Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2024 Jun 12;19(6):e0303770. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0303770

How big five personality traits influence information sharing on social media: A meta analysis

Hao Lin 1,2,3, Chundong Wang 1,2,3, Yongjie Sun 4,*
Editor: Simone Varrasi5
PMCID: PMC11168692  PMID: 38865331

Abstract

Research interest in information sharing behavior on social media has significantly increased over the past decade. However, empirical studies on the relationship between Big Five personality traits and information sharing behavior have yielded contradictory conclusions. We aimed to investigate how Big Five personality influences information sharing behavior on social media. This meta-analysis systematically reviewed high-quality studies indexed by web of science and CNKI from the past decade (n = 27, with 31969 samples) and performed a meta-analysis to examine the association between Big Five personality traits and information sharing behavior. The literature search was performed in September 2023. The meta-analysis results showed that extraversion (β = 0.05**) had a positive relationship with information sharing behavior on social media. Agreeableness (β = −0.06**), conscientiousness (β = −0.03**), and neuroticism (β = −0.03**) had negative relationships with information sharing behavior on social media. However, the relationship between openness and information sharing behavior was not clearly observed due to insufficient research. The meta-analysis results are made available to the scientific community to enhance research, comprehension, and utilization of social media.

1 Introduction

Social users continuously expand the scale of their presence as they engage in acquiring, sharing, and interacting with information, thereby maintaining, strengthening, or reconstructing their existing social relationships [1, 2]. Research on the constant dissemination of diverse information on social media can contribute to detecting rumors [3, 4], improving recommendation systems [5], marketing [6], managing social media [7, 8] and employee [9], and addressing other related areas.

The sharing behavior of social media users largely determines the dissemination of information on social media [10]. Personality traits, which are stable patterns of thoughts, feelings, and behaviors, have a significant influence on human cognitive patterns [1113]. This makes it an excellent starting point for studying information sharing behavior on social media. The Big Five personality model is the most commonly used personality taxonomy in information science [14]. The Big Five personality traits are often integrated into research methods in areas such as rumors, recommendation systems, employee management, etc. However, although many review studies have investigated the correlation between the Big Five personality traits and human behavior [11, 1517], the association between personality and information sharing behavior has not been thoroughly examined. In addition, there is no published meta-analysis on the relationship between two. The conclusions drawn between the two remain complex and unclear. This may lead to introducing personality traits in the method, which could be counterproductive. So, in this meta-analysis, we identified the connection between Big Five personality traits and information sharing behavior through a meta-analysis, which is a quantitative literature review method, to more effectively examine the human element in information science. The results may point to one of the bottlenecks faced by personalized research in information science.

This paper begins by providing definitions of Big Five personality traits and Information sharing behavior. Then, the research questions and hypotheses of this study are proposed. The adopted materials and methods of the meta-analysis are subsequently described. Section 5 presents the results of the meta-analysis, which is subsequently followed by a discussion of these results in section 6.

2 Theoretical review

2.1 Big five personality

Personality has been defined as “psychological qualities that contribute to an individual’s enduring and distinctive patterns of thinking, feeling, and behaving.” Various theorists have developed several models of personality, each representing different perspectives, such as Five Factor Model, Myers–Briggs Type Indicator, Eysenck’s three factor model, and seven-factor personality model.

The Five Factor Model, also known as the “Big Five” model of personality, is the most widely accepted and well-known theory within the dispositional perspective of personality. Big Five personality comprises five broad traits: extraversion (EXT), agreeableness (AGR), conscientiousness (CON), neuroticism (NEU) (or called emotional stability), and openness (OPN). The chaotic nature and lack of easy conclusions can be observed in agreeableness, conscientiousness, and openness and social media behavior. For instance, according to Indu et al. [18], individuals characterized by high extroversion and low agreeableness tend not to disseminate rumors. However, Buchanan [19] proposes that those who propagate false information feature lower agreeableness, yet exhibit higher levels of extroversion and neuroticism. As another example, various literature report the relationship between openness and information sharing behavior. However, contrary to these reports, a study by Zuniga et al. [20] found no significant correlation between these variables in a large sample. Meta-analysis is suitable for summarizing these confusing conclusions.

2.2 Information sharing behavior on social media

Due to its real-time, interactive, and diverse content characteristics, social media has gradually become an essential aspect of people’s daily lives over time. On these platforms, various types of information are shared, including entertainment information, health information, emergency information, political information, even rumor [18] and fake news [21]. The continuous sharing of such information enhances the appeal and usefulness of social media. In social media, information publishing, commenting, and forwarding all belong to information sharing behaviors, which transmit useful information to others.

Multiple psychometric measurement instruments have been created to evaluate individuals’ willingness to share information, including the Knowledge-sharing Behaviours Scale [22] and Information Exchange Scale [23]. However, no recognized specific scale has been developed to assess willingness to share information specifically on social media platforms. In addition to designing questionnaires, currently, researchers evaluate this willingness primarily through statistical design media data and interviews.

3 Research questions and hypothesis

The purpose of this meta-analysis was to consolidate all existing empirical evidence on the connection between Big Five personality traits and information sharing behavior on social media. So, RQ1:Do the information sharing behavior of users on social media correlate with their (a) extraversion, (b) agreeableness, (c) conscientiousness, (d) neuroticism, and (e) openness?

The extraversion trait distinguishes social, proactive individuals who are oriented towards themselves from silent, serious, shy, and quiet individuals. It is often considered highly positively correlated with the frequent use of social media [17, 24, 25]. Neuroticism reflects the individual emotional regulation process. Those with high neuroticism exhibit heightened reactivity to external stimuli compared to the general population, and they generally struggle with regulating and responding to emotions, often experiencing negative emotional states. It is often considered negatively correlated with the sharing behavior, since individual with high neuroticism tend to feel shy, anxious, insecure, and awkward in social situations [26, 27]. Agreeableness measures an individual’s attitude towards others. Conscientiousness distinguishes between individuals who are trustworthy and meticulous from those who are lazy and careless. Openness refers to an individual’s cognitive style, their capacity to tolerate unfamiliar situations, and their aptitude for exploration. So, based on our explanation of the Big Five personality traits and the previous literature of personality traits and human cognitive abilities [11], the following hypotheses were proposed for the meta-analysis:

  • H1: Extraversion is positively related to information sharing behavior on social media.

  • H2: Agreeableness is negatively related to information sharing behavior on social media.

  • H3: Conscientiousness is positively related to information sharing behavior on social media.

  • H4: Neuroticism is negatively related to information sharing behavior on social media.

  • H5: Openness is positively related to information sharing behavior on social media.

4 Material and methods

4.1 Literature searching and screening

A thorough literature search was conducted by two independent researchers in September 2023, among major databases involving Web of Science® and China National Knowledge Infrastructure® (CNKI). To ensure the quality of the literature, we only selected literatures indexed by Science Citation Index or Engineering Index (for literature written in English) and Core Journals of Peking University (http://hxqk.lib.pku.edu.cn/) (for literature written in Chinese). This rule filters out most low-quality articles in CNKI [28]. Due to the timeliness of social media research, we only adopt literature from the past decade (2012–2023). Given the main research objectives, we searched literature with multiple keywords, which include “personality”, “social media”, “information”, “sharing”, “dissemination”, “public opinion”, “人格” “传播”, “社交媒体” “舆情” and “信息共享” by Boolean search terms. We also gathered references from literature identified for inclusion in the meta-analysis, thereby adding five more papers to the final meta-analysis.

In addition to screening for journal papers and conference papers, relevant dissertations (e. g., [29]) were also screened to avoid potential bias. We subsequently screened the eligible papers using one criterion. That is, the paper must adopt Big Five as the personality taxonomy (e. g., [30]), and it’s research object must be the share behavior of social media. After eliminating duplicated articles resulting from the utilization of multiple databases and channels, the titles and abstracts of all retrieved papers were initially screened, which resulted in a total of 1421 papers at the preliminary stage, and a total of 15 papers and 27 studies in final. The selecetion process was also conducted by two independent researchers. The searching and selecetion process is shown in Fig 1.

Fig 1. PRISMA flow diagram of the meta-analysis.

Fig 1

A paper may present findings from numerous studies.

4.2 Effect size

Consistent with previous meta-analysis in personality [3133], our study has applied the standardized regression coefficient β and standard error SE as the primary metric to estimate effect size. Almost all paper report β between personality traits and information sharing behavior. The larger |β|, the greater the impact of the independent variable on the dependent variable. For papers reporting other indicators for measuring the degree of statistical distribution (i.e., t in t-distribution, P value, standard deviation SD, mean difference MD), we use the following formula to approximately convert these indicators to SE.

SE=SD/n. (1)
SE=MD/t. (2)

Where, n is total number of samples. t can be obtained by consulting the t-value distribution table with the P value and the degree of freedom (n − 1). When the paper does not provide the precise P value but instead presents the level of significance, we proceed with the following approximating.

P={Meaningless,P=NS0.05,P<0.050.01,P<0.010.001,P<0.001 (3)

4.3 Coding and data analysis

According to a predefined coding schema, every paper was coded for the following information: (1) the relevant bibliographic information including the author(s), year of publication, and the country where sample were collected was recorded; (2) the sample characteristics including the number of sample, mean age of the sample, and the percentage of males; (3) the Big Five personality scale used in the study (e.g., BFI, NEO-PI-R, MINI-IPIP, etc.); (4) the information Sharing Scale used in the study (e.g., Likert-type Scale); (5) the effect size of Big Five personality traits assessed in the study.

The random-effects model called DerSimonian-Laird [34] was used to determine whether the average correlations were statistically significant, considering the variation among the included studies. We adopted the Q statistic, H statistic, and I2 statistic to test the heterogeneity across effect sizes in our chosen papers. The Q statistic measures the difference between observed effect sizes and the estimated effect size. The H statistic is the correction of the freedom degree for Q. The I2 statistic represents the percentage of variability in effect sizes that is unrelated to sampling error. Forest plots visually depict the heterogeneity included in our meta-analysis. In addition, due to the research differences between the Big Five personality traits, we divided the literature into five subgroups for analysis based on the Big Five personality traits. This helps to reduce heterogeneity among the included studies.

4.4 Publication bias

Recognizing the publication bias toward positive findings in the personality research community [17], we conducted two methods to determine if any publication bias exists. Firstly, a funnel plot was utilized to visually assess if there were any missing studies with small effect sizes. Next, Begg’s test with non parametric rank correlation and Egger’s test was employed to provide statistical evidence of publication bias [35].

Overall, Tables 1 and 2 presented the information of all studies included in this meta-analysis. All calculations related to this meta-analysis were conducted in Stata 17.

Table 1. Studies included in the meta-analysis.

No. Study Country Year Object sample Mean age % of males Personality Scale Information Sharing Scale
1 David et al. 2012 (1) [40] World 2012 Social networking service 300 27 31 BFI-44 Likert-type (1∼7)
2 David et al. 2012 (2) [40] World 2012 Social networking servic 300 27 31 BFI-44 Likert-type (1∼7)
3 Chen 2016 [27] World 2016 Fake message 171 24 42.69 BFI-44 Likert-type (1∼7)
4 Liu et al. 2017 [41] China 2017 Social business information 267 22.04 44.94 TIPI-C Likert-type (1∼7)
5 Homero et al. 2017 [20] 20 country 2017 Message 21314 x x NEO-PI-R Likert-type (1∼10)
6 Deng et al. 2017 [42] China 2017 Message 311 21.96 42.1 NEO-PI Likert-type (1∼5)
7 Mohammad et al. 2018 [25] World 2018 Government information 257 38.91 62.01 NEO-PI-R Likert-type (1∼7)
8 Damien et al. 2019 (1) [43] World 2019 Message with emotional information 197 44.9 48.94 BFI-10 Binary questioning
9 Damien et al. 2019 (2) [43] World 2019 Message with facial expressions 197 44.9 48.94 BFI-10 Binary questioning
10 Buchanan et al. 2019 [44] USA, UK 2019 Fake message 409 x 31.5 IPIP Statistics on Facebook
11 Huang et al. 2020 (1) [45] China 2020 Entertainment information 317 30.26 50.16 Modified BFI Likert-type (1∼5)
12 Huang et al. 2020 (2) [45] China 2020 Social Communication Information 317 30.26 50.16 Modified BFI Likert-type (1∼5)
13 Huang et al. 2020 (3) [45] China 2020 Self-expression information 317 30.26 50.16 Modified BFI Likert-type (1∼5)
14 Yin et al. 2020 [46] China 2020 Negative news 215 22.45 41.4 BFI-44 Intention of RNI
15 Tom 2020 (1) [19] UK 2020 Later known fake message 672 44.92 47.2 41-item BFI Likert-type (0∼11)
16 Tom 2020 (2) [19] UK 2020 Later known fake message 674 38.95 46.3 41-item BFI Likert-type (0∼11)
17 Tom 2020 (3) [19] UK 2020 Later known fake message 650 33.07 34.6 41-item BFI Likert-type (0∼11)
18 Tom 2020 (4) [19] USA 2020 Later known fake message 638 44.91 44.4 41-item BFI Likert-type (0∼11)
19 Tom 2020 (5) [19] UK 2020 Known fake message 672 44.92 47.2 41-item BFI Likert-type (0∼11)
20 Tom 2020 (6) [19] UK 2020 Known fake message 674 38.95 46.3 41-item BFI Likert-type (0∼11)
21 Tom 2020 (7) [19] UK 2020 Known fake message 650 33.07 34.6 41-item BFI Likert-type (0∼11)
22 Tom 2020 (8) [19] USA 2020 Known fake message 638 44.91 44.4 41-item BFI Likert-type (0∼11)
23 Xiao et al. 2021 (1) [21] World 2021 Fake news published by social media 551 20.26 29.65 MINI-IPIP Likert-type (0∼6)
24 Xiao et al. 2021 (2) [21] World 2021 Fake news published by news media 551 20.26 29.65 MINI-IPIP Likert-type (0∼6)
25 Brinda et al. 2022 [47] India 2022 News 221 28.59 42 NEO-PI Likert-type (1∼5)
26 Ahmed1 et al. 2022 [48] Singapore 2022 News of COVID-19 500 >21 x BFI-44 Likert-type (1∼5)
27 Xu et al. 2023 [49] China 2023 Online public opinion on newly emerging infectious diseases 300 20–35 44 Random forest Statistics on WeiBo

× represents that the effect size cannot be calculated due to insufficient data. ∼ represents that the effect size cannot be calculated due to the personality trait has not been studied.

Table 2. Result of quality assessment.

Study Title Score
David et al. 2012 A tale of two sites: Twitter vs. Facebook and the personality predictors of social media usage 9
Chen 2016 The Influences of Personality and Motivation on the Sharing of Misinformation on Social Media 9
Homero et al. 2017 Personality Traits and Social Media Use in 20 Countries: How Personality Relates to Frequency of Social Media Use Social Media News Use, and Social Media Use for Social Interaction 10
Deng et al. 2017 How do personality traits shape information-sharing behaviour in social media? Exploring the mediating effect of generalized trust 9
Liu et al. 2017 社会化商务下个体心理因素对信息共享行为的影响——大五人格的调节作用 9
Mohammad et al. 2018 Sharing Political Content in Online Social Media: A Planned and Unplanned Behaviour Approach 10
Buchanan et al. 2019 Spreading Disinformation on Facebook: Do Trust in Message Source, Risk Propensity, or Personality Affect the Organic Reach of “Fake News”? 10
Damien et al. 2019 Willingness to Share Emotion Information on Social Media: Influence of Personality and Social Context 8
Huang et al. 2020 自媒体用户信息共享行为动机分析与实证 9
Tom 2020 Why do people spread false information online? The effects of message and viewer characteristics on self-reported likelihood of sharing social media disinformation 10
Yin et al. 2020 Reposting negative information on microblogs: Do personality traits matter? 9
Xiao et al. 2021 Wired to seek, comment and share? Examining the relationship between personality, news consumption and misinformation engagement 9
Brinda et al. 2022 Fake or real news? Understanding the gratifications and personality traits of individuals sharing fake news on social media platforms 10
Ahmed1 et al. 2022 Social Media News use and covid-19 misinformation engagement: Survey study 9
Xu et al. 2023 EID事件情境下情绪对信息分享行为的动态影响——人格特质的调节作用 8
Kim et al. 2014 Individual Differences in Social Media Use for Information Seeking 6
Luo 2018 社交媒体中用户人格特质对科学信息分享动机的影响与反思 7

Fig 2 visually displays the significant publication bias present in our chosen papers. We employed the Leave-one-out method for sensitivity analysis to remove the most extreme outlier studies [36, 37].

Fig 2. Funnel plot of our chosen papers (for EXT trait).

Fig 2

The highly asymmetric nature indicates a strong presence of publication bias. Funnel plots for other personality traits are similar to this.

4.5 Quality assessment

Although most quality checklists published in extant academic literature have primarily addressed medical studies, we sought to ensure the thorough evaluation of the selected studies by adhering to a combination of established guidelines. To rigorously assess the methodological quality of the studies included in this meta-analysis, we followed the guidelines of Kitchenham and Charters [38] as well as the meta-analysis on the existing quality assessment tools that are being used in meta-analysis in the area of Engineering [39]. The study suggested using a set of questions based on widely used checklists and guidelines for the design, conduct, analysis, and conclusions of each study in this meta-analysis. The study evaluation criteria were based on the questions presented below.

  • Q1: Are the aims of the research clearly defined?

  • Q2: Is there an adequate description of the context in which the research was carried out?

  • Q3: Was the research design appropriate to address the aims of the research?

  • Q4: Was there a control group?

  • Q5: Are the data collection methods adequately described?

  • Q6: Were all measures used in the study fully defined?

  • Q7: Is the experimental design appropriate and justifiable?

  • Q8: Does the study provide description and justification of the data analysis approaches?

  • Q9: Are the findings of the study clearly stated?

  • Q10: Does the study add value to academia or practice?

The scoring procedure assigned a value of 1 for “Yes” and 0 for “No”. Studies could score between 0 and 10 points. Papers receiving a score exceeding 8 (>8) were decided to be retained in this meta-analysis. The results of quality assessment are presented in Table 2.

5 Results

5.1 Description of the study

As shown in Tables 1 and 2, the studies were conducted over the world (29.6%), seven studies (25.9%) were conducted in China, and six studies (22.2%) were conducted in UK. In 14 studies (51.8%) social media users were recruited (mean age from 30 to 45 years), in 9 studies (33.3%), the sample comprised social media users (mean age from 20 to 30 years). And the mean age of the sample in 4 studies (14.8%) could not be accurately determined. Most of studies (96.3%) have recruited less than 1000 participants. The following Big Five Personality Scale were adopted: BFI-44 [50] in 4 papers (26.7%), NEO-PI [51] in 2 papers (13.3%), NEO-PI-R [52] in 2 papers (13.3%), 41-item BFI [53] in 2 papers (13.3%) and 9 studies, BFI-10 [54] in 1 paper (6.67%), Mini-IPIP [55] in 1 paper (6.67%), TIPI [56] in 1 paper (6.67%), and machine learning-based method in 1 paper (6.67%). The Big Five personality traits were studied in the 22 studies (81.5%). Likert-type scale were designed to assess users’ willingness of information sharing in 22 studies (81.5%). And statistical data on social media were adopted to assess the willingness in 2 studies (13.3%). There were no outliers (i.e., no studies with a SE exceeding 0.21).

5.2 Results of meta-analysis

EXT-information sharing

Fig 3 shows the results of DerSimonian-Laird model in EXT subgroup. The DerSimonian-Laird model yield a significant moderate effect size (β = 0.05, p(β) < 0.001). The 95% confidence interval (CI) ranged from 0.03 to 0.07. The effect of this subgroup is homogenous (I2 = 31.6% < 50%, H = 1.2 < 1.5, Q(16) = 25.28, p(Q) = 0.07 > 0.05).

Fig 3. Forest plot displays the average correlation between the EXT trait and information sharing behavior across multiple studies.

Fig 3

AGR-information sharing

Fig 4 shows the results of DerSimonian-Laird model in AGR subgroup. The DerSimonian-Laird model yield a significant moderate effect size (β = -0.06, p(β) < 0.001). The 95% CI ranged from -0.09 to -0.03. The effect of this subgroup is homogenous (I2 = 27.44% < 50%, H = 1.17 < 1.5, Q(13) = 19.59, p(Q) = 0.11 > 0.05).

Fig 4. Forest plot displays the average correlation between the AGR trait and information sharing behavior across multiple studies.

Fig 4

CON-information sharing

Fig 5 shows the results of DerSimonian-Laird model in CON subgroup. The DerSimonian-Laird model yield a significant small effect size (β = -0.03, p(β) < 0.001). The 95% CI ranged from -0.05 to -0.02. This subgroup has no heterogeneity (I2 = 9.33% < 25%, H = 1.04 < 1.2, Q(11) = 13.28, p(Q) = 0.28 > 0.1).

Fig 5. Forest plot displays the average correlation between the CON trait and information sharing behavior across multiple studies.

Fig 5

NEU-information sharing

Fig 6 shows the results of DerSimonian-Laird model in NEU subgroup. The DerSimonian-Laird model yield a significant small effect size (β = -0.03, p(β) < 0.001). The 95% CI ranged from -0.05 to -0.02. This subgroup has no heterogeneity (I2 = 21.35% < 25%, H = 1.12 < 1.2, Q(11) = 16.34, p(Q) = 0.13 > 0.1).

Fig 6. Forest plot displays the average correlation between the NEU trait and information sharing behavior across multiple studies.

Fig 6

OPN-information sharing

Fig 7 shows the results of DerSimonian-Laird model in OPN subgroup. The DerSimonian-Laird model yield a insignificant minor effect size (β = 0.01, p(β) = 0.3 > 0.01). The 95% CI ranged from -0.01 to 0.03. Specifically, the CI contains 0, indicating that the relationship between OPN and information sharing is not significant. The effect of this subgroup is homogenous (I2 = 29.79% < 50%, H = 1.19 < 1.5, Q(15) = 25.05, p(Q) = 0.053 > 0.05).

Fig 7. Forest plot displays the average correlation between the OPN trait and information sharing behavior across multiple studies.

Fig 7

Additionally, we conducted a cumulative meta-analysis that sorted by year in EXT, AGR, CON, NEU subgroups, and the results are presented in Fig 8. Obviously, the CIs of each subgroup converge cumulatively.

Fig 8. Cumulative forest plot.

Fig 8

5.3 Publication bias of subgroup analysis

First, the funnel plot (Fig 9) illustrated that the majority of the studies analyzed are evenly distributed in a symmetrical pattern near the center, suggesting no publication bias in our screened studies by Leave-one-out method.

Fig 9. Funnel plot of OPN subgroup analysis.

Fig 9

Futher, Table 3 presents the test results of Begg’s test and Egger’s test, providing the same conclusion as the above. Neither the Begg’s test (p = 0.19, 0.74,0.788,0.41, 0.65>0.05) nor the Egger’s test (p = 0.4347, 0.4161, 0.4372, 0.3632, 0.3020 > 0.05) was signiffcant, providing additional evidence to support the absence of publication bias. This also indicates that there is no publication bias in the studies we selected.

Table 3. Begg’s test and Egger’s test results of subgroup analysis.

Subgroup Sample Begg’s test Egger’s test
z p z p
EXT 17 1.32 0.1871 0.78 0.4347
AGR 14 -0.44 0.7418 -0.81 0.4161
CON 12 0.27 0.7834 0.78 0.4372
NEU 12 -0.96 0.4095 -0.91 0.3632
OPN 16 -0.54 0.6522 1.03 0.3020

6 Discussion

In the long run of research, most of the Big Five personality traits are believed to be related to information sharing behavior on social media. The positive correlation between EXT traits and information sharing behavior is the highest (β = 0.05). The negative correlation between AGR traits and information sharing behavior is the greatest (β = -0.06). Figs 36 report significant correlations, therefore, H1, H2, and H4 were supported, H3 was rejected, and H5 was not fully supported. As shown in the above forest plots, literature [20] (n = 21314) and literature [19] (n = 409) have a relatively high weight, since the large sample size. Significantly, no study carries enough weight to decisively influence the outcome, suggesting that our meta-analysis has low sensitivity. Overall, our findings are shown in the Fig 10.

Fig 10. Revised theoretical model.

Fig 10

Our research has established a linkage between information sharing behavior on social media and the Big Five personality traits. This evidence implies that future studies related to information sharing behavior, irrespective of their specific scenarios, should incorporate an additional focus on the influence of personality traits. This incorporation will provide a holistic understanding of information sharing behavior. Moreover, studying how personality influences information sharing behavior across different subfields is essential, given the diverse levels of interest that individuals have in various types of information. For example, individuals with high scores of conscientiousness are more likely to participate in discussions on political related information [57].

Second, the key research in current information science is personalized applications [5860], such as recommendation system and chat AI. The results of this study will contribute to the development of these personalized applications. This work also holds important implications for the field of security, particularly in addressing the prevalent issues of rumor spreading and online fraud. Current social landscape is marred by the substantial impact of these problems. The notion of “psychological persuasion” has gained attention in recent research [61], revealing the potency of personalized warnings in improving the efficacy of persuasion strategies [62, 63]. This study, along with machine learning-based automatic personality detection methods, enables the possibility of delivering personalized warnings on a large scale. Finally, the present study’s findings are particularly noteworthy in light of the current era of artificial intelligence-generated content (e.g., ChatGPT [64]). The fine-tuning of the large language model may also be based on the user’s personality traits. We also recommend personalized strategies when dealing with the dissemination of these generated content messages based on personality traits. Finally, we will discuss the limitations of this work.

Included studies

Some studies were excluded from this meta-analysis because they did not provide correlation coefficients or regression coefficients. Consequently, the number of included studies was reduced, potentially leading to biased meta-analysis results.

Cultural differences

Cultural differences will affect the test results of the Big Five personality traits [65, 66]. For example, Europeans and Americans tend to have higher EXT scores compared to Asians and Africans. Unfortunately, inadequate research poses a hindrance to performing subgroup analysis. Insufficient research can result in significant publication bias in meta-analysis.

Uncertain impact of openness

H5 was not fully supported. This meta-analysis further identified the uncertain impact of openness traits on information sharing behavior. At present, we were not well examined with enough studies to pass the test (Fig 7). To enhance the validation of these findings in future reviews, more studies published in other languages should be included, along with representative sampling methods.

Machine learning-based personality measurement

Applying machine learning for social user personality detection allows for a substantial increase in research sample size [67]. However, only one paper has utilized this technology [49], and the level of detection accuracy is concerning.

7 Conclusion

Although limited, this meta-analysis enhances understanding of the role of personality factors in information sharing behavior on social media in the existing studies. Based on the meta-analysis presented, we found that extraversion positively correlates with information sharing on social media, while agreeableness, conscientiousness, and neuroticism negatively correlate with it. In future studies, it will be important to investigate these personality traits more extensively.

Supporting information

S1 Checklist. PRISMA 2020 checklist.

(PDF)

pone.0303770.s001.pdf (63.5KB, pdf)
S1 Data

(DTA)

pone.0303770.s002.dta (29.6KB, dta)

Data Availability

All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting information files.

Funding Statement

This work was supported by the National Natural Science Foundation of China Joint Fund Project [U1536122], Key Special Project of Technology Boosts Economy 2020 by Ministry of Science and Technology [SQ2020YFF0413781], Pilot Demonstration Project of Big Data Industry Development [Big data intelligent analysis and service platform for language barrier regeneration applications], and Tian Jin Research Innovation Project for Postgraduate Students [2022BKY158].

References

  • 1. Li R. D., Guo Q., Zhang X. K., & Liu J. G. (2022). Reconstructing community structure of online social network via user opinions. Chaos: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Nonlinear Science, 32(5). doi: 10.1063/5.0086796 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Pellicani A., Pio G., Redavid D., & Ceci M. (2023). SAIRUS: Spatially-aware identification of risky users in social networks. Information Fusion, 92, 435–449. doi: 10.1016/j.inffus.2022.11.029 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Lu M., Huang Z., Li B., Zhao Y., Qin Z. & Li D., (2022). SIFTER: A Framework for Robust Rumor Detection, IEEE/ACM Transactions on Audio, Speech, and Language Processing, 30. 429–442. doi: 10.1109/TASLP.2022.3140474 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Liu Yao, Shen Hao & Shi Lei. (2023). A Review of Rumor Detection Techniques in Social Networks. Journal of Intelligent & Fuzzy Systems. 44(3). 3561–3578. doi: 10.3233/JIFS-221894 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Wu H., Yue K., Pei Y., Li B., Zhao Y., & Dong F. (2016). Collaborative topic regression with social trust ensemble for recommendation in social media systems. Knowledge-Based Systems, 97, 111–122. doi: 10.1016/j.knosys.2016.01.011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Luo Chengwen, Li Chaoxi, Hong Hande, Li Jianqiang, Li Wei, Ming Zhong, et al. (2018). SODAR: Nonobtrusive Off-Line Social Structure Reconstruction Through Passive Wireless Sensing, IEEE Transactions on Computational Social Systems, 5(3), 871–883. doi: 10.1109/TCSS.2018.2858799 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Nkhi N., Varachia Z., & Cerbone D. (2021). Impression management techniques employed by listed companies on the Johannesburg Stock Exchange when using Twitter. Communicare: Journal for Communication Sciences in Southern Africa, 40(2), 129–151. [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Varachia Z., & Nkhi N. (2021). Impression Management Strategies Used When Tweeting: An Analysis of Performance and Market Capitalisation. COMMUNICATIO-SOUTH AFRICAN JOURNAL FOR COMMUNICATION THEORY AND RESEARCH, 47(3), 104–132. [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Dhar S., Bose I. Corporate Users’ Attachment to Social Networking Sites: Examining the Role of Social Capital and Perceived Benefits. Inf Syst Front 25, 1197–1217 (2023). doi: 10.1007/s10796-022-10289-y [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Ahn H., & Park J.H. (2015). The structural effects of sharing function on twitter networks: focusing on the retweet function. Journal of Information Science, 41, 354–365. doi: 10.1177/0165551515574974 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Stanek K. C., & Ones D. S. (2023). Meta-analytic relations between personality and cognitive ability. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 120(23), e2212794120. doi: 10.1073/pnas.2212794120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Roberts B. W., & Jackson J. J. (2008). Sociogenomic personality psychology. Journal of personality, 76(6), 1523–1544. doi: 10.1111/j.1467-6494.2008.00530.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Piepiora Paweł, Piepiora Zbigniew, and Justyna Bagińska. (2022) Personality and sport experience of 20–29-year-old polish male professional athletes. Frontiers in Psychology, 13: 854804. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2022.854804 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Lin H., Wang C., & Hao Q. (2023). A novel personality detection method based on high-dimensional psycholinguistic features and improved distributed Gray Wolf Optimizer for feature selection. Information Processing & Management, 60(2), 103217. doi: 10.1016/j.ipm.2022.103217 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Cheng C., Lau Y. C., Chan L., & Luk J. W. (2021). Prevalence of social media addiction across 32 nations: Meta-analysis with subgroup analysis of classification schemes and cultural values. Addictive behaviors, 117, 106845. doi: 10.1016/j.addbeh.2021.106845 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Kun B., Takacs Z. K., Richman M. J., Griffiths M. D., & Demetrovics Z. (2021). Work addiction and personality: A meta-analytic study. Journal of behavioral addictions, 9(4), 945–966. doi: 10.1556/2006.2020.00097 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Rajesh T, Rangaiah B. Relationship between personality traits and facebook addiction: A meta-analysis. Heliyon. 2022, 8(8):e10315. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2022.e10315 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Indu V. and Thampi Sabu M. (2021). A Psychologically-inspired Fuzzy-based Approach for User Personality Prediction Rumor Propagation Across Social Networks’. Journal of Intelligent & Fuzzy Systems, 41(5), 5425–5439. doi: 10.3233/JIFS-189864 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Buchanan T. (2020). Why do people spread false information online? The effects of message and viewer characteristics on self-reported likelihood of sharing social media disinformation. Plos one, 15(10), e0239666. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0239666 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Gil de Zuniga H., Diehl T., Huber B., & Liu J. (2017). Personality traits and social media use in 20 countries: How personality relates to frequency of social media use, social media news use, and social media use for social interaction. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, and Social Networking, 20(9), 540–552. doi: 10.1089/cyber.2017.0295 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Xiao X., & Su Y. (2022). Wired to seek, comment and share? Examining the relationship between personality, news consumption and misinformation engagement. Online Information Review, 46(6), 1152–1166. doi: 10.1108/OIR-10-2021-0520 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Lu L., Leung K., & Koch P. T. (2006). Managerial knowledge sharing: The role of individual, interpersonal, and organizational factors. Management and Organization Review, 2(1), 15–41. doi: 10.1111/j.1740-8784.2006.00029.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Gong Y., Cheung S. Y., Wang M., & Huang J. C. (2012). Unfolding proactive processes for creativity: integration of employee proactivity, information exchange, and psychological safety perspectives. Journal of Management: Official Journal of the Southern Management Association, 36(5), 603–612. [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Ashiru Ja., Oluwajana D. & Biabor O.S. (2023). Is the Global Pandemic Driving Me Crazy? The Relationship Between Personality Traits, Fear of Missing Out, and Social Media Fatigue During the COVID-19 Pandemic in Nigeria. Int J Ment Health Addiction 21, 2309–2324. doi: 10.1007/s11469-021-00723-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Hossain M. A., Dwivedi Y. K., Chan C., Standing C., & Olanrewaju A. S. (2018). Sharing political content in online social media: A planned and unplanned behaviour approach. Information Systems Frontiers, 20, 485–501. doi: 10.1007/s10796-017-9820-9 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Mustafa Sohaib, Zhang Wen & Naveed Muhammad Mateen. (2023) How to mend the dormant user in Q&A communities? A social cognitive theory-based study of consistent geeks of StackOverflow. Behaviour & Information Technology, 1–20. doi: 10.1080/0144929X.2023.2237604 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Chen, X. (2016). The influences of personality and motivation on the sharing of misinformation on social media. IConference 2016 Proceedings. 10.9776/16145 [DOI]
  • 28. Olhnuud A, Liu Y, Makowski D, Tscharntke Teja, Westphal Catrin, Wu Panlong, et al. (2022) Pollination deficits and contributions of pollinators in apple production: A global meta‐analysis. Journal of Applied Ecology, 59(12), 2911–2921. doi: 10.1111/1365-2664.14279 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Xin Liu. Research on Netizen Multi-dimensional Classificationunder Public Health Emergency. Jilin University(Master of Medicine), 2021. 10.27162/d.cnki.gjlin.2021.007106 [DOI]
  • 30. Davenport S. W., Bergman S. M., Bergman J. Z., & Fearrington M. E. (2014). Twitter versus Facebook: Exploring the role of narcissism in the motives and usage of different social media platforms. Computers in Human Behavior, 32, 212–220. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2013.12.011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Hakulinen C., Elovainio M., Pulkki-Råback L., Virtanen M., Kivimäki M., & Jokela M. (2015). PERSONALITY AND DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMS: INDIVIDUAL PARTICIPANT META-ANALYSIS OF 10 COHORT STUDIES. Depression and Anxiety, 32(7), 461–470. doi: 10.1002/da.22376 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Pyhala R., Wolford E., Kautiainen H., Andersson S., Bartmann P., Baumann N., et al. (2017). Self-Reported Mental Health Problems Among Adults Born Preterm: A Meta-analysis. Pediatrics, 139(4), e20162690. doi: 10.1542/peds.2016-2690 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Bowden S., Tran V., Asgariroozbehani R., Brett McIntyre W., Agarwal M., Siskind D., et al. (2020). T207. METABOLIC ADVERSE EFFECTS OF OFF-LABEL USE OF SECOND-GENERATION ANTIPSYCHOTICS IN THE ADULT POPULATION: A SYSTEMATIC REVIEW AND META-ANALYSIS. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 46(Supplement_1), S311–S311. doi: 10.1093/schbul/sbaa029.767 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. DerSimonian R & Laird N. Meta-analysis in clinical trials. Control Clin Trials, 1986, 7(3): 177–188. doi: 10.1016/0197-2456(86)90046-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Egger M., Smith G. D., & Phillips A. N. (1997). Meta-analysis: Principles and procedures. BMJ, 315(7121), 1533–1537. doi: 10.1136/bmj.315.7121.1533 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Diress Alen G, Endalifer ML, Addisu A, & Belayneh M. Association between social supports and depression among patients with diabetes mellitus in Ethiopia: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Open, 2022;12:e061801. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2022-061801 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Ma S, Guo X, Wang C, Yin Y, Xu G, Chen H, et al. Association of Barrett’s esophagus with Helicobacter pylori infection: a meta-analysis. Therapeutic Advances in Chronic Disease. 2022, 13. doi: 10.1177/20406223221117971 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Kitchenham B, Charters S. Guidelines for performing Systematic Literature Reviews in Software Engineering (EBSE 2007–001). Tech Rep. 2007. 10.1145/1134285.1134500 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Zhou Y, Zhang H, Huang X, Yang S, Babar MA, Tang H. Quality assessment of systematic reviews in software engineering: a tertiary study. Proceedings of the 19th International Conference on Evaluation and Assessment in Software Engineering. 2015. p. 14. 10.1145/2745802.2745815 [DOI]
  • 40. Hughes D. J., Rowe M., Batey M., & Lee A. (2012). A tale of two sites: Twitter vs. Facebook and the personality predictors of social media usage. Computers in human behavior, 28(2), 561–569. doi: 10.1016/j.chb.2011.11.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. Renjin Liu, Huifang Cao, Linlin Liu. (2017). An Empirical Study on Psychological Factors of Information Sharing in Social Commerce Websites. Soft Science, 31(12), 101–107. 10.13956/j.ss.1001-8409.2017.12.23 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Deng S., Lin Y., Liu Y., Chen X., & Li H. (2017). How Do Personality Traits Shape Information-Sharing Behaviour in Social Media? Exploring the Mediating Effect of Generalized Trust. Information Research: An International Electronic Journal, 22(3), n3. http://www.informationr.net/ir/22-3/paper763.html. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Dupre, D., McKeown, G., Andelic, N., & Morrison, G. (2018, October). Willingness to share emotion information on social media: Influence of personality and social context. In 2018 IEEE 5th International Conference on Data Science and Advanced Analytics (DSAA) (pp. 665–672). IEEE. 10.1109/DSAA.2018.00086 [DOI]
  • 44. Buchanan T., & Benson V. (2019). Spreading disinformation on facebook: Do trust in message source, risk propensity, or personality affect the organic reach of “fake news”?. Social media + society, 5(4), 2056305119888654. https://doi.org/doi.org/10.1177/2056305119888654 [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Wei Huang, Zhenyuan Zhu, & Ye Xu. (2020). An Empirical Study on the Motives of Information Sharing among We Media Users Based on Big Five Mode. Library and Information Service, 64(04), 59–67. 10.13266/j.issn.0252-3116.2020.04.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. Yin C., Zhang X., & Liu L. (2020). Reposting negative information on microblogs: Do personality traits matter?. Information Processing & Management, 57(1), 102106. doi: 10.1016/j.ipm.2019.102106 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Sampat B., & Raj S. (2022). Fake or real news? Understanding the gratifications and personality traits of individuals sharing fake news on social media platforms. Aslib Journal of Information Management, 74(5), 840–876. doi: 10.1108/AJIM-08-2021-0232 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. Ahmed S., & Rasul M. E. (2022). Social Media News use and covid-19 misinformation engagement: Survey study. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 24(9), e38944. doi: 10.2196/38944 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Liwei Xu, Mingxing Han, qiu Jiangnan. (2023). Dynamic Effects of Emotions on Information Sharing Behavior in the EID Event Contexts: The Moderating Role of Personality Traits. Journal of Information Resources Management, 13(03), 79–99. 10.13365/j.jirm.2023.03.079 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.John, O. P., Donahue, E. M., & Kentle, R. L. (1991). The big five inventory—versions 4a and 54. Berkeley: University of California, Berkeley, Institute of Personality and Social Research.
  • 51.Costa, P. T., & McCrea, R. R. (1985). The NEO personality inventory manual. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  • 52.Costa, P. T., & McCrea, R. R. (1989). Revised NEO Personality Inventory (NEO PI-R) and NEO Five–Factor Inventory (NEO-FFI). Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources.
  • 53. Buchanan T, Johnson JA, & Goldberg LR. (2005). Implementing a Five-Factor Personality Inventory for Use on the Internet. European Journal of Psychological Assessment. 21: 115–127. doi: 10.1027/1015-5759.21.2.115 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Rammstedt B., & John O. P. (2007). Measuring personality in one minute or less: A 10-item short version of the Big Five Inventory in English and German. Journal of Research in Personality, 41(1), 203–212. doi: 10.1016/j.jrp.2006.02.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Donnellan M. B., Oswald F. L., Baird B. M., & Lucas R. E. (2006). The Mini-IPIP Scales: Tiny-yet-effective measures of the Big Five Factors of Personality. Psychological Assessment, 18(2), 192–203. doi: 10.1037/1040-3590.18.2.192 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Gosling S. D., Rentfrow P. J., & Swanm W. B. Jr. (2003). A very brief measure of the Big-Five personality domains. Journal of Research in Personality, 37, 504–528. doi: 10.1016/S0092-6566(03)00046-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Xianchao Wu, Xiuping Chen. Research on the Role of Personality Traits in Network Political Participation. Journal of Huazhong University of Science and Technology(Social Science Edition), 2019, 33(5): 133–140. [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Ma, Z., Dou, Z., Zhu, Y., Zhong, H., & Wen, J. R. (2021, July). One chatbot per person: Creating personalized chatbots based on implicit user profiles. In Proceedings of the 44th international ACM SIGIR conference on research and development in information retrieval, pp. 555–564.
  • 59. Kang J., Du H., Li Z., Xiong Z., Ma S., Niyato D., et al. (2022). Personalized saliency in task-oriented semantic communications: Image transmission and performance analysis. IEEE Journal on Selected Areas in Communications, 41(1), 186–201. doi: 10.1109/JSAC.2022.3221990 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Wang P., Li L., Wang R., Zheng X., He J., & Xu G. (2022). Learning persona-driven personalized sentimental representation for review-based recommendation. Expert Systems with Applications, 203, 117317. doi: 10.1016/j.eswa.2022.117317 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Dubois D., Rucker D. D., & Galinsky A. D. (2016). Dynamics of communicator and audience power: The persuasiveness of competence versus warmth. Journal of Consumer Research, 43(1), 68–85. doi: 10.1093/jcr/ucw006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Xing T., Sun F., Wang K., Zhao J., Wu M., & Wu J. (2020). Vulnerability to fraud among Chinese older adults: Do personality traits and loneliness matter? Journal of Elder Abuse & Neglect, 32(1), 46–59. doi: 10.1080/08946566.2020.1731042 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Judges R. A., Gallant S. N., Yang L., et al. (2017). The role of cognition, personality, and trust in fraud victimization in older adults. Frontiers in Psychology, 8, 588. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00588 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Semrl, N., Feigl, S., Taumberger, N., Bracic, T., Fluhr, H., Blockeel, C., et al. (2023). AI language models in human reproduction research: exploring ChatGPT’s potential to assist academic writing. Human Reproduction, dead207. [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 65. McCrae R. R., Terracciano A., & Personality Profiles of Cultures Project. (2005). Personality profiles of cultures: Aggregate personality traits. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 89(3), 407–425. doi: 10.1037/0022-3514.89.3.407 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Haas B. W., Abney D. H., Eriksson K., Potter J., & Gosling S. D. (2023). Person-Culture Personality Fit: Dispositional Traits and Cultural Context Explain Country-Level Personality Profile Conformity. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 14(3), 275–285. doi: 10.1177/19485506221100954 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Mehta Y., Majumder N., Gelbukh A. et al. (2020). Recent trends in deep learning based personality detection. Artif Intell Rev 53, 2313–2339. doi: 10.1007/s10462-019-09770-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Simone Varrasi

14 Feb 2024

PONE-D-23-41029How Big Five Personality Traits Influence Information Sharing on Social Media: A Meta AnalysisPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Sun,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Reviewers' comments must be addressed thoroughly with equal attention. They should be considered as complementary in the implementation of revisions. Please note that Reference 58 appears to be subject to an Expression of Concern relating to the data reported in that article. Please consider finding an alternative source to support the relevant statements, or if such a source is not available, please remove or revise the statements supported by this reference.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Mar 30 2024 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Simone Varrasi

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Thank you for stating the following financial disclosure:

“This work was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China-Joint Fund Project [U1536122], Key Special Project of “Technology Boosts Economy 2020” by Ministry of Science and Technology [SQ2020YFF0413781], and Pilot Demonstration Project of Big Data Industry Development [Big data intelligent analysis and service platform for language barrier rehabilitation applications].”

Please state what role the funders took in the study.  If the funders had no role, please state: "The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript."

If this statement is not correct you must amend it as needed.

Please include this amended Role of Funder statement in your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

3. Thank you for stating the following in the Acknowledgments Section of your manuscript:

“This work was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China-Joint Fund Project [U1536122], Key Special Project of “Technology Boosts Economy 2020” by Ministry of Science and Technology [SQ2020YFF0413781], and Pilot Demonstration Project of Big Data Industry Development [Big data intelligent analysis and service platform for language barrier rehabilitation applications].

We note that you have provided funding information that is currently declared in your Funding Statement. However, funding information should not appear in the Acknowledgments section or other areas of your manuscript. We will only publish funding information present in the Funding Statement section of the online submission form.

Please remove any funding-related text from the manuscript and let us know how you would like to update your Funding Statement. Currently, your Funding Statement reads as follows:

“This work was supported by National Natural Science Foundation of China-Joint Fund Project [U1536122], Key Special Project of “Technology Boosts Economy 2020” by Ministry of Science and Technology [SQ2020YFF0413781], and Pilot Demonstration Project of Big Data Industry Development [Big data intelligent analysis and service platform for language barrier rehabilitation applications].”

Please include your amended statements within your cover letter; we will change the online submission form on your behalf.

4. We note that your Data Availability Statement is currently as follows: [All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting Information files.]

Please confirm at this time whether or not your submission contains all raw data required to replicate the results of your study. Authors must share the “minimal data set” for their submission. PLOS defines the minimal data set to consist of the data required to replicate all study findings reported in the article, as well as related metadata and methods (https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-minimal-data-set-definition).

For example, authors should submit the following data:

- The values behind the means, standard deviations and other measures reported;

- The values used to build graphs;

- The points extracted from images for analysis.

Authors do not need to submit their entire data set if only a portion of the data was used in the reported study.

If your submission does not contain these data, please either upload them as Supporting Information files or deposit them to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of recommended repositories, please see https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/recommended-repositories.

If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent. If data are owned by a third party, please indicate how others may request data access.

5. PLOS requires an ORCID iD for the corresponding author in Editorial Manager on papers submitted after December 6th, 2016. Please ensure that you have an ORCID iD and that it is validated in Editorial Manager. To do this, go to ‘Update my Information’ (in the upper left-hand corner of the main menu), and click on the Fetch/Validate link next to the ORCID field. This will take you to the ORCID site and allow you to create a new iD or authenticate a pre-existing iD in Editorial Manager. Please see the following video for instructions on linking an ORCID iD to your Editorial Manager account: https://www.youtube.com/watch?v=_xcclfuvtxQ.

6. We notice that your supplementary figure and tables are included in the manuscript file. Please remove them and upload them with the file type 'Supporting Information'. Please ensure that each Supporting Information file has a legend listed in the manuscript after the references list.

Additional Editor Comments:

Dear Authors,

many thanks for your submission. Please reply to both the Reviewers and revise the manuscript accordingly, taking thoroughly into account their concerns.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Partly

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Dear Authors,

Your work provides cognitively interesting data on personality.

The study is legitimate, but the text is laconic.

It is typical of a Case Study Report, not a Research Article.

Therefore, please improve the editing style appropriate for a Research Article.

Also, please let me know if you used artificial intelligence in your work?

If so, to what extent?

In addition, please refer to the impact of experience on personality in the discussion.

I suggest referring to: https://doi.org/10.3389/fpsyg.2022.854804.

After all, verify the abstract and change the keywords to something other than the title.

Reviewer #2: This is a rather intriguing review, acknowledging the significant role of personality. The reviewers have put forth the following observations:

1. Elucidate the trajectory of your research in the introduction, and underscore why this trajectory is of consequence. Despite the current introduction being quite direct and detailing the structure of this paper, it falls short of a specific delineation of the research problem and background.

2. It is imperative to scrutinise current analogous reviews and accentuate the disparities between this manuscript and them.

3. The discussion warrants a more profound exploration.

4. Kindly delve deeper into the potential implications of the findings of this article.

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

PLoS One. 2024 Jun 12;19(6):e0303770. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0303770.r003

Author response to Decision Letter 0


7 Apr 2024

For point-to-point responses to reviewer comments, please refer to Response to Reviewerss.pdf.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers(2).pdf

pone.0303770.s004.pdf (201.2KB, pdf)

Decision Letter 1

Simone Varrasi

29 Apr 2024

PONE-D-23-41029R1How Big Five Personality Traits Influence Information Sharing on Social Media: A Meta AnalysisPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Sun,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please address the comments made by Reviewers. One of them recommended to reject the manuscript. Is it required to discuss their comments as limitations of you research, and to improve the structure of the article as suggested by both scholars. After these changes, your work will be finally evaluated for its suitability for the Journal.

Please submit your revised manuscript by Jun 13 2024 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Simone Varrasi

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: All comments have been addressed

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Dear Authors,

thank you for inviting me to review your work again and taking into account the suggestions for improvement. Since it is now an original article this excessive number of sections and subsections makes the content difficult to perceive. Therefore, consider rebuilding the content breakdown to a few main sections. Other than that, I have no objections to the content, methodology and pragmatics.

Reviewer #2: The topic on which this manuscript focuses is of great interest. However, the manuscript currently has the following problems: (1) the selected database is not representative, (2) the structure and presentation of the manuscript are not clear enough, (3) the manuscript does not fully discuss the issues of concern, and does not combine the analysis results There is also no focus on cultural heterogeneity across regions. In summary, the reviewers believe that the manuscript does not meet the journal's requirements.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Decision Letter 2

Simone Varrasi

1 May 2024

How Big Five Personality Traits Influence Information Sharing on Social Media: A Meta Analysis

PONE-D-23-41029R2

Dear Dr. Sun,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice will be generated when your article is formally accepted. Please note, if your institution has a publishing partnership with PLOS and your article meets the relevant criteria, all or part of your publication costs will be covered. Please make sure your user information is up-to-date by logging into Editorial Manager at Editorial Manager® and clicking the ‘Update My Information' link at the top of the page. If you have any questions relating to publication charges, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Simone Varrasi

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Additional Editor Comments (optional):

Reviewers' comments:

Acceptance letter

Simone Varrasi

15 May 2024

PONE-D-23-41029R2

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Sun,

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now being handed over to our production team.

At this stage, our production department will prepare your paper for publication. This includes ensuring the following:

* All references, tables, and figures are properly cited

* All relevant supporting information is included in the manuscript submission,

* There are no issues that prevent the paper from being properly typeset

If revisions are needed, the production department will contact you directly to resolve them. If no revisions are needed, you will receive an email when the publication date has been set. At this time, we do not offer pre-publication proofs to authors during production of the accepted work. Please keep in mind that we are working through a large volume of accepted articles, so please give us a few weeks to review your paper and let you know the next and final steps.

Lastly, if your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at customercare@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Dr. Simone Varrasi

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Checklist. PRISMA 2020 checklist.

    (PDF)

    pone.0303770.s001.pdf (63.5KB, pdf)
    S1 Data

    (DTA)

    pone.0303770.s002.dta (29.6KB, dta)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.pdf

    pone.0303770.s003.pdf (264.2KB, pdf)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers(2).pdf

    pone.0303770.s004.pdf (201.2KB, pdf)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers2.pdf

    pone.0303770.s005.pdf (321.7KB, pdf)

    Data Availability Statement

    All relevant data are within the manuscript and its Supporting information files.


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES