Socioeconomic disparities impact neurocognitive functions. Yet, despite decades of research, the relationship between socioeconomic disparities and social cognition (the mental processes that allow us to understand and interact with others) remains controversial. Fendinger and colleagues [1] examine evidence spanning from psychology to the neurosciences and conclude that lower social class is consistently associated with improved social cognition, even if it diminishes more general cognitive processes. The authors suggest that social and nonsocial cognition are dissociable at the behavioral and brain levels, advocating for a domain-specific approach. Here, we argue that the alleged effect of lower social class on improving social cognition is inconsistent. Finally, we discuss how population diversity and current brain–behavior frameworks [2] better account for the impact of socioeconomic disparities on social cognition.
As defined by Fendinger and colleagues, social class is a hierarchy in society based on wealth and social prestige, incorporating cultural aspects that influence norms, values, and behaviors within each class [1]. While usually linked to socioeconomic status, this is not always the case. For instance, cultural aspects such as bilingualism may be associated with higher socioeconomic status in Latin America but not India. However, authors typically evaluate social class using subjective or objective measures of socioeconomic status [3]. Subjective measures include social class identity (poor, middle-class, upper-class) and the MacArthur Ladder, in which individuals indicate their position relative to others regarding socioeconomic measures, such as money, education, and job prestige. Objective measures typically include income and education. Crucially, subjective and objective social class (or socioeconomic) measures are only modestly correlated [3].
Fendinger and colleagues state that ‘lower social class individuals consistently outperform their higher-class counterparts on tasks assessing social cognition’ [1] (p. 528). However, this conclusion draws on reports using subjective measures (social class identity and ladder ratings). With few exceptions, research using objective measures like income or education shows a different pattern, linking lower social class with worse social cognition (i.e., emotion perception and mentalizing) [4–6] and prosociality [7]. Select examples are provided in Table 1, but these are by no means exhaustive.
Table 1.
Key findings | Refs | |
---|---|---|
|
[4,7] | |
|
[4–6] | |
|
[4,7] | |
|
[5,6] |
The aforementioned evidence is not addressed in Fendinger’s work. The authors also neglect contradicting results in a report they cite [8]. In Bjornsdottir and colleagues [8], associations between lower subjective social class and better interpersonal accuracy disappeared when using objective measures and lower income predicted worse mentalizing. Thus, Fendinger’s conclusion might be valid only for subjective measures, which likely capture other factors (hierarchy, power) that may influence social cognition differently than socioeconomic disparities.
Another concern is population biases. Evidence scrutinized by Fendinger and colleagues largely stems from young and healthy students/workers in the USA. A different outlook emerges from the population diversity of non-WEIRD (western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic) nations (Table 1). There is no association between lower social class and higher emotion perception or enhanced prosocial behavior in more extensive and socio-demographically diverse samples [4,7]. As shown for cognition and functionality [9], socioeconomic disparities significantly burden social cognition in under-represented backgrounds [6,10]. Embracing population and cultural diversity seems critical to advance a global understanding of socioeconomic disparities and social cognition.
Finally, Fendinger’s domain-specific approach lacks current support. They state that social and nonsocial cognition are dissociable and rely on separate brain regions differentially impacted by social class. Lower social class may enhance social cognition by promoting activity in the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex. Conversely, lower social class either has no impact on nonsocial cognition or hinders it through other regions. However, at the behavioral level, evidence of blending between cognitive/executive functions and social cognition [2,10] contradicts such dissociation. Regarding neural correlates, social cognition broadly extends beyond the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex, involving ‘nonsocial regions’ like the premotor and intraparietal cortices [11]. Similarly, nonsocial cognition critically engages ‘social regions’, including the dorsomedial prefrontal cortex [11]. Socioeconomic disparities are associated with reduced cortical surface of multiple regions relevant to cognitive/executive functions and social cognition [12]. Socioeconomic-related brain differences may even not manifest quantitatively (i.e., less brain activity) but qualitatively (i.e., engaging different neurocognitive systems) [12]. Together, Fendinger’s assumption of a selective boost of lower social class on the ‘social brain’ seems oversimplistic. Assuming rigid anatomical categorizations without considering the degeneracy principle (same function performed by different structures) and network dynamics represents an outdated localizationist framework that can lead to inaccuracies [2].
At the very least, lower social class does not consistently improve social cognition but can indeed harm it. Lower subjective social class has been associated with enhanced social cognition in the USA. However, lower objective social class, particularly across diverse samples, seems to involve diminished social cognition. This conclusion has not only research relevance but also ethical: neglecting the negative impact of socioeconomic disparities in underserved populations can increase inequalities. A situated, diverse, and global approach is required to develop tailored models that surpass the failures of universal generalizations [9]. This may allow us to better understand how socioeconomic disparities shape the human brain and cognition.
Acknowledgments
A.I. is supported by grants from ReDLat [National Institutes of Health and the Fogarty International Center (FIC), National Institutes of Aging (R01 AG057234, R01 AG075775, R01 AG21051, CARDS-NIH), Alzheimer’s Association (SG-20-725707), Rainwater Charitable Foundation – The Bluefield project to cure FTD, and Global Brain Health Institute)], ANID/FONDECYT Regular (1210195, 1210176, and 1220995), and ANID/FONDAP/15150012. The contents of this publication are solely the author’s responsibility and do not represent the official views of these institutions.
Footnotes
Declaration of interests
None to declare.
References
- 1.Fendinger NJ et al. (2023) Beyond cognitive deficits: how social class shapes social cognition. Trends Cogn. Sci 27, 528–538 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Ibanez A (2022) The mind’s golden cage and cognition in the wild. Trends Cogn. Sci 26, 1031–1034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Tan JJ et al. (2020) The association between objective and subjective socioeconomic status and subjective well-being: a meta-analytic review. Psychol. Bull 146, 970–1020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Deveney CM et al. (2018) How generalizable is the inverse relationship between social class and emotion perception? PLoS One 13, e0205949. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Quesque F et al. (2022) Does culture shape our understanding of others’ thoughts and emotions? An investigation across 12 countries. Neuropsychology 36, 664–682 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Migeot J et al. (2022) Socioeconomic status impacts cognitive and socioemotional processes in healthy ageing. Sci. Rep 12, 1–11 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Korndörfer M et al. (2015) A large scale test of the effect of social class on prosocial behavior. PLoS One 10, e0133193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Bjornsdottir RT et al. (2017) The perceptive proletarian: subjective social class predicts interpersonal accuracy. J. Nonverbal Behav 41, 185–201 [Google Scholar]
- 9.Santamaria-Garcia H et al. (2023) Factors associated with healthy aging in Latin American populations. Nat. Med 29, 2248–2258 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Fittipaldi S et al. (2024) Heterogeneous factors influence social cognition across diverse settings in brain health and age-related diseases. Nat. Mental Health, Published online January 02, 2024. 10.1038/s44220-023-00164-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Barrett LF and Satpute AB (2013) Large-scale brain networks in affective and social neuroscience: towards an integrative functional architecture of the brain. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol 23, 361–372 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Farah MJ (2018) Socioeconomic status and the brain: prospects for neuroscience-informed policy. Nat. Rev. Neurosci 19, 428–438 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]