Skip to main content
The BMJ logoLink to The BMJ
. 1999 May 29;318(7196):1460–1467. doi: 10.1136/bmj.318.7196.1460

Psychosocial factors in the aetiology and prognosis of coronary heart disease: systematic review of prospective cohort studies

Harry Hemingway 1, Michael Marmot 1
PMCID: PMC1115843  PMID: 10346775

Do psychosocial factors cause coronary heart disease or affect survival among patients with coronary heart disease? Here we use an explicit methodological quality filter to review systematically the prospective cohort studies testing specific psychosocial hypotheses. This review of the epidemiological literature identifies the psychosocial factors that have been most rigorously tested. Only four psychosocial factors met the quality filter: type A/hostility, depression and anxiety, work characteristics, and social supports. The importance of other study designs—for example, ecological1 or nested case-control24 studies—is acknowledged. The review should be seen as complementary to existing reviews58 on single psychosocial factors and as a challenge to investigators in the field to ensure that the systematic review is made unbiased, kept up to date, and used to guide future hypothesis testing.

Summary points

  • In healthy populations, prospective cohort studies show a possible aetiological role for type A/hostility (6/14 studies), depression and anxiety (11/11 studies), psychosocial work characteristics (6/10 studies), social support (5/8 studies)

  • In populations of patients with coronary heart disease, prospective studies show a prognostic role for depression and anxiety (6/6 studies), psychosocial work characteristics (1/2 studies), and social support (9/10 studies); none of five studies showed a prognostic role for type A/hostility

  • Although this review can not discount the possibility of publication bias, prospective cohort studies provide strong evidence that psychosocial factors, particularly depression and social support, are independent aetiological and prognostic factors for coronary heart disease

What is a psychosocial factor?

A psychosocial factor may be defined as a measurement that potentially relates psychological phenomena to the social environment and to pathophysiological changes. The validity and reliability (precision) of the questionnaire based instruments used to measure psychosocial factors has been improved through the use of psychometric techniques. By avoiding the unhelpful general term of “stress,” recent work has developed theoretical models—for example, the job control-demands-support model of psychosocial work characteristics—which generate specific hypotheses that can be tested.

How might psychosocial factors be linked to coronary heart disease?

Evidence of mechanisms linking psychosocial factors with coronary heart disease (reviewed elsewhere9,10) is important in making causal inferences and therefore in designing preventive interventions. Psychosocial factors may act alone or combine in clusters11 and may exert effects at different stages of the life course.12 Broadly, three interrelated pathways may be considered. Firstly, psychosocial factors may affect health related behaviours such as smoking, diet, alcohol consumption, or physical activity, which in turn may influence the risk of coronary heart disease.13 If such behaviours do lie on the causal pathway between psychosocial factors and coronary heart disease, then treating them as confounding variables, as some studies do, must be questioned. Secondly, psychosocial factors may cause direct acute or chronic pathophysiological changes. Thirdly, access to and content of medical care may plausibly be influenced by, for example, social supports (but there is little direct evidence for this). Although it is beyond the scope of this review to consider the determinants of adverse psychosocial factors, socioeconomic status is inversely associated with coronary heart disease14 and also with certain psychosocial factors, and it has been proposed that psychosocial pathways may play a mediating role.15,16

Method of systematic review

A methodological quality filter was used to select studies for inclusion in the systematic review, so that the strength of evidence could be compared across psychosocial factors. Prospective cohort studies are the best observational design for questions of aetiology and prognosis. The studies included had a prospective cohort design; a population based sample (aetiological studies in healthy populations); at least 500 participants (aetiological studies) or 100 participants (prognostic studies in populations of patients with coronary heart disease); measurements of a psychosocial factor used in at least two different study populations; outcomes of fatal coronary heart disease or non-fatal myocardial infarction or (prognostic studies only) all cause mortality.

Articles were identified by Medline search (1966-97), manual searching of the bibliographies ofretrieved articles, previous review articles, writing to researchersin the field, and an in-house bibliographic database. No register of published and unpublished studies with psychosocial exposures exists, and hand searching of journals was not performed, so there is a serious potential for publication bias. For this reason as well as the lack of standardised methods of measurement of psychosocial factors, we carried out a narrative, rather than quantitative, systematic review. Given that randomised controlled trials, at least for primary prevention, are rarely feasible, observational studies are likely to remain the main type of evidence on which to base preventive action.

Evidence for specific psychosocial factors

Largely on the basis of studies in middle aged men (table 1), four groups of psychosocial factors were identified by using the predefined quality filter: psychological traits (type A behaviour, hostility), psychological states (depression, anxiety), psychological interaction with the organisation of work (job control-demands-support), and social networks and social support. In simple terms this corresponds to a spectrum with mainly psychological components at one end and a stronger social component at the other. The box summarises the key results.

Studies showing role of psychosocial factors

In healthy populations, prospective cohort studies suggest a possible aetiological role for:

  • Type A/hostility (6/14 studies)

  • Depression and anxiety (11/11 studies)

  • Psychosocial work characteristics (6/10 studies)

  • Social support (5/8 studies)

In coronary heart disease patient populations, prospective studies suggest a prognostic role for:

  • Type A/hostility (0/5 studies)

  • Depression and anxiety (6/6 studies)

  • Psychosocial work characteristics (1/2 studies)

  • Social support (9/10 studies)

Although this review cannot discount the possibility of publication bias, prospective cohort studies provide strong evidence that psychosocial factors, particularly depression and social support, are independent aetiological and prognostic factors for coronary heart disease.

Table 1.

Studies of type A behaviour, hostility, and coronary heart disease. References in this table are given on the BMJ website

Author, year, country Total sample (% women) Age at entry Exposure Follow up (years) No of events Type of events Adjustments Relative risk* Summary
Prospective aetiological studies
Jenkins 1974w1 USA 2750 (0) 39-59 Type A  4 120 Non-fatal MI + angina Age 1.8* +
Rosenman 1976w2 USA 3154 (0) 39-59 Type A 8.5 257 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI Age, smoking, cholesterol, family history, corneal arcus, schooling, β:α lipoprotein ratio 2.16* ++
Haynes 1980w3 USA  1674 (57) 45-77 Type A (Framingham)  8 170 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI + coronary insufficiency + angina Age, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol, glucose intolerance and other psychosocial factors 1.8*; among men, the effect was confined to white collar workers +
Shekelle 1983w4 USA 1877 (0) 40-55 Hostility (MMPI) 10 139 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI Age, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol, alcohol 1.47*, but effect not linear +
Cohen 1985w5 USA 2187 (0) 57.8 (mean) Type A (JAS)  8 190 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI + angina Smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol, body mass index, alcohol, and other biological factors 1.43, Type A associated with prevalence, not incidence or postmortem findings 0
Shekelle 1985w6 USA 3110 (0) 46 (mean) Type A (JAS) 7.1 554 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI Age, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol, alcohol, education 0.87 0
Johnston 1987w7 UK 5936 (0) 40-59 Type A (Bortner) 6.2 255 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI Age, social class 0.89 0
Ragland 1988w8 USA 3154 (0) 39-59 Type A (SI) 22 214 Fatal CHD Age, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol 0.98 0
Hearn 1989w9 USA 1399 (0) 19 Hostility (MMPI) 33  54 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI + angina + coronary surgery Smoking, hypertension, family history 1.1; no association in crude or risk factor adjusted analyses 0
Barefoot 1995w10 USA   730 (44) 50 Hostility (Cook-Medley) 27 122 Non-fatal MI Age, sex, smoking, blood pressure, triglycerides, exercise 1.26 (men) 2.95* (women) 0 (men) ++(women)
Bosma 1995w11 Lithuania and Netherlands 5817 (0) 45-60 Type A (JAS) 9.5 394 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI Age No association 0
Kawachi 1996w12 USA 1305 (0) 40-90 MMPI-2 anger content scale  7 110 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI + angina Age, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol, body mass index, family history, alcohol 2.66* ++
Everson 1997w13 Finland 1599 (0) 42-60 Cynical hostility (Cook-Medley)  6  60 First MI Age, biological, socioeconomic, behavioural, social support, prevalent diseases 1.43 (2.18* when adjusted for age only) 0
Tunstall-Pedoe 1997w14 Scotland 11659 (50) 40-59 Type A (Bortner) 7.6 581 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI + coronary surgery Age 0.82* in women, ie type A protective 0
Prognostic studies
Case 1985w15 USA 516 (18) patients <14 days post MI <70 Type A (JAS)  2  53 Fatal CHD and all cause mortality Age, sex, education, rales, ejection fraction, New York Heart Assocation functional class, ventricular premature beats 0.8 0
Shekelle 1985w16 USA 2314 (11) patients post MI 30-69 Type A (JAS)  3 294 Non-fatal MI and fatal CHD Smoking, previous MI, angina, fasting glucose No association 0
Ragland 1988w17 USA 257 (0) with MI or angina 39-70 Type A (SI) 11.5  91 Fatal CHD Age at initial event, follow up time, type of initial coronary event, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol 0.58*; type A protective 0
Barefoot 1989w18 USA 1467 (18) patients with angiographic disease mean 52 (SD 9) Type A (SI)  5 315 Fatal CVD + non-fatal MI Stratified on clinical prognostic factors No association with non-fatal MI 0
Jenkinson 1993w19 UK 1376 (22) 7 days post-MI 25-84 Type A  3 247 All cause mortality Age, previous MI,hospital complications, diabetes, hypertension, car ownership, sex No association 0

CHD=coronary heart disease; MI=myocardial infarction; SI=structured interview; JAS=Jenkins activity survey; MMPI=Minnesota multiphasic personality inventory. 

*

P<0.05. 0=no association—that is, relative risk not significantly different from unity; +=moderate association (relative risk >1⩽2.0); ++=strong association (relative risk >2.0). 

Hostility and type A behaviour

Type A behaviour pattern—the only personality trait which met the criteria of our review—is characterised by hard driving and competitive behaviour, a potential for hostility, pronounced impatience, and vigorous speech stylistics. The instruments for measurement of type A behaviour and hostility—the Jenkins activity scale, the structured interview, the Minnesota multiphasic personality inventory (MMPI), the Bortner hostility scale—have been subjected to psychometric testing and incorporated into many cardiovascular cohort studies, including some that have not reported results. Unlike other psychosocial factors, type A is distinguished by being the subject of numerous intervention trials.17 On the basis of early positive findings in the Framingham study18 and the Western Collaborative Group’s eight year follow up,19 among other evidence, the National Institutes of Health declared type A an independent risk factor for coronary heart disease. However, with the publication of negative findings2022 it was proposed that a more specific component of type A, namely hostility, might be aetiological, although there are conflicting studies. None of the five studies that examined type A or hostility in relation to prognosis among patients with coronary heart disease showed an increased risk; indeed, one suggested a protective effect.

graphic file with name hemh03eb.f1.jpg

SUE SHARPLES

Depression and anxiety

The relation between depression and anxiety and coronary heart disease differs from those of other psychosocial factors for several reasons. Firstly, unlike other psychosocial factors, depression and anxiety represent well defined psychiatric disorders, with standardised instruments for measurement. Secondly, depression and anxiety are commonly the consequence of coronary heart disease, and the extent to which they are also the cause poses important methodological issues. Thirdly, the ability to diagnose and treat such disorders makes them attractive points for intervention. Finally, depression and coronary heart disease could share common antecedents—for example, environmental stressors and social supports.

Table 2 shows the results from the 11 prospective studies that investigated depression or anxiety in the aetiology of coronary heart disease, all of which were positive. All three of the prospective studies examining the effect of anxiety in the aetiology of coronary heart disease had positive results. Intriguingly, there is some evidence that this effect is strongest specifically for phobic anxiety and sudden cardiac death. Wassertheil-Smoller23 reported the effect of depression in relation to cardiovascular events among 4367 healthy older people. An increase in depression symptoms (but not the baseline scores) predicted events, even when multiple covariates were controlled for. Such findings are compatible with the hypothesis that premonitory signs of coronary heart disease such as angina or breathlessness may have led to the increase in depression. Studies with longer periods of follow up are less likely to be confounded by the possibility of early disease causing depression, but raise further questions about the time course of exposure. For example, it is possible that there is a common trigger (such as viral illness) that precipitates both symptoms of depression and atherothrombotic processes. By examination of subclinical manifestations of coronary heart disease (using non-invasive measures of arterial structure and function, for example) before the onset of symptoms, the temporal sequence of the relation might be better understood.

Table 2.

Studies of depression and anxiety and coronary heart disease. References in the table are given on the BMJ website

Author, year, country Total sample (% women) Age at entry Exposure Follow up (years) No of events Type of events Adjustments Relative risk* Summary
Prospective aetiological studies
Hallstrom 1986w20 Sweden   795 (100) 38-54 Depression (Hamilton and psychiatric interview) 12  75 Non-fatal MI + angina + ischaemic changes on electrocardiograph Age, social class, marital status, conventional risk factors 5.4* severity of depression predicted angina but not other outcomes ++
Hagman 1987w21 Sweden 5735 (0) 55 (mean) Anxiety (“stress”) 2-7 162 Angina with or without MI Age, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol, relative weight Strong predictor for angina alone +
Haines 1987w22 UK 1457 (0) 40-64 Phobic anxiety (Crown-Crisp) 10 113 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI fibrinogen, cholesterol, factor VII, systolic blood pressure 3.77* for fatal CHD ++
Appels 1990w23 Netherlands 3877 (0) 39-65 Depression 4.2  59 Non-fatal MI + unstable angina + angina Age, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol 1.86* for unstable angina for combination of low mood, low energy, hopelessness, poor sleep (termed “vital exhaustion”) +
Anda 1993w24 USA  2832 (52) 45-77 Depression (General Well Being) 12 394 Fatal CHD + non-fatal CHD hospitalisations Age, sex, race, education, marital status, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol, body mass index, alcohol, exercise 1.6* +
Aromaa 1994w25 Finland  5355 (55) 40-64 Depression (GHQ and PSE) 6.6  91 Fatal CHD Age, pre-existing cardiovascular disease 3.36* (5.52 in those with pre-existing cardiovascular disease) ++
Kawachi 1994w26 USA 33999 (0) 42-77 Phobic anxiety (Crown Crisp)  2 168 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI Age, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol, body mass index, diabetes, parental history of MI, alcohol, exercise 3.01* (6.08 when sudden cardiac death examined) ++
Everson 1996w27 Finland 2428 (0) 42-60 Hopelessness  6  95 Non-fatal MI Age, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol education, income, exercise, alcohol, lipids, social supports, depression 2.05* ++
Wassertheil-Smoller 1996w28 USA  4367 (53) 72 (mean) Depression (CES-D) 4.5 321 Non-fatal MI + non-fatal strokes Age, smoking, baseline depression, sex, race, randomisation group, education, history of stroke, MI, diabetes, and baseline ADL 1.18* per 5 unit increase in depression score (baseline scores alone did not predict events) +
Barefoot 1996w29 Denmark   730 (44) 50 or 60 Depression (MMPI-obvious depression scale) 27 122 Non-fatal MI Age, conventional CHD risk factors, baseline CHD 1.7* for 2 SD difference in depression score +
Kubzansky 1997w30 USA 1759 (0) 21-80 Social conditions worry scale 20 323 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI + angina Age, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol, body mass index, family history, alcohol 1.23* per 1 point increase in social conditions worry scale +
Prognostic studies
Ahern 199031 USA 353 Depression (Beck), anxiety (Spielberger) 12 Fatal CHD Age, left ventricular dysfunction and previous MI 1.3* for depression +
Kop 199432 Netherlands 127 (17) patients 2 weeks after coronary angioplasty 56 (SD 9) Maastricht questionnaire for vital exhaustion 1.5  29 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI + further revascularisation + increase in coronary atherosclerosis + new angina Age, sex, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol, severity of coronary artery disease, clinical presentation 2.34 (P=0.06) +
Ladwig 199433 Germany 377 (0) 17-21 days after acute MI 29-65 Depression (interview) 0.5 Angina, not returning to work, continuing to smoke Age, social class, recurrent infarction, rehabilitation, cardiac events and helplessness 2.31* for the effect on angina; depression predicted all outcomes ++
Frasure-Smith 1995w34 USA 222 (21) patients 5-15 days after acute MI 24-88 Depression (diagnostic interview schedule) 1.5  21 All cause mortality and fatal CHD Age, Killip class, premature ventricular contractions and previous MI 6.64* effect of depression higher in those with (10 premature contractions per hour ++
Barefoot 1996w35 USA 1250 (18) patients with angiographic disease 52 (mean) Depression (Zung) 19.4 604 All cause mortality and fatal CHD Disease severity, initial treatment 1.66*, 1.84* and 1.72* in three follow up periods (year 1, 5-10 and >10 respectively) +
Denollet 1996w36 Belgium 303 (12) patients with angiographic disease 31-79 Type D personality (suppression of emotional distress), depression, social alienation 7.9  38 All cause mortality and fatal CHD Left ventricular function, number of diseased vessels, low exercise tolerance, lack of thrombolytic therapy 4.1* for type D and 2.7* for depression ++

CHD=coronary heart disease; MI=myocardial infarction; CES-D=Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression scale; GHQ=general health questionnaire; PSE=present state examination; MMPI=Minnesota multiphasic personality inventory. 

*

P<0.05. 0=no association—that is, relative risk not significantly different from unity; +=moderate association (relative risk >1⩽2.0); ++=strong association (relative risk >2.0) association. 

Depression in patients after myocardial infarction seems to be of prognostic importance beyond the severity of coronary artery disease. Although discrete major depressive episodes are not uncommon after a myocardial infarction, depressive symptoms are more prevalent. Given the graded relation between depression scores and risk, the long lasting nature of the effect, and the stability of the depression measured across time, it has been proposed that depression is a continuously distributed chronic psychological characteristic.

Psychosocial work characteristics

The longstanding observation that rates of coronary heart disease vary markedly among occupations—more than can be accounted for by conventional risk factors for coronary heart disease—has generated a quest for specific components of work that might be of aetiological importance. The dominant “job strain” model of psychosocial work characteristics, as proposed by Karasek and Theorell, grew out of secondary analyses of existing survey data on the labour force. This model proposes that people in jobs characterised by low control over work and high conflicting demands might be high strain. A subsequent addition to the model was that social support might buffer this effect. The advantage of the model is that it generates specific hypotheses for testing.

Table 3 shows prospective cohort studies that have examined the relation between job strain and coronary heart disease. Both self reports and ecological measurements (assigning a score on the basis of job title) of job strain have been made. Self reports may be biased by early manifestations of disease, and ecological measurements may lack precision. The finding that these methods tend to give reasonably consistent results suggests that they are complementary. Six of the 10 studies were had positive results. There is growing emphasis on the importance of low job control rather than on conflicting demands,24 and it seems likely that these empirical results will lead to a reformulation of the model. Alternative models of psychosocial work characteristics involve an imbalance between the effort at work and rewards received.25,26

Table 3.

Studies of psychosocial work characteristics and coronary heart disease. References in table are given on BMJ website

Author, year, country Total sample (% women) Age at entry Exposure Follow up (years) No of events Type of events Adjustments Relative risk* Summary
Prospective aetiological studies
LaCroix 1984w37 USA 876 (37) 45-64 Job control and demands (individual and ecological) 10 Not stated Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI + coronary insufficiency + angina Age, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol 2.9* all women (clerical women RR=5.2) no association in men. Ecological exposure was associated with risk in men and women +
Alfredsson 1985w38 Sweden 958 096 (51) 20-64 Hectic work and few possibilities for learning (ecological)  1 1201 Non-fatal MI (hospitalisation) Age, 10 sociodemographic factors, smoking, heavy lifting 1.5* +
Haan 1988w39 Finland 902 (33) factory workers 20-62 Job control, physical strain, variety (individual) 10   60 Fatal CHD + and non-fatal CHD Age, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol, alcohol, relative weight 4.95* for low control, low variety, high physical strain ++
Reed 1989w40 Hawaii (Japanese ancestry) 4737 (0) 45-65 Job control, demands and their interaction (ecological) 18  359 Fatal CHD and non-fatal MI Age No effect of control, demands or their interaction (ns trend for lower strain men to have higher CHD) 0
Netterstrom 1993w41 Denmark 2045 (0) bus drivers 21-64 Job variety, satisfaction 10   59 Fatal CHD Age 2.1*—high job variety and satisfaction associated with CHD risk 0
Suadicani 1993w42 Denmark 1752 (0) 59 (mean) Job influence, monotony, pace, satisfaction, ability to relax  3   46 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI None Only inability to relax after work associated with CHD 0
Alterman 1994w43 USA 1683 (0) 38-56 Job control, demands and their interaction (ecological) 25  283 Fatal CHD Age 1.4 for job strain 0
Bosma 1997w44 UK 10 308 (33) civil servants 35-55 Job control, demands (individual, assessed twice 3 years apart, and ecological)  5  654 Angina + doctor diagnosed ischaemia Age, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol, body mass index, employment grade 1.93* self reported or externally assessed low job control predicted CHD +
Lynch 1997w45 Finland 1727 (0) 42-60 Job demands, resources, income 8.1   89 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI Age, behavioural, biological and psychosocial covariates 1.57* for the effect of high demands, low resources and low income; 2.59 when adjustment made for age only +
Steenland 1997w46 USA 3575 (0) 25-74 Job control and demands (ecological) 14  519 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI Age, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol, education, body mass index, self reported diabetes 1.41* for low control +
Prognostic studies
Hlatky 1995w47 USA 1489 (24) employed patients undergoing coronary angiography 41-59 Job control, demands (individual)  5  112 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI prevalence of coronary artery disease Ejection fraction, extent of coronary atherosclerosis, myocardial ischaemia 0.96 for effect of job strain on events. Job strain was associated with normal coronary arteries -
Hoffmann 1995w48 Switzerland 222 (0) after first MI 30-60 Job work load, locus of control, social supports  1   19 All cause mortality, reinfarction, severe symptoms, or poor exercise capacity Age, severity of MI, exercise High workload and low external locus of control associated with outcome +

CHD=coronary heart disease; MI=myocardial infarction. 

*

P<0.05. 0=no association—that is, relative risk not significantly different from unity; +=moderate association (relative risk >1⩽2.0); ++=strong association (relative risk >2.0) association. 

Social network structure and quality of social support

Social supports and networks relate to both the number of a person’s social contacts and their quality (including emotional support and confiding support). Marital status—information routinely sought in clinical practice—is a simple measure of social support, and the ability of low social support to predict all cause mortality has long been recognised. It has been proposed that social supports may act to buffer the effect of various environmental stressors and hence increase susceptibility to disease,27 but most of the evidence supports a direct role.

Five of the eight prospective cohort studies that investigated aspects of social support in relation to the incidence of coronary heart disease were positive (table 4). Nine of the 10 prognostic studies were positive, and the relative risks for three of these studies exceeded 3. Despite the strength and consistency of these findings, the relative effect of structural and functional aspects of social supports has yet to be delineated.

Table 4.

Studies of social networks and social supports and coronary heart disease. References in table are given on BMJ website

Author, year, country Total sample (% women) Age at entry Exposure Follow up (years) No of events Type of events Adjustments Relative risk* Summary
Prospective aetiological studies
Medalie 1976w49 Israel 10 000 (0) >40 Perceived love and support from spouse  5 300 Angina Age, blood pressure, cholesterol, diabetes, ECG abnormalities 1.8* +
House 1982w50 USA 2754 (52) 35-69 Social relationships and activities 11 114 Fatal CHD Age, baseline CHD, smoking, forced expiratory volume at 1 second Not stated +
Berkman 1983w51 USA 4725 (53) 30-69 Social network index  9 120 Fatal CHD Age 2.13* ++
Reed 1983w52 USA 4653 (0) 52-71 Social network score  6 218 Fatal CHD + non-fatal Age, blood pressure, cholesterol, glucose, uric acid, forced vital capacity, body mass index, exercise, alcohol, complex carbohydrate Social network associated with CHD prevalence, but not incidence 0
Kaplan 1988w53 Finland 13301 39-59 Social network index  5 223 Fatal CHD Age, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol, prevalent illness, urban/ rural residence 1.34 for men but not women 0
Vogt 1992w54 USA 2603 (54)  18-75+ Network scope, network frequency, and network size 15 not stated Fatal CHD + non-fatal CHD Age, sex, SES, smoking and subjective health status at baseline 1.5* for effect of network scope on CHD incidence; all 3 measures predicted survival in those with CHD +
Orth-Gomer 1993w55 Sweden 736 (0) 50 Emotional support from close people and support from extended network (social integration)  6  25 Fatal CHD + non-fatal CHD Age, cholesterol treatment of hypertension, diabetes, body mass index< smoking, physical activity 3.8* for social integration 3.1 for emotional support ++
Kawachi 1996w56 USA 36 624 (0) 42-77 Social network index  4 403 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI Age, time period, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol, diabetes, angina, body mass index, family history, alcohol, exercise 1.14. Some evidence for association with fatal CHD (particularly non-sudden cardiac death) rather than non-fatal MI 0
Prognostic studies
Chandra 1983w57 USA 1401 Not stated Marital status 10 Not stated All cause mortality Age, race, smoking, severity of MI, medical care factors Married men and women had better in hospital and 10 year survival +
Ruberman 1984w58 USA 2320 (0) patients with MI 30-69 Social support, life stress  3 128 All cause mortality, sudden cardiac death Age, myocardial function, ventricular arrhythmia, smoking 4.5* for the effect of social isolation + high life stress on all cause mortality; 5.62 for sudden cardiac death ++
Wiklund 1988w59 Sweden 201(0) patients with first MI 32-60 Social support, depression and other psychosocial factors 8.3  85 All cause mortality + recurrent non-fatal MI Hypertension, smoking, angina Being single increased risk of death +
Case 1992w60 USA 1234 (38) participants in diltiazem post-MI trial 25-75 Living alone, disrupted marriage  2 226 Fatal CHD + recurrent non-fatal MI New York Heart Assocation functional class, ejection fraction, education, no β blockers, ventricular premature complexes, prior infarction 1.54* for effect of living alone. No effect of marital disruption +
Hedblad 199261 Sweden 98 (0) men with ischaemic 24 hour ECG 68 Social support and social network  5  17 Fatal CHD + non-fatal MI Age, smoking, blood pressure, cholesterol, alcohol, exercise, body mass index, triglycerides 5.6* and 4.1* for low informational support and low emotional support respectively ++
Williams 1992w62 USA 1368 (18) patients with angiographicdis ease 52 (median) Structural social support (marital status) and function social support  9 249 All cause mortality Age, ejection fraction, non-invasive myocardial damage index, conduction disturbance, pain/ ischaemic index, mitral regurgitation, number of diseased vessels, % stenosis of left main stem and left anterior descending artery 3.34* for effect of unmarried patients without confidant ++
Berkman 1992w63 USA 194 (48) patients with acute MI 65-85+ Emotional support 0.5  76 All cause mortality Age, sex, Killip class, ejection fraction, reinfarction, comorbidity, functional disability, previous MI, ventricular tachycardia 2.9* for lack of emotional support +
Gorkin 1993w64 USA 1322 (17) patients with previous MI + ventricular premature complexes 60.8 (SD 9.9) Social support 0.8 Not stated All cause mortality Ejection fraction, arrhythmia rates, CHD risk factors, 1.46* for 1 point decrease in social support +
Jenkinson 1993w19 UK 1376 (22) 7 days after MI 25-84 Social isolation, life stress, depression, type A  3 247 All cause mortality Age, previous MI, hospital complications, diabetes, hypertension, car ownership, sex 1.33 for social support; no effect of type A or depression 0
Friedman 1995w65 USA 369 (15) patients after acute MI with ventricular arrhythmias in the CAST 63 (SD 9) Social support, life events, depression, anxiety, type A, anger  1  20 All cause mortality Physiological severity, demographic and other psychosocial factors Not stated +

CHD=coronary heart disease; MI=myocardial infarction; CAST=cardiac arrhythmia suppression trial. 

*

P<0.05. 0=no association—that is, relative risk not significantly different from unity; +=moderate association (relative risk >1⩽2.0); ++=strong association (relative risk >2.0). 

Modification of psychosocial factors

The main implications of these findings for clinical practice are summarised in the box. A recent meta-analysis found that psychosocial interventions are associated with improved survival after myocardial infarction.28 However, two recent large randomised controlled trials of psychological rehabilitation after myocardial infarction found no difference in anxiety and depression, and this may in part explain the lack of effect on mortality.29,30 Randomised controlled trials of modification of social supports after myocardial infarction show a decrease in cardiac death or reinfarction rates.31 A patient’s social circumstances should be elicited as part of the history, and the doctor may have a role in mobilising social support. A multicentre trial of 3000 patients after myocardial infarction (ENRICHD—enhancing recovery in coronary heart disease) is currently under way in the United States. It will target patients at high psychosocial risk (those who are depressed or socially isolated) and enrol large numbers of women and ethnic minorities.

Psychosocial components of secondary prevention

Clinicians should consider:

  • Detecting and treating depression

  • Mobilising social support

  • Using socioeconomic status and psychosocial factors to risk stratify patients

The potential for primary prevention in relation to psychosocial factors lies largely outside the remit of clinicians. Psychosocial factors themselves are determined largely by social, political, and economic factors and it is therefore policy makers who influence the structure and function of communities—in the public and private domains—who may have scope for primary prevention.

Conclusion

Of the large number of psychosocial factors that have been studied, only four met the quality filter: type A/hostility, depression and anxiety, work characteristics, and social supports. While this review cannot discount the possibility of publication bias, the prospective observational studies show aetiological roles for social supports, depression and anxiety, and work characteristics and prognostic roles for social supports and depression. Further evidence of a causal role is provided by human and other primate evidence of biological and behavioural pathways mediating these effects. However, conflicting data exist on whether psychosocial interventions reduce mortality after myocardial infarction. This systematic review should be updated and expanded to include other observational study designs and other endpoints (for example, all cause mortality) in order to focus future research and, ultimately, policy. In this expanding area, future primary research might investigate the:

  • Interrelationships between different psychosocial factors

  • Effect of change in and cumulative exposure to psychosocial factors

  • Short and long term effects thoughout the life course

  • Differences by sex, ethnic group, and country

  • Behavioural and biological mechanisms involved

  • Effect of psychosocial factors on different clinical and subclinical outcomes

  • Appropriate primary and secondary preventive measures.

Supplementary Material

[extra: references]

Acknowledgments

The comments of Lisa Berkman, Ichiro Kawachi, Redford Williams, and Mandy Feeney are gratefully acknowledged. To keep this review complete and up to date, we would be grateful to be informed of studies which we may have missed.

Footnotes

This is the third of four articles

Funding: MM is supported by an MRC research professorship; his group investigating pyschosocial factors and health has been supported by the Agency for Health Care Policy and Research (5 RO1 HS06516); the New England Medical Centre-Division of Health Improvement; the National Heart Lung and Blood Institute (2RO1 HL36310); National Institute on Aging (RO1 AG13196-02); the John D and Catherine T MacArthur Foundation Research Network on Successful Midlife Development; the Institute for Work and Health, Ontario, Canada; the Volvo Research Foundation, Sweden; Medical Research Council; Health and Safety Executive; and British Heart Foundation (RG/28).

website extra: References in the tables are given on the BMJ website www.bmj.com

References

  • 1.Marmot MG. Improvement of social environment to improve health. Lancet. 1998;351:57–60. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)08084-7. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hecker MHL, Chesney MA, Black GW. Coronary prone behaviour in the Western Collaborative Group study. Psychosom Med. 1988;50:153–164. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198803000-00005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Dembroski TM, MacDougall JM, Costa PT., Jr Components of hostility as predictors of sudden death and myocardial infarction in the multiple risk factor intervention trial. Psychosom Med. 1989;51:514–522. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198909000-00003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Johnson JV, Stewart W, Hall EM, Fredlund P, Theorell T. Long-term psychosocial work environment and cardiovascular mortality among Swedish men. Am J Public Health. 1996;86:325–331. doi: 10.2105/ajph.86.3.324. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Kubzansky LD, Kawachi I, Weiss ST, Sparrow D. Anxiety and coronary heart disease: a synthesis of epidemiological, psychological and experimental evidence. Ann Behav Med. 1998;20:47–58. doi: 10.1007/BF02884448. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hayward C. Psychiatric illness and cardiovascular disease risk. Epidemiol Rev. 1995;17:129–138. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.epirev.a036169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Miller TQ, Smith TW, Turner CW, Guijarro ML, Hallet AJ. A meta-analytic review of research on hostility and physical health. Psychol Bull. 1996;119:322–348. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.119.2.322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Schnall PL, Landsbergis PA. Job strain and cardiovascular disease. Ann Rev Public Health. 1994;15:381–411. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pu.15.050194.002121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Schneiderman N, Skyler JS. Insulin metabolism, sympathetic nervous system regulation, and coronary heart disease prevention. In: Orth-Gomer K, Schneiderman N, editors. Behavioural medicine approaches to cardiovascular disease prevention. Mawah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1996. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Brunner EJ. Stress and the biology of inequality. BMJ. 1997;314:1472–1476. doi: 10.1136/bmj.314.7092.1472. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Williams RB, Barefoot JC, Blumenthal JA, Helms MJ, Luecken L, Pieper CF, et al. Psychosocial correlates of job strain in a sample of working women. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1997;54:543–548. doi: 10.1001/archpsyc.1997.01830180061007. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kuh D, Ben-Shlomo Y. A life course approach to chronic disease epidemiology. New York: Oxford University Press; 1997. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Pieper C, Lacroix AZ, Karasek RA. The relation of psychosocial dimensions of work with coronary heart disease risk factors: a meta-analysis of five United States data bases. Am J Epidemiol. 1989;129:483–494. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a115159. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Kaplan GA, Keil JE. Socioeconomic factors and cardiovascular disease: a review of the literature. Circulation. 1993;88:1973–1998. doi: 10.1161/01.cir.88.4.1973. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Marmot M, Bosma H, Hemingway H, Brunner EJ, Stansfeld SA. Contribution of job control and other risk factors to social variations in coronary heart disease incidence. Lancet. 1997;350:235–239. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(97)04244-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Mittleman MA, Maclure M, Nachnani M, Sherwood JB, Muller JE. Educational attainment, anger and the risk of triggering myocardial infarction onset. The Determinants of Myocardial Infarction Onset Study Investigators. Arch Intern Med. 1997;157:769–775. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Nunes EV, Frank KA, Kornfield DS. Psychologic treatment for the type A behaviour pattern and for coronary heart disease: a meta-analysis of the literature. Psychosom Med. 1987;48:159–173. doi: 10.1097/00006842-198703000-00006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Haynes SG, Feinleib M, Kannel WB. The relationship of psychosocial factors to coronary heart disease in the Framingham study: 3. Eight year incidence of coronary heart disease. Am J Epidemiol. 1980;111:37–58. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a112873. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Rosenman RH, Brand RJ, Sholtz RI, Friedman M. Multivariate prediction of coronary heart disease during 8.5 year follow-up in Western Collaborative Group Study. Am J Cardiol. 1976;37:903–909. doi: 10.1016/0002-9149(76)90117-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Shekelle RB, Hulley SB, Neaton JD, Billings J, Borhani NO, Gerace TA, et al. The MRFIT behavior pattern study. II. Type A behavior and incidence of coronary heart disease. Am J Epidemiol. 1985;122:559–570. doi: 10.1093/oxfordjournals.aje.a114135. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Johnston DW, Cook DG, Shaper AG. Type A behaviour and ischaemic heart disease in middle-aged British men. BMJ. 1987;295:86–89. doi: 10.1136/bmj.295.6590.86. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Hearn M, Murray DM, Luepker RB. Hostility, coronary heart disease and total mortality: a 33 year follow up study of university students. J Behav Med. 1989;12:105–121. doi: 10.1007/BF00846545. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Wassertheil-Smoller S, Applegate WB, Berge K, Chang CJ, Davis BR, Grimm R, Jr, et al. Change in depression as a precursor of cardiovascular events. Arch Intern Med. 1996;156:553–561. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bosma H, Marmot MG, Hemingway H, Nicholson A, Brunner EJ, Stansfeld S. Low job control and risk of coronary heart disease in the Whitehall II (prospective cohort) study. BM J. 1997;314:558–565. doi: 10.1136/bmj.314.7080.558. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Siegrist J, Peter R, Junge A, Cremer P, Seidel D. Low status control, high effort at work and ischemic heart disease: prospective evidence from blue-collar men. Soc Sci Med. 1990;31:1127–1134. doi: 10.1016/0277-9536(90)90234-j. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Bosma H, Peter R, Siegrist J, Marmot MG. Alternative job stress models and the risk of coronary heart disease: the effort-reward imbalance model and the job strain model. Am J Public Health. 1998;88:68–74. doi: 10.2105/ajph.88.1.68. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Alloway R. The buffer theory of social support-a review of the literature. Psychol Med. 1987;17:91–108. doi: 10.1017/s0033291700013015. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Linden W, Stossel C, Maurice J. Psychosocial interventions in patients with coronary artery disease: a meta-analysis. Arch Intern Med. 1996;156:745–752. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Jones DA, West RR. Psychological rehabilitation after myocardial infarction: multicentre randomised controlled trial. BMJ. 1996;313:1517–1521. doi: 10.1136/bmj.313.7071.1517. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Frasure Smith N, Lesperance F, Prince RH, Verrier P, Garber RA, Juneau M, et al. Randomised trail of home-based psychosocial nursing intervention for patients recovering from myocardial infarction. Lancet. 1997;350:473–479. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(97)02142-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bucher HC. Social support and prognosis following first myocardial infarction. J Gen Int Med. 1994;9:409–417. doi: 10.1007/BF02629526. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

[extra: references]

Articles from BMJ : British Medical Journal are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES