Abstract
When animals learn the association of a conditioned stimulus (CS) with an unconditioned stimulus (US), later presentation of the CS invokes a representation of the US. When the expected US fails to occur, theoretical accounts predict that conditioned inhibition can accrue to any other stimuli that are associated with this change in the US. Empirical work with mammals has confirmed the existence of conditioned inhibition. But the way it is manifested, the conditions that produce it, and determining whether it is the opposite of excitatory conditioning are important considerations. Invertebrates can make valuable contributions to this literature because of the well-established conditioning protocols and access to the central nervous system (CNS) for studying neural underpinnings of behavior. Nevertheless, although conditioned inhibition has been reported, it has yet to be thoroughly investigated in invertebrates. Here, we evaluate the role of the US in producing conditioned inhibition by using proboscis extension response conditioning of the honeybee (Apis mellifera). Specifically, using variations of a “feature-negative” experimental design, we use downshifts in US intensity relative to US intensity used during initial excitatory conditioning to show that an odorant in an odor–odor mixture can become a conditioned inhibitor. We argue that some alternative interpretations to conditioned inhibition are unlikely. However, we show variation across individuals in how strongly they show conditioned inhibition, with some individuals possibly revealing a different means of learning about changes in reinforcement. We discuss how the resolution of these differences is needed to fully understand whether and how conditioned inhibition is manifested in the honeybee, and whether it can be extended to investigate how it is encoded in the CNS. It is also important for extension to other insect models. In particular, work like this will be important as more is revealed of the complexity of the insect brain from connectome projects.
Animal studies of Pavlovian and operant conditioning have revealed important, generalizable principles of reinforcement learning (Sutton and Barto 2018). Many studies have now shown that animals assess the informative value of stimuli and then update behavioral decisions based on that assessment every time these stimuli occur. At its most fundamental level, associative (Pavlovian) conditioning involves pairing of a “neutral” conditioned stimulus (CS) with an important, salient unconditioned stimulus (US), such as food as an appetitive example or painful stimuli as aversive examples (Rescorla 1988). The predictive nature of a CS produces a conditioned behavior that prepares animals for the encounter with the US. These principles have been expressed in simple mathematical equations aimed at explaining how conditioning establishes associative strength VA to a CSA, which then is the basis for anticipatory behavior (Bush and Mosteller 1951; Rescorla and Wagner 1972). Although there are important differences between the different models, such as whether and how attention contributes to conditioning (Mackintosh 1975; Lubow 1989), these mathematical formulations share two important principles (Pearce and Hall 1980; Rescorla 1988). First, the equations assume that the change in associative strength ΔV can be either positive when the CS predicts the US will occur or, under at least some conditions, negative when the CS predicts that an expected US will not occur. Second, they all propose that ΔV on a given trial is proportional to the maximum amount of associative strength that a US can support (λ) minus an expectation of the summed total associative strength to all stimuli present—including contextual stimuli—that has already accrued regarding that US (VΣ = VA + VB + · · · + VN). Thus, associative strength V increases with each trial as long as λ > VΣ (i.e., as long as the US that is encountered is stronger than predicted by VΣ [a positive prediction error]). Once the US is fully predicted (λ = VΣ), associative strength stops being incremented, resulting in an asymptotic acquisition curve.
Behavioral studies have identified important manifestations of reinforcement expectation based on this type of model (Rescorla and Wagner 1972; Pearce and Hall 1980; Dunsmoor et al. 2015; Sutton and Barto 2018). When an animal experiences CSA after it has been associated with reinforcement, CSA evokes a prediction of the US that is proportional to VΣ. This amounts to an expectation that a US at least of value VΣ will occur. When a second CSX is combined with CSA, and the same reinforcement is presented, animals typically learn less about CSX than they normally would, which is called blocking (Mackintosh 1983). Additionally, it is important to establish what happens on trials when the US fails to occur despite being predicted by CSA (i.e., when there is a surprising reward omission) (Papini 2003). In particular, when a second CSX is present in the compound (i.e., CSAX) during reward omission, conditioned inhibition to CSX may ensue (Rescorla 1969). Essentially, on such CSAX trials, λ = 0 because the US is absent. However, VΣ > 0 because of the presence of CSA. Therefore, λ − VΣ is negative, reflecting that the US the animals encounter is less strong than what they expected. This negative prediction error serves as a teaching signal to establish a negative prediction for CSX (i.e., the prediction that the US will not occur). This is why CSX becomes a conditioned inhibitor.
Revealing whether and under what conditions an animal develops and expresses conditioned inhibition can be critical for understanding how that animal is capable of representing information about reinforcement, and in particular which of the several models of conditioning apply (e.g., whether there is a representation akin to VΣ or whether CSAX is perceived as a different stimulus altogether). Moreover, showing the existence of conditioned inhibition will be important for understanding how that capability is supported by neural connectivity in the brain.
However, it is not trivial to unambiguously demonstrate conditioned inhibition (Papini and Bitterman 1993; Sosa and Ramírez 2019). Minimally, a conditioned inhibitor should pass two types of tests (Rescorla 1969). First, once a stimulus (CSX) has taken on inhibitory properties, subsequent excitatory conditioning should proceed more slowly than it does with other stimuli that do not carry inhibitory properties. A lack of retardation or even its opposite, better excitatory conditioning of CSX, might indicate that more attention is devoted to the processing of CSX at the expense of other stimuli that are present. Second, CSX should pass a summation test. When CSX is presented in compound with a second CS that itself is capable of releasing a response, that response to the compound should be diminished by the inhibitory properties of CSX.
The first step in this process is to demonstrate behavior that is consistent with conditioned inhibition. Although the existence of reward expectations has been well documented in vertebrates (Sosa and Ramírez 2019), it has not been well studied via retardation and summation tests in invertebrates, in spite of their widespread use as models for reinforcement learning. Using the honeybee (Apis mellifera), Chandra et al. (2010) showed that unreinforced presentation of an odor X—as it would occur in an AX compound without reinforcement but where A predicts reinforcement—reliably produces retardation of acquisition. In other experiments, a reduction in expected reinforcement reduced learning of a target CS below that expected given the level of reinforcement (Smith 1997). Furthermore, backward pairing (Hellstern et al. 1998) and peak shifts (Fernandez et al. 2009) produce inhibition-like effects. These experiments seem to rule out attention as a factor. However, the focus of these works was not on conditioned inhibition, so the results of summation tests were not evaluated.
Here, we use variants of a feature-negative (A+|AX−) experimental design to test more directly for evidence of negative summation consistent with conditioned inhibition in the honeybee PER procedure. We show that when associated with the omission of expected reinforcement, X can reduce the response to another excitatory stimulus. We also show and discuss that individuals differ in expression of conditioned inhibition, which parallels other studies of individual differences in learning performance in honeybees, and it brings into focus how potentially multiple conditioning phenomena can affect behavior. Resolution of these differences will be necessary to more fully reveal and prove the existence of conditioned inhibition, which can also serve as a model for the extension of these kinds of studies to other insects. Finally, the use of more complex and well-controlled conditioning protocols to study learning should help map these phenomena onto the unexpected complexity of the fruit fly brain connectome (Li et al. 2020), and, in particular, it may reveal whether and how structures like the mushroom bodies are involved in reward expectation.
Results
Experiment 1: first test for negative summation
Subjects were initially conditioned in two phases. First, they were conditioned to A++ and B+ on separate trials that were pseudorandomly interspersed with one another (Fig. 1A, solid lines and filled symbols). For the second phase, subjects were conditioned to AX+ on trials that were similarly interspersed with the continued presentation of A++ (Fig. 1A, dashed lines and open symbols). The AX+ condition was designed to build inhibitory tendencies to X because of the decrease in reinforcement from A++ to AX+.
There was little difference between the preconditioning acquisition curves for odor A and odor B (Fig. 1A). In the compound conditioning phase, response to odor A alone (reinforced with high sucrose) remained high, whereas the percent proboscis extension in the AX+ group (AX reinforced with low sucrose) trended toward an overall decline. Because the strength of the US associated with A decreased between the preconditioning and compound conditioning phases in the AX+ trials, the decline in proboscis extension may already suggest an accrual of inhibition to odor X. If inhibition has accrued to X in the compound phase as a result of its association with a lower-than-expected US intensity, then this inhibitory effect should influence the subjects’ relative response to the otherwise excitatory odor B when it is paired with X during testing.
We then tested subjects with a blend of B and X. Under normal circumstances, the addition of an odorant to a previously conditioned B would decrease the response to B because of overshadowing or interactions in sensory transduction processes (Smith 1998). Therefore, we compare the response duration to B when it is combined with X to the response duration to B when it is combined with a novel odor (N). The inhibitory properties of X should decrease the response to B more than the addition of a novel odor N that is neutral.
In the final summation test, the response to odor B should be suppressed in the BX mixture (because of its pairing with putative inhibitor X) relative to the response to odor B when it is paired to the novel odor N. The results indicate that this is indeed the case (Fig. 1B). The duration of proboscis extension response to BX was significantly shorter than that to BN (W = 101.0, P < 0.01 one-tailed test). The distributions shown in Figure 1B reflect a number of subjects that failed to respond to one or both odors, which are the points lying on the x-axis.
Experiment 2: a feature-negative test of conditioned inhibition
Most studies of conditioned inhibition have used a variation of a feature-negative design (Papini and Bitterman 1993), which is a somewhat different procedure from that in our first experiment. In a feature-negative experiment we use here, one odorant (A++) is paired with reinforcement, whereas a mixture of that odorant with a target odorant (AX−) is not reinforced at all. We have used a similar procedure to demonstrate feature-negative conditioning in the honeybee (Chandra and Smith 1998). This procedure produces a much more dramatic discrepancy between the reinforcement signaled by A and the actual outcome on trials when X is present. As in the first experiment, we used a single treatment group of 24 subjects. Each subject experienced four different types of trials that were presented intermixed in a pseudorandom order. Two types of trials involved pairing an odorant with either ++ or + reinforcement (A++ and B+) on separate trials. In this experiment, we used B as an excitatory target odor against which to test for a summation effect. There were also two types of completely unreinforced trials (AX− and CY−). We predicted that inhibition would accrue to X on AX− trials because of the discrepancy between the signaled and actual outcomes. Furthermore, because the unreinforced presentation of a CS may reduce its potential excitatory properties via latent inhibition (Chandra et al. 2010), we present a second unreinforced odor blend (CY−). If the inhibition that accrues to X is simply due to attentional decrement from nonreinforcement (latent inhibition), then there should be no difference between the response levels BX and BY in a subsequent summation test. On the other hand, if the lack of reinforcement in an otherwise excitatory context produces conditioned inhibition, then we would expect that the response to BX would be less than that to BY. Furthermore, if afferent interaction in the AX compound reduces the response to BX, then there should be an equivalent reduction in BY because of afferent interaction in the CY condition.
Subjects showed rapid acquisition to A++ and to B+ (Fig. 2A), reaching an asymptotic level of response (∼70%) by trials 4 or 5. The response level to AX− was intermediate, reaching a final response level of ∼25%. The response to CY−, the components of which were never reinforced, was lowest. The somewhat elevated level of response to AX− as compared to CY− could be due to incomplete excitatory generalization from A to the AX compound. Furthermore, inhibition to X could have also contributed to the response to AX− in the sense that it would decrease the response to AX relative to A.
Indeed, the summation test reveals that there is most likely a contribution from conditioned inhibition. The response to BX was significantly less than that to BY (Fig. 2B; W = 102.0, P < 0.05 one-tailed test). Thus, odor X was capable of more significant negative summation when added to the conditioned excitor B than when Y was added.
Experiment 3: individual differences in expression of conditioned inhibition
Figures 1 and 2 reflect test performance across a sample of 24 or 30 subjects, which is standard procedure for presenting learning performance in PER studies (Burden et al. 2016). However, these values hide the diversity of performance on any learning task, with some subjects performing well and others not showing the expected outcome. These differences are reflected in the distributions of individual duration values shown in Figures 1B and 2B. Because of these individual differences, and its importance for a social colony (see Discussion; Cook et al. 2019), we decided to repeat the second experiment (Fig. 2) with a focus on individual differences.
Acquisition across the four odors in this replicate was similar to acquisition in the first experiment (Fig. 3). The response levels across trials to reinforced odors A++ and B+ were highest, reaching 70%–80% of subjects responding or higher by the fifth or sixth trials. As before, the response level to AX− was intermediate and the response to CY− was lowest.
In this replicate, differences between BX and BY failed to reach significance, even though the trend was in the same direction as in experiment 2 (median BX 2.7 vs. BY 3.2). The differences between experiments are a major point in our discussion below. In the meantime, some important clarification can be provided by a more detailed inspection of the data in both experiments.
In both experiments, a majority of subjects that responded to one or both odors (13 of 20 and 19 of 32, respectively) responded more strongly—that is, with longer response durations—to BY than to BX (Fig. 4A,B; X2 P < 0.01), which is the pattern expected for conditioned inhibition. Data from these subjects are plotted as “Inhibitors.” However, it is notable that several subjects in each experiment failed to show the pattern predicted by the conditioned inhibition hypothesis. First, some individuals failed to respond to either odor compound (4 and 9 in experiments 2 and 3, respectively). Second, some subjects (7 of 20 and 13 of 32 in experiments 2 and 3, respectively) showed shorter response durations to BY than to BX (right pairs of columns labeled “Noninhibitor”), which is opposite to the prediction for conditioned inhibition. In fact, in both experiments, some subjects (shaded symbols) responded to BX but failed to respond to BY. Obviously, because of the way they were selected, the differences within each group are statistically significant (P < 0.01). The implications of these differences are further discussed below.
Discussion
Our results suggest that a component of a binary odor blend can develop properties of a conditioned inhibitor (Rescorla 1969; Papini 2003; Sosa and Ramírez 2019). These properties develop when that component is associated with a lower level of reinforcement than that signaled by other components of the blend. However, as we discuss below, we must use caution at this point in reaching firm conclusions because of the individual variation that we report. In addition, there are several alternative interpretations that can account for a decrease in response to a target stimulus (Papini and Bitterman 1993; Sosa and Ramírez 2019). Those alternatives include modification of attention, afferent interaction, external inhibition, changes in generalized excitation, US habituation, and contextual conditioning/blocking. It is important now to address these issues in order to determine the reliability of the claim for inhibition in our experiments.
Rescorla (1969) has recommended a two-test strategy for revealing conditioned inhibition, which would serve as a necessary set of conditions for showing inhibition. A conditioned inhibitor would reveal retardation of acquisition when it is properly paired with reinforcement, and its inhibition would negatively summate with the excitatory properties of another stimulus that was associated with the same type of reinforcement used to produce the inhibition. If A+/AX− pairing changed attention to X, then X might pass one test but not the other. For example, if attention to X were enhanced because of the surprising omission of reinforcement in the A+/AX− condition, then acquisition would be expected to be more rapid to X in an A+/AX− group than in a control group (e.g., A+/X−). In this case, X would fail the retardation test but might still decrease the response to an excitatory stimulus because of X's ability to monopolize attention. On the other hand, if the effect of nonreinforcement of X in the A+/AX− condition were to decrease attention to X via latent inhibition (Lubow 1973), for example, then acquisition would be retarded, and X would pass the retardation test, but X might fail to reduce the excitatory properties of another stimulus leading to failure of the summation test. Only a conditioned inhibitor would be capable of passing both tests.
Interpretation of reduced response to BX in our experiments, relative to BN, because of increased attention to X is unlikely. If attention were a factor, then any modification of the US between the initial and compound trials will generate some form of surprise and hence enhanced attention to X (Pearce and Hall 1980; Mackintosh 1983). That is, either an up- or a downshift in reinforcement should be capable of generating a significant amount of learning about X on compound (AX) trials. However, when honeybees experience A++/AX++ training, they learn less about X relative to a control procedure in which A is not present during the compound phase (Smith 1997). Furthermore, when they experience a downshift in reinforcement (A++/AX+), they respond less to X relative to the A++/AX+ group that did not experience the downshift. The attention hypothesis would predict the opposite. Moreover, this procedure showed one-trial blocking. Those results are inconsistent with the interpretation that attention to X is enhanced by a downshift in reinforcement used in our current experiments (Rescorla and Holland 1982; Mackintosh 1983).
Latent inhibition (Lubow 1973) via retardation of acquisition also does not account for our results. Latent inhibition in the honeybee PER paradigm requires many more (∼20) unreinforced exposures to a CS (Chandra et al. 2010) than subjects were exposed to in our experiment. Nevertheless, we attempted to control for this effect in experiments 2 and 3 by exposing subjects to the CY− condition. If latent inhibition decreased response to X in the AX− condition, then it should have done so equally to Y in the CY− condition. But the test response to BX was lower than to BY, which argues at least for an additional source of inhibition to X.
Afferent interaction might reasonably be expected to be stronger in intramodal compounds than in intermodal compounds (Kehoe et al. 1994). We attempted to control for afferent interaction by the addition of a novel odorant during the test phase, as we previously have done for studies of blocking (Smith and Cobey 1994; Smith 1996, 1997; Thorn and Smith 1997). If we had simply tested BX versus B, then the decrease would be interpretable in terms of greater interaction in the compound, which occurs in mixtures of these same odorants used above (Smith 1998). Furthermore, we either partially or completely counterbalanced odorants in all of our experiments. It is, therefore, unlikely that the summation effect of X would arise because of greater afferent interaction with B than in the control groups.
Several previous attempts have been made to specifically study conditioned inhibition in honeybees, but the results were mixed. Some early experiments using different types of manipulations (Bitterman et al. 1983), including backward pairing (Hellstern et al. 1998) and peak shift (Fernandez et al. 2009), provided an indication of inhibition, but the experiments left open alternative interpretations. Couvillon et al. (1999) failed to find evidence for conditioned inhibition in freely flying honeybees. Yet, the use of modified, and perhaps more sensitive, experimental designs detected evidence of conditioned inhibition (Couvillon et al. 2003). Using restrained subjects in the PER paradigm provided indirect evidence for inhibition (Couvillon et al. 2005). Yet our experiments using PER reported here support the interpretation, over other alternative possibilities, that conditioned inhibition might still be found in honeybees. The design we adopted for experiments 2 and 3 was adopted from Couvillon et al. (1999). Therefore, differences across studies do not appear to be attributable to the use of freely flying versus restrained bees or to the use of intramodal versus intermodal stimulus mixtures.
A major difference between our study reported here and other studies using PER conditioning of restrained subjects (Couvillon et al. 2005) involves aspects of experimental design and use of different response measures. Our designs used longer phases of excitatory conditioning—18 or 32 trials versus 9. A similar difference can account for differences in the detection of latent inhibition (Bitterman et al. 1983; Chandra et al. 2010). Furthermore, studies of PER conditioning have mostly used the percentage of subjects in a treatment group that responded to the CS. This measure is frequently sensitive enough to detect differences between treatment groups. But failure to find differences, particularly when a large percentage of subjects respond, cannot be interpreted as unambiguous evidence of a lack of treatment effect. Although the response itself looks simple, detailed electromyographic (Smith and Menzel 1989) and video analyses like we have done here reveal several different features of response topology (e.g., response latency and duration) that differentiate treatment groups (Smith 1997), even when no differences are evident in response probability.
Here, we have tried to be careful to point out individual differences in the manifestation of conditioned inhibition, which are present in all studies of PER conditioning but are often not apparent when mean values are presented. Several other studies of PER in honeybees have also shown consistent individual differences in performance in several different learning protocols (Scheiner et al. 2017; Tait et al. 2019; Finke et al. 2021, 2023), which also broadly correlates to sensitivity to sugar as well as to other foraging preferences (Scheiner et al. 2001). Genotype accounts for the largest proportion of variance in learning performance among bees in the same colony (Bhagavan et al. 1994). Once genotype is more standardized, differences in learning performance between behavioral castes become more apparent (Ferguson et al. 2001). Because a queen mates with up to 20 drones (males), any colony will contain a mixture of several different patrilines (Page 2013). This genetic diversity within any colony is important for the fitness of the colony as it performs foraging tasks (Mosqueiro et al. 2017; Cook et al. 2020). However, this diversity must be taken into account in any study of conditioning, because a small sample (∼20 or 30) workers from a large population (up to 100,000) may not reflect the genetic diversity present in the large population. This could be the reason that inhibition was more difficult to show in experiment 3 than in experiment 2, for example.
A few loci in the honeybee genome correlate to learning performance, but one, in particular, has been highly correlated in two independent genetic mapping studies (Chandra et al. 2000; Latshaw et al. 2023). In this locus, a gene that encodes a receptor for the biogenic amine tyramine has a major impact on learning performance, and because of where that receptor is expressed (Sinakevitch et al. 2017), and its intracellular signaling pathway (Blenau et al. 2000), it is likely that it acts as a gain control to regulate inputs to circuitry that supports learning (Latshaw et al. 2023). The activity of this gene, or the lack of activity, would cause bees to display strong or poor learning performance. This model is also consistent with this gene having broad pleiotropic effects on other behaviors—for example, specializations in pollen versus nectar collation (Hunt et al. 1995). Because of this broad pleiotropy, it is possible that it affects expression of conditioned inhibition.
We confirm here that the groups of subjects selected for experiments 2 and 3 show differences across individuals in how the behavior is manifested. The majority of individuals responded more strongly to the BY compound than to the BX compound, which is the prediction for conditioned inhibition. In contrast, a minority of individuals in each experiment responded more strongly to BX than to BY, which is the opposite prediction from conditioned inhibition. This pattern (BX > BY) could arise if some individuals associated X with excitatory conditioning of A++, as would be expected for second-order conditioning (Mackintosh 1983), for example. In fact, the procedure we have used would also be used to study second-order conditioning. In that case, a stronger response to the BX compound could occur via positive summation instead of the negative summation predicted for conditioned inhibition.
These individual differences in our studies leave open possibilities for further investigation. For example, do some individuals in a colony show excitatory second-order conditioning over conditioned inhibition, or vice versa, when a stimulus is paired with a conditioned excitor? Does excitatory second-order conditioning drive responding over the first few conditioning trials and then switch to conditioned inhibition after several trials? In this case, some individuals may make this switch earlier than others, which would account for individual differences in our study. Within a colony genetic differences have been shown (e.g., for latent inhibition and reversal learning [Ferguson et al. 2001; Chandra et al. 2010]) and could carry over to other forms of conditioning. Our results emphasize that future studies need to describe individual differences in learning performance and test how other factors—age, experience, behavioral caste, and genetic background—affect the expression of the trait. At the very least, care must be taken to control for and standardize genetic background, which is not often done in studies with honeybees but is standard practice in studies with fruit flies and rodents.
Taken together, studies now show enough evidence that conditioned inhibition can be studied in honeybees, and now also in mollusks (Acebes et al. 2012). However, much needs to be done to verify it and more fully evaluate how conditioned inhibition is specifically manifested in behavior. One important contribution of our work is to provide guidance on how to study it—using the protocol in experiments 2 and 3—and raise some important questions going forward. For example, does conditioned inhibition show extinction, like in excitatory conditioning? What is the role of contextual cues in generating and releasing memory for conditioned inhibition? Are there differences in the manifestation of conditioned inhibition between intermodal and intramodal mixtures, as were used in our study? As noted above, is second-order conditioning expressed in the first few trials with conditioned inhibition more dominant with longer sequences of trails? Answers to these questions will be essential for understanding how different forms of learning are integrated into the ongoing decision processes involved in acquiring resources or avoiding threats (Bazhenov et al. 2013). Additionally, this information will interface with investigations into the neural mechanisms that underlie conditioned inhibition, such as have been initiated for latent inhibition (Locatelli et al. 2013) and blocking (Chen et al. 2015) in honeybees. These studies in insects such as honeybees and fruit flies, particularly if conditioned inhibition can be found in the latter, will help integrate the behavior into information now coming out of the fruit fly brain connectome projects (Li et al. 2020).
Materials and Methods
Subjects
The subjects were honeybee workers obtained from three to five colonies maintained out-of-doors in a genetically closed population (Page et al. 1985). Subjects were collected from colony entrances, as they departed from or returned to the colony, in the morning between 9 and 10 a.m. They were then set up in restraining harnesses and conditioned according to published procedures described below (and in Smith and Burden 2014). After a 15-min period to acclimate to the harness, subjects were fed 0.4 µL of a 1.25 M sucrose solution, and they then remained undisturbed for 2 h before the start of the conditioning procedure. All three experiments used a within-subjects design for testing.
Apparatus
Conditioning and testing were conducted in a 13-cm × 14.5-cm × 16.5-cm conditioning station illuminated with a fiber-optic light source. The station had an open front, top, and floor. The remaining sides were constructed of clear Plexiglas. To evacuate odors during conditioning, a 9.5-cm-diameter hole in the back of the chamber was connected to an exhaust hood via a dryer tube. The floor of the chamber consisted of a single strip of Plexiglas connecting the left and right sides of the conditioning station. In the center of this strip was a small Plexiglas peg on which harnessed subjects were placed during conditioning.
Subjects were positioned to face an odor cartridge at the front of the chamber. Odor cartridges were made fresh daily by placing a strip of filter paper containing 3 µL of pure odorant into a glass syringe. The open tip of the odor cartridge was positioned in front of and facing the subject. The ground glass fitting was connected to a three-way valve system that regulated the flow of air being generated by an aquarium air pump. Administration of the CS (odor) was controlled by computer or by an Arduino-based logic controller. When triggered, the computer operated a solenoid valve, thus shunting airflow away from an exhaust tube and through the odor cartridge. The odor-saturated air was ejected into the airstream that moved across the subject's antenna. Just behind the conditioning stage and out of the subject's view was a small LED used to signal the onset of odor delivery on videotape (see below). The US (0.4 µL of either 1.25 or 0.50 M sucrose-water solution) was administered using a Gilmont microliter syringe. The odors were hexanol, octanol, 2-hexanone, 2-octanone, geraniol, and eugenol; they were rotated across days into each of the odor types (labeled as A, B, X, N, Y, or C in different experiments) used in the conditioning phases described below. All odors are easily discriminable to honeybees in PER conditioning.
At the start of each trial, the subject was placed into the conditioning station 20 sec before the onset of the odor CS. It was also left in the conditioning station 30 sec after odor exposure. The intertrial interval for each subject was fixed at 6 min. A video camera was positioned above the conditioning stage to record each subject's response during the test phase of the experiment.
Procedure
Proboscis extension conditioning
In this protocol (Smith and Burden 2014), an odorant (CS) is blown across the subject's antenna for 4 sec. Three seconds after the onset of the odor CS, a 0.4-µL drop of sucrose-water (US) is lightly touched to the antenna. This stimulates sucrose taste receptors in the antenna and triggers the proboscis extension response. The subject is then allowed to consume the entire droplet, which always occurs within ∼1 sec. After three to four trials most subjects extend their proboscis to odor before sucrose reinforcement, which we registered as a positive conditioned response.
Experiment 1
The first experiment was divided into two acquisition phases during which six subjects were conditioned each day, so the intertrial interval was fixed at 6 min. In the initial (Pretraining) phase of all experiments, subjects were equivalently conditioned to six trials each of odor A and odor B presented in a pseudorandomized sequence (ABBABAABABBA). Each odor was appetitively reinforced, but with different sucrose concentrations. Odor A was reinforced with high (++; 1.25 M) sucrose and odor B was reinforced with low (+; 0.50 M) sucrose.
After pretraining, the first experiment consisted of a second acquisition phase followed by a test phase. For compound conditioning in the second phase, all subjects received 12 trials. Subjects were exposed to odor A followed by high sucrose (A++ condition) on six of the trials. This condition was a continuation of the A++ condition from the preconditioning phase. On the remaining six trials, subjects were exposed to the compound of A and X followed by low sucrose (AX+). Thus, X was reinforced with low sucrose in a context that predicted high sucrose reinforcement. The two trial types were presented in the pseudorandomized order used for pretraining.
After the second phase, each subject was tested for their response to the two odor compounds BX and BN without the presence of reinforcement. Responses to odor B in combination with odor X were compared to responses to odor B when it was mixed with the novel odor N. The odors used for A, B, X, and N, and the order of presentation of BX and BN during testing, were counterbalanced over days of the experiment.
Experiments 2 and 3
Each subject was exposed to 32 acquisition trials, which were comprised of eight trials each with A++, B+, AX−, and CY− in a pseudorandomized sequence across trials. The major qualitative differences between this experiment and those reported above were (1) that trials with A++ and AX− were pseudorandomly interspersed instead of presented in successive phases, and (2) the lack of reinforcement of the AX compound instead of reinforcement with a diminished US.
Following the acquisition trials, each subject was tested on unreinforced trials with BX and with BY in a random sequence across subjects. There are 24 different combinations of four odorants; therefore, each of the 24 subjects was conditioned to a different pattern of A, C, X, and Y. The fifth odorant (B), which was a common test stimulus, was the same for all subjects (geraniol).
Data analysis
After conditioning was complete, all subjects received an unreinforced presentation of odors BX and either BN (experiment 1) or BY (experiments 2 and 3) presented in a randomized order across days. All responses were video recorded for offline analysis. We then calculated the duration of proboscis extension during the 4-sec presentation of odor X and for 20 sec after the termination of odor presentation (Smith 1997, 1998). Statistical analysis of the response duration was used for all hypothesis testing (Smith and Menzel 1989; Smith 1998). Because of the distribution of the scores, statistical analyses were accomplished with Wilcoxon signed rank tests (Sokal and Rohlf 1994) in PRISM. Medians and interquartile ranges are shown for unreinforced tests with BX, BN, and BY in the figures along with symbols indicating durations from individual responses.
We present acquisition data for descriptive reasons. But we do not provide statistical tests of acquisition data, primarily because we do not use them for hypothesis testing. In particular, testing differences during acquisition trials, during which groups receive different treatment conditions, would confound treatment and testing conditions (Rescorla 1988). For example, A++ and B+ trials presented during acquisition differ in two factors; odor and concentration of sucrose in the reward. Bees learn the visual stimulus of the water droplet approaching them and the water vapor that surrounds it. On the B+ trials, when a lower concentration is presented, bees may generalize the association of the droplet with the high reward presented on the A++ trials to the B+ trials, which would elevate the response to B. In addition, the differences between rewarded trials and unreinforced (e.g., AX− or CY−) trials also involve two factors: odor and presence/absence of reward. The two-factor differences between these curves are necessary to set up different treatment conditions, but it makes them not comparable for hypothesis testing. To test for inhibition, we have added unreinforced trials at the end, which differ in only one factor (e.g., BX and BY). Furthermore, it would be impossible to calculate the duration of the conditioned response (CR) when its expression would be influenced by the presentation of the US before the offset of the CS. Given the similarity between the CR and the unconditioned response (UR) (Smith and Menzel 1989), it would be impossible to distinguish them. We did not insert extinction trials to measure duration during acquisition, because it was not our intent to study differences in response to stimuli presented during the conditioning phases, and doing that might have attenuated the ability of a CS to induce inhibition.
Therefore, the only response measure we used for acquisition was the percentage of subjects that responded to odor before the presentation of the CS. That measure has been used in many studies of PER conditioning in the honeybee (Menzel 1990), and when it reveals differences across treatment groups, those differences are robust with almost any measure of the CR (Smith 1998). But when it fails to reveal differences across treatments, the use of a more parametric measure such as duration or latency can reveal differences in the response topology (Smith and Menzel 1989).
Acknowledgments
This work was supported by awards to B.H.S. from the National Institutes of Health (GM 113967), the National Science Foundation (2014217) NeuroNex program, the National Science Foundation (2113179) CRCNS US–France (ANR) collaborative research program, and the Department of Energy (SC0021922) US–German (BMBF) collaborative research program.
Footnotes
Article is online at http://www.learnmem.org/cgi/doi/10.1101/lm.053915.124.
Freely available online through the Learning & Memory Open Access option.
References
- Acebes F, Solar P, Moris J, Loy I. 2012. Associative learning phenomena in the snail (Helix aspersa): conditioned inhibition. Learn Behav 40: 34–41. 10.3758/s13420-011-0042-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bazhenov M, Huerta R, Smith BH. 2013. A computational framework for understanding decision making through integration of basic learning rules. J Neurosci 33: 5686–5697. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4145-12.2013 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bhagavan S, Benatar S, Cobey S, Smith B. 1994. Effect of genotype but not of age or caste on olfactory learning performance in the honey bee, Apis mellifera. Anim Behav 48: 1357–1369. 10.1006/anbe.1994.1372 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Bitterman ME, Menzel R, Fietz A, Schäfer S. 1983. Classical conditioning of proboscis extension in honeybees (Apis mellifera). J Comp Psychol 97: 107–119. 10.1037/0735-7036.97.2.107 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Blenau W, Balfanz S, Baumann A. 2000. Amtyr1: characterization of a gene from honeybee (Apis mellifera) brain encoding a functional tyramine receptor. J Neurochem 74: 900–908. 10.1046/j.1471-4159.2000.0740900.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Burden CM, Elmore C, Hladun KR, Trumble JT, Smith BH. 2016. Acute exposure to selenium disrupts associative conditioning and long-term memory recall in honey bees (Apis mellifera). Ecotoxicol Environ Saf 127: 71–79. 10.1016/j.ecoenv.2015.12.034 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bush RR, Mosteller F. 1951. A mathematical model for simple learning. Psychol Rev 58: 313–323. 10.1037/h0054388 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chandra S, Smith BH. 1998. An analysis of synthetic processing of odor mixtures in the honeybee (Apis mellifera). J Exp Biol 201: 3113–3121. 10.1242/jeb.201.22.3113 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chandra SB, Hosler JS, Smith BH. 2000. Heritable variation for latent inhibition and its correlation with reversal learning in honeybees (Apis mellifera). J Comp Psychol 114: 86–97. 10.1037/0735-7036.114.1.86 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chandra SB, Wright GA, Smith BH. 2010. Latent inhibition in the honey bee, Apis mellifera: is it a unitary phenomenon? Anim Cogn 13: 805–815. 10.1007/s10071-010-0329-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Chen JY, Marachlian E, Assisi C, Huerta R, Smith BH, Locatelli F, Bazhenov M. 2015. Learning modifies odor mixture processing to improve detection of relevant components. J Neurosci 35: 179–197. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2345-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cook CN, Mosqueiro T, Brent CS, Ozturk C, Gadau J, Pinter-Wollman N, Smith BH. 2019. Individual differences in learning and biogenic amine levels influence the behavioural division between foraging honeybee scouts and recruits. J Anim Ecol 88: 236–246. 10.1111/1365-2656.12911 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cook CN, Lemanski NJ, Mosqueiro T, Ozturk C, Gadau J, Pinter-Wollman N, Smith BH. 2020. Individual learning phenotypes drive collective behavior. Proc Natl Acad Sci 117: 17949–17956. 10.1073/pnas.1920554117 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Couvillon PA, Ablan CD, Bitterman ME. 1999. Exploratory studies of inhibitory conditioning in honeybees (Apis mellifera). J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 25: 103–112. 10.1037/0097-7403.25.1.103 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Couvillon PA, Bumanglag AV, Bitterman ME. 2003. Inhibitory conditioning in honeybees. Q J Exp Psychol B 56: 359–370. 10.1080/02724990244000313 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Couvillon PA, Hsiung R, Cooke AM, Bitterman ME. 2005. The role of context in the inhibitory conditioning of honeybees. Q J Exp Psychol B 58: 59–67. 10.1080/02724990444000050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dunsmoor JE, Niv Y, Daw N, Phelps EA. 2015. Rethinking extinction. Neuron 88: 47–63. 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.028 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Ferguson HJ, Cobey S, Smith BH. 2001. Sensitivity to a change in reward is heritable in the honey bee, Apis mellifera. Anim Behav 61: 527–534. 10.1006/anbe.2000.1635 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Fernandez PC, Locatelli FF, Person-Rennell N, Deleo G, Smith BH. 2009. Associative conditioning tunes transient dynamics of early olfactory processing. J Neurosci 29: 10191–10202. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1874-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finke V, Baracchi D, Giurfa M, Scheiner R, Avarguès-Weber A. 2021. Evidence of cognitive specialization in an insect: proficiency is maintained across elemental and higher-order visual learning but not between sensory modalities in honey bees. J Exp Biol 224: jeb242470. 10.1242/jeb.242470 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Finke V, Scheiner R, Giurfa M, Avarguès-Weber A. 2023. Individual consistency in the learning abilities of honey bees: cognitive specialization within sensory and reinforcement modalities. Anim Cogn 26: 909–928. 10.1007/s10071-022-01741-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hellstern F, Malaka R, Hammer M. 1998. Backward inhibitory learning in honeybees: a behavioral analysis of reinforcement processing. Learn Mem 4: 429–444. 10.1101/lm.4.5.429 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hunt GJ, Page RE Jr., Fondrk MK, Dullum CJ. 1995. Major quantitative trait loci affecting honey bee foraging behavior. Genetics 141: 1537–1545. 10.1093/genetics/141.4.1537 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kehoe EJ, Horne AJ, Horne PS, Macrae M. 1994. Summation and configuration between and within sensory modalities in classical conditioning of the rabbit. Anim Learn Behav 22: 19–26. 10.3758/BF03199952 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Latshaw JS, Mazade RE, Petersen M, Mustard JA, Sinakevitch I, Wissler L, Guo X, Cook C, Lei H, Gadau J, et al. 2023. Tyramine and its Amtyr1 receptor modulate attention in honey bees (Apis mellifera). Elife 12: e83348. 10.7554/eLife.83348 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Li F, Lindsey JW, Marin EC, Otto N, Dreher M, Dempsey G, Stark I, Bates AS, Pleijzier MW, Schlegel P, et al. 2020. The connectome of the adult Drosophila mushroom body provides insights into function. Elife 9: e62576. 10.7554/eLife.62576 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Locatelli FF, Fernandez PC, Villareal F, Muezzinoglu K, Huerta R, Galizia CG, Smith BH. 2013. Nonassociative plasticity alters competitive interactions among mixture components in early olfactory processing. Eur J Neurosci 37: 63–79. 10.1111/ejn.12021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lubow RE. 1973. Latent inhibition. Psychol Bull 79: 398–407. 10.1037/h0034425 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lubow R. 1989. Latent inhibition and conditioned attention theory. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge. [Google Scholar]
- Mackintosh NJ. 1975. Blocking of conditioned suppression: role of the first compound trial. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 1: 335–345. 10.1037/0097-7403.1.4.335 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mackintosh N. 1983. Conditioning and associative learning. Clarendon Press, Oxford. [Google Scholar]
- Menzel R. 1990. Learning, memory, and ‘cognition’ in honeybees. In Neurobiology of comparative cognition, pp. 237–292. Lawrence Erlbaum, Hillsdale, NJ. [Google Scholar]
- Mosqueiro T, Cook C, Huerta R, Gadau J, Smith B, Pinter-Wollman N. 2017. Task allocation and site fidelity jointly influence foraging regulation in honeybee colonies. R Soc Open Sci 4: 170344. 10.1098/rsos.170344 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Page RE. 2013. The spirit of the hive: the mechanisms of social evolution. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA. [Google Scholar]
- Page RE, Erickson EH, Laidlaw HH. 1985. Closed population honey bee breeding 4. The distribution of sex alleles with top outcrossing. J Apic Res 24: 38–42. 10.1080/00218839.1985.11100646 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Papini MR. 2003. Comparative psychology of surprising nonreward. Brain Behav Evol 62: 83–95. 10.1159/000072439 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Papini MR, Bitterman ME. 1993. The two-test strategy in the study of inhibitory conditioning. J Exp Psychol Anim Behav Process 19: 342–352. 10.1037/0097-7403.19.4.342 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pearce JM, Hall G. 1980. A model for Pavlovian learning: variations in the effectiveness of conditioned but not of unconditioned stimuli. Psychol Rev 87: 532–552. 10.1037/0033-295X.87.6.532 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA. 1969. Pavlovian conditioned inhibition. Psychol Bull 72: 77–94. 10.1037/h0027760 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA. 1988. Behavioral studies of Pavlovian conditioning. Annu Rev Neurosci 11: 329–352. 10.1146/annurev.ne.11.030188.001553 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA, Holland PC. 1982. Behavioral studies of associative learning in animals. Annu Rev Psychol 33: 265–308. 10.1146/annurev.ps.33.020182.001405 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Rescorla RA, Wagner AR. 1972. A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement. In Classical conditioning II (ed. Black AH, Prokasy WF), pp. 64–99. Appleton-Century-Crofts, Norwalk, CT. [Google Scholar]
- Scheiner R, Page RE Jr., Erber J. 2001. Responsiveness to sucrose affects tactile and olfactory learning in preforaging honey bees of two genetic strains. Behav Brain Res 120: 67–73. 10.1016/S0166-4328(00)00359-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Scheiner R, Reim T, Søvik E, Entler BV, Barron AB, Thamm M. 2017. Learning, gustatory responsiveness and tyramine differences across nurse and forager honeybees. J Exp Biol 220: 1443–1450. 10.1242/jeb.152496 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sinakevitch IT, Daskalova SM, Smith BH. 2017. The biogenic amine tyramine and its receptor (AmTyr1) in olfactory neuropils in the honey bee. Front Syst Neurosci 11: 77. 10.3389/fnsys.2017.00077 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith BH. 1996. The role of attention in learning about odorants. Biol Bull 191: 76–83. 10.2307/1543065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith BH. 1997. An analysis of blocking in odorant mixtures: an increase but not a decrease in intensity of reinforcement produces unblocking. Behav Neurosci 111: 57–69. 10.1037/0735-7044.111.1.57 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith BH. 1998. Analysis of interaction in binary odorant mixtures. Physiol Behav 65: 397–407. 10.1016/S0031-9384(98)00142-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith BH, Menzel R. 1989. An analysis of variability in the feeding motor program of the honey bee; the role of learning in releasing a modal action pattern. Ethology 82: 68–81. 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1989.tb00488.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- Smith BH, Cobey S. 1994. The olfactory memory of the honeybee Apis mellifera. II. Blocking between odorants in binary mixtures. J Exp Biol 195: 91–108. 10.1242/jeb.195.1.91 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith BH, Burden CM. 2014. A proboscis extension response protocol for investigating behavioral plasticity in insects: application to basic, biomedical, and agricultural research. J Vis Exp 91: e51057. 10.3791/51057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sokal RR, Rohlf FJ. 1994. Biometry: the principles and practice of statistics in biological research. W.H. Freeman, New York. [Google Scholar]
- Sosa R, Ramírez MN. 2019. Conditioned inhibition: historical critiques and controversies in the light of recent advances. J Exp Psychol Anim Learn Cogn 45: 17–42. 10.1037/xan0000193 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sutton RS, Barto AG. 2018. Reinforcement learning: an introduction. MIT Press, Cambridge, MA. [Google Scholar]
- Tait C, Mattise-Lorenzen A, Lark A, Naug D. 2019. Interindividual variation in learning ability in honeybees. Behav Processes 167: 103918. 10.1016/j.beproc.2019.103918 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Thorn RS, Smith BH. 1997. The olfactory memory of the honeybee Apis mellifera. III. Bilateral sensory input is necessary for induction and expression of olfactory blocking. J Exp Biol 200: 2045–2055. 10.1242/jeb.200.14.2045 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]