each stimulus comprised 1100 circles across four distinct sizes: 0.54, 0.44, 0.32, and 0.22 degrees of visual angle. The spacing between circles was deliberately randomized with no overlap among them. Conversely, solid stimuli were meticulously fashioned in commercial image editor (Microsoft Paint, Microsoft, Palo Alto, USA). In each set of stimuli (mosaic and solid sets), a diverse array of six targets—circle, square, X letter, star, musical keynote, and number eight—was incorporated, along with a background figure. For both solid and mosaic stimuli, uniform chromaticity (CIE 1976 color space: u’ = 0.219; v’ = 0.48) was upheld, ensuring high luminance contrast exceeding 90% Weber contrast.

Experiment 2: Target detection

We employed a simple discrimination procedure to assess the capability of capuchin monkeys in detecting a target (S+) among eight other stimuli (S-) arranged in a 3 x 3 grid. This procedure was conducted employing both solid and mosaic stimuli, as depicted in Fig 4. We performed this evaluation in three stages. The first stage was to evaluate the detection of six distinct solid target in a session, encompassing 8 trials for each S+ per session. This stage was completed when the subject reached minimum 90% correct in one session. The order of targets presentation was randomly chosen in each session.

Fig 4. Simple discrimination task for stimulus target detection.

Fig 4

The same procedure was carried out using solid (A) and mosaic (B) stimuli. After the stimulus presentation a touch on the S+ was considered a correct response, and a pellet was delivered to the animal, a touch on the S- was considered an incorrect response and no reward was released. The touch on S+ or S- leads to a dark screen of 6 s (intertrial interval) followed by a new stimulus for simple discrimination trial. (C) Rationale used to guide the trials.

Subsequently, in the second stage, six simple discrimination sessions were carried out, and in each session a solid target stimulus was replaced by its correspondent mosaic target stimulus while retaining the other solid stimuli. In each session, there were 8 trials per target. The order of stimulus replacement was randomly chosen by the software in each session. Finally, the third stage involved a final simple discrimination session entailed 8 trials for each target in the mosaic.

For all stages, a correct response, acknowledged with a banana pellet reward, was defined as a tap on the S+. Conversely, a tap on any S- was considered an erroneous response, resulting in no reward release. After the subject response, a dark screen was displayed for 6 seconds (intertrial interval), followed by the presentation of a new stimulus for a fresh simple discrimination trial. Notably, touching the dark screen background during the trial presentation or intertrial interval bore no consequences. The predetermined criteria for completion of the test encompassed achieving either a 90% accuracy rate in one session or concluding 10 sessions without reaching the criterion. The same order of sessions was delivered for both animals, and inside each session the order of stimulus presentation was randomly chosen by the software.

Results of the Experiment 2

Assessment of stimulus target detection

Considering all the stages, Tico carried out 48 trials per solid target stimulus and 16 trials per mosaic target stimulus, and Raul carried out 72 trials per solid target stimulus and 16 per mosaic target stimulus. The difference between the number of trials per solid target stimulus carried out by the subjects occurred because Tico and Raul completed the first stage in 1 and 3 sessions, respectively. Table 2 illustrates the performance outcomes of Raul and Tico in the context of target detection within a simple discrimination task across all the stages. Notably, both subjects exhibited significant performance levels across both solid and mosaic stimuli, thereby indicating their adeptness at detecting the target within both design formats.

Table 2. Performance for target detection from both subjects.

The statistical significance was based on probability of success = 0.11, number of trials, and number of successes. The values represent the number of successes per number of trials.

Raul Tico
Stimulus Solid p-value Mosaic p-value Solid p-value Mosaic p-value
Circle 69/72 <0.001* 16/16 <0.001* 45/48 <0.001* 15/16 <0.001*
Square 67/72 <0.001* 15/16 <0.001* 48/48 <0.001* 15/16 <0.001*
Letter X 68/72 <0.001* 15/16 <0.001* 46/48 <0.001* 16/16 <0.001*
Star 65/72 <0.001* 12/16 <0.001* 45/48 <0.001* 16/16 <0.001*
Musical keynote 68/72 <0.001* 15/16 <0.001* 47/48 <0.001* 14/16 <0.001*
Number 8 60/64 <0.001* 20/24 <0.001* 45/48 <0.001* 16/16 <0.001*

*Significant number of successes.

Experiment 3. Shape simple discrimination

The third experiment implemented a simple discrimination procedure to investigate the discernment capabilities of capuchin monkeys concerning a stimulus featuring a distinct target (S+) set apart from eight identical targets (S-) within a 3x3 array, as depicted in Fig 5. This procedure was executed employing either solid or mosaic stimuli. The methodology for assessing shape discrimination retained the same underlying rationale as applied in the target detection experiment. Initially, a shape discrimination session employing solely solid stimuli was conducted, encompassing 48 trials with 8 trials per S+. A sequence of shape simple discrimination sessions ensued until the subject had a minimum performance of 90% correct in one session. After criterion, one array featured a mosaic design were presented, one by one, with remaining five arrays retained a solid design until all six mosaics were presented. In case the subject performed 10 consecutive sessions without reach the criterion, the test would be completed. The order of targets presentation was randomly chosen by the software.

Fig 5.

Fig 5

Simple discrimination (upper lines) and matching-to-sample (lower lines) procedures used to assess shape discrimination. The same procedures were carried out using solid and mosaic stimuli.

Consistent with the preceding experiment, a tap on the S+ signified a precise response and merited a banana pellet reward, whereas a tap on the S- signified an erroneous response, resulting in no reward dispensed. In case of tap occurred on either the S+ or the S-, a dark screen emerged for 6 seconds (intertrial interval), followed by the commencement of a new shape discrimination task. Taps on the dark screen during trial presentation or the intertrial interval held no impact.

Another experiment involving the matching-to-sample paradigm was undertaken to assess shape discrimination capabilities. This procedure aimed to ascertain whether capuchin monkeys could successfully match identical shapes presented through solid or mosaic stimuli. Each session encompassed 54 trials, consisting of 9 trials for each S+. At the initiation of each trial, a sample stimulus appeared on a dark screen, prompting the monkey to initiate the task by tapping twice on the sample. A correct response from the monkey prompted a 1-second delay, followed by the presentation of one S+ and two S- stimuli on a dark screen. The trial concluded upon the monkey’s tap on any stimulus (S+ or S-). If the monkey’s tap upon the S+, a banana pellet reward was dispensed. The intertrial interval persisted for 6 seconds, during which the monitor screen remained dark. Again, taps on the dark screen during this interval held no consequences. The procedural details of this experiment are illustrated in Fig 5. The performance criterion for terminating a session was either achieving a 90% accuracy rate or the completion of 5 sessions. For all conditions, the order of stimulus presentation was randomly chosen by the software in each session.

Results of the Experiment 3

Assessment of shape discrimination: Simple discrimination procedure

Presented in Table 3 are the performance outcomes of the subjects in the context of the shape discrimination test, conducted through a simple discrimination procedure. Raul reached performance criterion (90% correct) before Tico, so the total number of trials with solid stimuli was lower for Raul (96 trials distributed in 12 sessions) than for Tico (120 trials distributed in 15 sessions). Strikingly, both subjects exhibited superior performance when faced with solid figures as stimuli, achieving significance across all stimulus targets. Conversely, most of the mosaic stimuli they had poor performances, excepting Letter X for both subjects and Musical keynote target for Tico.

Table 3. Performance for simple shape discrimination from both subjects.

The statistical significance was based on probability of success = 0.11, number of trials, and number of successes. The values represent the number of successes per number of trials.

Raul Tico
Stimulus Solid p-value Mosaic p-value Solid p-value Mosaic p-value
Circle 73/96 <0.001* 1/16 0.84 79/120 <0.001* 0/16 1
Square 90/96 <0.001* 4/16 0.09 85/120 <0.001* 0/16 1
Letter X 93/96 <0.001* 9/16 <0.001* 97/120 <0.001* 13/16 <0.001*
Star 92/96 <0.001* 4/16 0.09 99/120 <0.001* 4/16 0.09
Musical keynote 85/96 <0.001* 0/16 1 99/120 <0.001* 5/16 0.02*
Number 8 81/96 <0.001* 6/16 0.055 79/120 <0.001* 6/16 0.055

*Significant number of successes.

Assessment of shape discrimination: Matching-to-sample procedure

Displayed in Table 4 are the subjects’ performance results concerning the shape discrimination test, as conducted through the delayed matching-to-sample paradigm. Once more, both subjects showcased noteworthy performance levels when discerning shapes depicted as solid figures. Moreover, even when faced with mosaic targets, their capacity for discrimination remained low for most of the targets, but they have again significant discrimination for Letter X and Number 8 targets. Notably, during this context, Tico additionally achieved significant discrimination prowess for the Star shape.

Table 4. Performance for shape discrimination using matching-to-sample procedure from both subjects.

The statistical significance was based on probability of success = 0.33, number of trials, and number of successes. The values represent the number of successes per number of trials.

Raul Tico
Stimulus Solid p-value Mosaic p-value Solid p-value Mosaic p-value
Circle 44/45 <0.001* 15/45 0.55 44/45 <0.001* 13/45 0.78
Square 42/45 <0.001* 11/45 0.92 45/45 <0.001* 14/45 0.67
Letter X 39/45 <0.001* 30/45 <0.001* 32/45 <0.001* 22/45 0.02*
Star 45/45 <0.001* 45/125 0.29 40/45 <0.001* 56/125 0.004*
Musical keynote 41/45 <0.001* 42/125 0.5 35/45 <0.001* 50/125 0.06
Number 8 43/45 <0.001* 62/125 <0.001* 38/45 <0.001* 76/125 <0.001*

*Significant number of successes.

Discussion

The present investigation reveals that the two capuchin monkeys showcased a limited capacity to consolidate the constituents of mosaic stimuli, displaying a restrained ability to respond guided by local cues. Intriguingly, these very subjects showcased pronounced proficiency in local perceptual processing during the assessment involving hierarchical stimuli, a trend that concurs with findings in the extant literature [5].

Based on the Gestalt principle of similarity, elements sharing attributes such as color, brightness, contrast, and texture tend to coalesce [30]. In the context of this study, the mosaic targets’ components possessed luminance similarities. It’s imperative to consider the Gestalt principle of proximity, which plays a pivotal role in the process of perceptual grouping [30]. Notably, the haphazard arrangement of elements within the mosaic stimulus plays a crucial role in target appearance, as it incorporates circles with varying luminances, potentially facilitating target perception and localization. Nevertheless, mere proximity between circles of the same luminance failed to ensure successful target shape discrimination. The random spatial distribution of mosaic elements may have contributed to the incomplete emergence of target perception by the two capuchin monkeys. Notably, it seems that the pivotal factor governing shape discrimination for both subjects pertains to the shape of certain objects. Intriguingly, although all mosaic targets were detected by the subjects, not all were successfully discriminated based on shape cues, be it in the delayed matching-to-sample task or the simple discrimination procedure. The precise reason behind the subjects’ significant discrimination performance for certain shapes while struggling with others remains enigmatic. For instance, both subjects demonstrated poor discrimination skills for rudimentary shapes like circles or squares. Notably, shapes such as stars and musical keynotes were significantly discriminated by Tico (musical keynote in the simple discrimination task, and star in the matching-to-sample procedure), while Raul failed to demonstrate similar aptitude. This disparity suggests that individual factors could be influencing the ability to discriminate these shapes. Intriguingly, both subjects showcased significant shape discrimination prowess when confronted with mosaic targets exhibiting shapes like the Letter X and Number 8 targets. Notably, the shapes of Number 8 and Letter X utilized in the experiments bear a striking resemblance due to their antiparallel diagonal line patterns. The authors have no suggestion about why specifically both targets elicited significant shape discrimination for both animals.

To date, the authors are unaware of any studies in non-human primates equivalent to the present study that have examined shape discrimination using mosaics. There is at least one article with humans in which the participant was asked to perform a task in which the shape of the target seen in the mosaic was compared with targets present in solid stimuli [31] although shape discrimination was not the focus of the study, which sought to estimate chromatic discrimination thresholds. The closest studies with nonhuman primates that we can relate to our findings are shape discrimination experiments using Kanizsa illusory shapes [32]. In these experiments, as in the experiment in our study, it was hypothesized that the emergence of illusory shapes would be due to global processing of the image, and that the animals’ good performance in these tasks would be a strong indicator that global processing of the visual scene was taking place. It should be noted that in these experiments, as in ours, there was variability in the animals’ performance in recognizing the illusory shapes.

It’s important to note that within the global and local processing hierarchical experiments, where the stimuli featured uniformly spaced elements composing the target, global perception did not manifest. The discerned prevalence of local control’s advantage within hierarchical stimuli, as revealed in this study, aligns cohesively with analogous findings in capuchin monkeys from other studies [1, 6, 14, 16, 18, 19], as well as in cotton top tamarins [4].

The present study diverges from earlier research examining the impact of local and global cues on image perceptual organization, owing to its utilization of mosaic stimuli. Unlike the hierarchically structured stimuli traditionally employed in such inquiries, mosaic stimuli introduce a significantly heightened level of complexity and, notably, have never been previously applied for this specific purpose. While mosaic stimuli have extensively found utility in appraising the color vision phenotype of non-human primates, their novel application in tasks necessitating shape discrimination is unprecedented. Mosaic stimuli present a multifaceted array of variables that can be manipulated within these tests, including the elemental dimensions constituting the mosaic, the spacing separating these elements, and diverse measures of color and luminance contrast. Therefore, a more comprehensive future investigation, exploring the impact of different stimulus parameters, could provide valuable insights into the underlying mechanisms of shape perception in capuchin monkeys. In the way these are put, it turns out to be mostly confounding variables in this study.

Our findings suggest that the monkeys’ responses are influenced by local contrast and, to a certain extent, by global contrast within the mosaic stimulus. However, it is plausible that additional manipulations employing mosaic stimuli would be necessary to definitively establish the potential for monkeys to exhibit more refined global perceptual grouping. An illustrative study conducted by Han et al. [33] explored neural grouping mechanisms rooted in proximity and similarity, leveraging brain event-related potentials (ERPs) as recorded from human participants. The results of their investigation unveiled that participants exhibited quicker and more accurate perceptions of grouping based on proximity compared to similarity. The ERP data revealed a brief latency positivity in the occipital cortex corresponding to proximity and a prolonged latency occipitotemporal negativity indicative of similarity. The authors posit the prevalence of proximity over similarity in the local element grouping for perceptual shape perception in humans. However, it remains essential to probe this issue within capuchin monkeys, as the existence of analogous mechanisms for perceptual grouping between humans and capuchins is far from guaranteed.

The congruence in results observed across the two subjects underscores the consistency between methodologies and outcomes. However, the limitation of a small subject pool in this study necessitates prudence in drawing comprehensive conclusions. To enhance the robustness of our findings, future investigations could encompass a larger sample size of monkeys, potentially including females and infants. The need for further exploration into the realm of visual processing within capuchin monkeys remains evident to fathom the intricacies of their visual cognition. The present study’s results offer preliminary indications that capuchin monkeys potentially discern shapes arising from the grouping of elements by luminance similarity within mosaics. This insight stands poised to guide forthcoming studies probing perceptual grouping employing mosaic stimuli. Importantly, such investigations must account for the distinct sensitivity to proximity and similarity between elements unique to each species.

Supporting information

S1 Data

(XLSX)

pone.0303562.s001.xlsx (46.5KB, xlsx)

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.

Funding Statement

DFV, GSS, OFG #431748/2016-0 Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq) https://www.gov.br/cnpq/pt-br Funding through FM, FACB, LM Finance Code 001 Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES) https://www.gov.br/capes/pt-br.

References

  • 1.De Lillo C, Palumbo M, Spinozzi G, Giustino G. Effects of pattern redundancy and hierarchical grouping on global-local visual processing in monkeys (Cebus apella) and humans (Homo sapiens). Behav Brain Res. 2012; 226(2): 445–455. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2011.09.040 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Fagot J, Tomonaga M. Global and local processing in humans (Homo sapiens) and chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes): use of a visual search task with compound stimuli. J Comp Psychol. 1999;113(1):3–12. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.113.1.3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Hopkins WD. Hemispheric specialization for local and global processing of hierarchical visual stimuli in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). Neuropsychologia. 1997;35(3):343–348. doi: 10.1016/s0028-3932(96)00089-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Neiworth JJ, Whillock KM, Kim SH, Greenberg JR, Jones KB, Patel AR, et al. Gestalt principle use in college students, children with autism, toddlers (Homo sapiens), and cotton top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). J Comp Psychol. 2014;128(2):188–198. doi: 10.1037/a0034840 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Spinozzi G, De Lillo C, Truppa V, Castorina G. The relative use of proximity, shape similarity, and orientation as visual perceptual grouping cues in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) and humans (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol. 2009;123(1):56–68. doi: 10.1037/a0012674 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Truppa V, Carducci P, De Simone DA, Bisazza A, De Lillo C. Global/local processing of hierarchical visual stimuli in a conflict-choice task by capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.). Anim Cogn. 2017;20(2):347–357. doi: 10.1007/s10071-016-1057-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Kimchi R. Primacy of holistic processing and global/local paradigm: a critical review. Psychol Bull. 1992;112(1):24–38. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.112.1.24 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Navon D. Forest before the tree: The precedence of global features in visual perception. Cogn Psychol. 1977;9(3):353–383. doi: 10.1016/0010-0285(77)90012-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.De Lillo C, Spinozzi G, Truppa V, Naylor D. A comparative analysis of global and local processing of hierarchical visual stimuli in young children and monkeys (Cebus apella). J Comp Psychol. 2005; 119(2): 155–165. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.119.2.155 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Fagot J, Deruelle C. Processing of global and local visual information and hemispheric specialization in humans (Homo sapiens) and baboons (Papio papio). J Exp Psychol Hum Percept Perform. 1997; 23(2): 429–442. doi: 10.1037//0096-1523.23.2.429 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Tomonaga M, Matsuzawa T. Perception of complex geometric figures in chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and humans (Homo sapiens): Analyses of visual similarity on the basis of choice reaction time. J Comp Psychol. 1992;106(1):43–52. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.106.1.43 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Hopkins WD, Washburn DA. Matching visual stimuli on the basis of global and local features by chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes) and rhesus monkeys (Macaca mulatta). Anim Cogn. 2002;5(1):27–31. doi: 10.1007/s10071-001-0121-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Neiworth JJ, Gleichman A, Olinick A, Lamp K. Global and local processing in adult humans (Homo sapiens), 5-year-old children (Homo sapiens), and adult cotton top tamarins (Saguinus oedipus). J Comp Psychol. 2006;120(4):323–330. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.120.4.323 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Spinozzi G, De Lillo C, Truppa V. Global and local processing of hierarchical visual stimuli in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella). J Comp Psychol. 2003;117(1):15–23. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.117.1.15 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.De Lillo C, Spinozzi G, Truppa V. Pattern recognition in tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella): the role of the spatial organization of stimulus parts. Behav Brain Res. 2007; 181(1): 96–109. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.03.030 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.De Lillo C, Spinozzi G, Palumbo M, Giustino G. Attention allocation modulates the processing of hierarchical visual patterns: a comparative analysis of capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) and humans. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process. 2011; 37(3): 341–352. doi: 10.1037/a0022989 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Spinozzi G, De Lillo C, Castelli S. Detection of grouped and ungrouped parts in visual patterns by tufted capuchin monkeys (Cebus apella) and humans (Homo sapiens). J Comp Psychol. 2004;118(3):297–308. doi: 10.1037/0735-7036.118.3.297 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Spinozzi G, De Lillo C, Salvi V. Local advantage in the visual processing of hierarchical stimuli following manipulations of stimulus size and element numerosity in monkeys (Cebus apella). Behav Brain Res. 2006;166(1):45–54. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2005.06.043 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Truppa V, De Simone DA, De Lillo C. Short-term memory effects on visual global/local processing in tufted capuchin monkeys (Sapajus spp.). J Comp Psychol. 2016;130(2):162–173. doi: 10.1037/com0000018 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Goulart PRK, Bonci DMO, Galvão ODF, Silveira LCDL, Ventura DF. Color discrimination in the Tufted Capuchin Monkey, Sapajus spp. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(4):4–7. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0062255 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Henriques LD, Oliveira JCP, Bonci DMO, Leão RC, Souza GS, Silveira LCL, et al. Behavioral and genetic color vision evaluation of an albino male capuchin monkey (Sapajus apella). J Comp Physiol A. 2019;205(4):529–536. doi: 10.1007/s00359-019-01342-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Mancuso K, Neitz M, Neitz J. An adaptation of the Cambridge Colour Test for use with animals. Vis Neurosci. 2006;23(3–4):695–701. doi: 10.1017/S0952523806233364 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Henriques LD, Hauzman E, Bonci DMO, Chang BS, Muniz JAPC, Souza GS, et al. Uniform trichromacy in Alouatta caraya and Alouatta seniculus: behavioural and genetic colour vision evaluation. Front Zool. 2021;18(36):1–10. doi: 10.1186/s12983-021-00421-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Brino ALF, Galvão OF, Picanço CRF, Barros RS, Souza CAB, Goulart PRK, et al. Generalized identity matching to sample after multiple-exemplar training in capuchin monkeys. Psychol Rec. 2014; 64(4): 693–702. doi: 10.1007/s40732-014-0035-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Galvão OF, Barros RS, Santos JR, Brino ALF, Brandão S, Lavratti CM, et al. Extent and limits of the matching concept in Cebus apella: A matter of experimental control? Psychol Rec. 2005;55(2):219–232. doi: 10.1007/BF03395507 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Leal TRC, Brino ALF, Costa LAA, Galvão OF, McIlvane WJ. Acquisition and maintenance of delayed-matching-to-sample in tufted capuchin monkeys. J Exp Anal Behav. 2020;113(3):549–564. doi: 10.1002/jeab.599 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Brino ALF, Barros RS, Galvão OF, Garotti M, da Cruz IR, Santos JR, et al. Sample stimulus control shaping and restricted stimulus control in capuchin monkeys: A methodological note. J Exp Anal Behav. 2011;95(3):387–398. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2011.95-387 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Man TSL. Efeito do treino de discriminação simples sobre o repertório de pareamento ao modelo por identidade de um macaco-prego (Cebus apella) [dissertação de mestrado]. Belém: Universidade Federal do Pará; 2007.
  • 29.Delage PEGA, Galvão OF. Generality of learning on tool use setting by a capuchin monkey. Psic Teor e Pesq. 2010; 26(4): 687–694. doi: 10.1590/S0102-37722010000400012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Wagemans J, Elder JH, Kubovy M, Palmer SE, Peterson MA, Singh M, et al. A century of Gestalt psychology in visual perception: I. Perceptual grouping and figure-ground organization. Psychol Bull. 2012;138(6):1172–1217. doi: 10.1037/a0029333 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Shin YJ, Park KH, Hwang JM, et al. A novel color vision test for detection of diabetic macular edema. Invest Ophthalmol Vis Sci. 2014;55:25–32. doi: 10.1167/iovs.13-11698 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Feltner KA, Kiorpes L. Global visual processing in macaques studied using Kanizsa illusory shapes. Vis Neurosci. 2010;27(3–4):131–138. doi: 10.1017/S0952523810000088 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Han S, Song Y, Ding Y, Yund EW, Woods DL. Neural substrates for visual perceptual grouping in humans. Psychophysiology. 2001;38(6):926–935. doi: 10.1111/1469-8986.3860926 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Carlos Tomaz

15 Feb 2024

PONE-D-24-01574Shape Discrimination in Mosaic Designs in Capuchin-Monkeys (Sapajus spp.): Indication of a preference for global processing?PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Mendes,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Mar 31 2024 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Carlos Tomaz, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Thank you for stating the following financial disclosure: 

"DFV, GSS, OFG

#431748/2016-0 

Conselho Nacional de Desenvolvimento Científico e Tecnológico (CNPq) 

https://www.gov.br/cnpq/pt-br

Funding through

FM, FACB, LM

Finance Code 001

Coordenação de Aperfeiçoamento de Pessoal de Nível Superior (CAPES) 

" ext-link-type="uri" xlink:type="simple">https://www.gov.br/capes/pt-br"

Please state what role the funders took in the study.  If the funders had no role, please state: ""The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript."" If this statement is not correct you must amend it as needed. 

Please include this amended Role of Funder statement in your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

3. We note that your Data Availability Statement is currently as follows: "All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files."

Please confirm at this time whether or not your submission contains all raw data required to replicate the results of your study. Authors must share the “minimal data set” for their submission. PLOS defines the minimal data set to consist of the data required to replicate all study findings reported in the article, as well as related metadata and methods (https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-minimal-data-set-definition).

For example, authors should submit the following data:

- The values behind the means, standard deviations and other measures reported;

- The values used to build graphs;

- The points extracted from images for analysis.

Authors do not need to submit their entire data set if only a portion of the data was used in the reported study.

If your submission does not contain these data, please either upload them as Supporting Information files or deposit them to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of recommended repositories, please see https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/recommended-repositories.

If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent. If data are owned by a third party, please indicate how others may request data access.

4. When completing the data availability statement of the submission form, you indicated that you will make your data available on acceptance. We strongly recommend all authors decide on a data sharing plan before acceptance, as the process can be lengthy and hold up publication timelines. Please note that, though access restrictions are acceptable now, your entire data will need to be made freely accessible if your manuscript is accepted for publication. This policy applies to all data except where public deposition would breach compliance with the protocol approved by your research ethics board. If you are unable to adhere to our open data policy, please kindly revise your statement to explain your reasoning and we will seek the editor's input on an exemption. Please be assured that, once you have provided your new statement, the assessment of your exemption will not hold up the peer review process.

5. Please include your full ethics statement in the ‘Methods’ section of your manuscript file. In your statement, please include the full name of the IRB or ethics committee who approved or waived your study, as well as whether or not you obtained informed written or verbal consent. If consent was waived for your study, please include this information in your statement as well. 

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: I Don't Know

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The manuscript evaluated shape discrimination capabilities of capuchin monkeys to gauge the dominance of local and global processing. Its main difference from previous studies seems to be on the type of stimuli used, such as mosaic stimuli. Results suggest that the monkeys primarily respond to local contrast and partly to global contrast in mosaic stimuli, which seems to differ from other studies using other classical paradigms.

The authors acknowledge the limitations of the study, especially that the results are only suggestive and inconclusive. But I believe rather than being an issue with the number of subjects, there are issues with the stimuli used. I was not able to understand what mosaic stimuli consisted of (perhaps the figure is not big enough also), and it is not clear if it’s a new paradigm or based on previous studies, since the references mentioned in comparison (19, 11, 13, and 14) don’t call their stimuli as mosaic (and also don’t seem to be the same type as the current study). Given this is an important feature of this study, more clarification is needed into what this stimulus is, as well as its importance in visual processing and in this study itself.

Another small issue is the different measures given for the stimuli, with some features measured in arc-seconds (degrees?), some in pixels, and others in cm. Some consistency could help with replication.

I commend the author's thorough description of some technicalities. Despite that, a few parts may not be relevant, such as the processor of the computer used (line 123), and mentioning the software “Microsoft paint" twice through the text. But I would like to make clear this does not make the manuscript any poorer and I’m not basing my evaluation on it, just as a suggestion.

One technical detail that does need better explanation is the order of the stimuli shown. Is it randomized, always the same order, and is it the same for both subjects?

In Experimental Procedures, line 133, there is again a reminder on the manuscript’s focus on the mosaic designs, but it is not clear through the paper the reasoning behind the choice of all these stimuli paired with the mosaic design.

In the explanation of the experiment in lines 185-204 it’s not clear if every session follows that order or if changes between sessions/subjects.

In line 214 should it be the third experiment? The inconsistent use of each of the 3 experiment names through the manuscript (for example between line 261 and 268) makes it sometimes hard to follow.

Are the outlier performances of each subject in specific shapes (lines 285-286 and 296-297) of any significance to the results? If so it would be better mentioned in the discussion. if not, either just grouped as outlier performances without known reason by the authors, or removed since it can be seen on the tables already.

As a general limitation, the authors acknowledged that mosaic stimuli involve various factors that could influence the monkeys' responses, such as the elemental dimensions, spacing, and color contrast. However, they did not systematically manipulate many of these variables to determine their specific effects on shape discrimination. A more comprehensive investigation, exploring the impact of different stimulus parameters, would have provided valuable insights into the underlying mechanisms of shape perception in capuchin monkeys. In the way these are put, it turns out to be mostly confounding variables in this study.

There is also a lack of comparisons between their findings with other research examining shape discrimination in mosaic stimuli in either human or non-human populations. This omission makes it challenging to contextualize the results and determine whether the observed patterns are unique to capuchin monkeys or are consistent with broader trends in visual perception. Including data from other studies would have provided a more comprehensive perspective and strengthened the conclusions.

As a whole, the manuscript needs to be more forthcoming as an exploratory research and better interconnect with other studies, or improve their experimental paradigm to give more conclusive objective results.

Reviewer #2: Thank you for the opportunity to review this manuscript, which details 4 screen-based studies of visual cognition in two captive capuchin monkeys. The manuscript provides key information about methods (including animal housing, ethical review, the experimental chamber and the touchscreen); and the figures and tables are useful (it is nice to see the experimental chamber, and examples of the stimuli help the reader understand the conditions and procedures). I think these studies have the potential to make a valuable contribution to the literature, once some questions are addressed.

First, in general throughout the manuscript it is not clear what is meant by ‘mosaic stimuli’. (At Line 71, it is stated that this type of stimulus is common in chromatic discrimination tasks.) From the figures provided, it appears that the mosaic stimuli are simply on a darker gray background than the linear images. It would be very helpful to describe these stimuli and give a clear exemplar in a figure early on in the manuscript (especially as it is claimed in the Discussion that this is a novel use of this type of stimulus). It may also be helpful to use a different name for them, like ‘dot stimuli’ or ‘dot-mosaic stimuli’, or similar (as previous examples of the term ‘mosaic stimuli’ in the literature do not always describe the same type of stimulus).

Second, as it stands I do not believe there is enough information in the Methods to replicate the procedure. Therefore, in some places more experimental detail is needed in order for the reader to understand the experiments. For example, at Lines 158-162 (matching to sample of hierarchical stimuli): How was it indicated to the monkeys which was the global and which the local condition? Were they visually identical other than the stimulus that was rewarded, or did they have different backgrounds (or other feature) that would let the monkey know which condition they were performing? Which stimuli were included in each condition (e.g., were there only 2 possible stimuli for each condition, and were these alternated as targets in some sort of pattern?)? How long was the delay between initial selection of the target and the availability of the responses? Line 158 states that this was ‘training’: were there separate test sessions also? It is stated (Lines 161-162) that there were 5 sessions with 12 trials each (60 trials) for each condition; in Table 1, each monkey appears to have received 120 trials in each condition. Can you please clarify? At Lines 188-189 (target discrimination): Were the different shapes in the target discrimination trials randomized throughout a session, or were they presented in groups?

Third, a suggestion for readability would be to combine the Methods Results for each experiment, as otherwise it can be hard to keep track of what’s happening.

Individual points

Title: Based on the results of the studies taken as a whole, the title’s second half (“Indication of a preference for local processing?”) doesn’t effectively summarize the findings.

Throughout manuscript: I believe it is now customary to refer to ‘New World monkeys’ as ‘platyrrhine monkeys’: would you be willing to adjust?

A specific example of a hierarchical stimulus (even just described in the text) at Lines 54-55 would be helpful to the reader.

Lines 60-61: can you clarify what you mean by ‘for inconsistent stimuli, the global level resulted in slower responses to the local level, but not vice versa’?

Line 64 (also Lines 337-338): the referenced paper’s title states that it is about ‘cotton-top tamarins’ (Saguinus oedipus) rather than ‘cotton-headed marmosets’. Please clarify.

Lines 88-89: Can you clarify whether monkeys’ experience with discrimination and MTS were with using a touchscreen?

Line 120: Which type of touch technology does the touchscreen use? (capacitive, resistive, infrared, other?)

Can you provide a statement about whether animals were food-deprived or kept at a certain percentage of free-feeding weight (or not) for the purposes of the study?

Lines 148-155 (Figure 2 legend?): S+ and S- are not mentioned in the figure.

For all tables: please indicate what the asterisks mean. If they indicate ‘above-chance performance’, can you please also state the chance level for each task in the table or legend somewhere?

Table 2 and 3: is there a reason that Raul had more solid target discrimination trials than Tico? And that Tico had more shape discrimination trials than Raul?

Line 232: Here you mention ‘gray fields’, but the shape discrimination experiment to my understanding did not contain gray fields: please clarify.

Lines 248-249: It is stated that ‘The performance criterion for terminating a session was either achieving a 90% accuracy rate or the completion of 5 sessions.’ Can you clarify? (Would a monkey have performed more than one 54-trial session in a single day?)

Line 250-255: Can you please clarify which the statistical tests you conducted, and how they were implemented (i.e., which statistical programme)?

In the discussion (Lines 341-345) it is stated that this is a novel application of mosaic stimuli. This information could usefully be introduced in the Introduction section.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2024 May 29;19(5):e0303562. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0303562.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


8 Apr 2024

Reviewer #1:

The manuscript evaluated shape discrimination capabilities of capuchin monkeys to gauge the dominance of local and global processing. Its main difference from previous studies seems to be on the type of stimuli used, such as mosaic stimuli. Results suggest that the monkeys primarily respond to local contrast and partly to global contrast in mosaic stimuli, which seems to differ from other studies using other classical paradigms.

The authors acknowledge the limitations of the study, especially that the results are only suggestive and inconclusive. But I believe rather than being an issue with the number of subjects, there are issues with the stimuli used. I was not able to understand what mosaic stimuli consisted of (perhaps the figure is not big enough also), and it is not clear if it’s a new paradigm or based on previous studies, since the references mentioned in comparison (19, 11, 13, and 14) don’t call their stimuli as mosaic (and also don’t seem to be the same type as the current study). Given this is an important feature of this study, more clarification is needed into what this stimulus is, as well as its importance in visual processing and in this study itself.

A. Thanks for the comment. We wrote a more detailed explanation of our choice of using mosaic arrangement to investigate the preference of global or local processing. Additionally, we added a Figure with an example of mosaic arrangement and more detailed paragraph describing why mosaic arrangements could be important to investigate the global and local processing.

Another small issue is the different measures given for the stimuli, with some features measured in arc-seconds (degrees?), some in pixels, and others in cm. Some consistency could help with replication.

A. Done.

I commend the author's thorough description of some technicalities. Despite that, a few parts may not be relevant, such as the processor of the computer used (line 123)

A. Right. We excluded the information.

, and mentioning the software “Microsoft paint" twice through the text. But I would like to make clear this does not make the manuscript any poorer and I’m not basing my evaluation on it, just as a suggestion.

A. We wrote again both parts. Thanks.

One technical detail that does need better explanation is the order of the stimuli shown. Is it randomized, always the same order, and is it the same for both subjects?

A. Thanks for the comment. We included that for each experiment the order of stimulus presentation was randomly chosen by the software.

In Experimental Procedures, line 133, there is again a reminder on the manuscript’s focus on the mosaic designs, but it is not clear through the paper the reasoning behind the choice of all these stimuli paired with the mosaic design.

A. We added to the introduction the rationale for the choice of the mosaic stimuli, and we hope to make clearer our purpose.

In the explanation of the experiment in lines 185-204 it’s not clear if every session follows that order or if changes between sessions/subjects.

A. We rewrote the sentence with “The same order of sessions was delivered for both animals. Inside each session the order of stimulus presentation was randomly chosen by the software.”

In line 214 should it be the third experiment? The inconsistent use of each of the 3 experiment names through the manuscript (for example between line 261 and 268) makes it sometimes hard to follow.

A. Thanks again. We searched for to name correctly the experiments in the revised version.

Are the outlier performances of each subject in specific shapes (lines 285-286 and 296-297) of any significance to the results? If so it would be better mentioned in the discussion. if not, either just grouped as outlier performances without known reason by the authors, or removed since it can be seen on the tables already.

A. Thanks for the suggestion. We stated in the Discussion that both outlier performances were without know by us.

As a general limitation, the authors acknowledged that mosaic stimuli involve various factors that could influence the monkeys' responses, such as the elemental dimensions, spacing, and color contrast. However, they did not systematically manipulate many of these variables to determine their specific effects on shape discrimination. A more comprehensive investigation, exploring the impact of different stimulus parameters, would have provided valuable insights into the underlying mechanisms of shape perception in capuchin monkeys. In the way these are put, it turns out to be mostly confounding variables in this study.

A. Thanks for the comment. We added this observation in the Discussion section.

There is also a lack of comparisons between their findings with other research examining shape discrimination in mosaic stimuli in either human or non-human populations. This omission makes it challenging to contextualize the results and determine whether the observed patterns are unique to capuchin monkeys or are consistent with broader trends in visual perception. Including data from other studies would have provided a more comprehensive perspective and strengthened the conclusions.

A. Done.

As a whole, the manuscript needs to be more forthcoming as an exploratory research and better interconnect with other studies, or improve their experimental paradigm to give more conclusive objective results.

Reviewer #2:

Thank you for the opportunity to review this manuscript, which details 4 screen-based studies of visual cognition in two captive capuchin monkeys. The manuscript provides key information about methods (including animal housing, ethical review, the experimental chamber and the touchscreen); and the figures and tables are useful (it is nice to see the experimental chamber, and examples of the stimuli help the reader understand the conditions and procedures). I think these studies have the potential to make a valuable contribution to the literature, once some questions are addressed.

First, in general throughout the manuscript it is not clear what is meant by ‘mosaic stimuli’. (At Line 71, it is stated that this type of stimulus is common in chromatic discrimination tasks.) From the figures provided, it appears that the mosaic stimuli are simply on a darker gray background than the linear images. It would be very helpful to describe these stimuli and give a clear exemplar in a figure early on in the manuscript (especially as it is claimed in the Discussion that this is a novel use of this type of stimulus). It may also be helpful to use a different name for them, like ‘dot stimuli’ or ‘dot-mosaic stimuli’, or similar (as previous examples of the term ‘mosaic stimuli’ in the literature do not always describe the same type of stimulus).

A. We rewrote the Introduction searching for clarifying the rationale of using mosaic stimulus.

Second, as it stands I do not believe there is enough information in the Methods to replicate the procedure. Therefore, in some places more experimental detail is needed in order for the reader to understand the experiments. For example, at Lines 158-162 (matching to sample of hierarchical stimuli): How was it indicated to the monkeys which was the global and which the local condition? Were they visually identical other than the stimulus that was rewarded, or did they have different backgrounds (or other feature) that would let the monkey know which condition they were performing?

A. The monkeys were instructed on which global and local clues to follow using a reward system that favored each condition.

Which stimuli were included in each condition (e.g., were there only 2 possible stimuli for each condition, and were these alternated as targets in some sort of pattern?)?

A. There were only two possible stimuli.

How long was the delay between initial selection of the target and the availability of the responses?

A. 1 second. We added to the text.

Line 158 states that this was ‘training’: were there separate test sessions also? It is stated (Lines 161-162) that there were 5 sessions with 12 trials each (60 trials) for each condition; in Table 1, each monkey appears to have received 120 trials in each condition. Can you please clarify?

A. There was no test. The animals were exposed to five sessions with global configuration trials and another five sessions with local configuration trials. The trials were repeated and there were differential consequences for success and error. Five 48-trial sessions had global configurations, in a total of 240 trials, 120 of global consistent and 120 of local consistent; the same occurred for the five 48-trial sessions of local configuration, 240 total trials, 120 local consistent and 120 global inconsistent.

At Lines 188-189 (target discrimination): Were the different shapes in the target discrimination trials randomized throughout a session, or were they presented in groups?

A. It was randomized.

Third, a suggestion for readability would be to combine the Methods Results for each experiment, as otherwise it can be hard to keep track of what’s happening.

A. Done.

Individual points

Title: Based on the results of the studies taken as a whole, the title’s second half (“Indication of a preference for local processing?”) doesn’t effectively summarize the findings.

A. We changed the title for Investigation of preference for local and global processing of Capuchin-Monkeys (Sapajus spp.) in shape discrimination in mosaic arrangements

Throughout manuscript: I believe it is now customary to refer to ‘New World monkeys’ as ‘platyrrhine monkeys’: would you be willing to adjust?

A. Done.

A specific example of a hierarchical stimulus (even just described in the text) at Lines 54-55 would be helpful to the reader.

A. Done. We included a figure with examples of hierarchical stimuli in the Introduction section.

Lines 60-61: can you clarify what you mean by ‘for inconsistent stimuli, the global level resulted in slower responses to the local level, but not vice versa’?

A. We excluded the sentence.

Line 64 (also Lines 337-338): the referenced paper’s title states that it is about ‘cotton-top tamarins’ (Saguinus oedipus) rather than ‘cotton-headed marmosets’.

Please clarify.

A. Thank you very much for the comment. We have changed the name.

Lines 88-89: Can you clarify whether monkeys’ experience with discrimination and MTS were with using a touchscreen?

A. Both subjects had extensive experience (at least 10 years) with discrimination and MTS were with using a touchscreen (references).

Line 120: Which type of touch technology does the touchscreen use? (capacitive, resistive, infrared, other?)

A. It was used a surface acoustic wave touchscreen-type device.

Can you provide a statement about whether animals were food-deprived or kept at a certain percentage of free-feeding weight (or not) for the purposes of the study?

A. Before the experiments the subject were kept at a certain percentage of free-feeding weight.

Lines 148-155 (Figure 2 legend?): S+ and S- are not mentioned in the figure.

A. Thanks. Done.

For all tables: please indicate what the asterisks mean. If they indicate ‘above-chance performance’, can you please also state the chance level for each task in the table or legend somewhere?

A. Done.

Table 2 and 3: is there a reason that Raul had more solid target discrimination trials than Tico? And that Tico had more shape discrimination trials than Raul?

A. Thanks for the questions. Raul had reached performance criterion before Tico. The total number of trials with solid stimuli was lower for Raul (96 trials distributed in 12 sessions) than for Tico (120 trials distributed in 15 sessions). We wrote again the sentence and we informed that was tried several sessions up to reach 90% correct in one session and when the performance was lower than this criterion in 10 consecutive sessions, the test was completed. Each subject performed one session (48 trials) per day.

Line 232: Here you mention ‘gray fields’, but the shape discrimination experiment to my understanding did not contain gray fields: please clarify.

A. Oh really, we replaced the gray fields by S-.

Lines 248-249: It is stated that ‘The performance criterion for terminating a session was either achieving a 90% accuracy rate or the completion of 5 sessions.’ Can you clarify? (Would a monkey have performed more than one 54-trial session in a single day?)

A. Thanks for the question. We wrote again the sentence and we informed that was tried several sessions up to reach 90% correct in one session and when the performance was lower than this criterion in 10 consecutive sessions, the test was completed. The subject was exposed to one session per day and the session was composed by 48 trials.

Line 250-255: Can you please clarify which the statistical tests you conducted, and how they were implemented (i.e., which statistical programme)?

A. Thanks. We added that we wrote a routine in Python language to calculate the statistical significance of the subject’s performance in each experiment by calculating the binomial probability

In the discussion (Lines 341-345) it is stated that this is a novel application of mosaic stimuli. This information could usefully be introduced in the Introduction section.

A. Done

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

pone.0303562.s002.docx (24.4KB, docx)

Decision Letter 1

Carlos Tomaz

29 Apr 2024

Investigation of preference for local and global processing of Capuchin-Monkeys (Sapajus spp.) in shape discrimination of mosaic arrangements

PONE-D-24-01574R1

Dear Dr. Mendes,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice will be generated when your article is formally accepted. Please note, if your institution has a publishing partnership with PLOS and your article meets the relevant criteria, all or part of your publication costs will be covered. Please make sure your user information is up-to-date by logging into Editorial Manager at Editorial Manager® and clicking the ‘Update My Information' link at the top of the page. If you have any questions relating to publication charges, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Carlos Tomaz, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

The authors have addressed all the comments and suggestions from the referees. Therefore, I understand that the MS can be now be accept for publication.

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: I Don't Know

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Thank you for directly addressing all the comments from the previous round of review.

The methods appear correct and the new information added enriches the manuscript and imparts importance to the study by making easier to compare to previous (and future) studies.

I'm specially satisfied with the new figures and clear explanation of the experiment and it's importance.

Despite that, I believe English editing is necessary in the manuscript in the current form, since Plos One does not perform language editing pre-publication. Besides a few grammatical mistakes, some parts make hard to understand what the authors intended to say.

Although this comment is mostly directed to the newly added parts, I must say it concerns me that some sentences and paragraphs appear disjointed from each other. I don't believe this issue is serious enough to compromise the scientific interpretation of the paper, but it is noticeable and could be easily fixed with an English revision.

Reviewer #2: Thank you for the opportunity to read and review this revised manuscript about capuchin monkeys’ visual cognition. The authors have addressed the majority of my questions in their responses and in the revised manuscript, which is improved in readability and clarity. Thank you also for providing the data file in Supporting Information. Figure 1 and the revised Figure 3 are nice additions/edits.

A few minor issues are outstanding.

By “significant” performance levels (throughout the manuscript), do you mean that their performance was better or worse than chance?

It is stated that the monkeys were kept at a certain percentage of free-feeding weight: which percentage? (i.e., just to confirm, they were food deprived to increase motivation?)

Were the statistical tests binomial tests?

Please check the sentences at lines 256-257 and 294-297; they could be clearer.

While the two experiments described together for Experiment 3 (shape discrimination and MTS) have a great deal in common, I wonder if they also could be described separately for clarity.

Check Figure 5 – the mosaic images are fairly dark in the PDF, but I wonder if this is due to the PDF-builder rather than an issue with the actual image file. *FOR EDITOR TO CHECK?

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

Acceptance letter

Carlos Tomaz

8 May 2024

PONE-D-24-01574R1

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Mendes,

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now being handed over to our production team.

At this stage, our production department will prepare your paper for publication. This includes ensuring the following:

* All references, tables, and figures are properly cited

* All relevant supporting information is included in the manuscript submission,

* There are no issues that prevent the paper from being properly typeset

If revisions are needed, the production department will contact you directly to resolve them. If no revisions are needed, you will receive an email when the publication date has been set. At this time, we do not offer pre-publication proofs to authors during production of the accepted work. Please keep in mind that we are working through a large volume of accepted articles, so please give us a few weeks to review your paper and let you know the next and final steps.

Lastly, if your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at customercare@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Carlos Tomaz

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Data

    (XLSX)

    pone.0303562.s001.xlsx (46.5KB, xlsx)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

    pone.0303562.s002.docx (24.4KB, docx)

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES