Abstract
Research suggests that discounting of delayed rewards (i.e., tendency to choose smaller immediate rewards over large later rewards) is a promising target of intervention to encourage compliance with public health measures (PHM), such as vaccination compliance. The effects of delay discounting, however, may differ across the types of PHMs, given that the benefits of vaccination, unlike other PHMs (physical distancing, handwashing, and mask-wearing), are more temporally delayed. Here, we examined whether delay discounting predicts engaging in COVID-19 PHMs in approximately 7,000 participants recruited from 13 countries in June–August 2021. After controlling for demographic and distress variables, delay discounting was a negative predictor of vaccination, but a positive predictor of physical distancing (when restrictions are in place) and handwashing. There was no significant association between delay discounting and frequency of mask-wearing. It is possible that increasing vaccination compliance may require greater emphasis on future benefits of vaccination, whereas promotion of physical distancing and hand hygiene may require greater focus on the present moment. Further research is needed to investigate the nature of this relationship and its implications for public health messaging.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1186/s41235-024-00566-6.
Keywords: Decision making, Delay discounting, Public health measures, Vaccination, COVID-19
Introduction
Widespread compliance with public health measures (PHMs) has been critical to containing the COVID-19 pandemic as well as other infectious diseases. Vaccination, mask-wearing, handwashing, and physical distancing are among the most commonly recommended PHMs designed to mitigate the impacts of the pandemic (World Health Organization, 2020). Widespread public compliance with PHMs is necessary to prevent severe illness and death, and alleviate hospital burden during pandemics, and is of increasing importance in light of global population aging. Understanding decisions to engage in protective behaviors during COVID-19 will help inform ways to encourage PHM compliance, with important implications for managing future pandemics.
Here, we investigate a well-established behavioral measure of decision making as a predictor of compliance with different PHMs during the COVID-19 pandemic. Delay discounting is a phenomenon where the subjective value of rewards decreases as a function of delay, such that individuals discount the worth of future rewards relative to immediate ones (Green & Myerson, 2004). Steep or high delay discounting indicates a tendency for immediate gratification and is often associated with impulsivity (Moreira & Barbosa, 2019). This tendency is associated with financial instability (Ruggeri et al., 2022) and problematic health behaviors (e.g., Barlow et al., 2016; Bickel et al., 2019; Daugherty & Brase, 2010; Robles et al., 2011; Rung & Madden, 2018), including those that were heightened during the COVID-19 pandemic (Cutler & Summers, 2020; Halilova et al., 2022; Wu et al., 2021; Zhang & Chen, 2021). By contrast, individuals with shallow or low delay discounting tend to exhibit greater levels of self-control and be better equipped to make decisions that align with their longer-term goals (e.g., Bickel et al., 2018).
There are several reasons why delay discounting is a promising behavioral economic measure for public health purposes and one that may be leveraged to address health crises on a global scale. Incorporating behavioral economic measures, such as delay discounting, into forecasting models of disease spread could enhance the accuracy of predictions and allow for the implementation of targeted interventions to mitigate the spread of COVID-19 (Marin-Lopez et al., 2022). Furthermore, unlike self-report measures that explicitly ask participants to reflect on their decision-making tendencies, delay discounting tasks measure decision-making behavior in a less conspicuous and more objective way (Myerson et al., 2001). As such, these tasks do not rely on participants’ awareness of their decision-making behaviors and are less likely to be affected by social desirability and other response biases (Ruggeri et al., 2024).
The promise of delay discounting as a predictor of compliance with COVID-19 PHMs has been recognized in recent research efforts involving diverse populations from different countries, tested at different times throughout the pandemic (Agrawal et al., 2023; Byrne et al., 2021; Calluso et al., 2021; DeAngelis et al., 2022; Halilova et al., 2022; Hudson et al., 2022; Krawiec et al., 2022; Lloyd et al., 2021; Strickland et al., 2022; Wismans et al., 2021). There is consistency in findings of a negative relationship between delay discounting and vaccination attitudes and status (Halilova et al., 2022; Hudson et al., 2022; Strickland et al., 2022). The relationship between delay discounting and other PHMs (e.g., handwashing/cleaning, physical distancing, and mask-wearing), however, has yielded mixed findings, with some studies showing a negative relationship between discounting of delayed rewards and PHMs (Agrawal et al., 2023; Byrne et al., 2021; DeAngelis et al., 2022; Lloyd et al., 2021), other studies showing a positive relationship between delay discounting and PHM compliance (Calluso et al., 2021; Wismans et al., 2021), and yet other studies indicating no relationship between delay discounting and compliance (Krawiec et al., 2022). As shown in Table 1, the picture is complicated by the study-specific differences, including being conducted in different populations at different time points during the pandemic under varying conditions that were evolving quickly. Moreover, the type of PHM examined and the delay discounting measure used varied across studies. In the current study, we investigated the relationships between delay discounting and multiple PHMs in the same sample of participants, and we recruited sufficient N’s per country (at least ~ 100 per country) to assess whether the effect generalized across countries. We measured delay discounting using the AuC, which is considered to be an atheoretical measure, unlike log-adjusted k values that other studies in this literature have used (Calluso et al., 2021; DeAngelis et al., 2022; Krawiec et al., 2022; Wismans et al., 2021).
Table 1.
Study | PHM(s) studied | Measure of discounting | n | Countr(ies) of data collection | Time of data collection | Correlation of delay discounting with PHMs |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Byrne et al., 2021 | Physical distancing, mask-wearing | Area-under-the-curve | 404 | USA | July–December 2020 | Greater delay discounting associated with less physical distancing and mask-wearing |
Lloyd et al., 2021 | Physical distancing | Delay discounting magnitude effect slope (m), and its intercept (c) | 442 | UK | April–May, 2020 | Greater delay discounting predicted poorer adherence to physical distancing measures |
DeAngelis et al., 2022 | Physical distancing, stockpiling | Log-transformed k value | 3,686 | 96 countries | March–May 2020 | Discounting negatively correlated with physical distancing and positively correlated with stockpiling |
Calluso et al., 2021 | Going out, hand sanitation, use of protective equipment | Log-transformed k value | 353 | Italy | May 2020 | Discounting rate was positively related to compliance physical distancing and mask- and glove-wearing |
Wismans et al., 2021 | Social distancing, hygiene | Log-transformed k value | 6,759 | Belgium, France, Ireland, Italy, the Netherlands, Portugal, Sweden | June 2020 | Discounting rate positively related to social distancing and hygiene compliance |
Krawiec et al., 2022 | Physical distancing, mask-wearing, disinfection | Log-transformed k value | 338 | Poland | December 2020–February 2021 | No significant correlation between delay discounting and any of the PHMs studied |
Agrawal et al., 2023 | Social distancing, mask-wearing | Area-under-the-curve | 12,906 | USA | March 26, 2020; April 2020; June 30, 2020; November 2, 2020 | Discounting rate negatively related to compliance with physical distancing, but not mask-wearing |
Delay discounting emerges across studies as a negative predictor of vaccination status, but the relationship between discounting and other PHMs is less consistent. In addition to the study-specific factors described above, it is possible that there is a more fundamental reason for the different relationships between delay discounting and vaccination versus other PHMs. It is possible that when deciding whether to engage in PHMs, an individual may consider shorter- and longer-term benefits of engaging in those behaviors (e.g., immediate side effects of a vaccine vs. long-term immunity). Importantly, the effect of delay discounting may differ across types of protective behaviors depending on the perceived temporal delay of PHM benefits, which may help to explain the mixed findings in the literature. For example, it has been well publicized that it takes approximately 2 weeks to develop immunity to the virus after receiving the vaccine and the benefits last for months, whereas physically distancing from others results in immediate reduction of virus transmission that is limited to that specific time (e.g., Bernal et al., 2021; Chea et al., 2021; Sun et al., 2022).
The current research examined the relationship between delay discounting and compliance in different PHMs (i.e., vaccination status, handwashing/cleaning, physical distancing, and mask-wearing) in mid-2021 in a large sample of adults from 13 countries that continued to promote PHMs in the face of the ongoing COVID-19 pandemic. To our knowledge, this is the first study to investigate the relationships between delay discounting and vaccination vs. other PHMs in the same sample of participants. We predicted that, after controlling for demographic variables, psychological distress, and intolerance of uncertainty, greater discounting of delayed rewards (i.e., more short-sighted thinking) would be associated with a reduced likelihood of being vaccinated but increased frequency of engaging in other PHMs that provide more immediate benefits, such as handwashing and cleaning, physical distancing, and mask-wearing. The multinational sample also allowed us to explore the consistency of the relationship between the variables across 13 industrialized countries that varied in pandemic severity, vaccination rates, and implementation of PHMs.
Method
Participants
Recruitment was conducted through an online platform (Prolific.co) from June 27, 2021 to August 31, 2021.1 Using Prolific’s built-in inclusion/exclusion filters, the study was available only to users meeting the following inclusion criteria: aged 18 years or older, fluent in English, normally residing in one of 14 target countries2 across North America, Europe, Australasia, and Africa, and free from neurological impairments or learning disabilities. Target countries were selected with the goal of capturing varying COVID-19 impact severity and a range of government mandates in place at the time of testing (Mathieu et al., 2020). Countries with fewer than 200 active participants on the recruitment platform were not included in the target list. Of the 7,667 participants who provided informed consent, data from 320 individuals were excluded due to failure to meet inclusion criteria (e.g., currently residing in a non-targeted country); non-completion of the survey (i.e., those who completed less than 95% of the survey); and/or responding incorrectly to more than one attention check item (see below). Data from 421 participants from South Africa were excluded due to a substantially different approach in government response, limited vaccine availability, and additional obstacles to compliance with PHMs (e.g., lack of access to clean water; Staunton et al., 2020). The final data set was composed of 6,926 participants who were on average 28.62 (sd = 10.18) years old; 58% were female, 40% male, and 2% non-binary. Approximately 35% of the sample had achieved secondary level education, 48% had an undergraduate degree, and 16% of the sample achieved postgraduate education. Approximately 23% of the sample self-identified as essential workers. Average subjective rating of relative income among participants (on a 100-point sliding scale; Adler et al., 2000; Smith et al., 2019) was 36.31 (sd = 23.8).
Measures
PHMs during COVID-19
Participants were asked a series of questions about their compliance with protective behaviors during COVID-19. Participants chose between five options in response to the question about their vaccination status: 1 = yes, I have received all necessary doses, 2 = yes, although I require another dose, 3 = no, but I am planning to get vaccinated, 4 = no, I am not planning to get vaccinated, 5 = prefer not to say. A binary vaccination status variable was created, distinguishing between those who were vaccinated (fully or partially) or not (including both those who were planning and not planning to get vaccinated in the future). Participants were asked to indicate on a 5-point scale the frequency with which they engaged in following five behaviors over the past week: (1) physical contact with (i.e., touching) and (2) being in close proximity with people they do not live with (0 = never, 1 = once, 2 = every several days, 3 = daily, 4 = more than once daily); (3) cleaning and disinfecting frequently touched surfaces (e.g., tables, doorknobs, light switches; 0 = not at all, 1 = every several days, 2 = every other day, 3 = daily, 4 = more than once daily); (4) cleaning their hands with sanitizer or soap and water (0 = never, 1 = a few times a week, 2 = daily, 3 = several times a day, 4 = at least once an hour); and (5) mask-wearing (0 = never, 1 = once, 2 = every several days, 3 = daily, 4 = more than once daily). The mask-wearing item and two composite variables—physical distancing (sum of reverse-coded physical contact and close proximity items), and cleaning (sum of cleaning and disinfecting frequently touched surfaces and cleaning hands)—were used in analyses.
Delay discounting task
In this well-established intertemporal choice procedure (Ciaramelli et al., 2021; Halilova et al., 2022; Mok et al., 2020), participants viewed pairs of monetary amounts and were asked to choose between smaller, immediate rewards which varied between trials, and a larger, delayed reward of $2,000. Participants were asked to make six choices at each of seven delays for the larger reward (waiting 1 week, 1 month, 3 months, 6 months, 1 year, 3 years, and 10 years before receiving the $2000 reward). An iterative, adjusting-amount procedure was used in which the amount of the immediate reward was increased or decreased based on the participant’s previous choice at that delay, converging on the amount of the immediate reward equivalent in subjective value to the delayed reward. The first adjustment was half of the difference between the immediate and delayed amounts presented on the first trial, with each subsequent adjustment being half of the preceding adjustment. For example, in the condition where a future reward of $2000 could be received in 3 years, the first choice presented to the participants would be “$1000 right now or $2000 in 3 years.” If the participant chose “$2000 in 3 years,” the choice on the second trial would be “$1500 right now or $2000 in 3 years.” If the participant then chose “$1500 right now”, the choice on the third trial would be “$1250 right now or $2000 in 3 years.” Following the sixth and final trial of each condition, the subjective value of the delayed reward was estimated as the amount of the immediate reward that would be presented on a seventh trial. A larger subjective value of the delayed reward indicated less discounting of delayed rewards. A smaller subjective value indicated a more short-sighted decision making. Degree of discounting was measured by examining the subjective values of reward across the seven delays and computing Area-under-the-curve (AuC), a single, theoretically-neutral measure of discounting (Myerson et al., 2001). Another advantage of using AuC as the measure of delay discounting is that it tends to generate approximately normally distributed scores (Myerson et al., 2001). The scores range from 0 to 1, with lower AuC representing a greater discounting rate (i.e., more short-sighted decision making).
Demographic questionnaire
Participants completed a demographic questionnaire assessing current country of residence, age, gender (female/male/non-binary), highest level of education obtained (no schooling/primary schooling/secondary schooling/undergraduate degree or professional equivalent/postgraduate degree), essential worker status (yes/no), and personal income. For essential worker status, participants indicated if they worked in an occupations supplying critical services during the pandemic: government; health and safety (e.g., healthcare, emergency response); utilities (e.g., water, energy, sanitation, transport, communications); food (e.g., supermarkets); and manufacturing. A subjective measure of relative income was used, such that participants estimated their current income on a sliding scale (0–100) marked by points representing low (0), average (50), and high (100) incomes in their own country/region (Adler et al., 2000; Smith et al., 2019).
Psychological distress index
Presence and severity of anxiety and depressive symptoms were assessed with the Generalized Anxiety Disorder 7-item (GAD-7) scale (Spitzer et al., 2006) and the Patient Health Questionnaire 9-item (PHQ-9) scale (Kroenke et al., 2001), respectively. Participants rated the frequency of symptoms experienced over the past two weeks on a four-point scale (0 = not at all; 3 = nearly every day). For each scale, a total score was computed, where higher scores reflect more severe symptoms. Total scores from these measures were standardized and then summed to create a psychological distress index.
Intolerance of uncertainty scale (IUS-12)
The IUS-12 is a 12-item measure of one’s difficulties tolerating uncertainty (Carleton et al., 2007). Participants used a 6-point scale (0 = not at all characteristic of me; 5 = entirely characteristic of me) to respond to items measuring two factors of intolerance of uncertainty: prospective anxiety, the cognitive component of intolerance of uncertainty that indicates one’s tendency to worry about future events (e.g., “I always want to know what the future has in store for me”) and inhibitory anxiety, the behavioral component of intolerance of uncertainty that represents avoidance tendencies in the face of uncertainty (e.g., “I must get away from all uncertain situations”; Carleton et al., 2007). Intolerance of uncertainty score was calculated as a sum of participants’ responses to IUS-12, ranging from 0 to 60.
Attention checks
Three items from the Conscientious Responder Scale (CRS; Marjanovic et al., 2014) were included at select points within the survey to identify random responders (e.g., “To answer this question, please choose option three, neither agree nor disagree.”).
Results
Descriptive statistics of the variables, including the frequency of engaging in physical distancing, cleaning, and mask-wearing broken down by vaccination status, are provided in Table 2 and Fig. 1. Country-level summary data are provided in Supplementary Materials. The correlations between physical distancing, cleaning, and mask-wearing are provided in Table 3.
Table 2.
Variables | Vaccinated (n = 4355) |
Unvaccinated (n = 2571) |
---|---|---|
Age | 29.58 (10.95) | 27.01 (8.49) |
Gender (female/male/non-binary) | 2661/1597/93 | 1333/1173/41 |
Education level (no education/primary/secondary/undergraduate/postgraduate) | 5/12/1495/2090/752 | 4/5/936/1249/357 |
Income (0–100) | 37.74 (24.15) | 35.21 (23.80) |
Essential workers (% yes) | 26.96% | 16.06% |
Intolerance of uncertainty | 35.19 (9.28) | 35.04 (9.31) |
Area-under-curve | 0.41 (0.25) | 0.37(0.25) |
Psychological distress | − 0.03 (1.88) | 0.04 (1.89) |
Cleaning (Range; Median) | 0–8; 4 | 0–8; 4 |
Physical distancing (Range; Median) | 0–8; 4 | 0–8; 5 |
Mask-wearing (Median) | 0–4; 3 | 0–4; 3 |
Table 3.
Cleaning | Mask-wearing | |
---|---|---|
Physical distancing | − 0.01 | − 0.01 |
Cleaning | – | 0.22 |
Vaccination
A multilevel logistic regression model was constructed using R packages lme4 (Bates et al., 2012) and lmerTest (Kuznetsova et al., 2017) with vaccination status (unvaccinated vs. vaccinated) as the outcome variable, and age, gender, education level, income, psychological distress index, essential worker status, intolerance of uncertainty, and AuC as predictors. To account for possible systematic differences across countries (e.g., COVID-related severity, population vaccination rates, government response), each participant’s vaccination status (Level 1) was nested within country (Level 2). A likelihood ratio test showed that the model accounted for significantly more variance in the data compared to an unconditional intercept-only model, χ2(13) = 225.77, p < 0.001. The tendency to choose larger future rewards over smaller immediate ones (larger AuC) significantly increased the odds of being vaccinated after controlling for other variables in the model (p < 0.001; Table 4). All of the continuous control variables in the model positively predicted the likelihood of being vaccinated (p < 0.05), with the exception of the psychological distress index (p = 0.754) and intolerance of uncertainty (p = 0.204), which were not significant predictors. After controlling for other variables, the results show that self-identifying as a male, compared to female, was associated with a significantly lower likelihood of being vaccinated. After controlling for other variables in the model, completing postgraduate studies compared to undergraduate studies was associated with greater likelihood of being vaccinated. The association between AuC and vaccination status was further explored by country. As evident in Fig. 2, the association between AuC and vaccination status was stronger in countries where the participant vaccination rate was moderate (e.g., France, Germany, Italy, the Netherlands, Australia). In contrast, this association appears weaker in countries where the participant vaccination rate was either very high (e.g., Canada, USA, and UK) or very low (e.g., Portugal) in our sample represented by nearly horizontal lines at the top or bottom of the graphs in Fig. 1, respectively. Poland appears to be the only country in our sample that showed a negative relationship between AuC and vaccination status.
Table 4.
Fixed effects | b | SE | z | p | OR | 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Intercept | 0.26 | 0.35 | 0.72 | .469 | 1.29 | [0.65, 2.58] |
Age† | 0.28 | 0.04 | 7.74 | < .001 | 1.32 | [1.23, 1.42] |
Gender (male) | − 0.31 | 0.06 | − 4.83 | < .001 | 0.73 | [0.65, 0.83] |
Gender (non-binary) | 0.32 | 0.22 | 1.44 | .149 | 1.38 | [0.89, 2.14] |
Education level (no schooling) | − 0.80 | 0.76 | − 1.06 | .287 | 0.45 | [0.10, 1.97] |
Education level (primary) | 0.24 | 0.60 | 0.40 | .691 | 1.27 | [0.39, 4.13] |
Education level (secondary) | − 0.05 | 0.07 | − 0.74 | .459 | 0.95 | [0.83, 1.09] |
Education level (postgraduate) | 0.33 | 0.09 | 3.60 | < .001 | 1.39 | [1.16, 1.66] |
Relative income† | 0.08 | 0.03 | 2.41 | .016 | 1.08 | [1.02, 1.15] |
Essential worker status | 0.51 | 0.08 | 6.43 | < .001 | 1.67 | [1.43, 1.95] |
Psychological distress† | − 0.01 | 0.04 | − 0.31 | .754 | 0.99 | [0.92, 1.06] |
Intolerance of uncertainty† | 0.04 | 0.04 | 1.27 | .204 | 1.05 | [0.98, 1.12] |
Delay discounting (AuC)† | 0.14 | 0.03 | 4.52 | < .001 | 1.15 | [1.08, 1.22] |
Random effects | Estimate | SD |
---|---|---|
Intercept error variance (country) | 1.58 | 1.26 |
Note. † The variable was standardized. AuC = Area-under-the-curve, CI = confidence interval; OR = odds ratio; SD = standard deviation; SE = standard error of the mean. Female was used as the reference category for gender. Undergraduate level of education was used as the reference category for level of education
Cleaning
Given that the remaining PHMs are measured on an ordinal scale, ordinal R package (Christensen, 2023) was used to implement cumulative link mixed model analysis. A multilevel model was constructed with the cleaning composite score as the outcome variable, and age, gender, education level, income, psychological distress index, essential worker status, intolerance of uncertainty, vaccination status, and AuC as predictors. Each participant’s frequency of cleaning behaviors (Level 1) nested within country (Level 2). A likelihood ratio test showed that the model accounted for significantly more variance in the data compared to an unconditional intercept-only model, χ2(14) = 160.72, p < 0.001. AuC negatively predicted the frequency of cleaning and handwashing after controlling for other variables in the model (p < 0.001; Table 5), indicating that shorter-sighted thinking was associated with increased engagement in cleaning behaviors. In contrast, female gender (compared to male), relative income, essential worker status, psychological distress, intolerance of uncertainty, and vaccination status were associated with a higher frequency of cleaning behaviors. The association between AuC and frequency of engaging in cleaning was further explored by country. As evident in Fig. 3, the relationship between the variables was negative in 10 out of 13 countries, while there was no relationship between AuC and frequency of engaging in cleaning behaviors in Germany, New Zealand, and Poland.
Table 5.
Coefficients | b | SE | z | P | OR | 95% CI | |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age† | 0.03 | 0.02 | 1.38 | .168 | 1.03 | [− 0.01, 0.08] | |
Gender (male) | − 0.31 | 0.05 | − 6.71 | < .001 | 0.73 | [− 0.41, − 0.22] | |
Gender (non-binary) | − 0.13 | 0.15 | − 0.83 | .408 | 0.88 | [− 0.43, 0.17] | |
Education level (no schooling) | − 2.48 | 0.61 | − 4.04 | < .001 | 0.08 | [− 3.68, − 1.28] | |
Education level (primary) | 0.31 | 0.46 | 0.68 | .499 | 1.36 | [− 0.59, 1.21] | |
Education level (secondary) | − 0.05 | 0.05 | − 0.94 | .347 | 0.95 | [− 0.14, 0.05] | |
Education level (postgraduate) | − 0.10 | 0.06 | − 1.54 | .125 | 0.91 | [− 0.22, 0.03] | |
Relative income† | 0.07 | 0.02 | 2.96 | .003 | 1.07 | [0.02, 0.12] | |
Essential worker status | 0.18 | 0.05 | 3.49 | < .001 | 1.20 | [0.08, 0.29] | |
Psychological distress† | 0.09 | 0.03 | 3.51 | < .001 | 1.10 | [0.04, 0.14] | |
Intolerance of uncertainty† | 0.07 | 0.03 | 2.77 | .005 | 1.07 | [0.02, 0.12] | |
Vaccination Status | 0.16 | 0.05 | 3.06 | .002 | 1.18 | [0.06, 0.27] | |
Delay discounting (AuC)† | − 0.08 | 0.02 | − 3.75 | < .001 | 0.92 | [− 0.13, − 0.04] |
Random effects | Estimate | SD | |
---|---|---|---|
Intercept error variance (country) | 0.21 | 0.46 |
† The variable was scaled to improve model fit. AuC = Area-under-the-curve; CI = confidence interval; SD = standard deviation; SE = standard error of the mean. Female was used as the reference category for gender. Undergraduate level of education was used as the reference category for level of education. Unvaccinated was used as the reference category for vaccination status
Physical distancing
A cumulative link mixed model was constructed with the physical distancing composite score as the outcome variable, and the same set of predictors and the same structure of random effects as in the earlier models. A likelihood ratio test shows that the model accounted for significantly more variance in the data compared to an unconditional intercept-only model, χ2(14) = 477.57, p < 0.001. Delay discounting (AuC) did not significantly predict the frequency of physical distancing after controlling for other variables in the model (p = 0.106; Table 6). Age, income, essential worker status, psychological distress, and intolerance of uncertainty significantly predicted the frequency of engaging in physical distancing (Table 6). The association between AuC and frequency of physical distancing behaviors was explored by country (Fig. 4). The UK was the only country with a positive slope in the relationship between AuC and physical distancing; notably this was also the only country in the sample that did not have physical distancing restrictions in place during the time of data collection (Mathieu et al., 2020). Running the model again without the UK data resulted in AuC becoming a significant negative predictor of frequency of physical distancing, b = − 0.06, SE = 0.02, z = − 2.35, p = 0.019, OR = 0.95, 95% CI [− 0.10, − 0.01], indicating that shorter-sighted thinking was associated with increased engagement in physical distancing. It was also notable that the associations between physical distancing and AuC were strongest in France, Spain, and Portugal (Fig. 4), countries with the most stringent physical distancing mandates (i.e., stay-at-home was a requirement with the exception of essentials) in place during the time of data collection (Mathieu et al., 2020).
Table 6.
Coefficients | b | SE | z | p | OR | 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age† | 0.27 | 0.02 | 11.38 | < .001 | 1.31 | [0.22, 0.32] |
Gender (male) | − 0.04 | 0.05 | − 0.95 | .344 | 0.96 | [− 0.13, 0.05] |
Gender (non-binary) | 0.36 | 0.15 | 2.35 | .019 | 1.43 | [0.06, 0.65] |
Education level (no schooling) | − 0.31 | 0.53 | − 0.58 | .565 | 0.74 | [− 1.35, 0.74] |
Education level (primary) | 0.92 | 0.44 | 2.10 | .036 | 2.51 | [0.06, 1.78] |
Education level (secondary) | − 0.05 | 0.05 | − 1.03 | .301 | 0.95 | [− 0.15, 0.05] |
Education level (postgraduate) | 0.14 | 0.06 | 2.17 | .030 | 1.15 | [0.01, 0.26] |
Relative income† | − 0.11 | 0.02 | − 4.76 | < .001 | 0.90 | [− 0.15, − 0.06] |
Essential worker status | − 0.83 | 0.05 | − 15.68 | < .001 | 0.43 | [− 0.94, − 0.73] |
Psychological distress† | 0.05 | 0.03 | 1.87 | .062 | 1.05 | [− 0.01, 0.10] |
Intolerance of uncertainty† | 0.14 | 0.02 | 5.46 | < .001 | 1.15 | [0.09, 0.19] |
Vaccination Status | − 0.10 | 0.05 | − 1.83 | .068 | 0.91 | [− 0.20, 0.01] |
Delay discounting (AuC)† | − 0.04 | 0.02 | − 1.62 | .106 | 0.97 | [− 0.08, 0.01] |
Random effects | Estimate | SD |
---|---|---|
Intercept error variance (country) | 0.26 | 0.51 |
†The variable was standardized. AuC = Area-under-the-curve; CI = confidence interval; SD = standard deviation; SE = standard error of the mean. Female was used as the reference category for gender. Undergraduate level of education was used as the reference category for level of education. Unvaccinated was used as the reference category for vaccination status
Mask-wearing
A multilevel model was constructed with mask-wearing as the outcome variable and the same set of predictors and the same structure of random effects as the earlier models. A likelihood ratio test showed that the model accounted for significantly more variance in the data compared to an unconditional intercept-only model, χ2(14) = 202.77, p < 0.001. Delay discounting (AuC) did not significantly predict the frequency of mask-wearing after controlling for other variables in the model (p = 0.071; Table 7). Among control variables, gender (female compared to male), essential worker status, psychological distress, and vaccination status were significant positive predictors of frequency of mask-wearing. On the other hand, age was a significant negative predictor of mask-wearing in the model. The association between AuC and frequency of mask-wearing was explored by country (Fig. 5). There were no strong relationships between delay discounting and mask-wearing among participants from most countries in our sample. In our sample, France and New Zealand appear as the countries with the strongest associations between AuC and mask-wearing, positive and negative, respectively. However, the association is not significant when fitting the model with the same set of control variables individually for these countries (for France, b = 0.36, p = 0.10 and for New Zealand, b = − 0.22, p = 0.15).
Table 7.
Coefficients | b | SE | z | p | OR | 95% CI |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Age† | − 0.07 | 0.03 | − 2.79 | .005 | 0.93 | [− 0.12, − 0.02] |
Gender (male) | − 0.16 | 0.05 | − 3.37 | < .001 | 0.85 | [− 0.30, − 0.07] |
Gender (non-binary) | 0.24 | 0.17 | 1.43 | .153 | 1.27 | [− 0.09, 0.57] |
Education level (no schooling) | − 1.06 | 0.56 | − 1.87 | .059 | 0.35 | [− 2.15, 0.04] |
Education level (primary) | − 0.27 | 0.45 | − 0.61 | .539 | 0.76 | [− 1.15, 0.60] |
Education level (secondary) | − 0.09 | 0.05 | − 1.81 | .071 | 0.91 | [− 0.20, 0.01] |
Education level (postgraduate) | 0.05 | 0.07 | 0.79 | .429 | 1.05 | [− 0.08, 0.18] |
Relative income† | 0.01 | 0.02 | 0.32 | .753 | 1.01 | [− 0.04, 0.06] |
Essential worker status | 0.37 | 0.06 | 6.63 | < .001 | 1.45 | [0.26, 0.48] |
Psychological distress† | 0.13 | 0.03 | 4.81 | < .001 | 1.14 | [0.08, 0.19] |
Intolerance of uncertainty† | 0.02 | 0.02 | 0.75 | .450 | 1.02 | [− 0.03, 0.07] |
Vaccination status | 0.44 | 0.06 | 7.72 | < .001 | 1.55 | [0.33, 0.55] |
Delay discounting (AuC)† | − 0.04 | 0.02 | − 1.81 | .071 | 0.96 | [− 0.09, 0.01] |
Random effects | Estimate | SD |
---|---|---|
Intercept error variance (country) | 1.17 | 1.08 |
†The variable was scaled to improve model fit. AuC = Area-under-the-curve; CI = confidence interval; OR = odds ratio; SD = standard deviation; SE = standard error of the mean. Female was used as the reference category for gender. Undergraduate level of education was used as the reference category for level of education. Unvaccinated was used as the reference category for vaccination status
Discussion
The current research investigated delay discounting as a predictor of PHMs, including protective behaviors with more delayed benefits (i.e., vaccination) and those with more immediate benefits (i.e., cleaning, physical distancing, and mask-wearing). A large multinational sample allowed us to detect differences in PHM compliance across 13 countries. As predicted, discounting of delayed rewards was a significant positive predictor of vaccination status: individuals who tend to choose more delayed rewards over smaller immediate rewards (i.e., far-sighted thinking) were more likely to be vaccinated (see also Halilova et al., 2022). Delay discounting was a significant predictor of the frequency of cleaning and handwashing behaviors. Also as predicted, the direction of the relationship was opposite to that for vaccination: individuals who tend to choose more immediate rewards over larger later rewards (i.e., near-sighted thinking) engaged in cleaning behaviors more frequently. Near-sighted thinking was also a significant positive predictor of physical distancing in the countries tested, other than the UK, where delay discounting was instead a negative predictor of physical distancing. Interestingly, this was the only country in our sample where there were no physical distancing mandates in place during the time of data collection (Mathieu et al., 2020). Lastly, delay discounting did not significantly predict likelihood of mask-wearing over and above the control variables.
The association between delay discounting and COVID-19 vaccination status extends recent findings in a subsample of the participants tested in the current study (Halilova et al., 2022) and is consistent with findings from other recent studies on the topic (Hudson et al., 2022; Strickland et al., 2022). The results are also consistent with research showing that even though nonmonetary outcomes (e.g., health) are generally discounted more steeply than monetary outcomes (Baker et al., 2003; Odum et al., 2020), discounting has trait-like qualities and may be context-independent (Odum et al., 2020). The findings suggest that delay discounting is an important behavioral economic predictor of compliance with PHMs designed to curb the spread of the virus.
The findings were somewhat more mixed when it comes to other PHMs. As hypothesized, the tendency to choose more immediate smaller rewards over larger later rewards predicted more frequent cleaning and handwashing behaviors across most countries. The findings are consistent with previous research showing a positive association between near-sighted thinking and hand hygiene in Belgium, France, Italy, the Netherlands, Portugal, and Sweden (Calluso et al., 2021; Wismans et al., 2021). Exploration of this relationship across the 13 countries revealed that delay discounting was not a significant predictor of cleaning behaviors in Germany, New Zealand, and Poland. This result could be explained by country-specific variability in compliance with hand hygiene during COVID-19 (Szczuka et al., 2021). Although there is no previous research systematically investigating the relationship between delay discounting and PHMs in Germany and New Zealand, our findings are consistent with previous research by Krawiec et al. (2022) showing no significant relationship between delay discounting and hand hygiene in Poland. This finding helps to rule out study-specific explanations (e.g., differences in research design, language, measures used) for the discrepancies in previous findings (e.g., Calluso et al., 2021; Krawiec et al., 2022; Wismans et al., 2021) related to the relationship between delay discounting and cleaning behaviors. This suggests that there may be a country-level variable moderating this relationship, which can be further explored in future research. For example, it is possible that severity of penalties for mandate violations could be one such variable. To the best of our knowledge, there is no global data source detailing such penalties and the enforcement of penalties varied across jurisdictions and institutions (e.g., workplaces). Future research would benefit from investigating the role of mandate violation penalties and their enforcement on PHMs and its relationship with delay discounting at a more regional level.
We also found that the tendency to choose immediate smaller rewards over larger later rewards predicted more frequent engagement in physical distancing behaviors during COVID-19, but only after the UK was excluded from the analysis due to the absence of physical distancing mandates. The findings are consistent with previous research showing a positive association between near-sighted thinking and willingness to engage in immediate protective measures (Calluso et al., 2021; Wismans et al., 2021). Although unexpected, the finding of a negative relationship between near-sighted decision making and physical distancing in the UK, where there were no physical distancing restrictions in place at the time of data collection in 2021, is also consistent with previous findings in a UK sample by Lloyd et al. (2021). It may be that when free to exercise personal choice over whether to physically distance or not, different factors are considered than when deciding to comply with PHMs that are mandated. Moreover, the removal of mandates may signal that immediate safety concerns have resolved and, thus, perhaps it is not surprising that people who tended to engage in physical distancing in this context were the ones who discounted the future less (i.e., those who tended to prioritize longer-term rewards). It should be noted that this potential moderating effect of mandates on the association between delay discounting and physical distancing limits generalizability of our findings to times when mandates are not in place. Future research would benefit from investigating more directly the moderating effects of various government mandates on the relationship between delay discounting and PHM compliance.
Interestingly, the finding of the positive relationship between delay discounting and physical distancing contradicts the results reported by Agrawal et al. (2023), who observed the opposite effect. One potential explanation for this discrepancy could be the timing of data collection. Agrawal et al. collected their data in 2020, a period before vaccines became widely available, and thus it is likely that at this time, adherence to physical distancing measures and mask-wearing were the primary protective strategies against infection. In contrast, our dataset was collected after vaccines became available, providing participants with more options in terms of engaging in protective behaviors. It is also possible that differences in measures, such as varying monetary amounts on delay discounting tasks and different scales to quantify PHMs, may also have contributed to the discrepant findings. Further exploration and reconciliation of these findings could provide valuable insights into the interplay between temporal discounting and health behaviors in varying contexts.
Although unexpected, the nonsignificant relationship between mask-wearing and delay discounting is consistent with some previous findings (Agrawal et al., 2023; Krawiec et al., 2022), but not others (Byrne et al., 2021; Calluso et al., 2021). One explanation is that compared to others protective measures (e.g., physical distancing and handwashing), mask-wearing mandates are socially enforced as non-compliance is easily detectable, thereby restricting people’s freedom of choice relating to compliance with this PHM. For example, if one is not wearing a mask in a hospital where masks are required, one may be asked to either put on their mask or be denied service. On the other hand, monitoring and enforcing compliance with physical distancing in public venues may be a greater challenge. Compliance with physical distancing, cleaning, and handwashing may therefore depend to a greater extent on one’s decision-making process than compliance with mask-wearing. It is also possible that because mask-wearing behavior is more observable compared to other PHMs, it is more likely to be subject to social conformity (Mladenović et al., 2023). In other words, people’s decisions to wear masks in public places may be influenced to a greater degree by social norms, rather than short- and long-term personal costs and benefits.
Another possible explanation for the different patterns of delay discounting in relation to vaccination vs. cleaning and, when mandates exist, physical distancing is risk compensation, the idea that individuals adjust their behavior to maintain a preferred level of risk (Mantzari et al., 2020). For example, if individuals perceive themselves as being at lower risk of contracting COVID-19 due to being vaccinated, they may engage in riskier behaviors, such as reduced compliance with physical distancing, mask-wearing, or hygiene protocols. We view this explanation as unlikely due to the positive relationship between vaccination and other PHMs. Consistent with other research (e.g., Goldszmidt et al., 2021), we do not observe evidence of risk compensations among participants in our sample.
Patterns of delay discounting might also differ across various PHMs due to the cost of engaging in protective behaviors in the present moment (Petherick et al., 2021). Given that mask-wearing is a relatively low-cost protective behavior, individuals may readily implement it without needing to assess the short- and long-term costs and benefits of that behavior. On the other hand, cleaning and handwashing behaviors may be more time-consuming (i.e., costly) to engage in and require an assessment of costs and benefits (e.g., spending the time engaging in other more enjoyable activities). Mandated physical distancing may be viewed as a higher-cost behavior that requires one to assess costs and benefits of avoiding interacting with others (e.g., missing an important event, working remotely). Additionally, it is worth noting that various PHMs can have short- and long-term gains and losses that may vary at an individual level, and that future research is needed to sort out these complexities.
The mixed findings in relation to different PHMs are consistent with previous research that indicates the importance of distinguishing between different protective behaviors, as some of them may be differently affected by one’s personality, demographic factors (e.g., Choi et al., 2022; MacIntyre et al., 2021), and economic considerations (Petherick et al., 2021). In a recent meta-analysis, intention–behavior relationships of physical distancing, hand hygiene, and mask-wearing were assessed (Liang et al., 2022). Although several studies reported a positive intention–behavior relationship when it comes to physical distancing and hand hygiene, there was a nonsignificant intention–behavior relationship found for mask-wearing (Liang et al., 2022). Overall, the literature suggests that decisions to comply with different PHMs may rely on different mechanisms and be influenced by different factors.
The multinational nature of the sample also allowed us to examine consistency of the relationship between delay discounting and frequency of engaging in PHMs across countries, helping to begin reconciling discrepancies in previous studies and exploring other factors influencing compliance with protective behaviors (e.g., government mandates). For example, examining the relationship across all of the PHMs in the current study, it appears that in Poland there is almost no association between discounting of delayed rewards and PHM compliance, which is consistent with previous literature (Krawiec et al., 2022). This discrepant finding suggests the need for further investigation of the impact of economic and cultural factors that have been previously found to influence delay discounting on PHM compliance (Du et al.., 2002; Ishii et al., 2016; Ruggeri et al., 2022).
Conclusion and future directions
Overall, delay discounting is a behavioral economic measure that predicts compliance with most COVID-related PHMs, including vaccination, cleaning and handwashing, and, when government restrictions are in place, physical distancing. Temporal delay of protective benefits of PHMs may determine the direction of the relationship between delay discounting and decisions to engage in the PHMs. This is the first study, to our knowledge, demonstrating the different relationships between delay discounting and vaccination acceptance and adherence to other protective measures within the same sample of participants. Delay discounting may be a useful behavioral feature to consider in constructing future forecasting models of the spread of COVID-19, as the existing models have been criticized for lack of integration of behavioral measures accounting for compliance with protective measures against the disease spread (Chen et al., 2020).
Given that delay discounting is a modifiable characteristic, future research should focus on investigating the effectiveness of different interventions targeting delay discounting (e.g., cuing individuals to imagine the future; Bromberg et al., 2017; Ciaramelli et al., 2021; Mok et al., 2020; Rung & Madden, 2018) to improve the compliance with various PHMs. The findings inform future policies designed to encourage PHM compliance and reinforce the utility of behavioral economic measures in orienting people toward making healthier choices that have global societal benefits. Future research would also benefit from empirically testing nonmonetary delay discounting when it comes to vaccination to assess whether individuals place greater emphasis on short-term vs. long-term rewards.
Supplementary Information
Acknowledgments
Significance statement
Human decision-making biases, particularly those that are correctable, should be factored into approaches addressing global health crises, but are not always considered. A near-universal tendency to choose a smaller immediate reward over a larger delayed reward, known as delay discounting, is a promising behavioral marker of health-related behaviors. By testing a large multi-national sample of participants during the COVID-19 pandemic, we show that delay discounting predicts various public health measures but in different ways. Specifically, individuals who tend to choose larger later rewards over more immediate smaller ones are more likely to be vaccinated, but less likely to engage in cleaning, handwashing, and (when restrictions are in place) physical distancing. No significant association was detected between delay discounting and frequency of mask-wearing. The results have potential implications for how to best promote engagement in different PHMs.
Abbreviations
- PHM
Public health measure
- AuC
Area-under-the-curve
- IUS-12
Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale-12
Author contributions
Julia G. Halilova was involved in conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, project administration, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review & editing. Samuel Fynes-Clinton helped in conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, methodology, project administration, writing—review & editing. Donna Rose Addis contributed to conceptualization, data curation, funding acquisition, investigation, methodology, project administration, supervision, writing—original draft, writing—review & editing. R. Shayna Rosenbaum assisted in conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, funding acquisition, investigation, methodology, project administration, supervision, writing—original draft, writing—review & editing.
Funding
Research reported in this manuscript was funded by the Canadian Institutes of Health Research (CIHR) Canadian 2019 Novel Coronavirus (COVID-19) Rapid Research Funding Opportunity Operating Grant (Grant 172681) and the York University-Beneva Anxiety Research Fund to R.S.R. and D.R.A. D.R.A is supported by the Canada 150 Research Chairs Program. R.S.R. is supported by a York Research Chair in Cognitive Neuroscience of Memory.
Availability of data and materials
Anonymized, raw, and cleaned data have been deposited in a public repository hosted by the Open Science Framework https://osf.io/nyt4g/?view_only=7482638ff0bd4963867ed3b1d6f0833d. The materials used in this study are either described in the method section or publicly available.
Declarations
Ethics approval and consent to participate
The research was approved by the York University and Baycrest Research Ethics Boards REB# 19-07 for research with human participants, and all research was conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Prior to their participation in the study, all of the participants signed a consent form, confirming their willingness to participate in the research.
Consent for publication
All participants provided their consent for their anonymized data to be presented in scientific conferences, journal articles, and published on the open access repository, Open Science Framework.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
A subset of the data (n = 5,193) collected from June 27, 2021 to July 16, 2021 was previously reported in Halilova et al., 2022.
USA, Canada, Mexico, UK, Italy, France, Portugal, the Netherlands, Spain, Germany, Poland, Australia, New Zealand, and South Africa.
Publisher's Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Donna Rose Addis and R. Shayna Rosenbaum have contributed equally to this work.
References
- Adler NE, Epel ES, Castellazzo G, Ickovics JR. Relationship of subjective and objective social status with psychological and physiological functioning: Preliminary data in healthy, White women. Health Psychology. 2000;19:586–592. doi: 10.1037/0278-6133.19.6.586. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Agrawal M, Peterson JC, Cohen JD, Griffiths TL. Stress, intertemporal choice, and mitigation behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General. 2023;152(9):2695–2702. doi: 10.1037/xge0001417. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Baker F, Johnson MW, Bickel WK. Delay discounting in current and never-before cigarette smokers: Similarities and differences across commodity, sign, and magnitude. Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 2003;112(3):382–392. doi: 10.1037/0021-843X.112.3.382. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Barlow P, McKee M, Reeves A, Galea G, Stuckler D. Time-discounting and tobacco smoking: A systematic review and network analysis. International Journal of Epidemiology. 2017;46(3):860–869. doi: 10.1093/ije/dyw233. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bates, D., Maechler, M., & Bolker, B. (2012). lme4: Linear mixed-effects models using S4 classes (R Package). http://cran.r-project.org/web/packages/lme4/index.html
- Bernal JL, Gower C, Andrews N, Public Health England Delta Variant Vaccine Effectiveness Study Group Effectiveness of COVID-19 vaccines against the B.1.617.2 (Delta) variant. The New England Journal of Medicine. 2021;385(25):e92. doi: 10.1056/NEJMc2113090. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bickel WK, Athamneh LN, Basso JC, Mellis AM, DeHart WB, Craft WH, Pope D. Excessive discounting of delayed reinforcers as a trans-disease process: Update on the state of the science. Current Opinion in Psychology. 2019;30:59–64. doi: 10.1016/j.copsyc.2019.01.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bickel WK, Moody LN, Koffarnus M, Thomas JG, Wing R. Self-control as measured by delay discounting is greater among successful weight losers than controls. Journal of Behavioral Medicine. 2018;41(6):891–896. doi: 10.1007/s10865-018-9936-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bromberg U, Lobatcheva M, Peters J. Episodic future thinking reduces temporal discounting in healthy adolescents. PLoS ONE. 2017;12(11):e0188079. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0188079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Byrne KA, Six SG, Anaraky RG, Harris MW, Winterlind EL. Risk-taking unmasked: Using risky choice and temporal discounting to explain COVID-19 preventative behaviors. PLoS ONE. 2021;16(5):e0251073. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0251073. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cairns JA. Valuing future benefits. Health Economics. 1994;3(4):221–229. doi: 10.1002/hec.4730030404. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Calluso C, Grande E, Erario A, Tosoni A, Committeri G. Effects of individual discount rate and uncertainty perception on compliance with containment measures during the COVID-19 pandemic. Brain Sciences. 2021;11(10):1256. doi: 10.3390/brainsci11101256. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Carleton RN, Norton MA, Asmundson GJ. Fearing the unknown: A short version of the Intolerance of Uncertainty Scale. Journal of Anxiety Disorders. 2007;21(1):105–117. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2006.03.014. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chea B, Bolt A, Agelin-Chaab M, Dincer I. Assessment of effectiveness of optimum physical distancing phenomena for COVID-19. Physics of Fluids (Woodbury, N.y.: 1994) 2021;33(5):051903. doi: 10.1063/5.0046429. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen S, Robinson P, Janies D, Dulin M. Four challenges associated with current mathematical modeling paradigm of infectious diseases and call for a shift. Open Forum Infectious Diseases. 2020;7(8):ofaa333. doi: 10.1093/ofid/ofaa333. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Choi SL, Martin P, Cho J, Ryou YJ, Heinz M. Personality and compliance with COVID-19 protective measures among older Americans: Moderating effects of age, gender, and race/ethnicity. Personality and Individual Differences. 2022;189:111499. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2022.111499. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Christensen R. (2023). Ordinal—Regression models for ordinal data. R package version 2023.12-4. https://CRAN.R-project.org/package=ordinal.
- Ciaramelli E, De Luca F, Kwan D, Mok J, Bianconi F, Knyagnytska V, Craver C, Green L, Myerson J, Rosenbaum RS. The role of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in reward valuation and future thinking during intertemporal choice. eLife. 2021;10:067387. doi: 10.7554/eLife.67387. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cutler DM, Summers LH. The COVID-19 pandemic and the $16 trillion virus. JAMA. 2020;324(15):1495–1496. doi: 10.1001/jama.2020.19759. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Daugherty JR, Brase GL. Taking time to be healthy: Predicting health behaviors with delay discounting and time perspective. Personality and Individual Differences. 2010;48(2):202–207. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2009.10.007. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- DeAngelis BN, Ben Salah A, al'Absi M. Stress and COVID-19 related behaviours: The mediating role of delay discounting. Stress and Health: Journal of the International Society for the Investigation of Stress. 2022;38(1):140–146. doi: 10.1002/smi.3060. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Du W, Green L, Myerson J. Cross-cultural comparisons of discounting delayed and probabilistic rewards. The Psychological Record. 2002;52:479–492. doi: 10.1007/BF03395199. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Green L, Myerson J. A discounting framework for choice with delayed and probabilistic rewards. Psychological Bulletin. 2004;130(5):769–792. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.130.5.769. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldszmidt R, Petherick A, Andrade EB, Hale T, Furst R, Phillips T, Jones S. Protective behaviors against COVID-19 by individual vaccination status in 12 countries during the pandemic. JAMA Network Open. 2021;4(10):e2131137. doi: 10.1001/jamanetworkopen.2021.31137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Halilova JG, Fynes-Clinton S, Green L, Myerson J, Wu J, Ruggeri K, Addis DR, Rosenbaum RS. Short-sighted decision-making by those not vaccinated against COVID-19. Scientific Reports. 2022;12(1):11906. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-15276-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hudson A, Hall PA, Hitchman SC, Meng G, Fong GT. Cognitive predictors of COVID-19 mitigation behaviors in vaccinated and unvaccinated general population members. Vaccine. 2022 doi: 10.1016/j.vaccine.2022.10.004. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ishii K, Gang L, Takahashi T. Cross-cultural comparisons of delay discounting of gain and loss. Neuroendocrinology Letters. 2016;37(6):427–432. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krawiec JM, Mizak S, Tagliabue M, Białaszek W. Delay discounting of money and health outcomes, and adherence to policy guidelines during the COVID-19 pandemic. Frontiers in Public Health. 2022;10:953743. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.953743. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kroenke K, Spitzer RL, Williams JB. The PHQ-9: Validity of a brief depression severity measure. Journal of General Internal Medicine. 2001;16(9):606–613. doi: 10.1046/j.1525-1497.2001.016009606.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kuznetsova A, Brockhoff PB, Christensen RHB. lmerTest Package: Tests in linear mixed effects models. Journal of Statistical Software. 2017;82(13):1–26. doi: 10.18637/jss.v082.i13. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Liang W, Duan Y, Li F, Rhodes RE, Wang X, Peiris DLIHK, Zhou L, Shang B, Yang Y, Baker JS, Jiao J, Han W. Psychosocial determinants of hand hygiene, facemask wearing, and physical distancing during the COVID-19 pandemic: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Annals of Behavioral Medicine. 2022;56(11):1174–1187. doi: 10.1093/abm/kaac049. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lloyd A, McKay R, Hartman TK, Vincent BT, Murphy J, Gibson-Miller J, Levita L, Bennett K, McBride O, Martinez AP, Stocks TVA, Vallières F, Hyland P, Karatzias T, Butter S, Shevlin M, Bentall RP, Mason L. Delay discounting and under-valuing of recent information predict poorer adherence to social distancing measures during the COVID-19 pandemic. Scientific Reports. 2021;11(1):19237. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-98772-5. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- MacIntyre CR, Nguyen PY, Chughtai AA, Trent M, Gerber B, Steinhofel K, Seale H. Mask use, risk-mitigation behaviours and pandemic fatigue during the COVID-19 pandemic in five cities in Australia, the UK and USA: A cross-sectional survey. International Journal of Infectious Diseases. 2021;106:199–207. doi: 10.1016/j.ijid.2021.03.056. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mantzari E, Rubin GJ, Marteau TM. Is risk compensation threatening public health in the covid-19 pandemic? BMJ (clinical Research Ed.) 2020;370:m2913. doi: 10.1136/bmj.m2913. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Marjanovic, Z., Struthers, C. W., Cribbie, R., & Greenglass, E. R. (2014). The conscientious responders scale: A new tool for discriminating between conscientious and random responders. Sage Open, 4(3). 10.1177/2158244014545964
- Marin-Lopez BA, Jimenez-Gomez D, Abellán-Perpiñán JM. Behavioral economics in the epidemiology of the COVID-19 pandemic: Theory and simulations. International Journal of Environmental Research and Public Health. 2022;19(15):9557. doi: 10.3390/ijerph19159557. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mathieu E., Ritchie H., Rodés-Guirao L., Appel C., Giattino C., Hasell J., Macdonald B., Dattani S., Beltekian D., Ortiz-Ospina E., & Roser M. (2020). Coronavirus Pandemic (COVID-19). Published online at OurWorldInData.org. Retrieved from: https://ourworldindata.org/coronavirus [Online Resource]
- Mladenović D, Jirásek M, Ondráček T, Opatrná Z, Štangová R. The influence of social conformity on mask-wearing behavior during the COVID-19 pandemic. Heliyon. 2023;9(3):e14496. doi: 10.1016/j.heliyon.2023.e14496. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mok JNY, Kwan D, Green L, Myerson J, Craver CF, Rosenbaum RS. Is it time? Episodic imagining and the discounting of delayed and probabilistic rewards in young and older adults. Cognition. 2020;199:104222. doi: 10.1016/j.cognition.2020.104222. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moreira D, Barbosa F. Delay discounting in impulsive behavior: A systematic review. European Psychologist. 2019;24(4):312–321. doi: 10.1027/1016-9040/a000360. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Myerson J, Green L, Warusawitharana M. Area under the curve as a measure of discounting. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 2001;76(2):235–243. doi: 10.1901/jeab.2001.76-235. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Odum AL, Becker RJ, Haynes JM, Galizio A, Frye CCJ, Downey H, Friedel JE, Perez DM. Delay discounting of different outcomes: Review and theory. Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior. 2020;113(3):657–679. doi: 10.1002/jeab.589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Petherick A, Goldszmidt R, Andrade EB, Furst R, Hale T, Pott A, Wood A. A worldwide assessment of changes in adherence to COVID-19 protective behaviours and hypothesized pandemic fatigue. Nature Human Behaviour. 2021;5(9):1145–1160. doi: 10.1038/s41562-021-01181-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Read D, Read NL. Time discounting over the lifespan. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes. 2004;94(1):22–32. doi: 10.1016/j.obhdp.2004.01.002. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Robles E, Huang BE, Simpson PM, McMillan DE. Delay discounting, impulsiveness, and addiction severity in opioid-dependent patients. Journal of Substance Abuse Treatment. 2011;41(4):354–362. doi: 10.1016/j.jsat.2011.05.003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruggeri K, Panin A, Vdovic M, Većkalov B, Abdul-Salaam N, Achterberg J, García-Garzon E. The globalizability of temporal discounting. Nature Human Behaviour. 2022;6(10):1386–1397. doi: 10.1038/s41562-022-01392-w. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ruggeri K, Stock F, Haslam SA, Capraro V, Boggio P, Ellemers N, Willer R. A synthesis of evidence for policy from behavioural science during COVID-19. Nature. 2024;625(7993):134–147. doi: 10.1038/s41586-023-06840-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rung JM, Madden GJ. Experimental reductions of delay discounting and impulsive choice: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General. 2018;147(9):1349–1381. doi: 10.1037/xge0000462. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scholten H, Scheres A, de Water E, Graf U, Granic I, Luijten M. Behavioral trainings and manipulations to reduce delay discounting: A systematic review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review. 2019;26(6):1803–1849. doi: 10.3758/s13423-019-01629-2. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Szczuka Z, Siwa M, Abraham C, Baban A, Brooks S, Cipolletta S, Danso E, Dombrowski SU, Gan Y, Gaspar T, Gaspar de Matos M, Griva K, Jongenelis M, Keller J, Knoll N, Ma J, Abdul Awal Miah M, Morgan K, Peraud W, Quintard B, Luszczynska A. Handwashing adherence during the COVID-19 pandemic: A longitudinal study based on protection motivation theory. Social Science & Medicine. 2023;317:115569. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2022.115569. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith ML, Kakuhikire B, Baguma C, et al. Relative wealth, subjective social status, and their associations with depression: Cross-sectional, population-based study in rural Uganda. SSM - Population Health. 2019;8:100448. doi: 10.1016/j.ssmph.2019.100448. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Spitzer RL, Kroenke K, Williams JB, Löwe B. A brief measure for assessing generalized anxiety disorder: The GAD-7. Archives of Internal Medicine. 2006;166(10):1092–1097. doi: 10.1001/archinte.166.10.1092. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Staunton C, Swanepoel C, Labuschaigne M. Between a rock and a hard place: COVID-19 and South Africa's response. Journal of Law and the Biosciences. 2020;7(1):lsaa052. doi: 10.1093/jlb/lsaa052. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Strickland JC, Reed DD, Dayton L, Johnson MW, Latkin C, Schwartz LP, Hursh SR. Behavioral economic methods predict future COVID-19 vaccination. Translational Behavioral Medicine. 2022;12(10):1004–1008. doi: 10.1093/tbm/ibac057. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sun KS, Lau TSM, Yeoh EK, Chung VCH, Leung YS, Yam CHK, Hung CT. Effectiveness of different types and levels of social distancing measures: A scoping review of global evidence from earlier stage of COVID-19 pandemic. British Medical Journal Open. 2022;12(4):e053938. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2021-053938. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Torgerson DJ, Raftery J. Economic Notes. Discounting. BMJ (clinical Research Ed.) 1999;319(7214):914–915. doi: 10.1136/bmj.319.7214.914. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wismans A, Letina S, Wennberg K, Thurik R, Baptista R, Burke A, Dejardin M, Janssen F, Santarelli E, Torrès O, Franken I. The role of impulsivity and delay discounting in student compliance with COVID-19 protective measures. Personality and Individual Differences. 2021;179:110925. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2021.110925. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- World Health Organization. (n.d.). Vaccines and immunization. In World Health Organization Health Topics. Retrieved June 8, 2023, from https://www.who.int/health-topics/vaccines-and-immunization#tab=tab_1
- Wu T, Jia X, Shi H, Niu J, Yin X, Xie J, Wang X. Prevalence of mental health problems during the COVID-19 pandemic: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Affective Disorders. 2021;281:91–98. doi: 10.1016/j.jad.2020.11.117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zhang SX, Chen J. Scientific evidence on mental health in key regions under the COVID-19 pandemic - meta-analytical evidence from Africa, Asia, China, Eastern Europe, Latin America, South Asia, Southeast Asia, and Spain. European Journal of Psychotraumatology. 2021;12(1):2001192. doi: 10.1080/20008198.2021.2001192. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Anonymized, raw, and cleaned data have been deposited in a public repository hosted by the Open Science Framework https://osf.io/nyt4g/?view_only=7482638ff0bd4963867ed3b1d6f0833d. The materials used in this study are either described in the method section or publicly available.