Skip to main content
The BMJ logoLink to The BMJ
. 2001 Jun 2;322(7298):1355–1357. doi: 10.1136/bmj.322.7298.1355

Why should women have lower reference limits for haemoglobin and ferritin concentrations than men?

D Hugh Rushton a, Robin Dover b, Anthony W Sainsbury c, Michael J Norris a, Jeremy J H Gilkes d, Ian D Ramsay d
PMCID: PMC1120434  PMID: 11387188

The need to transport oxygen and remove carbon dioxide from animal tissue is a fundamental requirement of life, independent of age or sex.1 The role of iron in humans and many other mammals is central to this process.2,3 Haemoglobin concentration and red blood cell count are important diagnostic indicators for anaemia in humans and animals.

In prepubertal humans no major differences can be found between the sexes in red blood cell count or haemoglobin and serum ferritin concentrations.4 Only after the onset of menstruation does a difference emerge.4 Not until 10 years after the menopause does this situation revert in women, when the haemoglobin concentration becomes similar to that of aged matched men.4,5 This situation is compounded by the fact that modern women have a different reproductive history from those in the past. They reach sexual maturity at an earlier age, have fewer pregnancies, and breast feed for shorter periods; as such they menstruate for more years than women in the past. Menstruation is the principal cause of iron loss in women.68 Furthermore, 90% of UK females of childbearing age do not achieve the recommended daily intake of elemental iron (14.8 mg) from their diet.9 Evaluation of the haemoglobin concentration and red blood cell count of women from Canada, Central America, China, and the United States shows that this situation is widespread.4,1014 Women worldwide are at risk of being in a negative iron balance, and by current criteria if their haemoglobin concentration is less than 115 g/l they are deemed to be anaemic, whereas in men the cut-off point is 130 g/l.15

As far as the authors are aware, of the primates only humans show a sex difference in haemoglobin concentration and red blood cell count. A survey of the haematology of mammals also failed to establish a difference.16 Whereas most mammals, including the New World primates, do not menstruate, the Old World primates, which includes the apes, do. As with humans, Old World primates have premenses, breeding, and menopausal phases, and the frequency of menstruation is similar to humans, with a menses around every 28 days.1618 Unlike humans, however, no significant difference was found between the lower haemoglobin concentration (table) or red blood cell count (data not presented) of female primates and aged matched males sampled by the Zoological Society of London.17

Summary points

  • It is considered “normal” to find lower red blood cell counts and lower haemoglobin and serum ferritin concentrations in menstruating women than in age matched men

  • No other mammal, including the menstruating primates, exhibits such a sex difference, and neither is there a difference in humans before puberty or after menopause

  • Menstruation is the main cause of iron loss in women; 90% of UK women do not achieve the daily recommended intake of iron from their diet

  • Populations used to establish reference ranges for women contained a large proportion of those with iron deficiency, thus the lower limits are too low

  • This hidden deficiency has fundamental implications for women's health, particularly adolescent girls

  • Male reference ranges for ferritin and haematological parameters may be of more value when assessing iron status in women

Haem synthesis occurs largely in the mitochondria.12 There is no evidence showing either a reduction in mitochondrial number or a reduced ability for haem synthesis in women of childbearing age than in men. It might therefore be argued that the hormonal environment is a possible cause for the observed sex difference in red blood cell count and haemoglobin and serum ferritin concentrations in humans. Increased serum erythropoietin concentrations occur in response to anaemia, which in turn increases iron absorption from the diet. Hallberg et al have shown that men and women of the same iron status absorb the same amounts of iron from the same diet.19 If they are correct then there should be no difference in the erythropoietin concentrations of men and women with “normal” serum ferritin concentrations matched for age and weight. Similarly no sex difference can be found for thyroxine; therefore, with the exception of iron and the sex hormones there is no evidence of lower concentrations for any other parameter involved in haem synthesis in women of childbearing age than there is in men.20 Further, the data from menstruating Old World primates suggest that a difference in sex hormones is unlikely to be responsible for the sex difference in humans. Consequently, of all the parameters involved, iron is the only one with a measured difference.

The mean upper limit for haemoglobin concentration in primates that menstruate is significantly higher in males than it is in females, similar to humans. However, this is not the case in primates that do not menstruate (table), suggesting that menses is responsible for limiting the upper haemoglobin concentration in non-human primates that menstruate.17 Perhaps the same may be true of humans, with an iron deficient diet significantly influencing the lower haemoglobin concentration in women.

No evidence supports a view that women of reproductive age have a lower biological requirement for any haem parameters than do aged matched men. The data from humans point to the possibility that the current lower reference levels for red blood cell count and haemoglobin and serum ferritin concentrations in women have been derived from sampling populations that are deficient in iron. Consequently, it would seem that a large number of women spend a major part of their lives with a negative iron balance arising from a dietary imbalance. Iron deficiency is the most important nutritional problem affecting humans around the world.21 The presence of haem iron in the diet of humans enhances the absorption of non-haem iron, and the trend towards diets with a lower haem iron content may add to the problem of dietary insufficiency.22 In addition, the typical UK diet contains many common products that can limit iron absorption—for example, dairy produce, cereals, and tannin21—thereby compounding the situation.

The deleterious effects of iron deficiency and iron deficient anaemia are due partly to impaired delivery of oxygen to the tissues and partly to a deficiency of iron containing compounds, especially enzymes, in various tissues.2 Restlessness and irritability in children who are deficient in iron has been related to increased concentrations of catecholamines, which return to normal after treatment with iron.22 Lower IQ scores have been found in UK and American adolescent girls who are deficient in iron.23,24 Abnormalities in response to infection have been shown in people who are deficient in iron, where decreased neutrophil function and impaired T cell proliferation occurs.3 Studies in humans have confirmed the findings from animals of reduced work capacity and poor work performance in iron deficient anaemia.25 Reduced thermoregulation has also been confirmed in people who are iron deficient where norepinephrine concentration was increased, and differences were found in concentrations of epinephrine, dopamine, triiodothyronine, and thyroxine. Normal concentrations for these parameters were restored after iron supplementation.25 Hair loss has been reported in women with iron deficiency, which responded to iron supplementation.26,27 The quantity of non-haem iron in the brain is to a large extent independent of the iron stores in the body.20 Overall, 10% of the iron complement of the adult brain is present at birth, and by the age of 10 this has reached 50%; however, not until the early 20s is the full adult complement achieved.22 Although most of the deleterious effects of iron deficiency can be reversed by appropriate iron supplementation, of concern is the possibility that if during the adolescent years a shortfall exists, then, as in rats, humans may not be able to make up the deficiency by iron supplementation, and adolescent girls may enter adult life with a compromised complement of iron in the brain.2025

We propose that the current “normal” values for red blood cell count and haemoglobin and serum ferritin concentration cited by laboratories were obtained from sampling populations that contained a large proportion of women with iron deficiency. We suggest using reference ranges for men for these parameters when assessing the haematological and iron status of women. By current criteria, iron deficiency is the commonest cause of anaemia in the world, with more than half a billion people experiencing adverse effects.28 If this hypothesis is accepted then noticeably more women will be classified as being iron deficient or anaemic. Reclassification of red blood cell count and haemoglobin and serum ferritin concentrations to normal values for men would be expected to have fundamental and positive implications for women's health and welfare.

Table.

Lower and upper limits of haemoglobin concentration from various non-human primates

Species Haemoglobin concentration (g/l)
Males
Females
Lower limit Upper limit Lower limit Upper limit
Non-menstruating primates:
 Lemurs (n=31) 143 203 140 204
 Lorises and bushbabies (n=25) 124 210 120 194
 Marmosets and tamarins (n=37) 125 196 125 188
 New World monkeys (n=150) 119 195 123 190
Mean 128 201 127* 194*
Menstruating primates:
 Old World Monkeys (n=121) 110 175 103 151
 Apes (n=166) 96 184 96 160
Means 103 180 99* 155
*

P>0.05. P<0.01. 

Acknowledgments

We thank Professor Hideo Uno, Dr Julian Barth and Dr Chris Gummer, Peter Cundall, and Nina Busuttil for their help, comments, and suggestions concerning the manuscript.

Footnotes

Competing interests: None declared.

References

  • 1.Bothwell TH, Charlton RW, Cook JD, Finch CA. Iron metabolism in man. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific; 1979. pp. 7–43. [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hallberg L. Iron absorption and iron deficiency. Human nutrition. Clin Nutr. 1982;36C:259–278. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Dallman PR. Biochemical basis for the manifestations of iron deficiency. Ann Rev Nutr. 1986;6:13–40. doi: 10.1146/annurev.nu.06.070186.000305. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Valberg LS, Sorbie J, Ludwig J, Pelletier O. Serum ferritin and the iron status of Canadians. Can Med Assoc J. 1976;114:417–421. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Myers AM, Saunders CRG, Chalmers DG. The haemoglobin level of fit elderly people. Lancet. 1968;iiAug 3:261–263. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(68)92358-1. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Hallberg L, Hogdahl AM, Nilsson L, Rybo G. Menstrual blood loss—a population study. Variation at different ages and attempts to define normality. Acta Obstet Gynec Scand. 1966;45:320–351. doi: 10.3109/00016346609158455. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Frassinelli-Gunderson EP, Margen S, Brown JR. Iron stores in users of oral contraceptive agents. Am J Clin Nutr. 1985;41:703–712. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/41.4.703. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Simon TL, Garry PJ, Hooper EM. Iron stores in blood donors. JAMA. 1981;245:2038–2043. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Ministry of Agriculture, Fisheries and Food. The dietary and nutritional survey of British adults—further analysis. London: HMSO; 1994. pp. 72–75. [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Viteri FE, de Tuna V, Guzman MA. Normal haematological values in the Central American population. Br J Haematol. 1972;23:189–204. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2141.1972.tb03472.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ji G, Su Z, Bo-ling S, Hui-min F, Li-hui H. Menstrual blood loss and hematologic indices in healthy Chinese women. J Reprod Med. 1987;32:822–826. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Looker AC, Dallman PR, Carroll MD, Gunter EW, Johnson CL. Prevalence of iron deficiency in the United States. JAMA. 1997;277:973–976. doi: 10.1001/jama.1997.03540360041028. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Cook JD, Finch CA, Smith N. Evaluation of the iron status of a population. Blood. 1976;48:449–455. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Cook JD, Skikne BS, Lynch SR, Reusser ME. Estimates of iron sufficiency in the US population. Blood. 1986;68:726–731. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Hoffbrand AV, Pettit JE. Essential haematology. 3rd ed. Oxford: Blackwell Scientific; 1993. p. 419. [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Jain NC, Jain AH. Essentials of veterinary hematology. Philadelphia: Lea and Febiger; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Lynx. Haematological and biochemical data base. London: Zoological Society, Jan; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Walker ML. Menopause in female rhesus monkeys. Am J Primatol. 1995;35:59–71. doi: 10.1002/ajp.1350350106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Hallberg L, Hultén L, Gramatkovski E. Iron absorption from the whole diet in men: how effective is the regulation of iron absoption? J Clin Nutr. 1997;66:347–356. doi: 10.1093/ajcn/66.2.347. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hallgren B, Sourander P. The effect of age on the non-haemin iron in the human brain. J Neurochem. 1958;3:41–51. doi: 10.1111/j.1471-4159.1958.tb12607.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Hallberg L, Sandström B, Aggett PJ. Iron, zinc and other trace elements. In: Garrow SJ, James WPT, editors. Human nutrition and dietetics, 9th ed. Edinburgh: Churchill Livingstone; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Dillmann E, Johnson DG, Martin J, Mackler B, Finch CA. Catecholamine elevation in iron deficiency. Am J Physiol. 1979;237:337–81R. doi: 10.1152/ajpregu.1979.237.5.R297. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Nelson M. First report from the House of Commons Education and Employment Committee. School meals. London: Stationery Office; 1999. Iron status and cognitive function in UK adolescent girls; an intervention study. . (HC 96.) [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Bruner AB, Joffe G, Duggan AK, Casella JF, Brandt J. Randomised study of cognitive effects of iron supplementation in non-anaemic iron deficient adolescent girls. Lancet. 1996;348:992–996. doi: 10.1016/S0140-6736(96)02341-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Srrimshaw NS. Functional consequences of iron deficiency in human populations [review] J Nutr Sci Vitamonol. 1984;30:47–83. doi: 10.3177/jnsv.30.47. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Hård S. Non-anemic iron deficiency as an etiological factor in diffuse loss of hair of the scalp in women. Acta Derm Venereo. 1963;43:562–569. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Rushton DH, Ramsay ID. The importance of adequate serum ferritin levels during oral cyproterone acetate and ethinyl oestradiol treatment of diffuse androgen-dependent alopecia in women. Clin Endocrinol. 1992;36:421–427. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2265.1992.tb01470.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Andrews NC. Medical progress: disorders of iron metabolism. N Engl J Med. 1999;341:1986–1995. doi: 10.1056/NEJM199912233412607. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Articles from BMJ : British Medical Journal are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES