Skip to main content

This is a preprint.

It has not yet been peer reviewed by a journal.

The National Library of Medicine is running a pilot to include preprints that result from research funded by NIH in PMC and PubMed.

Research Square logoLink to Research Square
[Preprint]. 2024 May 30:rs.3.rs-4271576. [Version 1] doi: 10.21203/rs.3.rs-4271576/v1

Is 70 the new 60? A longitudinal analysis of cohort trends inintrinsic capacity in England and China

John Beard 1, Hanewald Katja 2, Yafei Si 3, Jotheeswaran Thiyagarajan 4, Dario Moreno-Agostino 5
PMCID: PMC11160896  PMID: 38853854

Abstract

To understand how the health of older adults today compares to that of previous generations, we estimated intrinsic capacity and subdomains of cognitive, locomotor, sensory, psychological and vitality capacities in participants of the English Longitudinal Study on Ageing (ELSA) and the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS). We applied multilevel growth curve models to examine change over time and cohort trends. We found that more recent cohorts entered older ages with higher levels of capacity, and their subsequent age-related declines were somewhat compressed compared to earlier cohorts. These improvements in capacity were large, with the greatest gains being in the most recent cohorts. For example, a 68-year-old ELSA participant born in 1950 had higher capacity than a 62-year-old born just 10 years earlier. Trends were similar for men and women, and findings were generally consistent across English and Chinese cohorts.

INTRODUCTION

Over the past century, life expectancy has risen in almost every country, and longer lives are increasingly becoming the norm.1 Initially, this trend was driven by increased survival through childhood and childbirth, but in more developed countries, it is now mainly a consequence of longer survival at older ages.2 Yet, while these improvements in life expectancy are well documented, it is still uncertain how the health of older people today compares to that experienced by previous generations at similar ages.36

One reason for this uncertainty is that health is a multifaceted concept, and no consensus has been reached on how to frame or measure it.7 Temporal trends are particularly difficult to interpret when considered from the perspective of disease.6 Broader access to effective health care means that some people who would previously have died from a condition now survive into older ages, and these improvements in survival have led to greater disease prevalence.6 However, the advances in medical care that increase survival may have also reduced the impact that these conditions have on people’s lives. Treatments for other conditions, too, may have lessened their influence. For example, a person with osteoarthritis of the hip, who might have previously experienced severe disability, may now regain high levels of locomotor capacity following joint replacement.8 Other people may live much of their lives free of disease but still experience declines in physical or mental capacities.9

Furthermore, older people often report that the health outcome they value most is not the presence or absence of disease, or even life extension, but their level of functioning and independence.10 Yet trends in functioning have also been difficult to assess. For example, to explore changing patterns of disability, the Global Burden of Disease group has applied disability weightings to trends in disease prevalence. This analysis suggests that health-adjusted life expectancies are not keeping pace with increases in life expectancy.11. However, these disability weights are only indirect, generic estimates of a disease’s impact on functioning and cannot account for geographic or temporal variations in access to health care or other resources that may mitigate the consequences of these conditions.12

Meanwhile, most of the research that directly estimates functional trends has been limited to measures of severe disability, such as Activities of Daily Living (ADL) or Instrumental Activities of Daily Living (IADL). The findings of these studies have been varied and inconclusive.1317 Moreover, since loss of ADLs or IADLs only becomes apparent after very significant declines in functioning, these categorical outcomes tell us little about the functional status of the far broader population of older adults who have not experienced these major losses.

The World Health Organization (WHO) has proposed a framework for understanding ageing and health that allows a more nuanced consideration of trends in functioning and health across the full range of ageing populations. In this model, healthy ageing is considered not from the perspective of disease but based on an individual’s ability to be and do the things they value.1 This ability is understood to be determined by individual-level attributes – a person’s “intrinsic capacity”, as well as the environments they inhabit and the interaction between the individual and these environments.18 Since intrinsic capacity is framed as a continuum that can be considered across the second half of life, it can potentially be used to compare incremental changes among both relatively robust and severely disabled individuals.

We have previously examined intrinsic capacity in two large longitudinal studies of the English and Chinese populations: the English Longitudinal Study on Ageing (ELSA)9 and the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS).19 Both analyses identified an intrinsic capacity construct comprising subdomains of cognitive, locomotor, sensory and psychological capacity and a further subdomain labelled vitality, which may represent underlying age-related biological changes and energy balance. This structure is consistent with what had been previously suggested by gerontological theory.20 Our analyses showed intrinsic capacity to have strong construct validity and to be a powerful predictor of subsequent care dependence even after adjustment for multimorbidity, age, gender and socioeconomic status. Subsequent research has shown it to also predict mortality and specific conditions.21

The aim of this paper is to conduct a longitudinal analysis of cohort trends in intrinsic capacity in these same studies to determine whether older adults in England and China are experiencing the same, better or worse capacity than people of similar ages in the past. We undertook secondary analysis of data from ELSA using multilevel growth curve models to identify period and cohort trends in intrinsic capacity and its subdomains. We then applied the same methods for a comparative analysis of the CHARLS cohort.

RESULTS

Main analyses – ELSA

The sample for the main analyses (ELSA) included 14,710 different participants aged 60+, including 53.3% women (n = 7,841). The median birth year was 1940 (interquartile range, IQR: 1931–1948), and the median number of observations was 4 (IQR: 2–6).

CFA models with a bifactor structure consisting of intrinsic capacity and five subdomains (cognitive, locomotor, sensory, psychological and vitality, as in Beard et al. 2019), led to non-significant loadings of the locomotor indicators on their subdomain, suggesting the collapse of this specific factor once the common factor (intrinsic capacity) had accounted for the common variance across these indicators. Therefore, the bifactor model was respecified, removing the “locomotor” subdomain. The updated configural bifactor model in ELSA showed good fit (Table 1, upper section). Additional equality constraints to the loadings and thresholds across time points did not lead to a loss in fit (rather, fit increased due to the increased model parsimony). Based on these results, scalar invariance of the proposed intrinsic capacity bifactor model was assumed to hold, and similar evidence was found for the correlated factors model, thus enabling comparisons of the levels in the intrinsic capacity factors and all the different subdomains over time.

Table 1.

Results from the measurement invariance testing.

Model Invariance level Chi-2 (df) RMSEA (90% CI) CFI TLI ΔRMSEA ΔCFI
Main analysis (ELSA: n = 3,246) Bifactor model Configural 1691 (357) 0.034 (0.032, 0.036) 0.981 0.975
Scalar 1962 (441) 0.033 (0.031, 0.034) 0.978 0.977 0.001 −0.003
Correlated factors model Configural 1874 (375) 0.035 (0.034, 0.037) 0.978 0.973
Scalar 2374 (429) 0.037 (0.036, 0.039) 0.972 0.970 −0.002 −0.006
Comparative analysis (CHARLS: n = 5,57l) Bifactor model Configural 2378 (234) 0.041 (0.039, 0.042) 0.958 0.945
Scalar 2789 (289) 0.039 (0.038, 0.041) 0.951 0.948 0.002 −0.007
Correlated factors model Configural 2118 (253) 0.036 (0.035, 0.038) 0.963 0.956
Scalar 2967 (289) 0.041 (0.039, 0.042) 0.947 0.944 −0.005 −0.016

Note. df: degrees of freedom; RMSEA: Root Mean Square Error of Approximation; CFI: Comparative Fit Index; TLI: Tucker-Lewis Index; ΔRMSEA: difference in RMSEA; ΔCFI: difference in TLI. Scalar invariance is usually deemed to hold if the difference between the configural model in the RMSEA (ΔRMSEA) and the Comparative Fit Index (CFI) is smaller than 0.015 and 0.010, respectively. Configural invariance is usually deemed to hold if the values of the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) for the configural model were below 0.060 and above 0.950, respectively (24).

Factor scores were then derived from the bifactor model (intrinsic capacity) and correlated factor models (psychological, locomotor, vitality, cognitive, and sensory), including all observations with at least partial information between waves 1 and 9 of ELSA. The results of the multilevel growth curve models computed using these scores are shown in Table 2 (left section), and the corresponding marginal mean predicted levels for different birth cohorts are plotted by age in Fig. 1. All confidence intervals (CI) were constructed at a 95% confidence level.

Table 2.

Results from the multilevel growth curve models performed in ELSA and CHARLS

Main analyses (ELSA) Comparative analyses (CHARLS)
Coef. [95% Conf. Interval] p-value Coef. [95% Conf. Interval] p-value
Intrinsic Capacity Linear change −0.041 −0.055 −0.027 < 0.001 −0.033 −0.044 −0.022 < 0.001
Quadratic change −0.001 −0.002 0.000 0.031
Birth year 0.046 0.043 0.048 < 0.001 0.035 0.032 0.037 < 0.001
Linear change * birth year 0.001 0.000 0.002 0.004 0.002 0.001 0.002 < 0.001
Quadratic change * birth year 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.945
Intercept −0.706 −0.753 −0.659 < 0.001 −0.518 −0.551 −0.484 < 0.001
Slope variance 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.037 0.035 0.039
Intercept variance 0.397 0.376 0.419 0.572 0.551 0.593
Slope-intercept covariance 0.010 0.011 0.008 0.073 0.079 0.068
Psychological Linear change −0.004 −0.017 0.008 0.491 0.026 0.014 0.039 < 0.001
Quadratic change −0.002 −0.002 −0.001 < 0.001
Birth year 0.020 0.018 0.023 < 0.001 0.014 0.012 0.017 < 0.001
Linear change * birth year 0.001 0.000 0.001 0.135 −0.001 −0.002 0.000 0.013
Quadratic change * birth year 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.286
Intercept −0.455 −0.500 −0.411 < 0.001 −0.269 −0.304 −0.234 < 0.001
Slope variance 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.061 0.058 0.064
Intercept variance 0.355 0.336 0.374 0.683 0.659 0.709
Slope-intercept covariance 0.009 0.011 0.008 0.119 0.126 0.112
Locomotor Linear change −0.044 −0.060 −0.029 < 0.001 −0.020 −0.032 −0.007 0.003
Quadratic change −0.001 −0.002 0.000 0.175
Birth year 0.048 0.045 0.051 < 0.001 0.034 0.032 0.036 < 0.001
Linear change * birth year 0.001 0.000 0.002 0.011 0.003 0.003 0.004 < 0.001
Quadratic change * birth year 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.891
Intercept −0.719 −0.770 −0.668 < 0.001 −0.597 −0.632 −0.562 < 0.001
Slope variance 0.001 0.001 0.001 0.045 0.043 0.048
Intercept variance 0.420 0.396 0.446 0.529 0.506 0.552
Slope-intercept covariance 0.011 0.013 0.009 0.093 0.100 0.087
Vitality Linear change −0.020 −0.032 −0.008 0.001 −0.065 −0.079 −0.052 < 0.001
Quadratic change 0.000 −0.001 0.000 0.196
Birth year 0.043 0.040 0.045 < 0.001 0.037 0.034 0.039 < 0.001
Linear change * birth year −0.001 −0.002 0.000 0.001 0.003 0.002 0.003 < 0.001
Quadratic change * birth year 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.002
Intercept −0.576 −0.619 −0.533 < 0.001 −0.498 −0.533 −0.463 < 0.001
Slope variance 0.000 0.000 0.000
Intercept variance 0.280 0.265 0.296 0.409 0.398 0.421
Slope-intercept covariance 0.006 0.007 0.005
Cognition Linear change −0.027 −0.044 −0.011 0.001 −0.089 −0.102 −0.077 < 0.001
Quadratic change −0.003 −0.004 −0.002 < 0.001
Birth year 0.042 0.038 0.045 < 0.001 0.032 0.030 0.035 < 0.001
Linear change * birth year 0.002 0.001 0.003 < 0.001 0.002 0.001 0.003 < 0.001
Quadratic change * birth year 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.316
Intercept −0.673 −0.730 −0.616 < 0.001 −0.370 −0.404 −0.335 < 0.001
Slope variance 0.002 0.001 0.002 0.062 0.059 0.065
Intercept variance 0.388 0.366 0.413 0.683 0.654 0.714
Slope-intercept covariance 0.008 0.010 0.006 0.116 0.125 0.108
Sensory Linear change −0.013 −0.031 0.004 0.139 0.015 0.002 0.029 0.025
Quadratic change −0.002 −0.003 −0.001 0.003
Birth year 0.024 0.020 0.027 < 0.001 0.022 0.019 0.025 < 0.001
Linear change * birth year 0.000 −0.001 0.001 0.412 −0.001 −0.002 −0.001 0.001
Quadratic change * birth year 0.000 0.000 0.000 0.119
Intercept −0.382 −0.443 −0.320 < 0.001 −0.298 −0.336 −0.260 < 0.001
Slope variance 0.002 0.002 0.002 0.062 0.059 0.066
Intercept variance 0.489 0.462 0.518 0.719 0.691 0.748
Slope-intercept covariance 0.018 0.021 0.016 0.133 0.142 0.125

Note. Coef. and 95% Conf. Interval denote the coefficient and its 95% confidence intervals from multilevel growth curve models. For ELSA, longitudinal individual weights were used for multilevel growth curve models to ensure representativeness to participants aged 50 + and living in England in 2002 and still alive and residing in private households by wave 9. For CHARLS, survey weights were used for multilevel growth curve models to restore representativeness to participants aged 50 + and living in China. Random effects are included in italics.

Figure 1.

Figure 1

Intrinsic capacity and subdomains mean factor scores by birth cohort and age in ELSA (main analyses).

Note: Longitudinal individual weights were used for multilevel growth curve models to ensure representativeness to participants aged 50+ and living in England in 2002 who were still alive and residing in private households by wave 9. The point estimates and their 95% confidence intervals are displayed in the graph.

More recent cohorts entered older ages with significantly higher levels of intrinsic capacity (Birth year: 0.046 (CI: 0.043–0.048), p < 0.001). While intrinsic capacity levels declined with increasing age across all cohorts, these declines were less steep for more recent cohorts than for earlier cohorts (linear change * birth year: 0.001 (CI: 0.000–0.002), p = 0.004).

More recent cohorts also entered older ages with significantly higher levels for each subdomain of capacity, with these improvements being largest in the locomotor, vitality and cognition subdomains. As with intrinsic capacity as a whole, declines with increasing age were observed across all subdomains. However, declines in locomotor and cognitive subdomains were less steep among more recent cohorts. For vitality, they were initially faster among younger cohorts (linear change * birth year: −0.001 (CI: −0.002–0.000), p = 0.001) but subsequently followed more stable levels over time (Quadratic change * birth year: 0.000 (CI: 0.000–0.000), p = 0.002).

To quantify the observed improvements shown in Fig. 1, we calculated the marginal mean predicted levels for each age by cohort (Table 3 and Supplement 4). Even when comparisons are limited to cohort participants for whom data is available at the same age, the observed improvements are large. For example, intrinsic capacity of the cohort born in 1950 at age 68 was 0.280 (CI: 0.248–0.313), significantly higher than the 0.208 (CI 0.183–0.233) of the cohort born in 1940 at age 62. The pattern was similar for psychological, locomotor, cognitive and sensory capacities, although the improvement was most pronounced for cognitive capacity. The greatest improvements were between the most recent (1950) cohort and the 1940 cohort, although direct comparisons between earlier cohorts also showed significant improvements in the more recent cohort (Supplement 4). If these directly observed trends were extrapolated to compare the earliest with the most recent cohort, the improvements would be significantly greater than those we could observe directly.

Table 3.

Scores by birth cohort and age in ELSA (1940 and 1950 cohorts)

INTRINSIC CAPACITY LOCOMOTOR CAPACITY
1950sCohort 1940s Cohort 1950s Cohort 1940s Cohort
Age Mean Lower Higher Mean Lower Higher Mean Lower Higher Mean Lower Higher
52 0.665 0.619 0.711 0.71 0.662 0.758
54 0.642 0.609 0.675 0.678 0.644 0.713
56 0.612 0.586 0.638 0.641 0.614 0.667
58 0.575 0.551 0.599 0.597 0.573 0.622
60 0.531 0.506 0.555 0.548 0.522 0.573
62 0.479 0.454 0.504 0.208 0.183 0.233 0.492 0.466 0.517 0.233 0.208 0.259
64 0.42 0.394 0.446 0.164 0.145 0.183 0.43 0.404 0.455 0.182 0.163 0.201
66 0.354 0.327 0.381 0.113 0.098 0.129 0.361 0.334 0.389 0.125 0.109 0.141
68 0.280 0.248 0.313 0.055 0.04 0.07 0.287 0.254 0.32 0.062 0.047 0.077
70 −0.011 −0.026 0.005 −0.007 −0.023 0.009
72 −0.084 −0.099 −0.068 −0.082 −0.098 −0.066
74 −0.164 −0.18 −0.148 −0.163 −0.179 −0.147
76 −0.252 −0.269 −0.235 −0.249 −0.267 −0.231
78 −0.347 −0.368 −0.326 −0.342 −0.364 −0.319
VITALITY COGNITIVE CAPACITY
Age 1950s Cohort 1940s Cohort 1950s Cohort 1940s Cohort
52 0.709 0.663 0.754 0.573 0.516 0.629
54 0.609 0.576 0.641 0.611 0.573 0.65
56 0.518 0.493 0.542 0.635 0.606 0.664
58 0.437 0.415 0.458 0.644 0.618 0.671
60 0.365 0.344 0.386 0.638 0.61 0.666
62 0.303 0.282 0.324 0.280 0.255 0.306 0.618 0.588 0.647 0.157 0.126 0.189
64 0.251 0.23 0.272 0.2 0.181 0.218 0.582 0.551 0.613 0.162 0.14 0.183
66 0.208 0.186 0.23 0.124 0.109 0.138 0.532 0.498 0.566 0.149 0.132 0.166
68 0.174 0.148 0.201 0.053 0.04 0.066 0.467 0.425 0.508 0.12 0.103 0.136
70 −0.013 −0.025 0 0.074 0.056 0.091
72 −0.073 −0.086 −0.061 0.011 −0.007 0.029
74 −0.129 −0.141 −0.117 −0.069 −0.088 −0.05
76 −0.179 −0.193 −0.166 −0.165 −0.187 −0.144
78 −0.225 −0.242 −0.208 −0.278 −0.306 −0.251
PSYCHOLOGICAL CAPACITY SENSORY CAPACITY
Age 1950s Cohort 1940s Cohort 1950s Cohort 1940s Cohort
52 0.153 0.103 0.204 0.332 0.268 0.397
54 0.172 0.137 0.207 0.327 0.284 0.371
56 0.183 0.156 0.209 0.319 0.287 0.35
58 0.186 0.162 0.21 0.307 0.279 0.335
60 0.181 0.157 0.206 0.292 0.264 0.32
62 0.169 0.144 0.195 −0.05 −0.079 −0.02 0.274 0.245 0.303 0.094 0.058 0.13
64 0.15 0.125 0.175 −0.042 −0.063 −0.022 0.252 0.223 0.281 0.08 0.055 0.104
66 0.123 0.096 0.149 −0.044 −0.061 −0.028 0.227 0.195 0.259 0.059 0.04 0.077
68 0.088 0.056 0.119 −0.056 −0.071 −0.041 0.199 0.157 0.24 0.031 0.014 0.048
70 −0.076 −0.092 −0.061 −0.004 −0.021 0.014
72 −0.106 −0.122 −0.091 −0.045 −0.063 −0.027
74 −0.145 −0.161 −0.13 −0.092 −0.111 −0.074
76 −0.194 −0.21 −0.177 −0.147 −0.167 −0.126
78 −0.251 −0.272 −0.231 −0.208 −0.235 −0.18

Comparative analyses – CHARLS

The sample for the comparative analyses (CHARLS) included 11,411 participants aged 60+, including 50.0% women (n = 5,706). The median birth year was 1947 (IQR: 1941–1951), and the median number of observations was 2 (IQR: 1–3).

The results from the measurement invariance testing (Table 1, lower section) suggested that scalar invariance held for the bifactor model (intrinsic capacity). However, the restrictions imposed to fit the scalar model in the correlated factors model resulted in a substantial loss in fit according to the change in CFI (change in RMSEA was within boundaries). Factor scores for each subdomain were derived from but interpreted with additional caution in the case of the subdomains, as changes in the score levels could be due to differences in the measurement parameters across the time points. Therefore, trajectories in the subdomains for the CHARLS need to be considered with caution.

Coefficients from the multilevel growth curve models estimated with CHARLS data as part of the comparative analyses are shown in Table 2 (right section), and the corresponding marginal mean predicted levels for different birth years are plotted by age in Fig. 2. Consistent with the main analyses for ELSA, more recent cohorts entered older ages with higher levels of capacity (Birth year: 0.035 (CI: 0.032–0.037), p < 0.001). The largest improvements were found for the vitality subdomain, followed by the locomotor, cognition, sensory and, finally, psychological factors. Intrinsic capacity was found to decline significantly with age, and subsequent declines for more recent cohorts were less steep than for earlier cohorts (Linear change * birth year: 0.002 (CI: 0.001–0.002), p < 0.001). Findings for the subdomains may be at least partly attributed to the lack of measurement invariance of the subdomain analysis outlined above. As with the main analysis, steeper declines with increasing age were observed among earlier cohorts in locomotor, vitality, and cognition. However, for the psychological and sensory subdomains, changes over time were found to be positive with increasing age overall (Linear change), with more recent cohorts experiencing smaller improvements (Linear change * birth year).

Figure 2.

Figure 2

Intrinsic capacity and subdomains mean factor scores by birth cohort and age in CHARLS (comparative analyses).

Note: Survey weights were used to ensure representativeness to participants aged 50+ and living in China. Here we displayed the point estimates and their 95% confidence intervals from the multilevel growth curve models.

Sensitivity analyses

We performed sensitivity analyses in ELSA, replacing the longitudinal weights with cross-sectional survey weights and found similar results (Supplement 5). We also undertook additional analysis to determine whether the observed trajectories varied by gender. Measurement invariance did not hold across both waves and genders in either ELSA or CHARLS. However, measurement invariance was found to hold over time within genders in most cases except for the correlated factors model in CHARLS. Factor scores were therefore derived separately for each gender, and multilevel growth curve models were estimated by gender using the corresponding factor scores. Although direct comparisons across gender could not be made since they would be biased by differences in the measurement of the intrinsic capacity factor and its subdomains, within-gender trajectories mirrored those found in the overall analyses (Supplement 6).

Discussion

Our research suggests there have been significant improvements in functioning in more recent cohorts of older people in both England and China. Within ELSA, more recent cohorts entered older ages with higher levels of intrinsic capacity, and subsequent declines were less steep than for earlier cohorts. Improvements were seen in all subdomains. Trajectories were similar for males and females and largely consistent across both countries, although our analysis was limited by the lesser availability of data waves in CHARLS.

The observed improvements are substantial. To avoid undue extrapolation, we limited our assessment to direct comparisons of capacity in participants of different cohorts at the same age. Currently, the overlap between adjacent cohorts in the ELSA study is 6 years, and participants of non-adjacent cohorts cannot be directly compared. However, even with these limitations, we still found that a 68-year-old ELSA participant born in 1950 had higher intrinsic capacity than a 62-year-old born just 10 years earlier. Improvement in cognition was even more substantial. When comparing earlier cohorts, additional improvements are observed, although the gains between these cohorts are not quite as large as between the 1940 and 1950 cohorts. Thus, while our models suggest that today’s 70-year-olds have the equivalent functioning to substantially younger adults in previous generations (perhaps 70 really is the new 60), our direct assessments can only confirm that 68 is the new 62.

These observed improvements stand in contrast to previous research, which has found that increases in longevity have been accompanied by increased prevalence of chronic conditions in older age groups.6 This increased prevalence is likely driven, at least in part, by people who would have previously died from a condition such as heart disease now surviving into older ages. However, since the management and functional consequences of chronic conditions may also have changed, the implications of these trends on the day-to-day lives of older adults have been less clear.

Most previous research directly examining functional trends has been limited to studies of severe disability, and the findings have been inconsistent. For example, in the UK, a comparison of similar cohorts of people over age 65 between 1991 and 2011 suggested that only 36.4% of the extra years of life gained for men and 4.8% for women were experienced with no level of care dependency.13 On the other hand, analysis of ELSA data from 2002 to 2016 found that ADL limitations declined in those aged 55 to 64 years.14 In China, some studies have suggested the age-adjusted prevalence of ADL loss may be declining16, while others found that limitations in ADLs and IADLs may be increasing17 or that there may be a V-shaped trend for ADLs.15

These inconsistencies are likely to arise partly from the wide variety of measures used.22 Some measures (particularly IADLs) also have difficulty distinguishing between changes that may be occurring in the individual and those that might result from changes in the environment. For example, a common IADL question relates to how easy it is to use a phone, yet phone type and use have changed with time.23.

Another important influence on these past findings could be changing patterns of institutionalisation. In the UK, the number of nursing home beds per 100 people aged 75 and over fell by 12% between 2012 and 2022, and admissions for those aged 65 and over fell by 18% between 2014 and 2022.24. As older adults became less likely to be cared for in institutions and more likely to be cared for at home, the prevalence of severe disability in community-based samples would increase, even if the prevalence remained unchanged in the total population. Changing patterns of institutionalisation may have influenced our findings as both studies use community-based samples. However, the shift in institutionalisation in England would operate against the positive trends we observed, while in China, institutionalisation rates remain low at around 1%, and recent emphasis has been on community-based care services.25

Furthermore, the most fundamental limitation of studies of severe disability is that they cannot assess how increasing life expectancy and associated changes in disease prevalence might be impacting on the functioning of people in relatively robust health. This is where our findings shed new light. In contrast to previous research, the continuous nature of the intrinsic capacity construct and its subdomains allowed us to examine milder and earlier forms of age-related disability than previous analyses, and to consider individual-level changes independent of any changes that may have occurred in contextual factors.

The improvements in functioning that we identify could arise from multiple influences and have no obvious single driver. Greater access to healthcare or improved treatments may have played a role. Detection and management of biological risk factors may also have improved, reducing their impact (and potentially increasing their prevalence), but observed trends are inconsistent. In ELSA, rates of awareness of hypertension, treatment of hypertension, and the proportion of treated participants who achieved recommended targets have increased over time.26 Management of hypertension in China has also improved, although the age-standardised prevalence of high blood pressure appears to have increased significantly.27 A reported increase in the prevalence of diagnosed diabetes in ELSA participants from 7.7% in 2004 to 11.5% in 2012 would also be consistent with better detection and possibly management.28 However, between 2004 and 2012, there was also a significant rise in the prevalence of undiagnosed diabetes and only a very small decrease in the proportion of participants with diabetes who were unaware of this condition.28

Another possibility is that healthier behaviours may have slowed age-associated biological changes, strengthening biological reserves and limiting the impact of chronic conditions. However, if this were a major influence, it would likely also have served to reduce the incidence of chronic disease, which runs counter to observed trends. Moreover, behaviours and risk factors in the UK and China have trended in multiple directions over the past 25 years. Age-standardised prevalence estimates suggest that between 1990 (or 2000, depending on data availability) and 2015, tobacco use fell in the UK but remained relatively steady in China, while the prevalence of being overweight rose in both countries.29 Trends in physical activity in the UK and elsewhere are hard to determine, but may have declined over time.30,31 In China, physical activity from work and domestic activities may have fallen by around 50% between 1991 and 2011,32 although other analyses suggest a more stable long-term trend in China, at least from 2000 to 2015.33

Other explanations for the observed trends may lie in the more distant past. The cohorts included in these studies were born between 1920 and 1959 for ELSA and 1930 to 1955 for CHARLS, and early life experiences from these periods, which include World War II and the Chinese Civil War, may have played a role.34 Our own research in CHARLS has previously shown that early life events such as poor nutrition account for 16% of the variance in capacity observed in older adults in China.35 Even the antenatal experience of mothers may influence the risk of chronic conditions in their children.36,37

Higher early life peaks in capacity are likely to also provide greater reserves for people to draw on as they age, delaying overt declines in capacity. For example, greater educational opportunities in childhood have been suggested as one explanation for the 13% per decade decline in the incidence rate of dementia observed in Europe and North America over the past 25 years.38,39 In our analysis, more recent cohorts entered older age with higher capacity, and this would be consistent with an influence of early life factors such as education or nutrition.

Finally, one hypothesised cause of multiple chronic conditions is inflammaging, which is thought to be driven by multiple factors, including infections and nutrition.40 Changes in exposure to common pathogens across the life course related to better sanitation and other environmental factors could thus also have played a role.

In summary, the explanations for the improvements we have observed are likely to be complex and relate to changes that have occurred over most of the past century.

Our analysis has many strengths, including the representative nature of the samples. The instruments underpinning our measure are widely used and, where possible, objective. They distinguish between individual-level change and changes that might have occurred in the physical and social environments the individual habits.

However, when considering these findings, it is important to understand the limitations of our research. We explored the typical experience of cohorts, and this is likely to mask significant intra-cohort heterogeneity. We considered this possibility in our gender analysis which suggested the improvements we observed were not limited to one sex. However, previous research suggests that positive trends are likely to be greater for more advantaged socioeconomic groups, and we cannot exclude this possibility.41,42

It is also likely that participants with worse intrinsic capacity were disproportionately excluded from the study samples, particularly for older ages and cohorts. However, any resulting survivor bias would likely be greater for older cohorts, and any effect would be to underestimate the positive trends we observed.

Due to the complexity of the measurement models, we could not embed the latent variables themselves in the analyses of the longitudinal trajectories. Rather, we derived factor scores and analysed these over time. These factor scores are assumed to be free of error (as would be any other observed outcome), so it is important to acknowledge that measurement error may still be a source of bias in this study.

It is also possible that self-report effects may be at play in the psychological and sensory subdomains, and trends in the sensory domain may have been impacted by changes in access to hearing and visual supports. However, the steepest improvements in capacity were found in subdomains measured with objective indicators, suggesting they are not explained by reporting bias. Finally, attrition within the two studies also needs to be considered as a possible influence on our findings. However, sample attrition in ELSA has been previously shown not to significantly affect estimates of disease prevalence, suggesting any influence is likely to be minor.43

Our findings suggest several avenues for further research. If they can be replicated and the limitations addressed, future studies could examine whether trends vary between settings, how trends might be influenced by socioeconomic and other characteristics, such as race or ethnicity, and why these trends may be occurring. This might suggest interventions to ensure the trends we have observed are reinforced and equitably spread.

In the meantime, our analysis strongly suggests that increasing life expectancy is being accompanied by large increases in health expectancy among more recent cohorts, at least when focusing on people born between 1920 and 1959 This has positive implications for all of us, both as individuals and for society more broadly.

Online Methods

Sample

ELSA follows a nationally representative sample of the English population aged 50 and above, while CHARLS follows a nationally representative sample of the Chinese population aged 45 and above. Data collection in both cohorts was conducted through face-to-face assessments using computer-assisted personal interviews. In addition, objective measures and blood samples were collected by trained nurses in waves 2, 4 and 6 in ELSA and waves 1 and 3 in CHARLS. The response rates in both ELSA and CHARLS were reasonably high, although they varied across waves. The average follow-up length is about 4.84 years in ELSA and 2.18 years in CHARLS, with attrition rates of 36.3% in ELSA and 45.0% in CHARLS from wave 1 to wave 2. The details regarding follow-up information and missing information are provided in Supplement 2.

We included ELSA and CHARLS participants aged 60 + with valid information in at least one of the indicators used to measure intrinsic capacity in at least one wave. ELSA currently has nine waves of data available, while five waves are available for CHARLS. Because of the comprehensive measures included in this longitudinal study and the many years of follow-up, we made ELSA [waves 1 (2002) to 9 (2019)] the focus of our main analysis. We then applied the same methods to the CHARLS [waves 1 (2011) to 3 (2015)] cohort but given the shorter follow-up period, we report this as a comparative analysis. More recent data from CHARLS [waves 4 (2018) and 5 (2020)] were not included as, by design, none of the locomotor and vitality subdomain indicators was assessed. This research involved secondary analysis of previously collected data, and patients and the public were not involved in any way.

Measures

We used data from multiple self-reported and objectively measured tests to create scores for intrinsic capacity and subdomains of capacity consistent with the WHO model of intrinsic capacity.20 To maximise the comparability across the two cohort studies, we focused on the indicators that were present in both ELSA and CHARLS (see Supplement 1 for details and descriptive results).

Building on our previous analyses, we used a confirmatory factor analysis (CFA) approach to operationalise a set of relevant subdomains.9,19 These comprised locomotor capacity (measured by walking speed, chair-stand test and balance), cognitive capacity (immediate recall, delayed recall, time orientation/memory), sensory capacity (reported hearing and visual impairments), psychological capacity (affect and sleep as measured by Center for Epidemiological Studies-Depression (CES-D) scale44 items present in ELSA and CHARLS) and vitality (grip strength, forced expiratory volume, haemoglobin). Intrinsic capacity was operationalised as a latent common cause of the levels across all indicators (general factor under a bifactor structure) after accounting for the unique shared variance among subsets of indicators as captured by the subdomains. A visual depiction of the measurement model is available in Supplement 3.

Statistical analyses

To ensure that the constructs (i.e., the intrinsic capacity general factor and the subdomains) under study were equivalently measured over time, we first used a measurement invariance testing approach.45,46 In this approach, a multiple groups CFA model without constraints (i.e., configural model) was rst estimated to assess whether the same factor structure held across time points (i.e., configural invariance). Configural invariance was deemed to hold if the values of the root mean square error of approximation (RMSEA) and the Tucker-Lewis Index (TLI) for the configural model were below 0.060 and above 0.950, respectively47. Provided that configural invariance held, an additional level of invariance, scalar invariance, was tested, where factor loadings and item intercepts/thresholds were fixed to be equal across time points. Scalar invariance ensures that comparisons of the levels in the constructs are not biased due to differences in the way in which they are measured across time points45. Since the main aim of this study was to explore the trajectories in those constructs, ensuring that scalar invariance held was crucial. Scalar invariance was deemed to hold if the difference in t between the scalar and configural model was smaller than 0.015 and 0.010 in the RMSEA and the Comparative Fit Index (CFI), respectively48,49. These models were computed using the data from participants present in all the waves in which all indicators were present (i.e., waves 2, 4, and 6).

Once measurement invariance had been tested in the waves in waves 2, 4, and 6 in ELSA and waves 1 and 3 in CHARLS, the measurement models were extended to include the remaining waves (waves 1, 3, 5, 7, 8, 9 in ELSA, waves 2 in CHARLS), where only partial information was available by design. We used weighted least squares mean and variance adjusted (WLSMV) estimation with pairwise deletion to estimate these models and generate factor scores representing the individuals’ levels of intrinsic capacity and each of the subdomains based on the factor models with multiple indicators. The use of pairwise deletion allowed us to obtain estimates of the measurement models in the presence of partial information based on the pattern of relationships between the indicators across the waves, maximising the use of the information available for each individual.50 While this approach can provide biased estimates if the data are not missing completely at random, it retains more information from the available data, which maximises the reliability and validity of the model estimates and offers more plausible results compared to other approaches like listwise deletion.50 Using alternative estimation procedures with more plausible assumptions (e.g., full information maximum likelihood assuming data are missing at random) was not feasible due to the complexity of the measurement models.

Intrinsic capacity factor scores were derived from the bifactor models, while subdomain factor scores were estimated using correlated factors models where only the subdomains were present and allowed to correlate with each other. We used this approach for the subdomain factor scores because bifactor models would give ‘residualised’ versions of the subdomains, capturing what was left after accounting for the general factor.

Multilevel growth curve models were then used to model the change over time in intrinsic capacity and the five subdomains (psychological, locomotor, vitality, cognition, and sensory).51 Time was included in the models as the years elapsed since the first wave. Both linear (constant change) and quadratic (accelerated change) terms were included to allow for non-linear trajectories in ELSA. However, for CHARLS, only linear trajectories were analysed due to fewer repeated measurements available. Birth year (in years, centred at 1920 in ELSA and 1930 in CHARLS) was included in the models as a covariate to account for potential differences in the initial levels across cohorts. Interaction terms between birth year and the growth parameters (i.e., linear and quadratic) were included in the models to account for potential differences in the rates of change across cohorts. We acknowledged the heterogeneity in the intercepts and rates of change by modelling the random effects of both the intercepts and linear slopes, which were allowed to correlate. Variation in the rates of change over time was captured by the random effects for the linear/constant change (random effects for the quadratic/accelerated change could not be included due to model estimation/convergence issues).

All models were computed using survey weights to restore representativeness to each study’s population of reference. In the main ELSA analysis, to confirm the robustness of the results to the differential non-response to the different waves and to ensure representativeness to participants aged 50 + and living in England in 2002 and still alive and residing in private households by wave 9, we estimated the models using longitudinal weights.52 Because these longitudinal weights take into account the differential non-response to all the waves, analyses do not rely on the assumption of the data being missing completely at random but rather on them being missing at random after conditioning on the variables used to derive those weights. In CHARLS, survey cross-sectional weights were used for multilevel growth curve models to ensure representativeness to participants aged 50 + and living in China.53 []. All the weights are provided by the ELSA and CHARLS teams. To aid the interpretation of the results, marginal predicted levels were obtained from the models for each of the data collection time points and plotted in year and age vector plots. Furthermore, these marginal predicted levels were tabulated in age*cohort grids.

Acknowledgements

We acknowledge the financial support from the ARC Centre of Excellence in Population Ageing Research (CEPAR, project CE170100005) at the University of New South Wales (UNSW). DM is partly supported by the Economic and Social Research Council (ESRC) Centre for Society and Mental Health at King’s College London [ES/S012567/1]. YS is supported by the National Social Science Foundation of China (23AZD091). This analysis uses data or information from the Harmonized ELSA dataset and Codebook, Version G.2 as of July 2021 and the Harmonized CHARLS dataset and Codebook, Version D as of June 2021 developed by the Gateway to Global Aging Data. The development of the Harmonized ELSA was funded by the National Institute on Aging (R01 AG030153, RC2 AG036619, R03 AG043052), and the Harmonized CHARLS was funded by the National Institute on Aging (R01 AG030153, RC2 AG036619, R03 AG043052). For more information, please refer to https://g2aging.org/.The views expressed in this paper are those of the authors and do not necessarily reflect the views of WHO, the ESRC, or King’s College London.

Footnotes

Conflicts of Interest

We declare that none of the authors has any potential conflicts of interest.

Supplementary Files

This is a list of supplementary les associated with this preprint. Click to download.

Contributor Information

John Beard, Columbia University - Mailman School of Public Health.

Hanewald Katja, University of New South Wales.

Yafei Si, The University of New South Wales.

Jotheeswaran Thiyagarajan, World Health Organization.

Dario Moreno-Agostino, University College London.

Data availability

Data described in this paper are available at the website of the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA, https://www.elsa-project.ac.uk/) and the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS, http://charls.pku.edu.cn/en/). Ethical approval for all the ELSA waves was granted by NHS Research Ethics Committees under the National Research and Ethics Service (NRES). The Biomedical Ethics Review Committee of Peking University approved the CHARLS study (IRB00001052–11015). Furthermore, the current study received approval from the UNSW Ethics Committee (HC210472). The code scripts used in this study are available from the corresponding authors upon reasonable request.

References

  • 1.Organization WH (2015) World report on ageing and health. World Health Organization [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Christensen K, Doblhammer G, Rau R, Vaupel JW (2009) Ageing populations: the challenges ahead. Lancet 374:1196–1208. 10.1016/S0140-6736(09)61460-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Robine J-M, Jagger C, Crimmins EM, Saito Y, Oyen H (2020) V. Trends in health expectancies. Int Handb health expectancies, 19–34 [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fries JF, Bruce B, Chakravarty E (2011) Compression of morbidity 1980–2011: a focused review of paradigms and progress. J aging Res 2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Jagger C et al. (2015) A comparison of health expectancies over two decades in England: results of the Cognitive Function and Ageing Study I and II. Lancet [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Crimmins EM, Zhang YS, Kim JK, Levine ME (2021) in Handbook of the Biology of Aging (Ninth Edition) (eds Musi Nicolas & Hornsby Peter J.) 405–414 Academic Press [Google Scholar]
  • 7.McCartney G, Popham F, McMaster R, Cumbers A (2019) Defining health and health inequalities. Public Health 172:22–30 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Bindawas SM et al. (2014) Trajectories in functional recovery for patients receiving inpatient rehabilitation for unilateral hip or knee replacement. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 58:344–349. 10.1016/j.archger.2013.12.009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Beard JR, Jotheeswaran AT, Cesari M, de Araujo I (2019) The structure and predictive value of intrinsic capacity in a longitudinal study of ageing. BMJ Open 9:e026119. 10.1136/bmjopen-2018-026119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Stegmann ME et al. (2019) Using the Outcome Prioritization Tool (OPT) to assess the preferences of older patients in clinical decision-making: A review. Maturitas 128:49–52. 10.1016/j.maturitas.2019.07.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Hay SI et al. (2017) Global, regional, and national disability-adjusted life-years (DALYs) for 333 diseases and injuries and healthy life expectancy (HALE) for 195 countries and territories, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet 390:1260–1344. 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)32130-X [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Vos T et al. (2017) Global, regional, and national incidence, prevalence, and years lived with disability for 328 diseases and injuries for 195 countries, 1990–2016: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2016. Lancet 390:1211–1259. 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)32154-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kingston A et al. (2017) Is late-life dependency increasing or not? A comparison of the Cognitive Function and Ageing Studies (CFAS). Lancet 390:1676–1684. 10.1016/S0140-6736(17)31575-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Choi H, Schoeni R, Cho T-C, Langa K (2020) Trends in Disability Among Adults 55–64 in the United States and England From 2002 to 2016. Innov Aging 4:303–303. 10.1093/geroni/igaa057.970 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hu X et al. (2022) Trends in activities of daily living disability among Chinese older adults from 1998 to 2018: an ageperiod-cohort analysis. Eur J Ageing 19:1167–1179. 10.1007/s10433-022-00690-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Li Z-H et al. (2019) Trends in the Incidence of Activities of Daily Living Disability Among Chinese Older Adults From 2002 to 2014. Journals Gerontology: Ser A 75:2113–2118. 10.1093/gerona/glz221 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Pan C, Cao N, Kelifa MO, Luo S (2023) Age and cohort trends in disability among Chinese older adults. Front Public Health 11. 10.3389/fpubh.2023.998948 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Beard JR et al. (2016) The World report on ageing and health: a policy framework for healthy ageing. Lancet 387:2145–2154. 10.1016/s0140-6736(15)00516-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Beard JR, Si Y, Liu Z, Chenoweth L, Hanewald K (2022) Intrinsic Capacity: Validation of a New WHO Concept for Healthy Aging in a Longitudinal Chinese Study. J Gerontol Biol Sci Med Sci 77:94–100. 10.1093/gerona/glab226 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Cesari M et al. (2018) Evidence for the Domains Supporting the Construct of Intrinsic Capacity. Journals Gerontology: Ser A 73:1653–1660. 10.1093/gerona/gly011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Ramírez-Vélez R et al. (2023) Association of intrinsic capacity with respiratory disease mortality. Respir Med 212:107243. 10.1016/j.rmed.2023.107243 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Jagger C, Matthews R, Comas Herrera A, Morciano M, Hancock R (2009) Calibrating disability measures across UK national surveys
  • 23.Knoll K et al. (2023) The Prevalence and Trends of Instrumental Activities of Daily Living Impairments in the United States from 2008–2018. J Alzheimer’s disease Rep 7:271–278. 10.3233/ADR-220107 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Trust N (2023) Care home bed availability
  • 25.Yue Z, Xiang N, Li H, Liu E (2021) The evolution trend of availability of China’s community-based care services and its impact on the cognitive function of elderly people: 2008–2018. Int J Equity Health 20:203. 10.1186/s12939-021-01544-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Dregan A et al. (2016) Longitudinal Trends in Hypertension Management and Mortality Among Octogenarians. Hypertension 68:97–105. 10.1161/HYPERTENSIONAHA.116.07246 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Yi Q et al. (2021) Trends in the prevalence of hypertension according to severity and phenotype in Chinese adults over two decades (1991–2015). J Clin Hypertens (Greenwich) 23:1302–1315. 10.1111/jch.14306 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Huang YT, Steptoe A, Zaninotto P (2021) Prevalence of Undiagnosed Diabetes in 2004 and 2012: Evidence From the English Longitudinal Study of Aging. J Gerontol Biol Sci Med Sci 76:922–928. 10.1093/gerona/glaa179 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Organization WH (2022) (World Health Organization, Geneva [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Strain T, Milton K, Dall P, Standage M, Mutrie N (2020) How are we measuring physical activity and sedentary behaviour in the four home nations of the UK? A narrative review of current surveillance measures and future directions. Br J Sports Med 54:1269–1276. 10.1136/bjsports-2018-100355 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Stamatakis E, Ekelund U, Wareham NJ (2007) Temporal trends in physical activity in England: The Health Survey for England 1991 to 2004. Prev Med 45:416–423. 10.1016/j.ypmed.2006.12.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Zang J, Ng SW (2016) Age, period and cohort effects on adult physical activity levels from 1991 to 2011 in China. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Activity 13:40. 10.1186/s12966-016-0364-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Qinpei Z et al. (2020) Preplanned Studies: Trends in Leisure-Time Physical Activity Among Chinese Adults — China, 2000–2015. China CDC Wkly 2:135–139 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Ben-Shlomo Y (2020) Life course, exposure, and ageing populations. Oxf Textbook Neurologic Neuropsychiatric Epidemiol, 21 [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Si Y et al. (2023) Life-course inequalities in intrinsic capacity and healthy ageing, China. Bull World Health Organ 101:307–316C. 10.2471/BLT.22.288888 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Heindel JJ, Vandenberg LN (2015) Developmental origins of health and disease: a paradigm for understanding disease cause and prevention. Curr Opin Pediatr 27:248–253. 10.1097/mop.0000000000000191 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.He X et al. (2023) Secular trend of non-communicable chronic disease prevalence throughout the life span who endured Chinese Great Famine (1959–1961). BMC Public Health 23:1238. 10.1186/s12889-023-16142-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Clouston SAP et al. (2019) Education and Cognitive Decline: An Integrative Analysis of Global Longitudinal Studies of Cognitive Aging. Journals Gerontology: Ser B 75:e151–e160. 10.1093/geronb/gbz053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Wolters FJ et al. (2020) Twenty-seven-year time trends in dementia incidence in Europe and the United States. Alzheimer Cohorts Consortium 95:e519–e531. 10.1212/wnl.0000000000010022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Franceschi C, Garagnani P, Parini P, Giuliani C, Santoro A (2018) Inflammaging: a new immune–metabolic viewpoint for age-related diseases. Nat Reviews Endocrinol 14:576–590. 10.1038/s41574-018-0059-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Tawiah R, Jagger C, Anstey KJ, Kiely KM (2021) Trends in disability-free life expectancy at age 50 years in Australia between 2001 and 2011 by social disadvantage. J Epidemiol Community Health 75:1056–1062 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Bennett HQ et al. (2021) Healthy ageing for all? Comparisons of socioeconomic inequalities in health expectancies over two decades in the Cognitive Function and Ageing Studies I and II. Int J Epidemiol 50:841–851 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Banks J, Muriel A, Smith JP (2011) Attrition and health in ageing studies: Evidence from ELSA and HRS. Longit Life Course Stud 2. 10.14301/llcs.v2i2.115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Carleton RN et al. (2013) The center for epidemiologic studies depression scale: a review with a theoretical and empirical examination of item content and factor structure. PLoS ONE 8:e58067. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Gregorich SE (2006) Do self-report instruments allow meaningful comparisons across diverse population groups? Testing measurement invariance using the con rmatory factor analysis framework. Med Care 44. 10.1097/01.mlr.0000245454.12228.8f [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Millsap RE, Yun-Tein J (2004) Assessing Factorial Invariance in Ordered-Categorical Measures. Multivar Behav Res 39:479–515. 10.1207/S15327906MBR3903_4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Lt Hu, Bentler PM (1999) Cutoff criteria for fit indexes in covariance structure analysis: Conventional criteria versus new alternatives. Struct equation modeling: multidisciplinary J 6:1–55. 10.1080/10705519909540118 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Chen FF (2007) Sensitivity of Goodness of Fit Indexes to Lack of Measurement Invariance. Struct Equ Model 14:464–504. 10.1080/10705510701301834 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Cheung GW, Rensvold RB (2002) Evaluating goodness-of-fit indexes for testing measurement invariance. Struct Equ Model 9:233–255. 10.1207/s15328007sem0902_5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Asparouhov T, Muthén B (2010) Weighted Least Squares Estimation with Missing Data
  • 51.Bryk AS, Raudenbush SW (1987) Application of hierarchical linear models to assessing change. Psychol Bull 101:147–158. 10.1037/0033-2909.101.1.147 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Pacchiotti B, Hussey D, Bennett G (2012) The dynamics of ageing: The 2018/2019 English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (Wave 9) Technical ReportNatCen, London [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Phillips D et al. (2021) Harmonized CHARLS Documentation. Center for Economic and Social Research, Los Angeles [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Data described in this paper are available at the website of the English Longitudinal Study of Ageing (ELSA, https://www.elsa-project.ac.uk/) and the China Health and Retirement Longitudinal Study (CHARLS, http://charls.pku.edu.cn/en/). Ethical approval for all the ELSA waves was granted by NHS Research Ethics Committees under the National Research and Ethics Service (NRES). The Biomedical Ethics Review Committee of Peking University approved the CHARLS study (IRB00001052–11015). Furthermore, the current study received approval from the UNSW Ethics Committee (HC210472). The code scripts used in this study are available from the corresponding authors upon reasonable request.


Articles from Research Square are provided here courtesy of American Journal Experts

RESOURCES