Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2024 Jun 14.
Published in final edited form as: Neurobiol Dis. 2023 May 29;183:106171. doi: 10.1016/j.nbd.2023.106171

Social and non-social working memory in neurodegeneration

Agustina Legaz a,b,c, Pavel Prado d,f, Sebastián Moguilner a,d,e,m, Sandra Báez g, Hernando Santamaría-García h, Agustina Birba a,j,k, Pablo Barttfeld i, Adolfo M García a,e,l,m, Sol Fittipaldi a,d,e,m,*, Agustín Ibañez a,b,d,e,m,*
PMCID: PMC11177282  NIHMSID: NIHMS1996710  PMID: 37257663

Abstract

Although social functioning relies on working memory, whether a social-specific mechanism exists remains unclear. This undermines the characterization of neurodegenerative conditions with both working memory and social deficits. We assessed working memory domain-specificity across behavioral, electrophysiological, and neuroimaging dimensions in 245 participants. A novel working memory task involving social and non-social stimuli with three load levels was assessed across controls and different neurodegenerative conditions with recognized impairments in: working memory and social cognition (behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia); general cognition (Alzheimer’s disease); and unspecific patterns (Parkinson’s disease). We also examined resting-state theta oscillations and functional connectivity correlates of working memory domain-specificity. Results in controls and all groups together evidenced increased working memory demands for social stimuli associated with frontocinguloparietal theta oscillations and salience network connectivity. Canonical frontal theta oscillations and executive-default mode network anticorrelation indexed non-social stimuli. Behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia presented generalized working memory deficits related to posterior theta oscillations, with social stimuli linked to salience network connectivity. In Alzheimer’s disease, generalized working memory impairments were related to temporoparietal theta oscillations, with non-social stimuli linked to the executive network. Parkinson’s disease showed spared working memory performance and canonical brain correlates. Findings support a social-specific working memory and related disease-selective pathophysiological mechanisms.

Keywords: Working memory, social processing, social working memory, behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, Parkinson’s disease

1. Introduction

Working memory (WM) plays a critical role in cognition and social functioning by allowing the maintenance and manipulation of information (Christophel et al., 2017; Porcelli et al., 2019) within load-dependent limits (Cowan, 2017; Oberauer et al., 2016). Yet, it is still unclear whether social stimuli involve domain-general or -specific WM processes. Although most traditional studies have targeted non-social stimuli (Chai et al., 2018; Meyer and Lieberman, 2012), relevant works suggest social stimuli increase WM load resembling a ‘social impairment’ effect (i.e., reduced WM performance linked to social processing in comparison to non-social cues) (Fairfield et al., 2015; Garrison and Schmeichel, 2019; Pessoa, 2009; Plancher et al., 2019). However, the field is not without controversy. Social stimuli maintenance and manipulation have been linked to canonical non-social WM hubs -i.e., the frontoparietal executive network (EN) (Smith et al., 2017; Thornton and Conway, 2013; Xin and Lei, 2015) suggesting a domain-general WM for social stimuli. Few works have related WM for social stimuli to a broader network beyond executive regions -i.e., social processing hubs including medial frontal, cingulate, and temporoparietal areas (Meyer and Collier, 2020; Meyer et al., 2012; Meyer et al., 2015), proposing a domain-specific WM subsystem [or social WM (Meyer and Lieberman, 2012)]. This scenario limits the characterization of clinical conditions with deficits in both WM and social processes. Neurodegenerative lesion models can partially overcome correlational evidence by exposing direct associations between behavioral performance and critical brain regions (Birba et al., 2022; Cruzat et al., 2023; Legaz et al., 2022; Moguilner et al., 2022; Rorden and Karnath, 2004; Salamone et al., 2021; Santamaría-García et al., 2022). To our knowledge, no previous work has targeted the WM domain-specificity for social vs non-social stimuli across neurodegenerative conditions with different WM and socio-cognitive impairments, such as behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD), Alzheimer’s disease (AD), and Parkinson’s disease (PD) (Piguet et al., 2011; Salmi et al., 2020) –let alone with a multidimensional approach indexing behavioral, electrophysiological (resting-state electroencephalography [rsEEG]) and neuroimaging (resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging [rsfMRI]) methods. Answering these questions is relevant for both cognitive and translational neuroscience.

The bvFTD presents non-social WM alterations (Poos et al., 2018) predominantly related to frontal (Nissim et al., 2017) and secondarily to temporoinsular atrophy (Baez et al., 2019; Migeot et al., 2022; Possin et al., 2013). However, WM dynamics for social stimuli remain unexamined in bvFTD. Electrophysiological WM correlates also remain unassessed, beyond general frontotemporal theta (θ) oscillatory disruption (Caso et al., 2012; Metin et al., 2018). Social processing deficits are pervasive in bvFTD (Dodich et al., 2021; Ibáñez and Manes, 2012; Kipps et al., 2009; O’Callaghan et al., 2016; Piguet et al., 2011) and have been linked to frontoinsular and temporoparietal atrophy (Baez et al., 2019; Kumfor et al., 2017), reduced fronto-posterior electrophysiological activity (Melloni et al., 2016), and salience network (SN) dysfunctions (Rijpma et al., 2022; Toller et al., 2018). Nonetheless, it is unknown whether a domain-general or -specific WM disruption exists in this condition with both WM and social processing deficits.

AD presents non-social WM impairments (Kirova et al., 2015) comparable to bvFTD (Leslie et al., 2016; Ramanan et al., 2017a), although linked to left temporoparietal dysfunctions (Kobylecki et al., 2018) and to reduced frontoparietal θ oscillations (Goodman et al., 2019; Hata et al., 2016). Indeed, altered WM in AD may be explained by global cognitive deficits (Possin et al., 2013) and related to EN dysfunctions (Agosta et al., 2012; Ibáñez et al., 2021a; Moguilner et al., 2021; Tait et al., 2020; Zhao et al., 2019). Yet, how social stimuli modulate WM in AD has not been assessed. Particularly, social processing impairments related to default mode network (DMN) dysfunctions (Badhwar et al., 2017; Saris et al., 2021) have also been linked to AD severity and global cognitive deficits (Dodich et al., 2016; Musa et al., 2020; Ramanan et al., 2017b; Synn et al., 2018). In sum, the extent of social specificity over an altered WM remains unexamined in this general cognitive deficit model.

Finally, although non-social WM deficits have been reported in PD (Ramos and Machado, 2021), dopaminergic-medicated PD patients tend to perform as well as healthy controls (HCs) (Moustafa et al., 2013; Salmi et al., 2020) with increased compensatory frontal activity despite corticostriatal disruptions (Simioni et al., 2017). In fact, the EN is spared in this disease (Hou et al., 2018). Again, no previous work has studied how social stimuli modulate WM in PD. Indeed, social processing disturbances are still inconclusive (Argaud et al., 2018; Lewis and Ricciardi, 2021; Maresca et al., 2020). Altogether, it is not clear whether WM is selectively impaired for social stimuli in this unspecified disease model.

Briefly, the notion of distinct WM mechanisms for social relative to non-social stimuli remains untested in bvFTD, AD, and PD -let alone combining behavioral and neurofunctional dimensions. To fill this gap, we employed a novel domain-specific WM task with social and non-social stimuli across these conditions. The task required participants to identify the sameness between separately presented lists of social (e.g., cordial) and non-social (e.g., oval) adjectives, organized in three load levels: three (triplets), four (quartets), or five (quintets) words. In addition, we acquired offline rsEEG modulations to capture θ oscillations (~4–8 Hz) given their systematic association to verbal WM load (Dai et al., 2017; Pavlov and Kotchoubey, 2020) and social processing (Billeke et al., 2013; Gregory et al., 2021). Finally, offline rsfMRI was also obtained to investigate functional connectivity correlates of WM domain-specificity [three networks linked to WM and social processes: the SN, the EN, and the DMN (Feng et al., 2021); and two control networks: the visual network (VN), and the motor network (MN)].

We propose distinct hypotheses for each group. In HCs, we predicted a behavioral WM load effect (triplets > quartets > quintets), with lower performance for social vs non-social stimuli (Fairfield et al., 2015; Garrison and Schmeichel, 2019). Increased WM for social stimuli should be linked to higher θ oscillations in extended frontocinguloparietal regions [related to social processing (Billeke et al., 2013; Feng et al., 2021; Gregory et al., 2021) and WM (Constantinidis and Klingberg, 2016; Dai et al., 2020)]; as well as higher SN connectivity (Luo et al., 2014; Rijpma et al., 2021). Conversely, WM for non-social stimuli should be associated with θ oscillations in canonical right frontal regions (Dai et al., 2017; Pavlov and Kotchoubey, 2020) as well as higher EN and lower DMN connectivity (Chai et al., 2018; Liang et al., 2016; Maehara, 2017). A WM load-dependent enhancement of θ oscillations should be observed irrespective of stimulus type (Dai et al., 2017; Pavlov and Kotchoubey, 2020). Compared to HCs, each patient group should present distinct WM patterns. In bvFTD, we expected increased WM deficits for social vs non-social stimuli, linked to θ oscillations in fronto-posterior hubs, and to SN connectivity. In AD, we hypothesized WM deficits in both social and non-social stimuli, predominantly associated to temporoparietal θ oscillations and EN connectivity. In PD, we predicted preserved WM, associated with frontal θ oscillations. A behavioral WM load effect was also expected in all neurodegenerative groups. By testing these hypotheses, we aim to provide multimodal evidence of social specificity in WM across neurodegenerative models.

2. Methods

2.1. Participants

The study was coordinated by BrainLat (Duran-Aniotz et al., 2022) and comprised 245 participants: 90 HCs with preserved cognition and no history of neuropsychiatric diseases and/or substance abuse; 42 people fulfilling revised criteria for bvFTD (Rascovsky et al., 2011); 54 people with AD, each meeting the international NINCDS-ADRDA criteria (Dubois et al., 2007; McKhann et al., 2011); and 59 people with PD diagnosed in accordance with the United Kingdom PD Society Brain Bank criteria (Hughes et al., 1992). Power analyses confirmed the adequacy of our sample size (Material S1.1). Participants were recruited from four international clinics taking part in the Multi-Partner Consortium to Expand Dementia Research in Latin America (ReDLat) and assessed following harmonized procedures (Ibáñez et al., 2021b; Ibáñez et al., 2021c; Maito et al., 2023; Moguilner et al., 2023) as in previous reports (Donnelly-Kehoe et al., 2019; Ibáñez et al., 2021a; Legaz et al., 2022; Melloni et al., 2016; Salamone et al., 2021; Sedeno et al., 2017). Clinical diagnoses were established by experts through an extensive neurological, neuropsychiatric, and neuropsychological examination comprising semi-structured interviews and standardized cognitive assessments (Table 1). Participants with neurodegenerative conditions were in early/mild stages of the disease. They did not fulfill criteria for other neurological, psychiatric and/or primary language disorders, or a history of substance abuse. As verified by caregivers, bvFTD and AD participants were functionally impaired, with bvFTD exhibiting prominent changes in personality and social behavior. PD participants were medicated with antiparkinsonian therapy (dopaminergic medication) and evaluated during the ‘ON’ phase. Each neurodegenerative sample was comparable in sex, age, and years of formal education with HCs (Table 1). Finally, whole-brain GM was compared between each neurodegenerative group and HCs, showing a predominantly orbitofrontal-cingulate-temporal atrophy in bvFTD (Ibáñez and Manes, 2012; Whitwell et al., 2009), bilateral temporal with less extended frontoparietal atrophy in AD (Du et al., 2007; Landin-Romero et al., 2017; Pini et al., 2016), and no atrophy in PD (Huber et al., 1989; Price et al., 2004; Schulz et al., 1999) (Fig. 1A; Table S1.2). The institutional ethics committee of each recruitment center approved the study protocol. All participants provided signed informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Table 1.

Samples’ demographic and neurocognitive data.

HCs (n = 90) bvFTD (n = 42) AD (n = 54) PD (n = 59) Stats Post-hoc comparisons

Demographics
Sex (M:F) 39:51 26:16 22:32 35:24 χ2 = 7.87, P = 0.048* HCs-bvFTD: P = 0.071; HCs-AD: P = 0.896; HCs-PD: P = 0.081
Age a 71.21 (6.82) 69.45 (9.67) 74.65 (5.82) 67.9 (9.01) F = 7.69, P < 0.001*, ηp2 = 0.08 HCs-bvFTD: P = 0.618; HCs-AD: P = 0.051; HCs-PD: P = 0.054
Education 13.4 (3.92) 13.98 (4.61) 11.7 (4.82) 11.8 (4.7) F = 3.61, P = 0.013*, ηp2 = 0.04 HCs-bvFTD: P = 0.899; HCs-AD: P = 0.120; HCs-PD: P = 0.138
Handedness (R:L) 85:1 35:1 50:1 56:2
Cognitive assessment
MoCA a 25.72 (3.15) 20.69 (4.94) 17.27 (4.38) 23.33 (4.44) F = 50.22, P < 0.001*, ηp2 = 0.39 HCs-bvFTD: P < 0.001*; HCs-AD: P < 0.001*; HCs-PD: P = 0.003*
IFS a 22.08 (3.69) 18.81 (5.26) 14.95 (4.98) 19.21 (4.85) F = 27.39, P < 0.001*, ηp2 = 0.26 HCs-bvFTD: P = 0.001*; HCs-AD: P < 0.001*; HCs-PD: P = 0.001*

Results are presented as mean (SD). The asterisk (*) indicates significant differences with an alpha level of P < 0.05.

a

indicates variables with significant differences (P < 0.05) between neurodegenerative groups, precluding comparisons between them in our target measures. Demographic and cognitive data were assessed through ANOVAs and Tukey post-hoc pairwise comparisons –except for sex, which was analyzed via Pearson’s chi-squared (χ2) test. Effects sizes were calculated through partial eta (ηp2). AD: Alzheimer’s disease, bvFTD: behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia, HCs: healthy controls, IFS: INECO Frontal Screening, MoCA: Montreal Cognitive Assessment, PD: Parkinson’s disease.

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1.

GM atrophy, task design and behavioral results. (A)GM atrophy in patients. GM integrity was assessed via voxel-based morphometry, based on w-score maps of the normalized and smoothed DARTEL outputs (Chung et al., 2017; Jack et al., 1997; La Joie et al., 2012; Ossenkoppele et al., 2015; van Loenhoud et al., 2017). We ran two sample t-tests between each neurodegenerative group and HCs using the statistical non-parametric mapping (SnPM13) toolbox for SPM12 (5000 random permutations, P<0.001 for cluster-forming threshold, and P<0.05 FWE-corrected for cluster-wise threshold (Kim et al., 2020; Salamone et al., 2021; Shih et al., 2019)). BvFTD showed orbitofrontal-cingulate-temporal atrophy. AD showed bilateral temporal with less extended frontoparietal atrophy. No atrophy was found in PD (Table S1.2). Results are presented on MNI space using the AAL(Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002), in neurological convention. (B) Task design. Participants judged if adjectives from a second list (testing phase) were the same as those from a first list (encoding phase) after a retention phase. Adjectives were either social or non-social (stimulus type) and randomly presented in three load levels: quintets, quartets or triplets (dark, medium and light gray, respectively). Note: Adjectives were displayed and validated in Spanish (Material S2). English translations are simply communicative renditions for the benefit of non-Spanish readers. (C)Behavioral results: between-group comparisons. We compared the WM performance of HCs and patient groups via mixed model ANOVA (group[4]*type[2]*load[3]) and post-hoc Tukey comparisons using the normalized inverse efficiency score (IES). Significant results were found for group-by-type (plotted) and group-by-load interactions (Table 2, Fig. S1, Fig. S2, Material S4.) Dot-plots represent results for HCs (dark blue), bvFTD (turquoise), AD (light blue), and PD (pink). Vertical-dotted lines show mean (black dot) and standard deviation (lines). The asterisk indicates significant differences (P<0.05). The between-groups mean difference (effect size) between each patient group and HCs is reported below each result. AD: Alzheimer’s disease, bvFTD: behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia, GM: grey matter, HCs: healthy controls, L: left, PD: Parkinson’s disease, R: right, WM: working memory.

2.2. Experimental protocol

Participants completed a multimodal assessment protocol including a behavioral WM task, and offline resting-state high-density EEG recordings and MRI-fMRI sessions.

2.2.1. Behavioral data: domain-specific WM task

Behavioral data was obtained through a domain-specific WM task (Fig. 1B). It consists of the sequential presentation of two lists of words. Participants are asked to judge whether the words in the second list are all the same as those from the first list (beyond word’s order) by pressing predefined keys. The task comprises two stimulus types: social, where words are adjectives that describe the interaction between two persons (e.g., cordial, friendly, prudent); and non-social, where words are adjectives that cannot be used to describe a person during a social interaction (e.g., oval, rocky, printed). Moreover, the task includes three load levels: social and non-social adjectives are organized in lists of three (triplets), four (quartets) and five (quintets) words. We manipulated the content of the stimuli (social/non-social) in order to compare their effect on WM, and the load (triplets, quartets, quintets) to confirm task understanding and control for potential cognitive confounds. The task was designed based on previous WM paradigms (Fiebach et al., 2006; Parra et al., 2010; Pietto et al., 2016; Reuter-Lorenz et al., 2000). Social and non-social adjectives were validated through online surveys distributed across Spanish speaking countries. We controlled that words between social and non-social trials were statistically paired for lexical parameters (log frequency, number of letters, number of syllables) and for Levenshtein distance. Conversely, they were statistically different for sociability. We also controlled that triplets, quartets and quintets were statistically paired for lexical parameters within stimulus type. Full details of task design and validation are provided in Material S2.

Each trial consists of three phases: (a) encoding, (b) retention and (c) testing. They initiate with a fixation cross (random duration between 200 – 500 ms). (a) Then stimuli are presented (first list of triplets, quartets, or quintets). The duration depends on the stimuli load: 3000 ms for triplets, 4000 ms for quartets, and 5000 ms for quintets (Morrison et al., 2016). (b) Immediately after, a black screen appears for 5000 ms. (c) Finally, a second list of words (same load as the first list) is shown until the subject’s response, with ‘No’/’Yes’ options positioned to the bottom left and right of the screen next to left/right arrows, respectively. Participants have to respond the sameness of the words lists by choosing ‘No’/’Yes’ through the corresponding computer keyboard arrows with their dominant hand. In the testing phase, half of the lists are the same as in the encoding phase, and half are different. For the same lists, only the word’s order is changed. For the different lists, one word is replaced by another with the same initial syllable/letter (similarity between old and new words did not differ between social and non-social conditions, as assessed by Levenshtein distance [minimum number of changes required to convert one word into the other] – see Table S2.1.3). Instructions and a set of six practice trials are presented before the task. The order of trials per stimulus type, load level, and equality between first and second list is randomly assigned. In total, participants complete 60 trials: 20 trials per load (10 social, 10 non-social). The number of trials does not change according to performance. Approximately, the total duration of the task is 15 minutes. Accuracy and response time (RT) data were collected for each trial.

2.2.2. EEG: acquisition and signal preprocessing

We acquired 10-minute, high-density offline rsEEG (eye-closed) recordings from a 91-participants subsample. Signals were recorded using a Biosemi ActiveTwo 128-channel acquisition system with pre-amplified sensors and a DC coupling amplifier. Reference electrodes were set to linked mastoids. Analog filters were set at 0.03 and 100 Hz. Signals were sampled at 1024 Hz. The subsample comprised 19 bvFTD, 27 AD, and 12 PD, each group being demographically matched with HCs (n = 33) (see Table S3.1). Participants were instructed to remain still and awake, while sitting in a comfortable chair, inside a dimly lit sound-attenuated and electromagnetically-shielded EEG chamber.

rsEEG signals were preprocessed offline using standard procedures in MATLAB’s EEGLAB toolbox (Delorme and Makeig, 2004). Recordings were band-pass filtered at 0.5–40 Hz, and re-referenced to the average of all channels. Malfunctioning channels were identified and replaced using statistically weighted spherical interpolation (based on neighbor sensors) (Courellis et al., 2016). Data was down-sampled to 512 Hz. Eye movements or blink artifacts were corrected with independent component analysis (Kim and Kim, 2012) and with a visual inspection protocol (Birba et al., 2021; Dirlich et al., 1997; Garcia-Cordero et al., 2017; Garcia-Cordero et al., 2016; Pollatos and Schandry, 2004; Salamone et al., 2021; Schandry and Montoya, 1996).

2.2.3. Neuroimaging: acquisition and preprocessing

MRI and rsfMRI acquisition and preprocessing steps are reported as recommended by the Organization for Human Brain Mapping (Nichols et al., 2017; Poldrack et al., 2017). Following standard protocols (Garcia-Cordero et al., 2016; Gonzalez Campo et al., 2019), we obtained offline three-dimensional volumetric and 10-minute rsfMRI sequences from a subsample of 165 participants. These comprised 19 bvFTD, 32 AD, and 48 PD, each group being demographically matched with HCs (n = 66) (see Table S3.2). Recordings were performed in different scanners (for harmonization details see Table S3.3). Participants were asked not to think about anything in particular, move or fall asleep. To avoid noisy signals coming from the visual cortex, we chose the closed-eyes modality (Zou et al., 2015).

First, to ensure that magnetization achieved a steady state, we discarded the first five volumes of each subject’s resting-state recording. Then, images were preprocessed using the Data Processing Assistant for Resting-State fMRI (DPARSF V2.3) (Chao-Gan and Yu-Feng, 2010) open-access toolbox in MATLAB, which generates an automatic pipeline for fMRI analysis by calling the Statistical Parametric Mapping software (SPM12) and the Resting-State fMRI Data Analysis Toolkit (REST V.1.7). As in previous studies (Salamone et al., 2021; Yoris et al., 2018), preprocessing steps included slice-timing correction (using middle slice of each volume as the reference scan) and realignment to the first scan of the session to correct head movement (SPM functions) (Barttfeld et al., 2012; Garcia-Cordero et al., 2016; Melloni et al., 2016; Sedeno et al., 2016). Then, images were normalized to the MNI space using the default echo-planar imaging template from SPM12 (Ashburner and Friston, 1999), smoothed using an 8-mm full-width-at-half-maximum isotropic Gaussian kernel, and bandpass filtered between 0.01–0.08 Hz to correct and remove low-frequency drifts from the scanner. Finally, we regressed out six motion parameters, cerebrospinal fluid, and white matter signals to reduce motion and physiological artifacts such as cardiac and respiration effects (REST V1.7). Motion parameters were estimated during realignment, and cerebrospinal fluid and white matter masks were derived from the tissue segmentation of each subject’s T1 scan in native space with SPM12 (after co-registration of each subject’s structural image with the functional image). Finally, we excluded recordings with movements greater than 3 mm and/or rotation higher than 3° (Table S3.4) (Supekar and Menon, 2012; Supekar et al., 2008).

2.3. Statistical analysis

2.3.1. Behavioral analysis: domain-specific WM task

First, to improve the responses’ signal-to-noise ratio and avoid random responses, we removed trials with RTs < 250 ms and then excluded those trials falling three Median Absolute Deviations (MADs) away from the median of each subjects’ RT. This approach has proven to be more robust against outliers’ effect compared to SDs from the mean (Dave and Varma, 2014; Yang et al., 2019). Next, to ensure non-significant differences in the final number of trials between conditions, we randomly selected and kept the same number of trials per stimulus type and load level, for each subject (for data curation details see Fig. S1, Table S4.1, and Table S4.2). Finally, to quantify WM performance, we used the inverse efficiency score (IES) (Hesse et al., 2016; Hesse et al., 2019), a standard metric that combines accuracy and RT to holistically establish weighted behavioral outcomes (Jacques and Rossion, 2007; Jacquet and Avenanti, 2015), previously applied in neurodegenerative studies (Salamone et al., 2021). The IES is calculated by dividing the mean RT by the proportion of correct responses (Brozzoli et al., 2008; Mevorach et al., 2006), thus controlling for biases introduced by fast RTs with low accuracy and vice versa. Therefore, the higher the IES, the poorer the performance. Average IES scores were calculated for each stimulus type (social, non-social) and load level (triplets, quartets, quintets), per subject. Given that Shapiro-Wilk’s tests revealed a non-normal distributions for IES indexes, and that analyses based on non-normalized data may promote Type I and Type II errors (Rasmussen, 1985), IES scores were normalized using Ordered Quantile Normalization transformation (Table S4.3, Table S4.4), previously applied in literature (Das et al., 2022; Minnier et al., 2021). Based on a rank mapping of the observed data to the normal distribution, this technique guarantees normally distributed transformed data if ties are not present (Peterson and Cavanaugh, 2019).

Second, we run mixed ANOVA models of the IES across stimulus type and load level (2 [type] * 3 [load]) for each group separately. These analyses confirmed the task validation regarding the expected ‘social impairment’ effect in HCs and the load effect at each group (for further details see Table S4.5 and Fig. S2).

Finally, since our main hypotheses hinged on differences between each patient group and HCs, statistical analyses of behavioral data were performed to compare group pairs of patients and controls (bvFTD vs HCs, AD vs HCs, PD vs HCs) as in previous reports with neurodegenerative conditions (Chiong et al., 2016; Garcia-Cordero et al., 2016; Garcia-Cordero et al., 2019; Legaz et al., 2022; Salamone et al., 2021; Shany-Ur et al., 2014; Shany-Ur et al., 2012; Sollberger et al., 2014; Sollberger et al., 2009). IES was first compared between groups via mixed model ANOVAs, for each stimulus type and load level (4 [group] * 2 [type] * 3 [load]). Then, specific WM patterns in each patient group relative to HCs were assessed through pairwise comparisons (bvFTD-HCs, AD-HCs, and PD-HCs) via Tukey’s HSD tests. This procedure accounts for multiple comparisons reducing the probability of Type I error (Nanda et al., 2021). Effect sizes were reported with partial eta squared (ηp2). Behavioral analyses were performed using BestNormalize (Peterson and Cavanaugh, 2019), lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al., 2017), afex (Singmann et al., 2015), and effectsize (Ben-Shachar et al., 2020) packages in R software (Version 4.0.2, R Foundation for Statistical Computing). Figures were generated using the Seaborn Python package (Version 0.9.0) (Waskom, 2021).

2.3.2. EEG: source localization analysis

After preprocessing, we conducted a source analysis of the rsEEG in the frequency domain using the standardized Low-Resolution Electromagnetic Tomography method [sLORETA (Grech et al., 2008; Pascual-Marqui, 2002)] to examine associations between behavioral WM outcomes and θ oscillations correlates related to WM (Dai et al., 2017; Pavlov and Kotchoubey, 2020) and social processing (Billeke et al., 2013; Gregory et al., 2021). First, we computed the EEG cross-spectrum at the sensor level from the discrete Fourier transforms obtained for each EEG channel, using a 2s-length window. Then, the cross-spectrum was used to calculate the standardized current density maps. These maps were obtained using a three-concentric-spheres head model, in a predefined source space of 6239 voxels (voxel size of 5 × 5 × 5 mm3) of the MNI average brain (Evans et al., 1993). A brain segmentation of 82 anatomic compartments (cortical areas) was implemented using the automated anatomical labeling (AAL) atlas (Tzourio-Mazoyer et al., 2002). Current densities maps of each participant were frequency-wise normalized. For each frequency, the spectral power computed in each voxel was divided by the mean spectral power (6239 voxels’ average) (for further details see Material S5.1).

Normalized current densities maps computed in the θ frequency band of the EEG were correlated with behavioral outcomes (IES) for both, stimulus type (social and non-social) and the most extreme load level (quintets [high] and triplets [low]) conditions, using Pearson correlation tests. To adjust for multiple comparisons and reduce the probability of Type I error, we used false discovery rate (FDR) rate (P < 0.05 FDR-corrected) (Benjamini and Yekutieli, 2001; Bennett et al., 2009; Brancaccio et al., 2020). To increase behavioral variance and statistical power, analyses collapsing all groups together (HCs, bvFTD, AD, and PD) were added to the individual group’s correlation analyses (Garcia-Cordero et al., 2016; O’Callaghan et al., 2016; Sollberger et al., 2009).

2.3.3. Neuroimaging: resting-state functional connectivity analysis

After preprocessing, we used seed analyses to examine associations between behavioral WM outcomes and dynamic functional connectivity across three core brain networks associated to social processing and WM (Feng et al., 2021): the SN, related to salient-social information processing (Porcelli et al., 2019; Toller et al., 2018; Uddin, 2015); the EN, implicated in externally goal-oriented executive processes including WM (Constantinidis and Klingberg, 2016; Menon and D’Esposito, 2022); and the DMN, that supports internally-related processes (Smallwood et al., 2021) and inversely correlates with the EN activation during high load WM processes (Liang et al., 2016). To test the specificity of our predictions for these networks, we also examined associations between WM and connectivity along two additional unrelated networks: the VN and the MN. To calculate each resting-state network connectivity, we located bilateral seeds on different MNI coordinates for each network (Koslov et al., 2011) (for further details see Material S5.2). Then we employed a weighted Symbolic Dependence Metric (wSDM) non-linear correlation coefficient across the whole time series obtained in the resting-state acquisition which proved robust in neurodegenerative conditions (Moguilner et al., 2018). After that, we used standard masks (Shirer et al., 2012) to isolate the voxels that are typically involved in each resting-state network. Finally, we spatially averaged across all included voxels to obtain one feature per network. Resulting connectivity maps were correlated with behavioral outcomes (IES) for both, stimulus type (social and non-social) and the most extreme load level (quintets [high] and triplets [low]) conditions, through the SPM12 multiple regression module. To adjust for multiple comparisons, we used cluster-wise inference with false discovery rate (FDR) rate correction (P≤0.05 FDR-corrected) (Han et al., 2019). In line with the EEG analysis pipeline, in addition to individual groups correlation analyses, we performed analyses collapsing all groups together (HCs, bvFTD, AD, and PD) to increase behavioral variance and statistical power (Garcia-Cordero et al., 2016; O’Callaghan et al., 2016; Sollberger et al., 2009).

2.4. Availability of data and materials

The datasets supporting the conclusions of this article are publicly available in the OSF repository, http://osf.io/bx27h (Legaz, 2022).

3. Results

3.1. Behavioral results

Our analysis revealed significant main effects for group (F 3,240 = 25.22, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.24) and load (F 2,480 = 408.61, P < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.63), but not for type (F 1,240 = 1.87, P = 0.17, ηp2 = 0.007). On the other hand, a significant group-by-type interaction effect was found. Compared to HCs, participants with bvFTD performed significantly worse in both social and non-social stimulus types. The same pattern was observed in participants with AD. No significant differences were found in PD’s performance relative to HCs in either stimulus type (Table 2, Fig. 1C). The group-by-load interaction effect was also significant. Participants with bvFTD performed worse than HCs in all load levels (triplets, quartets, quintets). Similar results were observed in AD. PD participants presented significantly lower performance than HCs only in triplets (Table 2, Fig. S2). Finally, no significant load-by-type (F2,480 = 0.18, P = 0.83, ηp2 = 0.0007), neither group-by-type-by-load interaction effects were observed (F 6,480 = 0.81, P = 0.56, ηp2 = 0.01).

Table 2.

Statistical comparison between group * load * stimulus type.

Group-by-type

Stimulus type HCs bvFTD AD PD Stats Post-hoc comparisons
Social − 0.31 (0.84) 0.23 (0.99) 0.52 (0.97) − 0.05 (0.93) F3,240 = 4.99, P = 0.002*, ηp2 = 0.06 HCs-bvFTD: P = 0.001* HCs-AD: P < 0.001* HCs-PD: P = 0.10
Nonsocial − 0.44 (0.92) 0.35 (0.98) 0.53 (0.96) − 0.19 (0.95) HCs-bvFTD: P < 0.001* HCs-AD: P < 0.001* HCs-PD: P = 0.23

Group-by-load

Load level HCs bvFTD AD PD Stats Post-hoc comparisons

Triplets − 1.04 (0.76) − 0.19 (1.04) 0.1 (1.07) − 0.59 (0.92) F6,480 = 7.28, P < 0.001*, ηp2 = 0.08 HCs-bvFTD: P < 0.001* HCs-AD: P < 0.001* HCs-PD: P = 0.01*
Quartets − 0.42 (0.67) 0.21 (0.92) 0.56 (0.89) − 0.26 (0.83) HCs-bvFTD: P < 0.001* HCs-AD: P < 0.001* HCs-PD: P = 0.69
Quintets 0.35 (0.58) 0.85 (0.68) 0.92 (0.72) 0.5 (0.71) HCs-bvFTD: P < 0.001* HCs-AD: P < 0.001* HCs-PD: P = 0.41

Results are presented as mean (SD). The asterisk (*) indicates significant differences with an alpha level of P < 0.05. Between-group comparison on WM performance (normalized inverse efficiency score [IES]) for stimulus type (social, non-social) and load level (triplets, quartets, quintets) was assessed through a mixed model ANOVA (type III) and Tukey post-hoc comparisons. Effects sizes were calculated through partial eta (ηp2). Results are plotted in Fig. 1C. AD: Alzheimer’s disease, bvFTD: behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia, HCs: healthy controls, PD: Parkinson’s disease.

3.2. Theta oscillatory correlates of social and non-social WM

In all groups together, better WM for social stimuli correlated to increased θ oscillations in extended bilateral frontocingulate areas (Table 3, Fig. 2). Better WM for non-social stimuli was associated with increased θ oscillations in a specific right frontal cluster (Table 4, Fig. 2). Also, better high load (quintets) WM was associated with increased bilateral frontocingulate and left parietal θ oscillations. Meanwhile, better low load (triplets) WM predominantly correlated with increased bilateral frontocingulate θ oscillations (Table S6.1, Fig. S3).

Table 3.

Associations between EEG θ oscillations and WM for social stimuli.

Regions r P-FDR MNI coordinates
BA
x y z

Social
All groups together
 Superior frontal gyrus R − 0.528 0.041 25 35 45 8
 Superior frontal gyrus L − 0.525 0.044 − 10 − 55 40 9
 Middle frontal gyrus R − 0.529 0.041 45 20 50 8
 Middle frontal gyrus L − 0.527 0.042 − 25 45 35 9
 Medial orbital frontal L − 0.527 0.042 − 5 − 65 0 10
 Medial superior frontal gyrus L − 0.527 0.042 − 10 − 60 30 10
 Inferior frontal (pars opercularis) R − 0.527 0.042 50 20 40 8
 Anterior cingulum L − 0.527 0.042 − 10 35 30 8
 Middle cingulum L − 0.527 0.042 − 5 − 20 35 8
HCs
 Superior frontal gyrus R − 0.556 0.044 20 − 10 − 60 6
 Superior frontal gyrus L − 0.551 0.046 − 25 10 60 6
 Middle frontal gyrus L − 0.551 0.046 − 25 − 15 − 50 6
 Supplementary motor area R − 0.558 0.041 5 − 25 60 6
 Supplementary motor area L − 0.583 0.045 − 15 − 15 60 6
 Precentral gyrus L − 0.589 0.041 − 25 − 20 60 6
 Parahippocampal gyrus L − 0.601 0.039 − 20 − 25 − 20 36
 Middle cingulum L − 0.589 0.041 − 10 − 25 50 6
 Paracentral lobe R − 0.556 0.044 5 − 25 65 4
 Paracentral lobe L − 0.600 0.039 − 15 − 30 60 4
 Postcentral gyrus L − 0.597 0.040 − 20 − 30 55 1
 Precuneus L − 0.641 0.035 − 15 − 35 60 7
 Superior parietal lobe L − 0.553 0.045 − 20 − 40 65 1
BvFTD
 Cuneus L 0.707 0.029 − 15 − 75 20 18
 Calcarine fissure L 0.722 0.027 − 20 − 70 20 19
 Fusiform gyrus L 0.729 0.027 − 25 − 75 20 19
 Superior occipital gyrus L 0.728 0.027 − 20 − 75 20 19
 Middle occipital gyrus L 0.709 0.029 − 30 − 80 20 19
 Fusiform gyrus L 0.727 0.027 − 20 − 80 20 18
 Lingual gyrus L 0.699 0.030 − 15 − 85 15 18
AD
 Supramarginal gyrus L − 0.550 0.044 − 65 − 45 30 39
 Inferior parietal lobe L − 0.541 0.044 − 60 − 50 40 40
 Angular gyrus L − 0.532 0.045 − 60 − 55 35 40
PD
 Inferior frontal (pars triangularis) R − 0.792 0.021 40 35 15 46
 Medial superior frontal gyrus R − 0.778 0.022 5 30 40 6
 Superior frontal gyrus R − 0.815 0.019 20 30 30 9
 Middle frontal gyrus R − 0.805 0.019 35 25 35 9
 Inferior frontal (pars opercularis) R − 0.810 0.019 35 20 35 9
 Insula R − 0.799 0.021 30 20 15 45
 Anterior cingulum R − 0.840 0.016 5 10 30 24
 Anterior cingulum L − 0.837 0.017 − 5 10 25 24
 Middle cingulum R − 0.832 0.017 5 10 25 32
 Middle cingulum L − 0.796 0.020 − 5 10 35 32
 Supplementary motor area R 0.789 0.021 15 5 45 6

Pearson correlations (P ≤ 0.05 FDR-corrected) were performed to test the association between normalized current density maps in the EEG θ frequency band and WM performance (normalized inverse efficiency score [IES]), for social stimulus type. Analyses were run in all groups together and individually per group (HCs, bvFTD, AD and PD). Results are plotted in Fig. 2.A. These results were obtained with a demographically matched sample (see Table S3.1). AD: Alzheimer’s disease, BA: Brodmann area, bvFTD: behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia, HCs: healthy controls, PD: Parkinson’s disease.

Fig. 2.

Fig. 2.

Associations between EEG θ oscillations and WM stimulus types. Pearson’s correlations between frequency-wise normalized current densities maps computed in the EEG θ frequency band, and WM performance (normalized IES) for social and non-social stimulus types (P≤ 0.05 FDR-corrected). Analyses were run in all groups together and individually per group (HCs, bvFTD, AD and PD). For further details see Table 3 and Table 4. Results are plotted in top, front, back, right external, right internal, left external, and left internal views of the brain. For results in high (quintets) and low (triplets) load levels see Fig. S3. Results were obtained with a demographically matched sample (Table S3.1). Associations between source space θ oscillations and WM for (A) social stimuli and (B) non-social stimuli. AD: Alzheimer’s disease, bvFTD: behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia, HCs: healthy controls, IES: inverse efficiency score, PD: Parkinson’s disease.

Table 4.

Associations between EEG θ oscillations and WM for non-social stimuli.

Regions r P-FDR MNI coordinates
BA
x y z

Non-social
All groups together
 Superior frontal gyrus R − 0.525 0.044 30 5 65 6
 Middle frontal gyrus R − 0.527 0.042 45 10 55 8
 Precentral gyrus R − 0.525 0.044 45 − 5 60 6
HCs
 Superior frontal gyrus R − 0.672 0.032 3 51 8 6
 Frontal inferior operc. R − 0.626 0.037 35 15 35 8
 Middle frontal gyrus R − 0.685 0.031 25 15 50 8
 Middle cingulum R − 0.641 0.035 15 10 40 8
 Supplementary motor area R − 0.656 0.034 15 5 45 6
 Precentral gyrus R − 0.660 0.034 30 − 5 50 6
BvFTD
 Calcarine fissure L 0.623 0.037 − 10 − 100 − 10 18
 Lingual gyrus L 0.671 0.032 − 10 − 100 − 15 18
AD
 Rolandic operculum R 0.552 0.045 50 5 0 44
 Insula R 0.558 0.044 40 5 − 10 13
 Inferior temporal gyrus R 0.567 0.043 35 5 − 45 38
 Caudate R 0.565 0.043 35 5 − 15 38
 Fusiform gyrus R 0.575 0.042 25 5 − 45 38
 Middle temporal pole R 0.567 0.042 25 5 − 35 36
 Superior temporal pole R 0.572 0.041 25 5 − 20 34
 Amygdala R 0.577 0.041 20 0 − 15 38
 Parahippocampal gyrus R 0.572 0.041 15 0 − 15 36
 Postcentral gyrus R − 0.553 0.045 65 − 5 15 4
 Superior temporal gyrus R − 0.563 0.043 65 − 5 0 21
 Superior temporal gyrus L − 0.579 0.041 − 55 − 45 20 22
 Hippocampus R 0.570 0.042 15 − 5 − 15 28
 Middle frontal gyrus L − 0.551 0.045 − 25 − 15 − 50 6
 Inferior parietal lobe L − 0.616 0.038 − 60 − 50 40 39
 Supramarginal gyrus L − 0.627 0.037 − 60 − 50 − 35 39
 Middle temporal gyrus L − 0.579 0.041 − 50 − 50 20 39
 Angular gyrus L − 0.616 0.038 − 60 − 55 35 39
PD
 Medial superior frontal gyrus R − 0.621 0.038 5 40 55 8
 Superior frontal gyrus R − 0.606 0.039 15 30 60 6
 Supplementary motor area R − 0.600 0.039 10 25 60 6

Pearson correlations (P ≤ 0.05 FDR-corrected) were performed to test the association between normalized current density maps in the EEG θ frequency band and WM performance (normalized inverse efficiency score [IES]), for non-social stimulus type. Analyses were run in all groups together and individually per group (HCs, bvFTD, AD and PD). Results are plotted in Fig. 2.B. These results were obtained with a demographically matched sample (see Table S3.1). AD: Alzheimer’s disease, BA: Brodmann area, bvFTD: behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia, HCs: healthy controls, PD: Parkinson’s disease.

In HCs, better WM for social stimuli was associated with increased θ oscillations in bilateral frontocingulate and left parietal regions (Table 3, Fig. 2). In contrast, better WM for non-social stimuli was associated with increased θ oscillations in specific right frontocingulate areas (Table 4, Fig. 2). Moreover, better high load (quintets) WM was associated with increased θ oscillations in a specific right frontal cluster, while better low load (triplets) WM was associated with bilateral frontocingulate θ increment (Table S6.1, Fig. S3).

In bvFTD, WM deficits for social stimuli were associated with increased θ oscillations in left posterior regions (Table 3, Fig. 2), and WM deficits for non-social stimuli with a left posterior cluster (Table 4, Fig. 2). The same pattern was found among load levels, with significant associations between both high load (quintets) and low load (triplets) WM deficits, and increased θ oscillations in left posterior areas (Table S6.1, Fig. S3).

In AD, WM deficits for social stimuli were associated with decreased θ oscillations in the temporoparietal junction (Table 3, Fig. 2). In contrast, WM deficits for non-social stimuli were linked to decreased θ oscillations in extended temporoparietal areas, and to increased θ oscillations in right temporolimbic regions (Table 4, Fig. 2). High load (quintets) WM alterations were associated with decreased θ oscillations in left posterior regions, and low load (triplets) WM deficits correlated with a left parietal θ decrement (Table S6.1, Fig. S3).

In PD, we observed significant associations between better WM for social stimuli and increased θ oscillations in frontocingulate regions, while better WM for non-social stimuli was significantly associated with increased θ oscillations in a specific right frontal cluster (Table 3, Table 4, Fig. 2). Better high load (quintets) WM was associated with increased bilateral frontocingulate θ oscillations, while better low load (triplets) WM correlated with increased θ oscillations in a less extended bilateral frontocingulate cluster (Table S6.1, Fig. S3).

3.3. Brain network correlates of social and non-social WM

For all groups together (Fig. 3), seed analyses revealed that the better the WM for social stimuli, the higher the SN connectivity (r = −0.443, P-FDR = 0.03). In contrast, better WM for non-social stimuli was significantly associated with increased EN (r = −0.424, P-FDR = 0.03) and decreased DMN (r = 0.387, P-FDR = 0.04) connectivity. Also, better high load (quintets) WM was associated with increased EN connectivity (r = −0.401, P-FDR = 0.05). No significant associations were found for control networks (VN and MN) neither with low load (triplets) (see Table 5, Fig. S4). HCs presented non-significant associations.

Fig. 3.

Fig. 3.

Associations between networks and WM. Seed analyses over five networks (SN, EN, DMN, VN, MN, P≤0.05 FDR-corrected) were performed to test the association between each network’ functional connectivity and WM performance (normalized IES) for social and non-social stimuli, and for high (quintets) and low (triplets) load levels. Analyses were run in all groups together and individually for HCs, bvFTD, AD and PD (Table 5). (A) Target network associations for stimulus type. Associations are plotted between target networks (SN, EN and DMN) and WM for social and non-social stimuli. WM for social stimuli was significantly linked to the SN in all groups together and in bvFTD. WM for non-social stimuli was significantly linked to the EN in all groups together and in AD, and to the DMN in all groups together. Black-border squares indicate significant associations. For HC and PD non-significant associations see Fig. S4. (B) Correlation matrix for each stimulus type. Correlation matrix for social and non-social stimulus type across all groups together and individual groups. Black-border squares indicate significant associations. (C) Functional connectivity associations for each stimulus type and load level. Circular plots represent all functional connectivity correlations between groups (upper semi-circle) and brain networks (lower semi-circles). Links represent significant group-network correlation. They are color-coded according to the network. High load (quintets) WM significantly correlated to the EN in all groups together and in AD. Non-significant results were found across associations with low load (for further details see Fig. S4). Results were obtained with a demographically matched sample (Table S3.2) and across scanners (Table S3.3). Standard masks (Shirer et al., 2012) were used to isolate the voxels involved in each network in MNI anatomical space. None of the participants showed head movements greater than 3 mm and/or rotations higher than 3° (Table S3.4). AD: Alzheimer’s disease, bvFTD: behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia, DMN: default mode network, EN: executive network, HCs: healthy controls, IES: inverse efficiency score, MN: motor network, PD: Parkinson’s disease, SN: salience network, VN: visual network, wSDM: weighted Symbolic Dependence Metric.

Table 5.

Associations between functional connectivity networks and WM.

Network Social
Non-social
High load (quintets)
Low load (triplets)
r P-FDR r P-FDR r P-FDR r P-FDR

All groups together
SN − 0,443 0,036* − 0,090 0,216 0,037 0,554 − 0,025 0,848
EN − 0,068 0,297 − 0,424 0,037* − 0,401 0,050* − 0,068 0,286
DMN − 0,121 0,160 0,387 0,041* 0,035 0,571 0,048 0,416
VN 0,013 0,536 − 0,035 0,558 − 0,122 0,164 − 0,062 0,320
MN − 0,123 0,165 − 0,033 0,589 − 0,088 0,222 0,116 0,174
HCs
SN − 0,094 0,219 − 0,069 0,282 − 0,120 0,160 − 0,049 0,434
EN − 0,060 0,323 0,049 0,418 − 0,109 0,188 − 0,033 0,580
DMN 0,092 0,216 − 0,061 0,322 0,031 0,648 − 0,063 0,311
VN − 0,113 0,175 0,116 0,177 0,094 0,230 − 0,043 0,459
MN 0,033 0,609 − 0,090 0,227 0,042 0,399 − 0,040 0,485
BvFTD
SN − 0,431 0,038* 0,021 0,951 − 0,123 0,161 0,092 0,215
EN − 0,092 0,217 0,044 0,459 − 0,019 0,448 − 0,113 0,179
DMN 0,057 0,351 0,077 0,266 − 0,071 0,277 0,033 0,603
VN 0,102 0,196 0,047 0,425 0,056 0,358 0,107 0,183
MN − 0,121 0,165 − 0,025 0,764 0,027 0,742 0,104 0,199
AD
SN 0,057 0,357 0,095 0,214 0,022 0,904 − 0,116 0,176
EN − 0,032 0,650 − 0,395 0,041* − 0,372 0,046* 0,119 0,169
DMN − 0,117 0,178 0,375 0,064 − 0,089 0,223 0,080 0,254
VN 0,120 0,168 − 0,034 0,586 0,094 0,213 − 0,063 0,310
MN 0,080 0,258 − 0,064 0,316 0,017 0,974 − 0,055 0,356
PD
SN 0,098 0,209 0,074 0,276 − 0,033 0,585 − 0,073 0,272
EN 0,039 0,524 0,103 0,193 0,102 0,196 0,089 0,222
DMN − 0,073 0,271 − 0,065 0,307 0,122 0,169 0,035 0,579
VN 0,088 0,227 − 0,068 0,285 0,093 0,216 0,105 0,195
MN 0,035 0,574 − 0,045 0,432 0,032 0,649 0,119 0,168

Seed analyses over five networks (SN, EN, DMN, MN, VN, P ≤ 0.05 FDR-corrected) were performed to test the association between the functional connectivity of each network and WM performance (normalized inverse efficiency score [IES]), for social and non-social stimulus types, and the most extreme load level (quintets [high load] and triplets [low load]) conditions. Analyses were run in all groups together and individually per group (HCs, bvFTD, AD and PD). The asterisk (*) indicates significant association with an alpha level of P-FDR ≤ 0.05. Results are plotted in Fig. 3. and in Fig. S4. These results were obtained with a demographically matched sample (see Table S3.2). Standard masks were used to isolate the voxels that are typically involved in each resting-state network, based on the MNI anatomical space. None of the participants showed head movements greater than 3 mm and/or rotations higher than 3 (see Table S3.4). AD: Alzheimer’s disease, bvFTD: behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia, DMN: default mode network, EN: executive network, HCs: healthy controls, MN: motor network, PD: Parkinson’s disease, SN: salience network, VN: visual network.

In bvFTD, WM deficits for social stimuli were associated to reduced SN connectivity (r = −0.431, P-FDR = 0.03). In AD, significant associations were found between WM deficits for non-social stimuli (r = −0.395, P-FDR = 0.04) and for high load (quintets) level (r = −0.372, P-FDR = 0.04) and reduced EN connectivity. PD presented non-significant associations. Finally, non-significant results were found in any individual group for neither low load (triplets) associations (Table 5, Fig. S4).

4. Discussion

We examined the WM domain-specificity for social vs non-social stimuli in healthy participants and neurodegenerative conditions at behavioral, oscillatory, and functional connectivity levels. In HCs, a WM load effect and decreased WM for social stimuli confirmed our hypotheses. Considering all groups together, WM for social stimuli was associated with higher frontocingulate θ oscillations and SN connectivity. Conversely, WM for non-social stimuli was linked to canonical right frontal θ oscillations, higher EN and lower DMN connectivity. Relative to HCs, bvFTD presented generalized WM deficits associated with increased θ oscillations in posterior regions (being abolished in canonical frontal hubs), and WM for social stimuli specifically linked to lower SN connectivity. In AD, generalized WM deficits were related to temporoparietal θ oscillations, with WM for non-social stimuli particularly linked to lower EN connectivity. PD showed preserved WM, with social and non-social stimuli related to frontocingulate and frontal θ oscillations, respectively. Together, these multimodal findings reveal specific social and non-social stimuli mechanisms in WM across different pathophysiological models sensitive to WM and social processing impairments (bvFTD), generalized cognitive deficits (AD), and unspecific alterations (PD).

4.1. Domain-specific WM for social stimuli in controls

In HCs, behavioral and brain correlates confirmed the task’s robustness and our predictions. WM performance was reduced by high load and social stimuli (social impairment effect) (Cowan, 2017; Garrison and Schmeichel, 2019; Plancher et al., 2019). WM social modulation increased θ oscillations in extended regions beyond canonical hubs (Dai et al., 2017; Pavlov and Kotchoubey, 2020), including: frontal [involved in action-execution (Gu et al., 2019), WM (Marvel et al., 2019), and social-saliency processing (Van der Molen et al., 2017)]; cingulate [related to error-prediction and ‘other-oriented’ cues (Apps et al., 2016; Billeke et al., 2013)]; and left parietal [engaged in social processing (Billeke et al., 2014) and verbal WM (Eriksson et al., 2015; Li et al., 2017)] hubs. This broader network suggests θ oscillations are involved in the integration of bottom-up (stimulus-driven) and top-down (cognitive) processes across distributed areas (Brenner et al., 2014). The need for more efficient propagation of information in order to cope with socially-demanding WM processes might recruit this extended oscillatory network. Regarding rsfMRI, WM for social stimuli was related to SN connectivity. Insular-cingulate hubs (Seeley, 2019; Seeley et al., 2007) are critical for executive and social processing (Uddin, 2015) by mediating the interaction between bottom-up and top-down WM processes for social stimuli (Luo et al., 2014). Then, the SN seems to be critical to successful WM for externally-perceived social stimuli. This network would dynamically reallocate resources between the DMN (related to social cognition) and the EN (linked to WM) to optimize responses to salient (social) stimuli (Maehara, 2017; Uddin, 2015). In contrast, WM for non-social stimuli involved the predicted EN-DMN anticorrelation (Chai et al., 2018; Liang et al., 2016). Moreover, the lack of associations with control networks (VN and MN) confirmed the differential impact of social and non-social stimuli over WM. All in all, results in HCs and in all groups together support a relative domain-specific WM for social stimuli with increased behavioral demands indexed by frontocinguloparietal θ oscillations and the SN connectivity, compared to frontal θ oscillations and EN-DMN anticorrelation for non-social stimuli.

4.2. Distinct WM mechanisms across neurodegenerative models

In bvFTD, contrarily to the social stimuli selectivity hypothesis, WM deficits were generalized. Although to some extent unexpected, this is consistent with the syndrome’s well-known, overall, and sui generis dysexecutive profile (Beeldman et al., 2018; Gonzalez-Gomez et al., 2021; Kamath et al., 2019). Similarly, overall absent frontal θ correlates suggest a generalized WM alteration linked to impaired oscillatory mechanisms. Indeed, WM decay was linked to higher posterior visual-encoding θ oscillations (Takase et al., 2022), interpreted as a paradoxical response to frontal θ abolition (Caso et al., 2012; Metin et al., 2018) necessary for WM compensation (Dai et al., 2017). Beyond bvFTD lack of domain-specificity across behavioral and oscillatory correlates, impaired WM for social stimuli was specifically linked to lower SN connectivity. This network is distinctly disrupted in bvFTD relative to HCs and AD (Brown et al., 2020; Moguilner et al., 2021; Pini et al., 2022; Salamone et al., 2021; Seeley, 2019; Zhou et al., 2010) and has been related to their inability to hold and manipulate social cues in complex social situations (Baez et al., 2014; Zhou and Seeley, 2014). Then, SN abnormalities may disrupt the ‘switch’ between the EN and DMN during executive processes involving social stimuli (Maehara, 2017; Uddin, 2015). Altogether, a primary (domain-neutral), sui generis WM impairment in bvFTD is accompanied by a distinctive social domain-specific WM pathophysiological mechanism related to the SN.

In AD, results supported the hypothesis of unspecific WM decline across behavioral and brain correlates, possibly explained by overall cognitive deficits (Ibáñez et al., 2021b; Ramanan et al., 2017b). Resulting socially-related temporoparietal θ decay confirmed the DMN-related attentional and social processes engagement (Igelström and Graziano, 2017) already impaired in AD (Lattanzio et al., 2021). Moreover, non-social stimuli deficits were linked to broader temporoparietal θ decrease (Goodman et al., 2019; Kobylecki et al., 2018) and to unexpected temporolimbic θ increase. Recent works in AD have reported both positive and negative θ dynamics, in terms of hypo- and hyperconnectivity (Herzog et al., 2022; Prado et al., 2023). The observed mixed correlations may be explained by temporo-posterior atrophy and decreased dynamic range of cortical activity (Prado et al., 2023). Positive θ associations could be triggered by functional reorganization (Parra et al., 2017) including temporolimbic θ hyperconnectivity (Prado et al., 2023), and overall cognitive deficits (Musaeus et al., 2018). Finally, canonical frontal θ correlates were absent during both stimulus types, previously related to AD cognitive withdrawal (Hata et al., 2016). Regarding rsfMRI, the observed link between non-social WM deficits and lower EN connectivity adds evidence of a disrupted frontoparietal mechanism (Zhao et al., 2019) underlying this condition’ WM decay (Wang et al., 2015). In contrast to bvFTD, AD social deficits appear to be related to general-cognitive alterations. Thus, results support the overall WM alteration hypothesis in AD, primarily indexed by temporoparietal θ and EN dysfunctions that jointly aligns with a domain-general WM pathophysiological mechanism.

In PD, the predicted spared WM suggests a cognitive compensation possibly related to dopaminergic medication (Moustafa et al., 2013; Salmi et al., 2020). Social modulation over WM was comparable to HCs, indicating that WM for social stimuli is not primarily altered in this disease. In fact, social processing deficits in PD might be mediated by general executive functions (Kosutzka et al., 2019; Maggi et al., 2022; Romosan et al., 2019). Similarly, preserved frontocingulate and frontal θ correlates in both stimulus type can also be explained by the overall-brain θ regularization triggered by dopaminergic medication (Moustafa et al., 2013; Orcioli-Silva et al., 2020; Ramos and Machado, 2021; Simioni et al., 2017). Lack of neurofunctional correlates (rsfMRI) is not surprising considering the more lenient impact of atrophy, the relative cognitive preservation, and the ‘ON’ dopaminergic-medication state of our sample (Cole et al., 2013; Dang et al., 2012; Kalpouzos et al., 2012; Wolters et al., 2019). In fact, absent connectivity correlates of executive performance in PD is not novel (Engels et al., 2018). Multimodal results converge in a spared dopaminergic-medicated PD model with predominant frontal θ correlates supporting WM and social processing preservation.

4.3. Theoretical and clinical implications

Our findings support theoretical accounts of a relative domain-specific WM for social stimuli, with higher behavioral demands and extended social brain regions beyond canonical WM hubs. The cingulate cortex was engaged in all domain-specific neurofunctional mechanisms: the distributed θ oscillations and the SN. This hub may play a pivotal role during WM for social stimuli. Specifically, it may index error-prediction WM mechanisms that are particularly modulated by the integration of socio-contextual information (Lavin et al., 2013). Then, when social and cognitive processes operate simultaneously and compete with each other for the limited neural resources (Hur et al., 2017), the cingulate may coordinate their interaction allowing the successful maintenance and manipulation of social cues. This interpretation fits well with externally-perceived social stimuli, contrasting other WM reports based on internally-generated social cues (Meyer and Lieberman, 2012; Meyer et al., 2012; Meyer et al., 2015). Our findings support the idea that previous controversies regarding WM domain-specificity (Meyer and Collier, 2020; Thornton and Conway, 2013; Xin and Lei, 2015) are primarily explained by differences in the internal vs external stimulus sources (Smith et al., 2017).

The detection of specific WM mechanisms for social stimuli carries clinical implications providing better characterization of neurodegenerative conditions. Comparable bvFTD and AD behavioral deficits were indexed by different domain-specific and -general pathophysiological mechanisms, respectively. Then, findings suggest differential therapeutic strategies, including: (a) general-WM training as a potential tool to induce EN enhancement (Constantinidis and Klingberg, 2016) and to improve daily-functioning in AD (Hernes et al., 2021); and (b) both general- and social specific-WM training to boost everyday social competence in bvFTD (Maehara, 2017; Meyer and Lieberman, 2012). Moreover, spared neurocognitive mechanisms in PD highlight the essential role of dopaminergic medication in WM preservation beyond striatal alterations. In sum, we offer relevant evidence of distinct neural correlates underlying social modulation over WM across neurodegenerative models, allowing differential diagnosis and treatment.

4.4. Limitations and further research

We acknowledge certain limitations to our study and outline new avenues for further research. First, although our work is based on a modest sample size, it is larger than those of other multimodal neurodegenerative reports (Birba et al., 2021; Garcia-Cordero et al., 2016; Hughes et al., 2011; Legaz et al., 2022; Melloni et al., 2016; Moretti et al., 2009; Salamone et al., 2021). Moreover, strict control of demographic and clinical variables, as well as systematic diagnostic procedures, counteract this limitation. Also, our sample size power analysis, the results consistency across dimensions together with moderate-to-large effect sizes further attests to their robustness. In any case, future studies should replicate and extend these results with larger samples. Second, we focused on θ oscillations since research strongly supports its involvement in WM (Pavlov and Kotchoubey, 2020) across neurodegeneration (Goodman et al., 2019), and social processing (Gregory et al., 2021; van der Velde et al., 2021). However, future works should also target alpha/gamma oscillations and cross-frequency coupling also related to WM (Dai et al., 2017; Roux and Uhlhaas, 2014) and reported impaired in neurodegenerative conditions (Güntekin et al., 2022; Ishii et al., 2017; Kitchigina, 2018). Third, beyond the novel contributions of convergent rsEEG and rsfMRI methodologies across multiple neurodegenerative models, future works should also include active paradigms to better elucidate brain networks directly engaged in social WM modulation. Finally, our work rises new evidence regarding the domain-specificity of a critical daily-life process (Maresca et al., 2020; Porcelli et al., 2019). Our findings call for a more synergic understanding of social cognition and WM blending. These processes are not isolated, but integrated across different dimensions. New studies should examine how WM for social stimuli differentially impacts everyday functioning across neurodegenerative profiles with an ecological approach. This would better capture implicit socio-contextual modulations over WM and social cognition dynamics in real-life settings (Ibáñez, 2022).

5. Conclusions

Our multimodal neurodegenerative lesion model approach reveals convergent evidence of social and non-social effects over WM across healthy controls and neurodegenerative conditions. Findings support a relative domain-specific WM for social stimuli indexed by frontocinguloparietal θ oscillations and the SN that contrast with canonical frontal θ correlates and EN-DMN anticorrelation for non-social stimuli. Also, results provide different pathophysiological mechanisms, including a bvFTD primary WM alteration but specific network mechanisms linked to WM for social stimuli, domain-general WM deficits linked to cognitive deficits and related pathological brain correlates in AD, and behavioral and neurofunctional preservation in dopaminergic-medicated PD. Further research may bring a new clinical agenda favoring differential diagnosis and treatment among neurodegenerative conditions with common WM and social processing alterations.

Supplementary Material

1

Acknowledgement

Authors thank the ReDLat participants and their families invaluable time and commitment to our study.

Funding

This work is partially supported by grants of Takeda [CW2680521]; CONICET; FONCYT-PICT [2017–1818, 2017–1820]; ANID/FONDECYT Regular [1210195, 1210176, 1220995]; ANID/FONDAP [15150012]; ANID/PIA/ANILLOS ACT210096; and the Multi-Partner Consortium to Expand Dementia Research in Latin America (ReDLat), funded by the National Institutes of Aging of the National Institutes of Health and the Fogarty International Center (FIC) under award number R01AG057234, an Alzheimer’s Association grant [SG-20-725707-ReDLat], the Rainwater Foundation, The Bluefield foundation to cure FTD; and the Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI). Sol Fittipaldi is an Atlantic Fellow for Equity in Brain Health at the Global Brain Health Institute (GBHI) and is supported with funding from GBHI. Adolfo M. García is supported with funding from GBHI, Alzheimer’s Association, and Alzheimer’s Society [GBHI ALZ UK-22-865742]; ANID [FONDECYT Regular 1210176]; and Programa Interdisciplinario de Investigación Experimental en Comunicación y Cognición (PIIECC), Facultad de Humanidades, USACH. The content of the article is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not represent the official views of these institutions.

Abbreviations:

AD

Alzheimer’s disease

BvFTD

behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia

HCs

healthy controls

IES

inverse efficiency score

DMN

default mode network

EN

executive network

MN

motor network

NINCDS-ADRDA

National Institute of Neurological and Communicative Disorders and Stroke - Alzheimer’s Disease and Related Disorders Association

PD

Parkinson’s disease

rsEEG

resting-state EEG

rsfMRI

resting-state functional magnetic resonance imaging

sLORETA

standardized Low-Resolution Electromagnetic Tomography method

SN

salience network

SPM12

Statistical Parametric Mapping software v.12

θ

theta

VN

visual network

WM

working memory

Footnotes

Declaration of Competing Interests

The authors declare that they have no competing interests.

Ethics approval

The institutional ethics committee of each recruitment center approved the study protocol. All participants provided signed informed consent in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki.

Appendix A. Supplementary data

Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.nbd.2023.106171.

CRediT authorship contribution statement

Agustina Legaz: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Investigation, Resources, Data curation, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Visualization. Pavel Prado: Formal analysis. Sebastián Moguilner: Formal analysis, Data curation, Visualization. Sandra Báez: Methodology, Resources. Hernando Santamaría-García: Methodology, Resources. Agustina Birba: Investigation, Data curation. Pablo Barttfeld: Investigation, Resources, Data curation, Visualization. Adolfo M. García: Methodology, Resources, Writing – review & editing. Sol Fittipaldi: Conceptualization, Investigation, Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Supervision. Agustín Ibañez: Conceptualization, Methodology, Resources, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Funding acquisition.

Data availability

The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available in the OSF repository, http://osf.io/bx27h (Legaz, 2022).

References

  1. Agosta F, et al. , 2012. Resting state fMRI in Alzheimer’s disease: beyond the default mode network. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 1564–1578. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Apps MA, et al. , 2016. The anterior cingulate gyrus and social cognition: tracking the motivation of others. Neuron. 90, 692–707. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Argaud S, et al. , 2018. Facial emotion recognition in Parkinson’s disease: a review and new hypotheses. Mov. Disord. 33, 554–567. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ashburner J, Friston KJ, 1999. Nonlinear spatial normalization using basis functions. Hum. Brain Mapp. 7, 254–266. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Badhwar A, et al. , 2017. Resting-state network dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease: a systematic review and meta-analysis. In: Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring, Vol. 8, pp. 73–85. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Baez S, et al. , 2014. Comparing moral judgments of patients with frontotemporal dementia and frontal stroke. JAMA Neurol. 71, 1172–1176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Baez S, et al. , 2019. Brain structural correlates of executive and social cognition profiles in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and elderly bipolar disorder. Neuropsychologia. 126, 159–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Barttfeld P, et al. , 2012. State-dependent changes of connectivity patterns and functional brain network topology in autism spectrum disorder. Neuropsychologia. 50, 3653–3662. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Beeldman E, et al. , 2018. The cognitive profile of behavioural variant FTD and its similarities with ALS: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 89, 995–1002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Benjamini Y, Yekutieli D, 2001. The control of the false discovery rate in multiple testing under dependency. Ann. Stat. 1165–1188. [Google Scholar]
  11. Bennett CM, et al. , 2009. The principled control of false positives in neuroimaging. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 4, 417–422. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Ben-Shachar MS, et al. , 2020. effectsize: Estimation of effect size indices and standardized parameters. J. Open Source Softw. 5, 2815. [Google Scholar]
  13. Billeke P, et al. , 2013. Oscillatory brain activity correlates with risk perception and predicts social decisions. Cereb. Cortex 23, 2872–2883. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Billeke P, et al. , 2014. Someone has to give in: theta oscillations correlate with adaptive behavior in social bargaining. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 9, 2041–2048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Birba A, et al. , 2021. Multimodal neurocognitive markers of naturalistic discourse typify diverse neurodegenerative diseases. Cereb. Cortex 32 (16), 3377–3391. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Birba A, et al. , 2022. Allostatic-interoceptive overload in frontotemporal dementia. Biol. Psychiatry 92, 54–67. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Brancaccio A, et al. , 2020. Cortical source localization of sleep-stage specific oscillatory activity. Sci. Rep. 10, 6976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Brenner CA, et al. , 2014. The role of encoding and attention in facial emotion memory: an EEG investigation. Int. J. Psychophysiol. 93, 398–410. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Brown CL, et al. , 2020. Comparing two facets of emotion perception across multiple neurodegenerative diseases. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 15, 511–522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Brozzoli C, et al. , 2008. Touch perception reveals the dominance of spatial over digital representation of numbers. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 105, 5644–5648. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Caso F, et al. , 2012. Quantitative EEG and LORETA: valuable tools in discerning FTD from AD? Neurobiol. Aging 33, 2343–2356. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Chai WJ, et al. , 2018. Working memory from the psychological and neurosciences perspectives: a review. Front. Psychol. 9, 401. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Chao-Gan Y, Yu-Feng Z, 2010. DPARSF: a MATLAB toolbox for “pipeline” data analysis of resting-state fMRI. Front. Syst. Neurosci. 4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Chiong W, et al. , 2016. Neuroeconomic dissociation of semantic dementia and behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 139, 578–587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Christophel TB, et al. , 2017. The distributed nature of working memory. Trends Cogn. Sci. 21, 111–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Chung J, et al. , 2017. Normalization of cortical thickness measurements across different T1 magnetic resonance imaging protocols by novel W-Score standardization. Neuroimage. 159, 224–235. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Cole DM, et al. , 2013. Differential and distributed effects of dopamine neuromodulations on resting-state network connectivity. Neuroimage. 78, 59–67. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Constantinidis C, Klingberg T, 2016. The neuroscience of working memory capacity and training. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 17, 438–449. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Courellis HS, et al. , 2016. EEG channel interpolation using ellipsoid geodesic length. In: 2016 IEEE Biomedical Circuits and Systems Conference (BioCAS). IEEE, pp. 540–543. [Google Scholar]
  30. Cowan N, 2017. The many faces of working memory and short-term storage. Psychon. Bull. Rev. 24, 1158–1170. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Cruzat J, et al. , 2023. Temporal irreversibility of large-scale brain dynamics in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurosci. 43, 1643–1656. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Dai Z, et al. , 2017. EEG cortical connectivity analysis of working memory reveals topological reorganization in theta and alpha bands. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Dai C, et al. , 2020. Effects of sleep deprivation on working memory: change in functional connectivity between the dorsal attention, default mode, and frontoparietal networks. Front. Hum. Neurosci. 360. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Dang LC, et al. , 2012. Dopamine supports coupling of attention-related networks. J. Neurosci. 32, 9582–9587. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Das A, et al. , 2022. Electrophysiological foundations of the human default-mode network revealed by intracranial-EEG recordings during resting-state and cognition. NeuroImage. 118927. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Dave D, Varma T, 2014. A review of various statistical methods for outlier detection. Int. J. Comput. Sci. Eng. Technol. 5, 137–140. [Google Scholar]
  37. Delorme A, Makeig S, 2004. EEGLAB: an open source toolbox for analysis of single-trial EEG dynamics including independent component analysis. J. Neurosci. Methods 134, 9–21. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Dirlich G, et al. , 1997. Cardiac field effects on the EEG. Electroencephalogr. Clin. Neurophysiol. 102, 307–315. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Dodich A, et al. , 2016. Differential impairment of cognitive and affective mentalizing abilities in neurodegenerative dementias: evidence from behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia, Alzheimer’s disease, and mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 50, 1011–1022. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Dodich A, et al. , 2021. Diagnostic accuracy of affective social tasks in the clinical classification between the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia and other neurodegenerative disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 80, 1401–1411. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Donnelly-Kehoe PA, et al. , 2019. Robust automated computational approach for classifying frontotemporal neurodegeneration: multimodal/multicenter neuroimaging. In: Alzheimer’s & Dementia: Diagnosis, Assessment & Disease Monitoring, Vol. 11, pp. 588–598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Du A-T, et al. , 2007. Different regional patterns of cortical thinning in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 130, 1159–1166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Dubois B, et al. , 2007. Research criteria for the diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease: revising the NINCDS–ADRDA criteria. Lancet Neurol. 6, 734–746. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Duran-Aniotz C, et al. , 2022. The Latin American Brain Health Institute, a regional initiative to reduce the scale and impact of dementia. Wiley Online Library. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Engels G, et al. , 2018. Dynamic functional connectivity and symptoms of Parkinson’s disease: a resting-state fMRI study. Front. Aging Neurosci. 388. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Eriksson J, et al. , 2015. Neurocognitive architecture of working memory. Neuron. 88, 33–46. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Evans AC, et al. , 1993. 3D statistical neuroanatomical models from 305 MRI volumes. In: 1993 IEEE Conference Record Nuclear Science Symposium and Medical Imaging Conference. IEEE, pp. 1813–1817. [Google Scholar]
  48. Fairfield B, et al. , 2015. Running with emotion: when affective content hampers working memory performance. Int. J. Psychol. 50, 161–164. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Feng C, et al. , 2021. Common brain networks underlying human social interactions: evidence from large-scale neuroimaging meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 126, 289–303. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Fiebach CJ, et al. , 2006. Modulation of inferotemporal cortex activation during verbal working memory maintenance. Neuron. 51, 251–261. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Garcia-Cordero I, et al. , 2016. Feeling, learning from and being aware of inner states: interoceptive dimensions in neurodegeneration and stroke. Philos. Trans. R. Soc. Lond. Ser. B Biol. Sci. 371. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Garcia-Cordero I, et al. , 2017. Attention, in and out: scalp-level and intracranial EEG correlates of interoception and exteroception. Front. Neurosci. 11, 411. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Garcia-Cordero I, et al. , 2019. Explicit and implicit monitoring in neurodegeneration and stroke. Sci. Rep. 9, 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Garrison KE, Schmeichel BJ, 2019. Effects of emotional content on working memory capacity. Cognit. Emot. 33, 370–377. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Gonzalez Campo C, et al. , 2019. Fatigue in multiple sclerosis is associated with multimodal interoceptive abnormalities. Mult. Scler. J. 26 (14), 1845–1853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Gonzalez-Gomez R, et al. , 2021. Neurocognitive factorial structure of executive functions: evidence from neurotypicals and frontotemporal dementia. Cortex. 145, 79–96. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Goodman MS, et al. , 2019. Changes in theta but not alpha modulation are associated with impairment in working memory in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. J. Alzheimers Dis. 68, 1085–1094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Grech R, et al. , 2008. Review on solving the inverse problem in EEG source analysis. J. Neuroeng. Rehabil. 5, 1–33. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Gregory SE, et al. , 2021. EEG alpha and theta signatures of socially and non-socially cued working memory in virtual reality. Soc. Cogn. Affect. 17 (6), 531–540. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Gu R, et al. , 2019. Love is analogous to money in human brain: coordinate-based and functional connectivity meta-analyses of social and monetary reward anticipation. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 100, 108–128. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Güntekin B, et al. , 2022. Are there consistent abnormalities in event-related EEG oscillations in patients with Alzheimer’s disease compared to other diseases belonging to dementia? Psychophysiology. 59, e13934. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Han H, et al. , 2019. Evaluating alternative correction methods for multiple comparison in functional neuroimaging research. Brain Sci. 9, 198. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Hata M, et al. , 2016. Functional connectivity assessed by resting state EEG correlates with cognitive decline of Alzheimer’s disease–An eLORETA study. Clin. Neurophysiol. 127, 1269–1278. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Hernes SS, et al. , 2021. Working memory training in amnestic and non-amnestic patients with mild cognitive impairment: preliminary findings from genotype variants on training effects. Front. Aging Neurosci. 13, 22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Herzog R, et al. , 2022. Genuine high-order interactions in brain networks and neurodegeneration. Neurobiol. Dis. 175, 105918. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Hesse E, et al. , 2016. Early detection of intentional harm in the human amygdala. Brain. 139, 54–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Hesse E, et al. , 2019. Consistent gradient of performance and decoding of stimulus type and valence from local and network activity. IEEE Trans. Neural Syst. Rehabil. Eng. 27, 619–629. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Hou Y, et al. , 2018. Resting-state network connectivity in cognitively unimpaired drug-naive patients with rigidity-dominant Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Sci. 395, 147–152. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Huber SJ, et al. , 1989. Magnetic resonance imaging in dementia of Parkinson’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 52, 1221–1227. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Hughes AJ, et al. , 1992. Accuracy of clinical diagnosis of idiopathic Parkinson’s disease: a clinico-pathological study of 100 cases. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 55, 181–184. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Hughes LE, et al. , 2011. Magnetoencephalography of frontotemporal dementia: spatiotemporally localized changes during semantic decisions. Brain. 134, 2513–2522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  72. Hur J, et al. , 2017. Emotional influences on perception and working memory. Cognit. Emot. 31, 1294–1302. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Ibáñez A, 2022. The min d’s golden cage and cognition in the wild. Trends Cogn. Sci. 26 (12), 1031–1034. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Ibáñez A, Manes F, 2012. Contextual social cognition and the behavioral variant of frontotemporal dementia. Neurology. 78, 1354–1362. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Ibáñez A, et al. , 2021a. Predicting and characterizing neurodegenerative subtypes with multimodal neurocognitive signatures of social and cognitive processes. J. Alzheimers Dis. 1–22. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Ibáñez A, et al. , 2021b. Dementia caregiving across Latin America and the Caribbean and brain health diplomacy. Lancet Healthy Longev. 2, e222–e231. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Ibáñez A, et al. , 2021c. The Multi-partner consortium to expand dementia research in Latin America (ReDLat): driving multicentric research and implementation science. Front. Neurol. 12, 303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Igelström KM, Graziano MS, 2017. The inferior parietal lobule and temporoparietal junction: a network perspective. Neuropsychologia. 105, 70–83. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Ishii R, et al. , 2017. Healthy and pathological brain aging: from the perspective of oscillations, functional connectivity, and signal complexity. Neuropsychobiology. 75, 151–161. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Jack CR, et al. , 1997. Medial temporal atrophy on MRI in normal aging and very mild Alzheimer’s disease. Neurology. 49, 786–794. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Jacques C, Rossion B, 2007. Early electrophysiological responses to multiple face orientations correlate with individual discrimination performance in humans. Neuroimage. 36, 863–876. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Jacquet PO, Avenanti A, 2015. Perturbing the action observation network during perception and categorization of actions’ goals and grips: state-dependency and virtual lesion TMS effects. Cereb. Cortex 25, 598–608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Kalpouzos G, et al. , 2012. Local brain atrophy accounts for functional activity differences in normal aging. Neurobiol. Aging 33, 623. e1–623. e13. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Kamath V, et al. , 2019. A meta-analysis of neuropsychological, social cognitive, and olfactory functioning in the behavioral and language variants of frontotemporal dementia. Psychol. Med. 49, 2669–2680. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  85. Kim D, Kim S-K, 2012. Comparing patterns of component loadings: Principal Component Analysis (PCA) versus Independent Component Analysis (ICA) in analyzing multivariate non-normal data. Behav. Res. Methods 44, 1239–1243. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  86. Kim G-W, et al. , 2020. A pilot study of brain morphometry following donepezil treatment in mild cognitive impairment: volume changes of cortical/subcortical regions and hippocampal subfields. Sci. Rep. 10, 1–11. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Kipps C, et al. , 2009. Understanding social dysfunction in the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia: the role of emotion and sarcasm processing. Brain. 132, 592–603. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Kirova A-M, et al. , 2015. Working memory and executive function decline across normal aging, mild cognitive impairment, and Alzheimer’s disease. Biomed. Res. Int. 2015. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Kitchigina VF, 2018. Alterations of coherent theta and gamma network oscillations as an early biomarker of temporal lobe epilepsy and Alzheimer’s disease. Front. Integr. Neurosci. 36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Kobylecki C, et al. , 2018. Functional neuroanatomical associations of working memory in early-onset Alzheimer’s disease. Int. J. Geriat. Psychiatry 33, 176–184. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  91. Koslov K, et al. , 2011. Asymmetry in resting intracortical activity as a buffer to social threat. Psychol. Sci. 22, 641–649. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Kosutzka Z, et al. , 2019. Neurocognitive predictors of understanding of intentions in Parkinson disease. J. Geriatr. Psychiatry Neurol. 32, 178–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Kumfor F, et al. , 2017. Assessing the “social brain” in dementia: applying TASIT-S. Cortex. 93, 166–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  94. Kuznetsova A, et al. , 2017. lmerTest package: tests in linear mixed effects models. J. Stat. Softw. 82, 1–26. [Google Scholar]
  95. La Joie R, et al. , 2012. Region-specific hierarchy between atrophy, hypometabolism, and β-amyloid (Aβ) load in Alzheimer’s disease dementia. J. Neurosci. 32, 16265–16273. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Landin-Romero R, et al. , 2017. Disease-specific patterns of cortical and subcortical degeneration in a longitudinal study of Alzheimer’s disease and behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia. Neuroimage. 151, 72–80. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  97. Lattanzio L, et al. , 2021. The emergent relationship between temporoparietal junction and anosognosia in Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurosci. Res. 99, 2091–2096. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Lavin C, et al. , 2013. The anterior cingulate cortex: an integrative hub for human socially-driven interactions. Front. Neurosci. 7, 64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Legaz A, 2022. Social and Non-Social Working Memory in Neurodegeneration osf.io/bx27h. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  100. Legaz A, et al. , 2022. Multimodal mechanisms of human socially reinforced learning across neurodegenerative diseases. Brain. 145, 1052–1068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Leslie F, et al. , 2016. FRONTIER Executive Screen: a brief executive battery to differentiate frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Neurol. Neurosurg. Psychiatry 87, 831–835. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  102. Lewis S, Ricciardi L, 2021. Social cognition in Parkinson’s disease. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 85, 122–123. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Li S, et al. , 2017. Theta and alpha oscillations during the retention period of working memory by rTMS stimulating the parietal lobe. Front. Behav. Neurosci. 11, 170. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Liang X, et al. , 2016. Topologically reorganized connectivity architecture of default-mode, executive-control, and salience networks across working memory task loads. Cereb. Cortex 26, 1501–1511. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Luo Y, et al. , 2014. Emotion perception and executive control interact in the salience network during emotionally charged working memory processing. Hum. Brain Mapp. 35, 5606–5616. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  106. Maehara Y, 2017. Psychological and neural functions of working memory in social contexts. In: Memory in a Social Context, pp. 27–42. [Google Scholar]
  107. Maggi G, et al. , 2022. Neuropsychological, neuropsychiatric, and clinical correlates of affective and cognitive theory of mind in Parkinson’s disease: a meta-analysis. Neuropsychology 36 (6), 483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  108. Maito MA, et al. , 2023. Classification of Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia using routine clinical and cognitive measures across multicentric underrepresented samples: a cross sectional observational study. Lancet Reg. Health-Am. 17, 100387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Maresca G, et al. , 2020. Understanding the role of social cognition in neurodegenerative disease: a scoping review on an overlooked problem. J. Clin. Neurosci. 77, 17–24. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Marvel CL, et al. , 2019. How the motor system integrates with working memory. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 102, 184–194. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. McKhann GM, et al. , 2011. The diagnosis of dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease: Recommendations from the National Institute on Aging-Alzheimer’s Association workgroups on diagnostic guidelines for Alzheimer’s disease. Alzheimers Dement. 7, 263–269. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Melloni M, et al. , 2016. Your perspective and my benefit: multiple lesion models of self-other integration strategies during social bargaining. Brain. 139, 1–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Menon V, D’Esposito M, 2022. The role of PFC networks in cognitive control and executive function. Neuropsychopharmacology. 47, 90–103. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Metin SZ, et al. , 2018. The use of quantitative EEG for differentiating frontotemporal dementia from late-onset bipolar disorder. Clin. EEG Neurosci. 49, 171–176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  115. Mevorach C, et al. , 2006. Opposite biases in salience-based selection for the left and right posterior parietal cortex. Nat. Neurosci. 9, 740. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Meyer ML, Collier E, 2020. Theory of mind s: managing mental state inferences in working memory is associated with the dorsomedial subsystem of the default network and social integration. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 15, 63–73. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Meyer ML, Lieberman MD, 2012. Social working memory: neurocognitive networks and directions for future research. Front. Psychol. 3, 571. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Meyer ML, et al. , 2012. Evidence for social working memory from a parametric functional MRI study. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. 109, 1883–1888. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Meyer ML, et al. , 2015. Social working memory and its distinctive link to social cognitive ability: an fMRI study. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 1338–1347. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Migeot JA, et al. , 2022. A predictive coding framework of allostatic–interoceptive overload in frontotemporal dementia. Trends Neurosci. 45 (11), 838–853. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Minnier J, et al. , 2021. Associations between lipids in selected brain regions, plasma miRNA, and behavioral and cognitive measures following 28Si ion irradiation. Sci. Rep. 11, 1–30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Moguilner S, et al. , 2018. Weighted Symbolic Dependence Metric (wSDM) for fMRI resting-state connectivity: a multicentric validation for frontotemporal dementia. Sci. Rep. 8, 11181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Moguilner S, et al. , 2021. Dynamic brain fluctuations outperform connectivity measures and mirror pathophysiological profiles across dementia subtypes: a multicenter study. Neuroimage. 225, 117522. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Moguilner S, et al. , 2022. Multi-feature computational framework for combined signatures of dementia in underrepresented settings. J. Neural Eng. 19, 046048. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  125. Moguilner S, et al. , 2023. Visual deep learning of unprocessed neuroimaging characterises dementia subtypes and generalises across non-stereotypic samples. EBioMedicine. 90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Moretti L, et al. , 2009. Reward and social valuation deficits following ventromedial prefrontal damage. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 21, 128–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Morrison AB, et al. , 2016. Variation in strategy use across measures of verbal working memory. Mem. Cogn. 44, 922–936. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Moustafa AA, et al. , 2013. The effects of clinical motor variables and medication dosage on working memory in Parkinson’s disease. Brain Cogn. 82, 137–145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  129. Musa G, et al. , 2020. Alzheimer’s disease or behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia? Review of key points toward an accurate clinical and neuropsychological diagnosis. J. Alzheimers Dis. 73, 833–848. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Musaeus CS, et al. , 2018. EEG theta power is an early marker of cognitive decline in dementia due to Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 64, 1359–1371. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Nanda A, et al. , 2021. Multiple comparison test by Tukey’s honestly significant difference (HSD): Do the confident level control type I error. IJAMS. 6, 59–65. [Google Scholar]
  132. Nichols TE, et al. , 2017. Best practices in data analysis and sharing in neuroimaging using MRI. Nat. Neurosci. 20, 299–303. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Nissim NR, et al. , 2017. Frontal structural neural correlates of working memory performance in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci. 8, 328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  134. O’Callaghan C, et al. , 2016. Fair play: social norm compliance failures in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 139, 204–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Oberauer K, et al. , 2016. What limits working memory capacity? Psychol. Bull. 142, 758. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  136. Orcioli-Silva D, et al. , 2020. Levodopa facilitates prefrontal cortex activation during dual task walking in Parkinson disease. Neurorehabil. Neural Repair 34, 589–599. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Ossenkoppele R, et al. , 2015. The behavioural/dysexecutive variant of Alzheimer’s disease: clinical, neuroimaging and pathological features. Brain. 138, 2732–2749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Parra MA, et al. , 2010. Visual short-term memory binding deficits in familial Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 133, 2702–2713. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Parra MA, et al. , 2017. Brain information sharing during visual short-term memory binding yields a memory biomarker for familial Alzheimer’s disease. Curr. Alzheimer Res. 14, 1335–1347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Pascual-Marqui RD, 2002. Standardized low-resolution brain electromagnetic tomography (sLORETA): technical details. Methods Find. Exp. Clin. Pharmacol. 24, 5–12. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Pavlov YG, Kotchoubey B, 2020. Oscillatory brain activity and maintenance of verbal and visual working memory: A systematic review. Psychophysiology. e13735. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Pessoa L, 2009. How do emotion and motivation direct executive control? Trends Cogn. Sci. 13, 160–166. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Peterson RA, Cavanaugh JE, 2019. Ordered quantile normalization: a semiparametric transformation built for the cross-validation era. J. Appl. Stat. 1–16. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Pietto M, et al. , 2016. Behavioral and electrophysiological correlates of memory binding deficits in patients at different risk levels for Alzheimer’s disease. J. Alzheimers Dis. 53, 1325–1340. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Piguet O, et al. , 2011. Behavioural-variant frontotemporal dementia: diagnosis, clinical staging, and management. Lancet Neurol. 10, 162–172. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Pini L, et al. , 2016. Brain atrophy in Alzheimer’s Disease and aging. Ageing Res. Rev. 30, 25–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Pini L, et al. , 2022. Brain network modulation in Alzheimer’s and frontotemporal dementia with transcranial electrical stimulation. Neurobiol. Aging 111, 24–34. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Plancher G, et al. , 2019. Effect of negative emotional content on attentional maintenance in working memory. Cognit. Emot. 33, 1489–1496. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Poldrack RA, et al. , 2017. Scanning the horizon: towards transparent and reproducible neuroimaging research. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 18, 115–126. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Pollatos O, Schandry R, 2004. Accuracy of heartbeat perception is reflected in the amplitude of the heartbeat-evoked brain potential. Psychophysiology. 41, 476–482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Poos JM, et al. , 2018. Meta-analytic review of memory impairment in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 24, 593–605. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Porcelli S, et al. , 2019. Social brain, social dysfunction and social withdrawal. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 97, 10–33. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Possin KL, et al. , 2013. Dissociable executive functions in behavioral variant frontotemporal and Alzheimer dementias. Neurology. 80, 2180–2185. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Prado P, et al. , 2023. Source space connectomics of neurodegeneration: one-metric approach does not fit all. Neurobiol. Dis. 179, 106047. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Price S, et al. , 2004. Voxel-based morphometry detects patterns of atrophy that help differentiate progressive supranuclear palsy and Parkinson’s disease. Neuroimage. 23, 663–669. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Ramanan S, et al. , 2017a. Longitudinal executive function and episodic memory profiles in behavioral-variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 23, 34–43. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Ramanan S, et al. , 2017b. Determinants of theory of mind performance in Alzheimer’s disease: a data-mining study. Cortex. 88, 8–18. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Ramos AA, Machado L, 2021. A comprehensive meta-analysis on short-term and working memory dysfunction in Parkinson’s disease. Neuropsychol. Rev. 31, 288–311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Rascovsky K, et al. , 2011. Sensitivity of revised diagnostic criteria for the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia. Brain. 134, 2456–2477. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Rasmussen JL, 1985. Data transformation maximizing homoscedasticity and within-group normality. Behav. Res. Methods Instrum. Comput. 17 (3), 411–412. [Google Scholar]
  161. Reuter-Lorenz PA, et al. , 2000. Age differences in the frontal lateralization of verbal and spatial working memory revealed by PET. J. Cogn. Neurosci. 12, 174–187. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Rijpma MG, et al. , 2021. Salience driven attention is pivotal to understanding others’ intentions. Cog. Neuropsychol. 38, 88–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Rijpma MG, et al. , 2022. Influence of periaqueductal gray on other salience network nodes predicts social sensitivity. Hum. Brain Mapp. 43 (5), 1694–1709. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Romosan A-M, et al. , 2019. Affective theory of mind in Parkinson’s disease: the effect of cognitive performance. Neuropsychiatr. Dis. Treat. 15, 2521. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Rorden C, Karnath HO, 2004. Using human brain lesions to infer function: a relic from a past era in the fMRI age? Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 5, 813–819. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Roux F, Uhlhaas PJ, 2014. Working memory and neural oscillations: alpha–gamma versus theta–gamma codes for distinct WM information? Trends Cogn. Sci. 18, 16–25. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  167. Salamone PC, et al. , 2021. Interoception primes emotional processing: multimodal evidence from neurodegeneration. J. Neurosci. 41, 4276–4292. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Salmi J, et al. , 2020. Disentangling the role of working memory in Parkinson’s disease. Front. Aging Neurosci. 287. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Santamaría-García H, et al. , 2022. Neurocognitive patterns across genetic levels in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia: a multiple single cases study. BMC Neurol. 22, 1–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Saris IM, et al. , 2021. Social dysfunction is transdiagnostically associated with default mode network dysconnectivity in schizophrenia and Alzheimer’s disease. World J. Biol. Psychiatry 1–14. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  171. Schandry R, Montoya P, 1996. Event-related brain potentials and the processing of cardiac activity. Biol. Psychol. 42, 75–85. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  172. Schulz JB, et al. , 1999. Magnetic resonance imaging–based volumetry differentiates idiopathic Parkinson’s syndrome from multiple system atrophy and progressive supranuclear palsy. Ann. Neurol. 45, 65–74. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  173. Sedeno L, et al. , 2016. Brain network organization and social executive performance in frontotemporal dementia. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc. 22, 250–262. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  174. Sedeno L, et al. , 2017. Tackling variability: a multicenter study to provide a gold-standard network approach for frontotemporal dementia. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 3804–3822. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  175. Seeley WW, 2019. The salience network: a neural system for perceiving and responding to homeostatic demands. J. Neurosci. 39, 9878–9882. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  176. Seeley WW, et al. , 2007. Divergent social functioning in behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer disease: reciprocal networks and neuronal evolution. Alzheimer Dis. Assoc. Disord. 21, S50–S57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  177. Shany-Ur T, et al. , 2012. Comprehension of insincere communication in neurodegenerative disease: lies, sarcasm, and theory of mind. Cortex. 48, 1329–1341. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  178. Shany-Ur T, et al. , 2014. Self-awareness in neurodegenerative disease relies on neural structures mediating reward-driven attention. Brain. 137, 2368–2381. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  179. Shih Y-W, et al. , 2019. Effects of positive and negative expectations on human pain perception engage separate but interrelated and dependently regulated cerebral mechanisms. J. Neurosci. 39, 1261–1274. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  180. Shirer WR, et al. , 2012. Decoding subject-driven cognitive states with whole-brain connectivity patterns. Cereb. Cortex 22, 158–165. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  181. Simioni AC, et al. , 2017. Effects of levodopa on corticostriatal circuits supporting working memory in Parkinson’s disease. Cortex. 93, 193–205. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  182. Singmann H, et al. , 2015. Package ‘afex’. Vienna. [Google Scholar]
  183. Smallwood J, et al. , 2021. The default mode network in cognition: a topographical perspective. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 22, 503–513. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  184. Smith R, et al. , 2017. Maintaining the feelings of others in working memory is associated with activation of the left anterior insula and left frontal-parietal control network. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 12, 848–860. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  185. Sollberger M, et al. , 2009. Neural basis of interpersonal traits in neurodegenerative diseases. Neuropsychologia. 47, 2812–2827. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  186. Sollberger M, et al. , 2014. Neural substrates of socioemotional self-awareness in neurodegenerative disease. Brain Behav. 4, 201–214. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  187. Supekar K, Menon V, 2012. Developmental maturation of dynamic causal control signals in higher-order cognition: a neurocognitive network model. PLoS Comput. Biol. 8, e1002374. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  188. Supekar K, et al. , 2008. Network analysis of intrinsic functional brain connectivity in Alzheimer’s disease. PLoS Comput. Biol. 4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  189. Synn A, et al. , 2018. Mental states in moving shapes: distinct cortical and subcortical contributions to theory of mind impairments in dementia. J. Alzheimers Dis. 61, 521–535. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  190. Tait L, et al. , 2020. EEG microstate complexity for aiding early diagnosis of Alzheimer’s disease. Sci. Rep. 10, 1–10. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  191. Takase R, et al. , 2022. Processing time affects sequential memory performance beginning at the level of visual encoding. PLoS One 17, e0265719. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  192. Thornton MA, Conway AR, 2013. Working memory for social information: Chunking or domain-specific buffer? Neuroimage. 70, 233–239. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  193. Toller G, et al. , 2018. Individual differences in socioemotional sensitivity are an index of salience network function. Cortex. 103, 211–223. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  194. Tzourio-Mazoyer N, et al. , 2002. Automated anatomical labeling of activations in SPM using a macroscopic anatomical parcellation of the MNI MRI single-subject brain. Neuroimage. 15, 273–289. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  195. Uddin LQ, 2015. Salience processing and insular cortical function and dysfunction. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 16, 55–61. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  196. Van der Molen M, et al. , 2017. Why don’t you like me? Midfrontal theta power in response to unexpected peer rejection feedback. NeuroImage. 146, 474–483. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  197. van der Velde B, et al. , 2021. The emergence of a theta social brain network during infancy. NeuroImage. 240, 118298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  198. van Loenhoud AC, et al. , 2017. A neuroimaging approach to capture cognitive reserve: application to Alzheimer’s disease. Hum. Brain Mapp. 38, 4703–4715. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  199. Wang P, et al. , 2015. Aberrant intra-and inter-network connectivity architectures in Alzheimer’s disease and mild cognitive impairment. Sci. Rep. 5, 1–12. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  200. Waskom ML, 2021. Seaborn: statistical data visualization. J. Open Source Softw. 6, 3021. [Google Scholar]
  201. Whitwell JL, et al. , 2009. Distinct anatomical subtypes of the behavioural variant of frontotemporal dementia: a cluster analysis study. Brain. 132, 2932–2946. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  202. Wolters AF, et al. , 2019. Resting-state fMRI in Parkinson’s disease patients with cognitive impairment: a meta-analysis. Parkinsonism Relat. Disord. 62, 16–27. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  203. Xin F, Lei X, 2015. Competition between frontoparietal control and default networks supports social working memory and empathy. Soc. Cogn. Affect. Neurosci. 10, 1144–1152. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  204. Yang J, et al. , 2019. Outlier detection: how to threshold outlier scores?. In: Proceedings of the International Conference on Artificial Intelligence, Information Processing and Cloud Computing, pp. 1–6. [Google Scholar]
  205. Yoris A, et al. , 2018. Multilevel convergence of interoceptive impairments in hypertension: new evidence of disrupted body-brain interactions. Hum. Brain Mapp. 39, 1563–1581. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  206. Zhao Q, et al. , 2019. Functional segregation of executive control network and frontoparietal network in Alzheimer’s disease. Cortex. 120, 36–48. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  207. Zhou J, Seeley WW, 2014. Network dysfunction in Alzheimer’s disease and frontotemporal dementia: implications for psychiatry. Biol. Psychiatry 75, 565–573. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  208. Zhou J, et al. , 2010. Divergent network connectivity changes in behavioural variant frontotemporal dementia and Alzheimer’s disease. Brain. 133, 1352–1367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  209. Zou Q, et al. , 2015. Reliability comparison of spontaneous brain activities between BOLD and CBF contrasts in eyes-open and eyes-closed resting states. Neuroimage. 121, 91–105. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

1

Data Availability Statement

The datasets supporting the conclusions of this article are publicly available in the OSF repository, http://osf.io/bx27h (Legaz, 2022).

The datasets generated during and/or analysed during the current study are available in the OSF repository, http://osf.io/bx27h (Legaz, 2022).

RESOURCES