Skip to main content
PLOS ONE logoLink to PLOS ONE
. 2024 Jun 13;19(6):e0295373. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0295373

Anodal M1 tDCS enhances online learning of rhythmic timing videogame skill

Anthony W Meek 1,*, Davin R Greenwell 1, Hayami Nishio 2, Brach Poston 3, Zachary A Riley 1
Editor: Sandra Carvalho4
PMCID: PMC11175489  PMID: 38870202

Abstract

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) has been shown to modify excitability of the primary motor cortex (M1) and influence online motor learning. However, research on the effects of tDCS on motor learning has focused predominantly on simplified motor tasks. The purpose of the present study was to investigate whether anodal stimulation of M1 over a single session of practice influences online learning of a relatively complex rhythmic timing video game. Fifty-eight healthy young adults were randomized to either a-tDCS or SHAM conditions and performed 2 familiarization blocks, a 20-minute 5 block practice period while receiving their assigned stimulation, and a post-test block with their non-dominant hand. To assess performance, a performance index was calculated that incorporated timing accuracy elements and incorrect key inputs. The results showed that M1 a-tDCS enhanced the learning of the video game based skill more than SHAM stimulation during practice, as well as overall learning at the post-test. These results provide evidence that M1 a-tDCS can enhance acquisition of skills where quality or success of performance depends on optimized timing between component motions of the skill, which could have implications for the application of tDCS in many real-world contexts.

Introduction

Motor learning is fundamental in everyday life for acquiring and honing skills that range in complexity from relatively simple, like reaching and pressing a button, to more complex skills requiring coordinated sequential actions such as learning a piece of music on an instrument or throwing a ball. Motor skill improvements can accrue during a single practice session (online) or after practice is completed (offline), with both contributing to long-term retention over time [1]. The online and offline skill changes comprise the fast and slow stages of learning, with fast stage learning occurring early on, markedly during skill acquisition, and slow stage occurring later with incremental gains over multiple practice sessions [2,3]. The motor learning process is underpinned by neuroplastic changes across a spatially distributed network of interconnected brain regions [4]. Which areas are involved and the extent to which plasticity within them may subserve learning, largely depends upon the characteristics of task and the stage learning [5].

The primary motor cortex (M1) appears to be crucial in the early stages of learning for fast and precise motions associated with skillful performance [6,7]. When learning a new motor skill, task-associated neurons are recruited and their repeated, sequential activation drives plastic changes synaptic strength, reinforcing connectivity [8,9]. This change in corticomotor excitability has been previously demonstrated using transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) in conjunction with electromyography (EMG) [10,11]. Even short bouts of skill practice (10 to 30 minutes) have been shown to elicit acute increases in M1 excitability [11,12]. Although these early, practice-related changes are relatively transient in the short term [13], it is believed that this is an important initial step of fast motor learning and skill acquisition [2,11]. Based on these short-term mechanisms as well as the more permanent structural and functional cortical reorganization associated with extensive practice and skill expertise [10,1416], M1 is a site of particular interest for investigating motor learning.

Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) is a non-invasive brain stimulation technique which is proposed to modulate cortical excitability by passing a weak, sub-threshold electrical current between two electrodes placed on or near the scalp [17]. This current passes between electrodes by passing through the surface tissues and into at underlying cortical surface [18]. The outcome of this stimulation depends on the placement and directional flow of current between electrodes with anodal tDCS (a-tDCS) typically being regarded as facilitatory, while cathodal tDCS (c-tDCS) is generally regarded as inhibitory [19]. Studies have shown that a-tDCS is able to enhance cortical excitability and LTP-like plasticity in M1 and improve motor function [2022]. Additionally, tDCS has been shown to be particularly relevant in the acquisition and early consolidation phase of motor learning and evidence suggests that it is safe for facilitating motor learning in healthy individuals [22,23] as well as those suffering from neurological disorders [24,25].

In a recent meta-analysis by Patel and colleagues (2018), the application of tDCS to M1 during skill practice was found to enhance performance of several upper limb motor tasks through changes in speed (reaction or movement time), accuracy, the relationship between those two qualities, or reduced variability [21]. However, much of the tDCS literature reporting positive effects of M1 stimulation on performance have used simple laboratory-based tasks, such as the serial reaction time task (SRTT), sequential finger tapping task (SFTT), and sequential visual isometric pinch force task (SVIPT) that, while highly controlled, fail to represent the complexity of real-world movements [21,22,26]. Despite the non-generalizability of such findings to more complex movements, these studies lend support to M1’s important role in the fast stage of learning and as a potential as a target for tDCS.

Recently, research has shown that M1 tDCS may be able to enhance performance and learning in more complex tasks like surgical skills and or videogames [27,28]. Additionally, our lab recently demonstrated that a-tDCS of the non-dominant M1 enhances online learning in both a dexterous, fine-motor tweezer task [29] as well as a complex multi-joint dart throwing task [30] performed with the non-dominant arm. Though these sorts of tasks are more generalizable to real-world skills, performance outcomes are more variable and harder to objectively measure than simpler laboratory tasks. Videogames, which share traits of both conventional laboratory motor tasks and more dynamic real-world movements, are a novel way to study the effects of tDCS on motor learning. Videogames involve diverse combinations of perceptual, attentional, cognitive, and motor skills, and practice can lead to training-induced learning [31,32]. Although there are reports of competitive gamers using tDCS to enhance performance [33], the effect of M1 tDCS in the context of gaming-based motor skill has only recently been initially investigated [27].

Our study seeks to further investigate the effects of M1 tDCS on the early stages of learning a complex videogame task. Given the task dependency of tDCS effects for relatively simple tasks [21,34,35], it is unclear how M1 tDCS will influence the acquisition of a videogame task with rhythmic sequence tapping movements combined with complex visuomotor and auditory processing demands. Based upon our previous work showing that a-tDCS of M1 enhances the early stages of learning in complex motor tasks [29,30] performed with the non-dominant arm, we hypothesized that a-tDCS of M1 would also enhance learning in a complex videogame task performed with the non-dominant arm. Our investigation was largely exploratory as, to our knowledge, this was the first study to utilize a rhythmic, sequence tapping videogame to study the effects of tDCS on motor learning.

Methods

Participants

A total of 60 volunteers were recruited for this study (n = 30 per group; age 22.27 ± 2.78 yrs). Of these, a total of 58 were included in analysis. One subject’s data excluded due significant outliers in performance scores and another was excluded due to a software issue during data collection which resulted in incomplete data. All participants were free of neurological or musculoskeletal impairments that could impact performance of the task and any volunteers who were taking medications that could influence learning, such as stimulants for ADHD, were excluded from participating in the study. Handedness was determined with the Edinburgh Handedness Inventory (EHI) [36]. Right-hand dominance was identified with scores > +40, left-hand dominance with scores < -40, while scores ≥-40 and ≤+40 were ambidextrous. In cases of ambidextrousness, the subject’s preferred writing hand was considered their dominant. (See Table 1 below for subject characterization).

Table 1. Subject characterization.

Condition Male Female
Handedness
a-tDCS 14 15
Right 12 14
Left 2 1
SHAM 15 14
Right 11 13
Left 4 1

a-tDCS = Anodal transcranial direct-current stimulation.

SHAM = Sham transcranial direct-current stimulation.

This was a prospective study which utilized a randomized, single-blinded, between-subjects, SHAM controlled design. Using MathWorks MATLAB software, a randomly generated number sequence was created and used during screening to assign subjects into either the anodal tDCS group (a-tDCS) or SHAM stimulation group (SHAM) based upon their order of recruitment. Subjects were blinded to the condition that they received for the duration of testing. The two groups were relatively matched for age (a-tDCS = 22.70 ± 2.96 yrs and SHAM = 22.34 ± 2.64 yrs) and sex (a-tDCS = 14 males, 15 females; SHAM = 15 males, 14 females). For handedness, considering preferred writing hand in the case of an ambidextrous inventory result, each group had 25 right-handed individuals and 4 left-handed individuals. All procedures were approved by Indiana University’s IRB and conducted according to the Declaration of Helsinki.

Procedures

Participants completed a single testing session while receiving a-tDCS or SHAM stimulation over M1 contralateral to the non-dominant (active) hand. During the session, subjects performed a timing-based dexterous video game task with the non-dominant hand. The protocol included 2 familiarization pre-test blocks, 5 practice blocks while receiving either a-tDCS or SHAM, and a post-test block 5 min after cessation of practice. Familiarization pre-test scores were used to determine whether the task difficulty was adequate to demonstrate motor learning during the practice period. If the task was too easy or too hard based on the familiarization trial performance, the difficulty was increased or decreased accordingly in the video game settings, and one additional familiarization trial was given at the new speed (Fig 1B).

Fig 1. Step mania overview & study timeline.

Fig 1

(a) Screen shot of Step Mania gameplay showing scrolling arrow icons and stationary arrow silhouettes. (b) Study procedures timeline.

Step Mania task

Step Mania (https://www.stepmania.com/) is an open-source videogame where sequences of directional arrow icons (up, down, left, right) scroll upwards toward stationary arrow silhouettes (Fig 1A) and the objective is to press the appropriate arrow keys whenever a corresponding scrolling icon is perfectly aligned with (i.e. centered on) its silhouette.

Participants sat at a desk with a keyboard positioned with the arrow keys in front of the non-dominant hand. Subjects were instructed to only use one hand to tap the keys and to only tap keys one time per cue. However, no explicit instructions were given assigning specific digits to particular keys. A brief demonstration of the gameplay was shown to the participants as the objective was explained (i.e. press correct keys with optimal timing and avoid unnecessary/incorrect key strokes).

The same 219 cue pattern was used for all test blocks. One of the 219 cues required the left and right arrow key to be struck simultaneously, thus the pattern consisted of 55 up, 56 down, 55 left and 54 right key strokes (220 total key taps). This could not be changed through the available settings. Each testing block comprised one complete pattern. Each keystroke was categorized relative to a time window centered (time = 0s) on perfect overlap of the scrolling icon and its stationary silhouette. These timing windows were built into the program as flawless (0 to ± 0.0225s), perfect (±0.0225 to ±0.045s), great (±0.045 to ±0.090s), good (±0.090 to ±0.135s), boo (±0,135s to ± 0.180s), and miss (> ± 0.180s). To note, the first two time windows (flawless and perfect) were 22.5ms, while the other windows were 45ms.

Baseline assessment and testing

Subjects performed 2 familiarization trials with 500 continuous modifier (CMOD) difficulty (sets the arrow scroll speed; “arrow heights” moved per minute). An initial proficiency score was calculated (Eq 1) indicative of the number of cues being met with a tap within scoring distance. This allowed for difficulty adjustment to ensure that the task was appropriately difficult for performance gains to be demonstrated within a single practice session. It was determined in pilot testing that if too many cues were entirely missed or nearly unscored based on the inbuilt timing windows, the task overwhelmed participants and they struggled to improve. Conversely, individuals who, in their familiarization trials demonstrated very precise timing and few misses, had little room for growth.

Familiarization Proficiency

FP=(Flawless*1)+(Perfect*0.75)+(Great*0.5)+(Good*0.25)+(Boo*0)+(Miss*1)219*100 (Eq 1)

For familiarization proficiency scores between 20 and 60, the default 500 CMOD was maintained throughout testing. For scores > 60 or < 20, the scrolling speed was adjusted up or down by 200, respectively (Fig 1B). The music tempo (210 bpm) was not affected by the CMOD change, it only affected the cue scrolling speed. Skill-adjusted subjects performed an additional familiarization block at the new speed that was used for all subsequent blocks. The last familiarization block was used as the pre-test measure for all subjects.

After familiarization and pre-test, tDCS electrodes were positioned during a 5 min break. Subjects then completed a 20 min practice period while they received either a-tDCS or SHAM during 5 practice blocks with 2 minutes of rest between blocks. The stimulation electrodes were then removed and subjects completed a post-test trial 5 minutes after completing practice.

Evaluating performance

At the end of each trial, the game provides feedback in the form of a numerical "game score" as well as an aggregate overview of the categorization of each keystroke timing (i.e. miss, boo, good, great, perfect, and flawless). However, this fails to account for any excess or aberrant keystrokes that fall outside of the inbuilt timing windows. For this reason, we opted to collect additional data using a separate keystroke logging software. This allowed us to devise an alternate scoring mechanism, or performance index (PI), that better reflects changes in task performance.

Two aspects of Step Mania performance are related to skill–hitting only the correct inputs and hitting them at the correct time. Both are interrelated as qualities of the task, but a change in one does not necessarily precipitate a proportional change in the other. With the guiding premise that newly acquired movement sequences are segmented, inaccurate, and jerky, whereas learned sequences are cohesive, accurate and smooth [6], a PI was calculated to incorporate incorrect inputs (Key Error Rate, KER) and temporal accuracy (2 values: Temporal Accuracy, TA; Tap Distribution Ratio, TDR) into a single value to reflect the skill associated with a certain quality of performance:

Performance Index (PI)

PI=(TAKER)×TDR (Eq 2)

A description is provided below for each of the constituent variables in the PI equation and how they are derived from the data provided by the game. Example data are shown after each equation to illustrate each step of the process.

Temporal Accuracy (TA)

TA was quantified by assigning point values to the timing windows, so that “flawless” keystrokes awarded 1.0 pt and each subsequent window awarded 0.2 pts less, resulting in 0 pts for each “miss”. TA provided a base value reflecting temporal accuracy of the entire 220 constituent inputs of each trial.

This is different from the Familiarization Proficiency equation we used to evaluate initial ability because we did not want to penalize a “miss” with a negative score and instead just not award any points in that case. This distinction was made because in the initial familiarization, if the speed of the game was too fast for the subject they would get overwhelmed and miss several arrows in a row without even attempting any inputs, in which case a measure was needed to clearly inform us to slow the game down. In the case of the timing accuracy measure (Eq 2), the subject would typically only miss an arrow occasionally and therefore just not be awarded points for it.

Temporal Accuracy (TA)

TA=((Flawless*1)+(Perfect*0.8)+(Great*0.6)+(Good*0.4)+(Boo*0.2)+(Miss*0)) (Eq 3)

For a subject with 72 Flawless, 69 Perfect, 58 Great, 10 good, 3 Boo and 7 miss:

TA=(72*1)+(69*0.8)+(58*0.6)+(10*0.4)+(3*0.2)+(7*0)=166.6

Key Error Rate (KER)

KER evaluated execution errors such as pressing multiple keys simultaneously, tapping the same key several times, or pressing a cycle of keys ‘searching’ for the correct one. Imprecise and unstable finger movements relate to learning in aspects of the skill, irrespective of timing (i.e. key sequence order, hand positioning, etc). Utilizing the extra keystroke data collected during the game, the differences between actual key stroke totals and the number of times the arrow keys appear in the sequence were calculated for each trial and used in Eq 3 to produce a scaled error value relative to the total number of cues.

Key Error Rate

KER=(up55)+(down56)+(left55)+(right54)220*100 (Eq 4)

For a subject with 56 up, 57 down, 53 left and 53 right arrow:

KER=(|5655|)+(|5756|)+(|5355|)+(|5354|)220*100=2.273

Tap Distribution Ratio (TDR)

In rhythm gaming, ratios of the tap totals in the different timing windows are commonly used in conjunction with timing scores because, as a consequence of binning taps into scored timing windows that are relatively wide, attempts can yield arbitrarily similar TAs despite the distribution of taps in the windows reflecting different levels of play. We adapted this strategy to further differentiate skill in the task and enhance sensitivity in the measure to changes in the quality of performance. TDR adjusts scores based on the concentration of taps in the two best and two worst categories, with additional weighting based on the proportion of those that are in the best (Flawless; most skillful) and worst (Miss; least skillful) timing windows. To properly illustrate the function of this variable, the original TA data from above will be used as well as a second example that produces an identical 166.6 TA value.

Tap Distribution Ratio (TDR)

TDR=1+Flawless+Perfect)219*1.5+Flawless2191+Boo+Miss219*1.5+Miss219 (Eq 5)

For a subject with 72 Flawless, 69 Perfect, 58 Great, 10 good, 3 Boo and 7 Miss:

TDR=1+(72+69)219*1.5+722191+3+7219*1.5+7219=1.877

For a second subject with 82 Flawless, 46 Perfect, 67 Great, 18 Good, 2 Boo, 4 Miss:

TDR=1+(82+46)219*1.5+822191+2+4219*1.5+4219=1.904

Assuming a similar KER value for both of these trials, the PI for each would be:

PI=(TAKER)*TDR
A.)PI=(166.62.273)*1.877=307.32

or

B.)PI=(166.62.273)*1.904=311.7

In utilizing a TDR factor to modify PI scores, we are able to better reflect the more subtle changes in subject performance that might otherwise be missed when relying on TA and KER values alone.

tDCS

A Soterix Medical 1x1 Low Intensity transcranial DC stimulator was used to deliver a-tDCS with parameters previously determined to be effective and safe (duration 20 min; current 1mA; active electrode over M1 contralateral to the non-dominant hand and reference electrode over the ipsilateral supraorbital). These parameters were chosen because previous research, including work from our lab, has shown that 1mA of current delivered for 20 minutes is sufficient to elicit changes in cortical excitability and motor performance and that higher doses (longer durations and/or higher intensities) may result in diminishing or inverse effects [3741]. Current was delivered through a pair of 5cm x 5cm (25cm2) rubber electrodes which were placed inside of saline-soaked sponges and affixed to the head with rubber straps. In the a-tDCS condition, the current intensity was ramped up over the course of 30s, maintained at 1mA for 20 minutes, and followed by a 30s ramp down. For the SHAM condition, a 30 second ramp up and then down was performed at the beginning and end of the 20-minute period to simulate the sensation of stimulation.

Data analysis and statistics

While we recognize the importance of performing an a priori sample size calculation, our study was largely exploratory, and no effect size of stimulation or task performance score could be estimated from previous literature. For this reason, no initial sample size calculation was performed. Instead, we estimated a sample size based on our two previous studies which showed significant effects of a-tDCS on motor task performance in a non-dominant hand tweezer pegboard task (n = 40) [37] and a non-dominant hand, dart throwing task (n = 58) [38]. Notably, between the dart throwing and tweezer task, the dart throwing was characterized by more degrees of freedom and, in turn, more task performance variability. Since the Step Mania task involved less upper arm movement and fewer degrees of freedom than our dart throwing task but more cognitive and technical complexity than our tweezer task, we determined that an appropriate recruitment target would likely lay somewhere between the 40 and 58 subjects. Still, because we were unsure, we opted to recruit 60 with the hopes that this number would provide us with sufficient statistical power for our analyses.

A PI value was calculated for each trial. PI gain scores (block PI–baseline PI) were calculated for each practice block as well as the post-test and used observe the change in performance over time relative to baseline performance. Normality was tested with a Shapiro-Wilk test. Baseline PI scores were compared between groups with an independent t-test to test for baseline differences before gain scores were calculated.

Separate comparisons were performed for the practice blocks and the post-test block. A mixed ANOVA (2 groups x 5 blocks) with repeated measures on block was used to compare the practice gain scores and effect size were calculated as partial eta squared (ηp2). For the post-test block, an independent t-test was used to compare the gain scores between the conditions and effect size was calculated as Cohen’s d. A p-value of 0.05 was considered statistically significant. Bonferroni post-hoc tests were used to determine differences for multiple comparisons. Data are presented as mean ± standard deviation in text and as mean ± standard error in figures. All data analysis was performed with SPSS 24.

Results

Practice blocks

Over the course of practice (B1 through B5), both groups gradually improved TA (range: 147.78 to 160.72) and TDR also modestly increased (range: 1.56 to 1.80). The rate of errors (KER) also improved (i.e. reduced) across practice for both groups (range: 3.83 to 2.71) (see Table 2). The calculated PI score for each block incorporated all 3 of these metrics.

Table 2. TA, KER, & TDR scores across time.

Practice
Baseline B1 B2 B3 B4 B5 Post-Test
a-tDCS
TA 135.66(19.81) 149.84(19.16) 152.85(20.02) 154.31(15.99) 159.42(15.04) 160.72(16.01) 166.63(15.57)
KER 5.36(4.53) 3.83(3.42) 3.30(2.59) 3.15(2.06) 3.26(2.33) 2.82(1.91) 2.63(1.93)
TDR 1.35(0.29) 1.59(0.32) 1.65(0.34) 1.66 (0.28) 1.76(0.28) 1.80(0.31) 1.92(0.31)
SHAM
TA 134.96(27.41) 147.78(22.42) 150.78(22.32) 154.21(19.79) 152.66(17.71) 155.36(19.73) 159.62(18.98)
KER 3.82(2.99) 3.32(1.97) 2.74(2.33) 2.90(2.16) 2.98(2.30) 2.71(1.82) 2.03(1.79)
TDR 1.37(0.36) 1.56(0.35) 1.62(0.35) 1.68(0.35) 1.64 (0.32) 1.69(0.37) 1.78(0.37)

TA ‐ Temporal Accuracy; KER ‐ Key error rate; TDR ‐ Tap Distribution Ratio.

Practice block PI gain scores were calculated relative to baseline to evaluate how overall performance of the two groups changed during practice. An independent t-test on the baseline PI values showed a-tDCS (181.80 pts ± 66.04) and SHAM (188.97 pts ± 79.39) performance was similar prior to practice (t(56) = -0.37, P = 0.71). An a-tDCS subject was missing error data for two practice trials and thus was excluded from the practice block analysis (a-tDCS n = 28, SHAM n = 29).

A Shapiro-Wilk test showed normally distributed gain scores (P = 0.278 to 0.977) for all cells of the design except a-tDCS B1 (P = 0.004) and a-tDCS B4 (P = 0.01). Since ANOVA type 1 error rate is fairly robust to normality violations [4244] the mixed ANOVA was still performed. Homogeneity of variances was confirmed by Levene’s test (P = 0.25 to 0.65). A Huynh-Feldt correction was used to adjust for a sphericity violation (P = 0.003). There was a statistically significant interaction between the stimulation group and block on practice gain scores (F[3.466, 190.605] = 3.042, P = 0.024, ηp2 = 0.052). Post hoc analysis of the simple main effects for group revealed a significant difference between conditions at B4 (P = 0.008) and B5 (P = 0.046), where a-tDCS (B4: 100.02pts ± 47.21; B5: 109.05pts ± 60.92) resulted in greater PI improvement than SHAM (B4: 62.05pts ± 55.75; B5: 76.58pts ± 59.16). Post hoc analysis for block showed that B3 and B5 SHAM gain scores were significantly different from B1 (P = 0.002 and P = 0.005). For a-tDCS, B4 and B5 gain scores were statistically significantly higher than blocks 1 (both, P < 0.001), 2 (P = 0.049 and P = 0.008) and 3 (P = 0.005 and P = 0.002).

Pre-test to post-test

One SHAM subject was missing error data for the post-test and thus was excluded from analysis (a-tDCS n = 29, SHAM n = 28). Normality of the gain scores was confirmed with a Shapiro-Wilk test (P = 0.661 to 0.540). An independent t-test on the gain scores from baseline to post-test showed a statistically significant difference between conditions (t(55) = 2.234, P = 0.030, Cohen’s d = 0.592), with a-tDCS (137.844pts ± 66.218) leading to greater improvement compared to SHAM (99.501pts ± 63.28) (Fig 2).

Fig 2. Change in Performance Index (PI) gain score across time.

Fig 2

Data show the practice block PI gain scores relative to baseline (i.e. last familiarization block) for a-tDCS (n = 28) and SHAM (n = 29) conditions. The gain scores from baseline to the post-test block for a-tDCS (n = 29) and SHAM (n = 28) are also shown. Whiskers denote the standard error. Both groups displayed significant increases in performance across practice. a-tDCS gain scores were significantly greater than SHAM at B4 and B5. At the post-test, a-tDCS resulted in significantly greater gains in performance compared to SHAM. *Indicates significant difference from SHAM (P < 0.05).

Discussion

Our results indicate that, compared to SHAM stimulation, M1 a-tDCS has a beneficial effect on the acquisition of a dexterous timing-based video game. Task improvement was indicated by higher gain scores, representing increased cumulative performance with better temporal accuracy and reduced error rate. Although both conditions demonstrated significant differences in performance across practice, the level of improvement with a-tDCS at B4 and B5 was significantly greater than that attained by SHAM (Fig 2). Likewise, considering only the change in performance from baseline to post-test, a-tDCS during practice resulted in a greater overall change in PI compared to SHAM stimulation (Fig 2).

While the a-tDCS group showed greater improvements than the SHAM group in their global PI score, this metric, alone, lacks detail about how execution of the SM task was changed to improve performance. Visual inspection of the general raw data trends (TA windows and KER) suggests that changes in both speed and accuracy elements of the task were factors driving and differentiating performance gains between the groups (Fig 3A–3C). Despite what appear to be disparities in baseline KER scores between groups (Fig 3C), these differences were non-significant (p = 0.149), resulted in only marginal (∼7 points) differences in the baseline PI scores between groups, and were largely attenuated by the end of the initial practice block. Since both groups demonstrated rapid initial KER changes with similar trends across practice and post-test (Fig 3C), it can be inferred that both groups acquired the sequence order similarly. Additionally, both a-tDCS and SHAM groups had more taps in the best TA window with a concurrent decrease in the worst window (Fig 3B). However, by practice B5 and the post-test, there is a clear TA advantage favoring a-tDCS with 10.9 and 13.5 more taps on average than SHAM in the best two timing windows combined and 3.1 and 2.3 fewer misses in B5 and the post-test, respectively (Fig 3A). These results suggest that the greater performance improvements within the a-tDCS group may have been attributed to the enhanced “chunking” of Step Mania subsequences,

Fig 3. Change in subject performance characteristics.

Fig 3

Data illustrate changes in the concentration of taps in the timing windows across all trials (A, B) and the key error rates (C) for both conditions. The color gradient table (A) shows the difference between the average a-tDCS tap distribution and average SHAM tap distribution (a-tDCS total–SHAM total) for each trial. Darker red tones signify differences favoring a-tDCS (positive values) and darker grey tones signify differences favoring SHAM (more negative). The stacked bar graph (B) shows the total number of taps in the best (“Flawless”, 0 to ± 0.0225s) and worst (“Miss”, > ± 0.180s) timing windows across trials. The total height of the bars represents the combined total number of taps falling within those two windows. The dark bottom portions of the bars indicate the proportion of flawless taps and the lighter top portions represent misses. The line graph (C) shows the KER trend from baseline, across practice to the post-test. Whiskers denote standard error.

The chunk formation process can be thought of as a transition from high uncertainty to low uncertainty in the execution of a motor skill that leads to sequence accuracy increases as the transition occurs [45]. In early stages of sequence learning with internally cued movements, efficient learning appears to prioritize learning of spatial elements and then shift to temporal components [45]. Previous work has demonstrated that M1 tDCS can accelerate chunk formation in early motor learning within the first training session [46]. Our results support this pattern for externally cued sequences as well, as evident by the quick initial improvements in KER and the similar learning curve in both groups (Fig 3C). Whether timing-based performance differences resulted from enhanced encoding of temporal sequence elements across the motor network or enhanced local M1 circuitry (enlarged representation or improved activity pattern), the behavioral output that increased PI scores resulted from M1 a-tDCS, which supports the important role of M1 for fast motor learning and as a sequence learning substrate [47].

It is also possible that tDCS may have augmented Step Mania skill acquisition by improving the transitions between taps through the enhancement of movement coarticulations. Coarticulation is the tendency for one element of a movement sequence to be generated in a manner which facilitates preceding or subsequent elements [48]. In our study, this was observed in subjects who appeared to develop anticipatory finger motions which allowed them to prepare for subsequent key presses. It is therefore plausible that, through enhanced chunking of movement sequences, M1 tDCS accelerated the emergence of specific coarticulations which facilitated more rapid and accurate transitions between taps.

Compared with SHAM, improved task component chunking and enhanced movement coarticulation in the M1 tDCS group appears to have resulted in superior relative timing of SM task performance. This is important because relative timing, appears to be critically important in motor skill learning and performance in rhythm based tasks [49,50]. Apolinário-Souza and colleagues (2016) previously demonstrated that M1 tDCS only enhances absolute timing [51], however, taken in the context of our current findings, we believe that M1 tDCS effects on relative timing may be dependent on task complexity. Apolinário-Souza and colleagues observed contextual changes during a single-finger transfer task, while our study analyzed the effects on learning under the practiced conditions with a comparatively complex multi-finger task. This is a particularly relevant distinction because it has been previously shown that complex movements may be more susceptible to tDCS accelerated learning than simpler tasks [52]. Overall, our results reinforce the idea that M1 is an important neural substrate for motor sequence learning, especially when the quality or outcome success of the skill relies directly upon optimized relative timing (rhythm) between the component movements (playing a musical instrument or shooting a jump shot), rather than the fastest possible performance of correctly ordered elements (typing on a keyboard or a serial reaction timing task).

Our results align with previous findings that indicate M1 a-tDCS is a beneficial tool to enhance acquisition and performance of dexterous motor skills [21,34,53]. However, most of the previous evidence is based on simple laboratory tasks that do well isolating and measuring specific learning processes, but lack the variety of parameters many real-world motor skills involve. Step Mania is a complex rhythmic timing video game that provides ample explicit feedback. Even though M1 is important in skill acquisition, the effects of tDCS on motor learning are known to be task-dependent [34,35] and when several areas outside M1 influence task performance, it has been suggested that M1 modulation alone may be insufficient to enhance motor learning [21]. Within this context, our finding that M1 was a viable target for tDCS to enhance acquisition and performance of a novel complex task is particularly interesting.

To our knowledge this is only the second study demonstrating M1 tDCS can enhance acquisition of video gaming motor skill [27], and the first specifically within the rhythm gaming genre. Video gaming inputs (i.e. controllers, keyboard and mouse) require fine, dexterous hand movements and gross arm motor skills that, when combined with the complex visual processing and attentional demands of video games, may provide greater generalizability than traditional paradigms for studying motor learning [27]. Additionally, tDCS has reportedly begun to see commercial use (i.e. foc.us or Halo devices) as a performance enhancing tool in the lucrative market of competitive gaming and eSports [33]; however, the sensory, cognitive, and motor demands between different games can vary drastically and the ideal stimulation parameters for these different gaming demands are unclear. Though our results support and extend the evidence that M1 tDCS can improve gaming motor skills, it is not clear whether our chosen stimulation parameters are optimal or generalizable to other types of video games. Furthermore, our study only investigated the effects of a single bout of M1 tDCS on videogame performance in the non-dominant hand while most videogames involve some degree of bimanual control and regular, repeated practice. Since bimanual motor tasks are generally more complex and involve broader, network-wide activity than unimanual tasks [5456], and since the effects of tDCS stimulation appear differ between hemispheres [5759], it challenging to say whether or not our chosen, stimulation parameters would translate to a more complex, bimanual videogame task. Finally, while our results may also provide further indication that M1 tDCS may be able to enhance rehabilitation motor outcomes, our research was conducted only on a healthy population and further research, particularly in conjunction with videogame-based rehabilitation interventions should be conducted [60].

Conclusion

tDCS shows promise as a motor learning adjuvant in many settings, but due to the task specific nature of tDCS effects on simplified laboratory tasks, there is little consensus on ideal stimulation targets to enhance motor learning for more complex real-world tasks. We have demonstrated that, compared to SHAM stimulation, M1 a-tDCS can significantly improve acquisition and performance of a complex dexterous video game skill within a single practice session. In contrast to simpler sequence learning tasks frequently used to study tDCS effects, Step Mania better represents real world skills where timing between component motions and processing incoming information relevant to task success are pivotal for learning and performance. In light of recent work highlighting that M1 might not be the primary site for plasticity that supports learning in traditional lab-based sequence tasks [61,62], and that lab-based tasks may not probe learning a skilled continuous sequential action with high fidelity [63], the observed effects in the present study provide interesting evidence that supports the potential of M1 a-tDCS for practical real-world applications.

Data Availability

All of our data files are available from the BioStudies database (accession number S-BSST1355.)

Funding Statement

The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.

References

  • 1.Dayan E, Cohen L. Neuroplasticity subserving motor skill learning. Neuron. 2011;72:443–54. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.10.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Karni A, Meyer G, Rey-Hipolito C, Jezzard P, Adams MM, Turner R, et al. The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1998;95(3):861–8. doi: 10.1073/pnas.95.3.861 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC33809. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Doyon J, Benali H. Reorganization and plasticity in the adult brain during learning of motor skills. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005;15:161–7. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Hikosaka O, Nakamura K, Sakai K, Nakahara H. Central mechanisms of motor skill learning. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2002;12:217–22. doi: 10.1016/s0959-4388(02)00307-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Lohse K, Wadden K, Boyd L, Hodges N. Motor skill acquisition across short and long time scales: A meta-analysis of neuroimaging data. Neuropsychologia. 2014;59:130–41. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2014.05.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Penhune V, Steele C. Parallel contribution os cerebellar, striatal and M1 mechanisms to motor sequence learning. Behav Brain Res. 2012;226:579–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Krakauer J, Mazzoni P. Human sensorimotor learning: adaptation, skill, and beyond. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2011;21:636–44. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2011.06.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ziemann U, Iliac T, Pauli C, Menintzschel F, Ruge D. Learning modifies subsequent induction of long-term potentiation-like and long-term depression-like plasticity in human motor cortex. J Neurosci. 2004;24(7):1666–72. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5016-03.2004 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Sanes JN, Donoghue JP. Plasticity and primary motor cortex. Annu Rev Neurosci. 2000;23:393–415. Epub 2000/06/09. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.393 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Tyc F, Boyadjian A. Plasticity of motor cortex induced by coordination and training. Clin Neurophysiol. 2011;122:153–62. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2010.05.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Classen J, Liepert J, Wise SP, Hallett M, Cohen LG. Rapid plasticity of human cortical movement representation induced by practice. Journal of neurophysiology. 1998;79(2):1117–23. Epub 1998/04/18. doi: 10.1152/jn.1998.79.2.1117 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Meek A, Perez J, Poston B, Riley Z. Cortical representation and excitabilty increases for a thenar muscle mediate improvement in short-term cellular phone text messaging ability. Brain Sci. 2021;11(3):406. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11030406 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Monfils M, Plautz E, Kleim J. In search of the motor engram: motor map plasticity as a mechanisms for encoding motor experience. Neuroscientist. 2005;11:471–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Tyc F, Boyadjian A, Devanne H. Motor cortex plasticity induced by extensive training revealed by transcranial magnetic stimulation in human. Eur J Neurosci. 2005;21:259–66. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2004.03835.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Nudo R, Milliken G, Jenkins W, Merzenich M. Use-dependent alterations of movement representations in primary motor cortex of adult squirrel monkeys. J Neurosci. 1996;16:785–807. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.16-02-00785.1996 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Pearce A, Thickbroom G, Byrnes M, Mastaglia F. Functional reorganization of the corticomotor projection to the hand in skilled racquet players. Exp Brain Res. 2000;130:238–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Paulus W. Transcranial electrical stimulation (tES ‐ tDCS; tRNS, tACS) methods. Neuropsychol Rehabil. 2011;21(5):602–17. Epub 20110805. doi: 10.1080/09602011.2011.557292 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Sadleir RJ, Vannorsdall TD, Schretlen DJ, Gordon B. Transcranial direct current stimulation (tDCS) in a realistic head model. Neuroimage. 2010;51(4):1310–8. Epub 20100327. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.03.052 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Lang N, Nitsche MA, Paulus W, Rothwell J, Lemon R. Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation over the human motor cortex on corticospinal and transcallosal excitability. Exp Brain Res. 2004;156:439–43. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1800-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Kidgell D, Goodwill A, Frazer A, Daly R. Induction of cortical plasticity and improved motor performance following unilateral and bilateral transcranial direct current stimulation. BMC Neurosci. 2013;14:64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Patel R, Ashvroft J, Patel A, Ashrafian H, Woods A, Singh H, et al. The impact of transcranial direct current stimulation on upper-limb motor performance in healty adults. Front Neurosci. 2019;13:1213. 10.3389/fnins.2019.01213. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Buch E, Santarnecchi E, Antal A, Born J, Celnik P, Classen J, et al. Effects of tDCS on motor learning and memory formation: a consensus and critical position paper. Clin Neurophysiol. 2017;128:589–603. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2017.01.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Reis J, Fritsch B. Modulation of motor performance and motor learning by transcranial direct current stimulation. Curr Opin Neurol. 2011;24(6):590–6. doi: 10.1097/WCO.0b013e32834c3db0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bastani A, Jaberzadeh S. Does anodal transcranial direct current stimulation enhance excitability of the motor cortex and motor functions in healthy individuals and subjects with stroke: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Neurophysiol. 2012;2012(123). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Floel A. tDCS-enhanced motor and cognitive function in neurological diseases Neuroimage. 2014;85:934–47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ammann C, Spampinato D, Ruiz J. Modulating motor learning through transcranial direct-current stimulation: an integrative view. Front Psychol. 2016;7(1981):1–15. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01981 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Toth A, Ramsbottom N, Constantin C, Milliet A, Campbell M. The effect of expertise, training, and neurostimulation on sensory-motor skill in esports. Compt Human Behav. 2021;121. 10.1016/j.chb.2021.106782. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Hung C, Zeng B, Zeng B, Sun C, Cheng Y, Su K, et al. The efficacy of transcranial direct current stimulation in enhancing surgical skill acquisition: A preliminary meta-analysis of randomized controlled trials. Brain Sci. 2021;11(6):707. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11060707 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Wilson M, Greenwell D, Meek A, Poston B, Riley Z. Neuroenhancement of a dextrous motor task with anodal tDCS. Brain Res. 2022;1790:147993. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Meek A, Greenwell D, Poston B, Riley Z. Anodal tDCS accelerates on-line learning of dart throwing. Neurosci Lett. 2021;764:136211. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2021.136211 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Bediou B, Adams D, Mayer R, Tipton E, Green C, Bavelier D. Meta-analysis of action video game impact on perceptual, attentional, and cognitive skills. Psychol Bull. 2018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Achtman R, Green C, Bavelier D. Video games as a tool to train visual skills. Restor Neurol Neurosci. 2008;26(4–5):435–46. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Burstyner N, Carter A, Farrell A. Brain stimulation is getting popular with gamers ‐ is it time to regulate it? 2016. Available from: https://theconversation.com/brain-stimulation-is-getting-popular-with-gamers-is-it-time-to-regulate-it-66845. [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Saucedo Marquez C, Zhang X, Swinnen S, Meesen R, Wenderoth N. Task-specific effect of transcranial direct current stimulation on motor learning. Front Hum Neurosci. 2013;7:333. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2013.00333 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Karok S, Fletcher D, Witney A. Task-specificity of unilateral anodal and dual-M1 tDCS effects on motor learning. Neurospsychologia. 2017;8(94):84–95. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2016.12.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory. Neuropsychologia. 1971;9(1):97–113. Epub 1971/03/01. doi: 10.1016/0028-3932(71)90067-4 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Wilson MA, Greenwell D, Meek AW, Poston B, Riley ZA. Neuroenhancement of a dexterous motor task with anodal tDCS. Brain Res. 2022;1790:147993. Epub 20220626. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2022.147993 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Meek AW, Greenwell D, Poston B, Riley ZA. Anodal tDCS accelerates on-line learning of dart throwing. Neurosci Lett. 2021;764:136211. Epub 20210902. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2021.136211 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Rawji V, Ciocca M, Zacharia A, Soares D, Truong D, Bikson M, et al. tDCS changes in motor excitability are specific to orientation of current flow. Brain Stimul. 2018;11(2):289–98. Epub 20171107. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.11.001 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5805821. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Esmaeilpour Z, Marangolo P, Hampstead BM, Bestmann S, Galletta E, Knotkova H, et al. Incomplete evidence that increasing current intensity of tDCS boosts outcomes. Brain Stimul. 2018;11(2):310–21. Epub 20171213. doi: 10.1016/j.brs.2017.12.002 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7050474. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Batsikadze G, Moliadze V, Paulus W, Kuo MF, Nitsche MA. Partially non-linear stimulation intensity-dependent effects of direct current stimulation on motor cortex excitability in humans. J Physiol. 2013;591(7):1987–2000. Epub 20130121. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.249730 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3624864. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Glass GV, Peckham PD, Sanders JR. Consequences of failure to meet assumptions underlying fixed effects analyses of variance and covariance. Rev Educ Res. 1972;42:237–88. [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Harwell M, Rubinstein E, Hayes W, Olds C. Summarizing Monte Carlo results in methodological research: the one- and two-factor fixed effects ANOVA cases. J Educ Stat. 1992;17:315–39. [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Lix L, Keselman J, Keselman H. Consequences of assumption violations revisited: A quantitative review of alternatives to the one-way analysis of variance F test. Rev Educ Res. 1996;66:579–619. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Maceira-Elvira P, Timmermann J, Popa R, Schmid A, Krakauer J, Morishita T, et al. Dissecting motor skill acquisition: Spatial coordinates take precedence. Sci Adv. 2022;8:eabo 3505. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.abo3505 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Greeley B, Barnhoorn J, Verwey W, Seidler R. Multi-session transcranial direct current stimulation over primary motor cortex facilitates sequence learning, chunking, and one year retention. Front Hum Neurosci. 2020;14(75). doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2020.00075 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Hamano Y, Sugawara S, Fukunaga M, Sadato N. The integrative role of the M1 in motor sequence learning. Neurosci Lett. 2021;760:136081. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2021.136081 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Winges S, Furuya S, Faber N, Flanders M. Patterns of muscle activity for digital coarticulation. Journal of neurophysiology. 2013;110:230–42. doi: 10.1152/jn.00973.2012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Wang L. Relative Timing and Rhythm: A Key to Motor Skill Learning. The Shield: Research Journal of Physical Education & Sports sciences. 2010;5:120–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Shin JC, Ivry RB. Concurrent Learning of Temporal and Spatial Sequences. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn. 2002;28(3):445–57. doi: 10.1037//0278-7393.28.3.445 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Apolinário-Souza T, Romano-Silva MA, de Miranda DM, Malloy-Diniz LF, Benda RN, Ugrinowitsch H, et al. The primary motor cortex is associated with learning the absolute, but not relative, timing dimension of a task: A tDCS study. Physiol Behav. 2016;160:18–25. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.03.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Seidel O, Ragert P. Effects of transcranial direct current stimulation of primary motor cortex on reaction time and tapping performance: A comparison between athletes and non-athletes. Front Hum Neurosci. 2019;13:103. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00103 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Nitsche MA, Schauenburg A, Lang N, Liebetanz D, Exner C, Paulus W, et al. Facilitation of implicit motor learning by weak transcranial direct current stimulation of the primary motor cortex in the human. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience. 2003;15(4):619–26. doi: 10.1162/089892903321662994 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Walsh RR, Small SL, Chen EE, Solodkin A. Network activation during bimanual movements in humans. Neuroimage. 2008;43(3):540–53. Epub 20080722. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2008.07.019 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2655207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Koeneke S, Lutz K, Wüstenberg T, Jäncke L. Bimanual versus unimanual coordination: what makes the difference? NeuroImage. 2004;22(3):1336–50. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2004.03.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Guiard Y. Asymmetric division of labor in human skilled bimanual action: the kinematic chain as a model. J Mot Behav. 1987;19(4):486–517. doi: 10.1080/00222895.1987.10735426 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Vines BW, Nair D, Schlaug G. Modulating activity in the motor cortex affects performance for the two hands differently depending upon which hemisphere is stimulated. European Journal of Neuroscience. 2008;28(8):1667–73. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.2008.06459.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Vines BW, Cerruti C, Schlaug G. Dual-hemisphere tDCS facilitates greater improvements for healthy subjects’ non-dominant hand compared to uni-hemisphere stimulation. BMC Neurosci. 2008;9:103. Epub 20081028. doi: 10.1186/1471-2202-9-103 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2584652. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Vines BW, Nair DG, Schlaug G. Contralateral and ipsilateral motor effects after transcranial direct current stimulation. Neuroreport. 2006;17(6):671–4. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200604240-00023 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Kim J, Lee M, Yim J. Approach to transcranial direct current stimulation in improving cognitive motor learning adn hand function with the nintendo switch in stroke survivors. Med Sci Monit. 2019;25:9555–62. doi: 10.12659/MSM.921081 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Berlot N, Popp J, Diedrichsen J. A critical re-evaluation of fMRI signatures of motor sequence learning. Elife. 2020;9. doi: 10.7554/eLife.55241 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Yokoi A, Arbuckle S, Diedrichsen J. The role of human primary motor cortex in the production of skilled finger seqeunces. J Neurosci. 2018;38(6):1430–42. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Wong A, Krakauer J. Why are sequence representations in primary motor cortex so elusive. Neuron. 2019;103:956–8. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.09.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Sandra Carvalho

14 Feb 2024

PONE-D-23-37863Anodal M1 tDCS enhances online learning of rhythmic timing videogame skillPLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Greenwell,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process. 

Please submit your revised manuscript by Mar 30 2024 11:59PM. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at plosone@plos.org. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Sandra Carvalho, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

1. When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and 

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. For studies involving third-party data, we encourage authors to share any data specific to their analyses that they can legally distribute. PLOS recognizes, however, that authors may be using third-party data they do not have the rights to share. When third-party data cannot be publicly shared, authors must provide all information necessary for interested researchers to apply to gain access to the data. (https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-acceptable-data-access-restrictions

For any third-party data that the authors cannot legally distribute, they should include the following information in their Data Availability Statement upon submission:

a) A description of the data set and the third-party source

b) If applicable, verification of permission to use the data set

c) Confirmation of whether the authors received any special privileges in accessing the data that other researchers would not have

d) All necessary contact information others would need to apply to gain access to the data

3. Please amend your authorship list in your manuscript file to include author Dr. Hayami Nishio.

4. Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

[Note: HTML markup is below. Please do not edit.]

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The present study sought to evaluate the impact of M1 tDCS on a single session motor practice. The authors were very successful in choosing the research problem, study design, method and articulation of results with the literature. The work is clear, objective, detailed and as a reader, there are no doubts about the theoretical basis, method or how the data were analyzed. The work clearly makes a contribution to the literature, its reading is fluid and the reader finds it easy to reproduce its detailed method. I believe that this work should be accepted by the journal, since it will contribute significantly to the field, both in clinical practice and research in neuromodulation. Below I send 5 comments to improve the work.

Major comments

1) The authors clearly describe the literature that supports the objectives, but do not present the hypotheses in order to verify whether the results are compatible or not with the hypotheses. I suggest describing in detail and referencing the literature, the hypotheses raised a priori and relating them to the analyzes described;

2) The authors describe that they chose a sample of 58 participants, but do not describe how they arrived at this value. Furthermore, it is not clear whether there was any sample loss or even the exclusion of any participant for any reason (such as an outlier);

3) The characterization of the collected sample was not presented, for example in a table;

4) The authors do not describe the limitations of the study;

5) Although the authors have provided an excellent discussion of the results, the length of this topic is not compatible with the other topics in the work. I suggest reviewing the discussion completely, seeking to synthesize it significantly, in order to make it more objective and direct.

Reviewer #2: I have thoroughly reviewed the manuscript titled "Anodal M1 tDCS enhances online learning of rhythmic timing videogame skill" and found the study to be a valuable contribution to our understanding of motor learning, particularly within the context of complex real-world tasks. The manuscript is well-written and provides significant insights into the potential of M1 anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (a-tDCS) to enhance motor learning, especially in the domain of complex real-world tasks, such as the Step Mania video game. However, to strengthen the manuscript, I recommend considering the following points for incorporation.

Introduction:

I suggest refining the introduction to lead the reader more effectively to understand the formulation of the research question.

For instance, the authors referenced lab-based tasks on page 4, yet I missed a discussion on the other studies conducted with more complex tasks to offer additional context, since they have only mentioned an increasing number of tasks expanding complexity but not explored these studies.

It would also be valid to ensure clarity in articulating the specific study's objectives and specify hypotheses related to anticipated changes in skill learning resulting from M1 a-tDCS application.

Finally, focus the introduction more explicitly on the study’s objective. For instance, information about multiple sections (page 3, line 1-2) may be considered less pertinent than the other points I mentioned and could be streamline for better alignment with the study’s main focus.

Methods:

The method is well-written, with detailed and easily-followed methodology. I appreciate the care taken in creating a score and reporting it in such a meticulous manner. However, I would like to offer some suggestions:

Please include information on whether a sample size calculation was conducted.

Could the authors provide clarification on whether the study was retrospective? Additionally, it would be valuable to know if the data analyzed in this study has also been considered in another publication.

It would be appreciated if the authors could provide details on the randomization procedure and specify if participation was voluntary. Consider following guidelines for reporting your experiment.

Could the authors enhance the resolution of Figure 1?

An explanation of the rationale behind choosing current intensity and stimulation duration would be helpful.

Could the authors provide clarification on whether participants' naivety to rhythm games served as an control variable in the study? If not, I think it would be beneficial to include a comment on this aspect in the discussions. However, in the discussion (pag 23), it is mentioned that participants were naive to rhythm games – was this an exclusion criterion then? If so, it would be helpful to add this information to the description of exclusion criteria.

Results:

Consider incorporating the results of the post-test into Figure 2 to provide an overarching view of the experiment and the longevity of effects post-training.

Discussion:

The discussion section of the paper addresses various crucial points regarding the outcomes derived from the application of M1 a-tDCS in acquiring motor skills within a timing-based video game. Here are some suggestions to further elaborate on the discussion:

Elaborate on the possibility that the a-tDCS group started with more errors in baseline in Figure 4C.

Discuss additional limitations and suggest areas for future research.

Given that the study's sample comprises a healthy population, consider rephrasing the last sentence of the discussion (page 24, l. 14-16) to emphasize the need for additional research within a rehabilitation context.

Minor comments:

Italicize the p-values

Spell out PM and SMA before using the acronyms (pag 22)

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Lucas Murrins Marques

Reviewer #2: No

**********

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at figures@plos.org. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: plosone02024.pdf

pone.0295373.s001.pdf (119KB, pdf)
PLoS One. 2024 Jun 13;19(6):e0295373. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0295373.r002

Author response to Decision Letter 0


8 Mar 2024

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

________________________________________

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

________________________________________

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: No

________________________________________

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

________________________________________

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: The present study sought to evaluate the impact of M1 tDCS on a single session motor practice. The authors were very successful in choosing the research problem, study design, method and articulation of results with the literature. The work is clear, objective, detailed and as a reader, there are no doubts about the theoretical basis, method or how the data were analyzed. The work clearly makes a contribution to the literature, its reading is fluid and the reader finds it easy to reproduce its detailed method. I believe that this work should be accepted by the journal, since it will contribute significantly to the field, both in clinical practice and research in neuromodulation. Below I send 5 comments to improve the work.

Major comments

1) The authors clearly describe the literature that supports the objectives, but do not present the hypotheses in order to verify whether the results are compatible or not with the hypotheses. I suggest describing in detail and referencing the literature, the hypotheses raised a priori and relating them to the analyzes described;

Thank you for catching this. This was an oversight on our part. We hypothesized that, compared to SHAM, a-tDCS would enhance motor task learning. This was based largely on our previous work which demonstrated that M1 targeted a-tDCS enhanced motor learning in tasks performed with the non-dominant arm (A. Meek et al., 2021; M. Wilson et al., 2022). However, at the time of conducting this study, to our knowledge, no previous literature had explored the effects of a-tDCS in a comparable videogame task. Therefore, the main purpose of our investigation was exploratory.

2) The authors describe that they chose a sample of 58 participants, but do not describe how they arrived at this value. Furthermore, it is not clear whether there was any sample loss or even the exclusion of any participant for any reason (such as an outlier);

This is a good point that we seem to have overlooked in our manuscript. While we recognize the importance of performing initial sample size calculations, our current project was largely exploratory, and we did not have any comparable literature from which to estimate a range for task performance or the effect sizes of stimulation.

Therefore, we estimated a sample size based on our two previous studies which showed significant effects of a-tDCS on motor task performance in samples of 40 subjects in a non-dominant hand tweezer pegboard task and of 58 subjects in a non-dominant hand, dart throwing task. Notably, between the dart and tweezer task, the dart task was characterized by more degrees of freedom and, in turn, more task performance variability. Since the Step Mania task involved less upper arm movement and fewer degrees of freedom than the dart task but more cognitive and technical complexity than the tweezer task, we determined that an appropriate recruitment target would likely lay somewhere between the 40 and 58 subjects. Still, because we were unsure, we opted to recruit 60 with the hopes that this number would provide us with sufficient statistical power for our analyses. One subject demonstrated extreme outliers in performance scores and due to a hardware error, another subject’s data was partially lost. As a result, a total of 2 subjects were excluded from analysis.

3) The characterization of the collected sample was not presented, for example in a table;

Table 1: Subject Characterization

Condition Male Female

Handedness

a-tDCS 14 15

Right 12 14

Left 2 1

SHAM 15 14

Right 11 13

Left 4 1

a-tDCS = Anodal transcranial direct-current stimulation

SHAM = Sham transcranial direct-current stimulation

4) The authors do not describe the limitations of the study;

Thank you for pointing this out. This was an oversight on our part. We have revised the final paragraph of the discussion to include a better summary of the study’s limitations.

5) Although the authors have provided an excellent discussion of the results, the length of this topic is not compatible with the other topics in the work. I suggest reviewing the discussion completely, seeking to synthesize it significantly, in order to make it more objective and direct.

Thank you again for your insight. We have completely revised and synthesized the discussion the hopes that is more direct and to the point. We agree that there were several components that, while maybe tangentially related, were not directly relevant to the discussion at hand.

Reviewer #2: I have thoroughly reviewed the manuscript titled "Anodal M1 tDCS enhances online learning of rhythmic timing videogame skill" and found the study to be a valuable contribution to our understanding of motor learning, particularly within the context of complex real-world tasks. The manuscript is well-written and provides significant insights into the potential of M1 anodal transcranial direct current stimulation (a-tDCS) to enhance motor learning, especially in the domain of complex real-world tasks, such as the Step Mania video game. However, to strengthen the manuscript, I recommend considering the following points for incorporation.

Introduction:

I suggest refining the introduction to lead the reader more effectively to understand the formulation of the research question.

For instance, the authors referenced lab-based tasks on page 4, yet I missed a discussion on the other studies conducted with more complex tasks to offer additional context, since they have only mentioned an increasing number of tasks expanding complexity but not explored these studies.

It would also be valid to ensure clarity in articulating the specific study's objectives and specify hypotheses related to anticipated changes in skill learning resulting from M1 a-tDCS application.

Finally, focus the introduction more explicitly on the study’s objective. For instance, information about multiple sections (page 3, line 1-2) may be considered less pertinent than the other points I mentioned and could be streamline for better alignment with the study’s main focus.

Thank you for your suggestions. We have revised the introduction accordingly and believe that it now aligns better with the study’s main focus. We have attempted to remove less pertinent information and included additional details about the more complex task with which tDCS has been used to modulate motor learning. We have also added details about our specific objectives and our central hypothesis. It is our hope that these changes will provide the reader with a better context for our research and help make clear the purpose of our research.

Methods:

The method is well-written, with detailed and easily-followed methodology. I appreciate the care taken in creating a score and reporting it in such a meticulous manner. However, I would like to offer some suggestions:

Please include information on whether a sample size calculation was conducted.

While we recognize the importance of performing initial sample size calculations, our current project was largely exploratory, and we did not have any comparable literature from which to estimate a range for task performance or effect sizes of stimulation. Therefore, we estimated a sample size based on our two previous studies which showed significant effects of a-tDCS on motor task performance in samples of 40 subjects in a non-dominant hand tweezer pegboard task and of 58 subjects in a non-dominant hand, dart throwing task. Notably, between the dart and tweezer task, the dart task was characterized by more degrees of freedom and, in turn, more task performance variability. Since the Step Mania task involved less upper arm movement and fewer degrees of freedom than the dart task but more cognitive and technical complexity than the tweezer task, we determined that an appropriate recruitment target would likely lay somewhere between the 40 and 58 subjects. Still, because we were unsure, we opted to recruit 60 with the hopes that this number would provide us with sufficient statistical power for our analyses.

Could the authors provide clarification on whether the study was retrospective? Additionally, it would be valuable to know if the data analyzed in this study has also been considered in another publication.

This study was prospective, and the data has not been considered in another publication.

It would be appreciated if the authors could provide details on the randomization procedure and specify if participation was voluntary. Consider following guidelines for reporting your experiment.

Using MathWorks MATLAB software, a randomly generated number sequence was created and used during screening to assign subjects into either the anodal tDCS group (a-tDCS) or SHAM stimulation group (SHAM) based upon their order of recruitment.

Could the authors enhance the resolution of Figure 1?

Per your suggestion, we have resubmitted Figure 1 in higher resolution.

An explanation of the rationale behind choosing current intensity and stimulation duration would be helpful.

Added the following to the Methods section under the subheading, tDCS;

“These parameters were chosen because previous research, including work from our lab, has shown that 1mA of current delivered for 20 minutes is sufficient to elicit changes in cortical excitability and motor performance and that higher doses (longer durations and/or higher intensities) may result in diminishing or inverse effects (Batsikadze et al., 2013; Esmaeilpour et al., 2018; A. W. Meek et al., 2021; Rawji et al., 2018; M. A. Wilson et al., 2022).”

Could the authors provide clarification on whether participants' naivety to rhythm games served as a control variable in the study? If not, I think it would be beneficial to include a comment on this aspect in the discussions. However, in the discussion (pag 23), it is mentioned that participants were naive to rhythm games – was this an exclusion criterion then? If so, it would be helpful to add this information to the description of exclusion criteria.

I believe our phrasing was a bit confusing on this topic. Subjects were naïve to playing rhythm games with their non-dominant hand, not necessarily naïve to rhythm games entirely. All subjects reported having never played a unimanual rhythm game (like Step Mania) with their non-dominant hand. This point has been reworded for better clarity and to note that subjects’ prior exposure to these types games may have influenced their ability to learn with the non-dominant hand.

Results:

Consider incorporating the results of the post-test into Figure 2 to provide an overarching view of the experiment and the longevity of effects post-training.

Figures 2 and 3 have been combined so that the post-test results are displayed within the context of the rest of the experiment.

Discussion:

The discussion section of the paper addresses various crucial points regarding the outcomes derived from the application of M1 a-tDCS in acquiring motor skills within a timing-based video game. Here are some suggestions to further elaborate on the discussion:

Elaborate on the possibility that the a-tDCS group started with more errors in baseline in Figure 4C.

This is a good point. We acknowledge that this difference in KER appears significant, however, it doesn’t seem to have impacted our results. In an attempt to provide more clarity, we’ve added the following sentence to the discussion section addressing this point; “Despite what appear to be disparities in baseline KER scores between groups (Fig. 4C), these differences were non-significant (p = 0.149), resulted in only marginal (∼7 points) differences in the baseline PI scores between groups, and were largely attenuated by the end of the initial practice block.”

Discuss additional limitations and suggest areas for future research.

Given that the study's sample comprises a healthy population, consider rephrasing the last sentence of the discussion (page 24, l. 14-16) to emphasize the need for additional research within a rehabilitation context.

We have revised the final paragraph of the discussion to include a better summary of the study’s limitations. Additionally, a sentence about the need for research within a rehab context has been added.

Minor comments:

Italicize the p-values

Spell out PM and SMA before using the acronyms (pag 22)

Both of these have been addressed.

Attachment

Submitted filename: SM1 Response to Reviewer 2.docx

pone.0295373.s002.docx (27.6KB, docx)

Decision Letter 1

Sandra Carvalho

16 May 2024

Anodal M1 tDCS enhances online learning of rhythmic timing videogame skill

PONE-D-23-37863R1

Dear Dr. Greenwell,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice will be generated when your article is formally accepted. Please note, if your institution has a publishing partnership with PLOS and your article meets the relevant criteria, all or part of your publication costs will be covered. Please make sure your user information is up-to-date by logging into Editorial Manager at Editorial Manager® and clicking the ‘Update My Information' link at the top of the page. If you have any questions relating to publication charges, please contact our Author Billing department directly at authorbilling@plos.org.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

Kind regards,

Sandra Carvalho, Ph.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Acceptance letter

Sandra Carvalho

24 May 2024

PONE-D-23-37863R1

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Greenwell,

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now being handed over to our production team.

At this stage, our production department will prepare your paper for publication. This includes ensuring the following:

* All references, tables, and figures are properly cited

* All relevant supporting information is included in the manuscript submission,

* There are no issues that prevent the paper from being properly typeset

If revisions are needed, the production department will contact you directly to resolve them. If no revisions are needed, you will receive an email when the publication date has been set. At this time, we do not offer pre-publication proofs to authors during production of the accepted work. Please keep in mind that we are working through a large volume of accepted articles, so please give us a few weeks to review your paper and let you know the next and final steps.

Lastly, if your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact onepress@plos.org.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at customercare@plos.org.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Professor Sandra Carvalho

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Attachment

    Submitted filename: plosone02024.pdf

    pone.0295373.s001.pdf (119KB, pdf)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: SM1 Response to Reviewer 2.docx

    pone.0295373.s002.docx (27.6KB, docx)

    Data Availability Statement

    All of our data files are available from the BioStudies database (accession number S-BSST1355.)


    Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES