Skip to main content
Schizophrenia Bulletin Open logoLink to Schizophrenia Bulletin Open
. 2022 Nov 4;3(1):sgac068. doi: 10.1093/schizbullopen/sgac068

Hallucinations in the Child and Adolescent “Ultra-High Risk” population: A Systematic Review

Michelangelo Di Luzio 1,, Maria Pontillo 2, Cristina Di Vincenzo 3, Domenica Bellantoni 4, Francesco Demaria 5, Stefano Vicari 6,7
PMCID: PMC11205958  PMID: 39144782

Abstract

Background and Hypothesis

“Ultra-high risk” for psychosis young adults are assumed to be at higher risk of developing a psychotic spectrum disorder. Predominantly, the ultrahigh-risk population is aged 18–35 years, but it may also include younger children and adolescents. Individuals in this population experience psychosis prodromes in the form of attenuated or brief psychotic symptoms (particularly perceptual abnormalities). Albeit diagnosis is made via structured interviews, such measures fail to sufficiently assess the precise form and content of perceptual abnormalities, especially as they manifest in children and adolescents.

Study Design

The present study involved a systematic review of the literature on perceptual abnormalities (particularly hallucinations) in ultrahigh-risk children and adolescents.

Results

The analysis reviewed five studies and drew conclusions about the perceptual abnormalities (ie, hallucinations) experienced by the study samples, focusing on form, content, and associations with other symptoms. Of note, 2 of the investigated studies suggested a relationship between hallucinations and experiences of childhood trauma.

Conclusions

The transition to psychosis and experiences of childhood trauma could correspond to different types of hallucinations in ultrahigh-risk children and adolescents. This knowledge could improve the identification of prodromal states in the young, ultrahigh-risk population.

Keywords: transition, prodromes, perception, psychosis, UHR, trauma

Introduction

Over the past 20 years, increasing attention has been placed on potential predictive factors of psychotic disorders (eg, schizophrenia). As part of this work, the “ultra-high risk” (UHR) state—also known as the “at-risk mental state” (ARMS) or “clinical high risk” (CHR) state—has been defined as a risk condition for developing full-blown psychosis in young adults.1–3 UHR individuals manifest typical symptoms of schizophrenia spectrum disorders (eg, thought disorders, perceptual abnormalities) in attenuated or brief forms. In addition, some UHR individuals could have only a genetic risk (first-degree relatives with a psychotic disorder) associated with a decrease in global functioning. Some authors pointed out as even a genetic liability for psychosis can lead to typical gray matter alterations in the brain structure.4 Accordingly, researchers have developed the Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental States (CAARMS)1 and the Structured Interview for Prodromal Symptoms (SIPS)5 to assess symptoms associated with the UHR state6,7 and to identify individuals at higher risk of developing a schizophrenia spectrum disorder.

Importantly, both the CAARMS and the SIPS assess changes in perception, with respect to sub-threshold hallucinations (in frequency and/or intensity). Hallucinations are “a perception-like experience with the clarity and impact of a true perception but without the external stimulation of the relevant sensory organ.” 8 In a true psychotic disorder, hallucinations are not caused by psychoactive substances or organic pathologies.8 In particular, schizophrenia is associated with verbal auditory hallucinations. However, perceptual abnormalities and hallucinations may also be nonspecific, especially when they appear isolated, and several studies have accounted for their presence in even healthy populations or among individuals who do not go on to develop psychotic disorders or require specialist medical assistance—in particular children and adolescents9–12

The CAARMS and SIPS items that assess hallucinations (eg, P4 in the SIPS) seem to be not enough specific related to the risk of conversion in psychosis.13–15 For this reason, it is important to differentiate between perceptual abnormalities of varying forms and with varying contents, in order to improve our understanding of the relationship between specific forms of perceptual abnormalities and other psychotic symptoms and the conversion to psychosis, especially in children and adolescents.

Indeed, the UHR child and adolescent population represent an age group that shows a specific clinical pattern with respect to the adult population.16–18 Several studies17,18 have reported that, in addition to attenuated positive symptoms, UHR children and adolescents exhibit several negative symptoms (especially social anhedonia) and significant functional impairment. For these patients, worse global and academic functioning will likely present in the first year,17 similar to the profile of First-Episode Psychosis (FEP) children and adolescents. Furthermore, UHR states at younger ages showed less specificity in predicting conversion to psychosis.19–21 In light of the above observations, the aim of this review was to examine the perceptual abnormalities in UHR children and adolescents, with a focus on hallucinations, as these are considered a close approximation of psychotic disorder symptomatology. Such insight would allow us to refine the tools used to identify UHR children and adolescents, and thus improve the early detection of psychosis. Therefore it seems important to deepen this topic as there are few literatures about how prevention in younger people is an important issue in mental health services.

Methods

Search Strategy

We searched PubMed, EMBASE, and PsycINFO for studies published between database inception and January 1, 2022, using the search terms: “High AND risk AND psychosis AND clinical AND child AND adolescent AND hallucination.” All members of the research team agreed on the search strategy and contributed to the literature review. The selected articles met the following eligibility criteria: (1) they were original research studies, (2) they included children and young adults with a UHR diagnosis,3 as assessed by the SIPS or the CAARMS, (3) they applied interviews or questionnaires to assess hallucinations and discriminated between various forms of hallucination (ie, auditory, visual, tactile, somatic, and olfactory), (4) they provided separate data for UHR individuals with at least 2 different types of hallucination, and (5) they assessed children, adolescent and young adults populations with a maximum age of 21 years and mean age of 18 years.

Eligibility and Study Selection

The following studies were excluded: (1) reviews and meta-analyses (however, the reference lists of these studies were reviewed to identify possible eligible studies that eluded the initial database search) (ie, “Review”), (2) studies that did not assess individuals with a UHR diagnosis (eg, studies that only assessed patients with full-blown psychosis) (ie, “No UHR”), (3) studies that did not assess hallucinations or perceptual abnormalities and did not discriminate between different forms of hallucinations (ie, “No Ha”), (4) studies that assessed a UHR population with an age of 22 years and/or a mean age of 19 years or older (ie, “Adults”), (5) editorials, letters to the editor with no data, opinion articles not supported by data, and surveys of doctor opinions about hallucinations in UHR young adults (ie, “Opinions”), (6) studies with no relation to the subject matter (ie, “Unrelated”), (7) case reports or case series (ie, “Case”), (8) studies that did not provide separate data for UHR subjects versus healthy controls or individuals with other psychiatric diagnoses (ie, “Lumping), (9) protocols and ongoing studies, (10) qualitative studies with no data; (11) surveys or consensus meetings; (12) studies that employed a research design that was inadequate to assess change, and (13) corrections to a previous study (although such corrections were taken into account, where necessary, for the review of the original study).

All authors agreed on the study inclusion and exclusion criteria, which were established through two Delphi rounds. The research was conducted according to the Preferred Reporting Items for Systematic reviews and Meta-Analyses (PRISMA) guidelines.22 The online supplementary includes the PRISMA checklist and flowchart, as well as the detailed results and inclusion/exclusion data (see online supplementary, supplementary figures 1 and 2).

Data Extraction and Synthesis

Data from the selected articles were extracted and recorded on a standardized spreadsheet. Specifically, the following variables were captured: first author, year of publication, sample size, age of participants, sex ratio (male/female), study design (including the type of interview or questionnaire used to gather details about perceptual abnormalities), and findings regarding the association between perceptual symptomatology and the UHR profile in young adults.

Risk of Bias Assessment

To assess the degree of reliability and quality of the review and to critically examine the results of the selected studies, a risk of bias analysis was conducted using the indications and criteria proposed by the Agency for Health Care Research and Quality.23 The online supplementary set out the criteria for risk of bias assessment. The assessment of biases according to the reported criteria (selection bias, performance bias, detection bias, attrition bias, and reporting bias) has been conducted on each study and a bias level has been identified for each study (low, medium, or high). M. Di Luzio, D. Bellantoni, and C. Di Vincenzo evaluated the studies included independently, the differences in the evaluation were resolved by discussion.

Results

Search Results

The abovementioned search retrieved an initial set of 35 articles, ranging in publication from May 2000 to November 2021. An additional 11 studies were identified from a review of reference lists, ranging in publication from December 2004 to August 2020. The exclusion criteria led to the rejection of 41 articles, resulting in a final set of 5 articles (table 1)(for each study reason of rejection see online supplementary, supplementary table 1), which ranged in publication from April 2013 to July 2019. All of the included articles were in English, although the non-English language was not established as an exclusionary criterion. The PRISMA flowchart (see Online supplementary, supplementary figure 1) displays the full search results, including the reasons for exclusion, where relevant.

Table 1.

Summary of Included Studies

Study Population Design Results Conclusions/Observations
Niles et al.,24 164 ids with UHR (F 66;
M 98; age x̄ =16.7 ± 4.1).
Prospective observational study.
SIPS
Separate component ratings for auditory, visual, somatic/tactile, and gustatory/olfactory PA were coded.
Auditory PA is further characterized in AVH and NAVH.
CHR followed for 6–24 months or until conversion to psychosis.
105 ids had auditory Ha, of these 32 had AVH and 73 NVAH.
None of the P4 modality component scores significantly predicted days to conversion in the multivariate Cox survival analyses (P > .05). Cox proportional hazards regression demonstrated a significant effect for AVH (P = .004) and an opposite effect for NVAH (P = .008).
None of the P4 component subscales demonstrated a significant relationship to conversion status in a multivariate regression (P > .05).
No significant evidence that the SIPS perceptual abnormality (P4) score, taken as the original composite or split into modality-specific sub-scores, predicts conversion to psychosis or time to conversion in a sample of 164 UHR.
Significant interaction between auditory Ha severity and verbal content, with severity of AVH predicting conversion and the presence of NAVH conferring protection against conversion.
While hearing voices may indicate increased risk, many types of perceptual (auditory or not) abnormal experiences often considered pathological may not be so.
Thompson et al.29 120 ids with UHR (F 71; M 49; age x̄ =18.3 ± 2.7).
Cases 60, Controls 60.
Retrospective case-control study.
An auditing tool was developed to assess psychotic symptoms and specifically symptom content.
Separate perceptual abnormalities assessed: Any Ha, auditory Ha, non-affective auditory illusions, hypnagogic/hypnopompic Ha, visual illusion/distortions, visual Ha, other Ha (tactile, somatic, olfactory, gustatory, complex).
Retrospective study differentiating ids who had transitioned to psychosis and who did not.
None of the PA assessed were significantly associated with transition to psychosis at univariate analysis.
Any Ha (P = .56), auditory Ha (P = .52), non-affective auditory illusions (P = .17), hypnagogic/hypnopompic Ha (P = .29), visual illusion/distortions (P = .15), visual Ha (P = 1.0), other Ha (P = .63).
None of the PA assessed were significantly associated with transition to psychosis.
O’ Connor et al.30 118 ids with UHR (F 69; M 49;
age x̄ =18.3 ± 2.7).
Cases:59, Controls:59.
Retrospective case-control study.
An auditing tool was developed to assess psychotic symptoms and specifically symptom content.
PA were divided into: all PA, APA, VPA, and “other” PA (tactile, olfactory, somatic, gustatory). In addition, verbal APA were divided into: abusive, second-person, third person and command. Logistic regression analysis was used to assess association between comorbid diagnosis or an adverse event and PA variables.
PA significantly more in F (P = .001), in comorbidity with depression (P .004) and in employment (P = .001).
APA most common type (50.8%) with VPA also common (46.6%).
PA is associated with higher GAF scores (P = .001).
DD is associated with all PA (P = .004) and in particular with VPA (P = .02).
BPD associated with APA (P = .04).
Childhood trauma associated with all PA (P = .000), VPA (P = .01) and APA (P = .02).
Childhood bullying is associated with all PA (P = .01).
UHR with PA are mostly F, in employment and with higher GAF.
UHR with PA have more DD and have a history of trauma or a history of bullying.
No association was found between trauma and specific hallucinatory content.
BPD associated with second-person APA (P = .04).
No association between a childhood history of trauma or bullying and hallucinatory content (all P > .1).
O’Connor et al.25 118 ids with UHR (F 69; M 49;
age x̄ =18.3 ± 2.7).
Cases:59, Controls:59
Retrospective case-control study.
An auditing tool was developed to assess psychotic symptoms and specifically symptom content.
Ha is divided into any Ha, auditory Ha, visual Ha, and associated or not with different psychotic symptoms (delusion, thought disorder, ego boundary disorder, negative symptoms, dissociation) or substance use.
At univariate analysis, Ha alone at baseline are not associated with a reduced risk of transition to psychosis (P = .67).
Any Ha and no dissociation (P = .03), visual Ha with no ego boundary disorder (P = .044), and visual Ha with no substance use (P = .045) were associated with a reduced risk of transition to psychosis (hazard ratio < 1).
At multivariate analysis, Ha without any thought disorder (P = .045) and visual Ha without substance misuse (P = .045) were associated with a reduced risk of transition to psychosis.
UHR ids who present with Ha alone were not found to be at significantly lower risk of transition to psychosis.
Ha in the absence of thought disorder reduces the risk of transition to psychosis by around 40%.
Individual forms of Ha (eg, auditory and visual) in isolation may not be associated with increased or reduced risk of transition to psychosis.
Visual Ha in the absence of substance use were associated with a 55% reduced likelihood of transition to psychosis.
Velthorst et al.31 127 ids with UHR (F 74;M 53; age x̄ =18.2 ± 2.7). Retrospective case-control study.
An auditing tool was developed to assess psychotic symptoms and specifically symptom content.
Relationship between Ha form and content and trauma [physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional abuse and neglect and other (ie, verbal abuse, domestic violence, witnessing of shooting, watching someone suicide, death of a close relative/friend, car accident, severe physical injury)]
Higher PA intensity in history of physical abuse (P = .023) and of “others” abuse (P = .007) but physical abuse was no longer significant when other types of trauma were adjusted for, instead “others” abuse remains significant (P = .039).
No significant difference with respect to auditory Ha.
Instead, visual Ha linked with physical abuse (P = .02) but this association was no longer significant after adjusting for other types of trauma (P = .07).
Sexual trauma associated with
PA with abusive content (P = .008). This association remains significant after adjustment for other types of traumas (P = .02).
Small but significant associations were found between a history of particular types of trauma and intensity of attenuated psychotic symptoms. Specifically, having experienced physical abuse or an “other type” of trauma during childhood was associated with a higher intensity of PA although the physical abuse association must be considered cautiously.
Stronger association between physical abuse and visual as opposed to auditory Ha.

Abbreviations: APA, auditory perceptual abnormalities; AVH, auditory verbal hallucinations; BPD, borderline personality disorder; CAARMS, Comprehensive Assessment of At-Risk Mental States; DD, depressive disorder; F, females; GAF, global assessment of functioning; Ha, hallucination/s; ids, individuals; M, males; NAVH, non-verbal auditory hallucinations; OPCRIT, Operational Criteria for Psychotic Illness; PA, perceptual abnormalities; P4, fourth positive symptoms item of the SOPS; SIPS, Structured Interview for Psychosis-risk Syndromes; x̄, mean age; UHR, ultrahigh risk for psychosis; VPA, visual perceptual abnormalities.

Overview of the Included Studies

Three of the included studies focused on the relationship between perceptual abnormalities and the risk of developing full-blown psychosis. The remaining 2 studies investigated the link between hallucinations and childhood trauma. The following 2 sub-sections discuss each of these sets of studies, respectively.

Hallucination and the Risk of Developing Full-Blown Psychosis

Niles et al.24 focused on the role of perceptual abnormalities in the risk of transitioning to full-blown psychosis in 164 UHR individuals with a mean age of 16.7 years. The SIPS was used to assess both the UHR state and perceptual abnormalities, and the P4 score was taken as the original composite, split into 4 sub-scores: Auditory, visual, somatic/tactile, and gustatory/olfactory. Furthermore, auditory hallucinations were characterized as either verbal auditory hallucinations (AVH) or nonverbal auditory hallucinations (NAVH). UHR outpatients were prospectively assessed at follow-ups (6–24 months) or until conversion to psychosis. The structured clinical interview for the DSM-IV (SCID-IV) was used to diagnose psychosis. The results showed that neither the P4 score nor any of the four sub-scores predicted conversion to psychosis or time to conversion. While the presence of AVH was significant in predicting the transition to psychosis, the presence of NAVH seemed to confer protection against this transition.24 These data come from 105 APS patients with auditory hallucinations, of these 32 with AVH and 73 with NAVH. 15 of 32 subjects with AVH had a conversion with a conversion rate of 46.9%. Instead in NAVH individuals, only 13 of 73 showed psychosis with a conversion rate of 17.8%. The conversion rate of the total population was 27%.

In 2016, O’Connor et al.25 retrospectively analyzed data for 118 UHR out-patients with a mean age of 18.3 years, using the CAARMS, the Operational Criteria for Psychotic Illness (OPCRIT),26 and an original audit tool (AT).25 Both the CAARMS27,28 and the OPCRIT were used to assess the transition to full-blown psychosis, while the CAARMS, alone, was used to diagnose the UHR state. The AT was used to gather detailed information on participants’ hallucinations, with reference to all of the criteria assessed in the OPCRIT as well as additional symptoms related to hallucinatory content.25 Hallucinations were divided into three subgroups (ie, “any,” auditory, and visual) and associated with psychotic symptoms (ie, delusion, thought disorder, ego boundary disorder, negative symptoms, and dissociation) and substance abuse. The authors also evaluated the association between transition to psychosis and each hallucination sub-group, both independently and in conjunction with each psychotic symptom and substance use. For this purpose, the UHR sample was divided into those who ultimately transitioned to psychosis (cases) and those who did not (controls). The results showed that hallucinations, alone, did not significantly lower the risk of transition to psychosis. However, some associations between hallucination and other conditions could reduce the risk of conversion. This “protection” effect comes from the analysis of the hazard ratio for each symptomatology association. Those associations with a significant hazard ratio < 1 were considered as a protective factor to conversion. In this way hallucinations in the absence of thought disorders were significant in reducing this risk. The same was observed for hallucinations without dissociation symptoms or ego boundary disorders. Moreover, visual and auditory hallucinations, alone, seemed unrelated to the risk of psychosis. However visual hallucinations in the absence of drug misuse were associated with a reduced risk of conversion to psychosis.25

In 2013, Thompson et al.29 addressed psychosis conversion risk in young UHR outpatients, using the CAARMS to assess UHR state. They divided the sample into those who ultimately developed a frank psychotic illness and those who did not (ie, controls). Both the CAARMS and the OPCRIT were used to diagnose psychosis. The AT developed by O’Connor et al. in 201625 was used to assess hallucinations. The authors retrospectively assessed 120 UHR subjects (mean age 18.3 years) presenting with a range of perceptual abnormalities, including: Any hallucinations, auditory hallucinations, non-affective auditory illusions, hypnagogic/hypnopompic hallucinations, visual illusion/distortions, visual hallucinations, and “other” hallucinations (ie, tactile, somatic, olfactory, gustatory, and complex). The statistical analyses showed that none of the perceptual abnormalities were significantly associated with transition to psychosis.29

Hallucinations and Trauma

In 2017, O’Connor et al.30 retrospectively observed the correlations between a history of childhood trauma, perceptual abnormality variables, and comorbid conditions in a sample of 118 UHR outpatients with a mean age of 18.3 years. The CAARMS was used to diagnose the UHR state. The authors divided the UHR sample into a case group of participants who ultimately transitioned to psychosis and a control group who did not (according to the CAARMS). An AT was applied to measure perceptual abnormalities25 in the following categories: “any,” auditory, visual, and “other” (ie, tactile, olfactory, somatic, and gustatory). In addition, the authors differentiated between abusive, second-person, third-person, and command verbal auditory hallucinations. The logistic regression analysis showed that visual perceptual abnormalities were associated with depressive disorder, while auditory hallucinations were mainly linked to borderline personality disorder. Childhood trauma was significantly associated with perceptual abnormalities, in general, and visual and auditory hallucinations, specifically; however, childhood bullying was only associated with perceptual abnormalities, in general. No association was found between specific verbal hallucinatory contents and trauma, and only borderline personality disorder emerged as correlated with second-person verbal auditory hallucinations.30

Finally, Velthorst et al.31 retrospectively assessed 127 UHR outpatients with a mean age of 18.2 years. Participants were divided into control and case groups, depending on whether or not they ultimately transitioned to psychosis. The CAARMS was used to assess both the UHR state and the transition to full-blown psychosis. An AT25 was applied to investigate the relationship between hallucination form and content and different types of trauma (ie, physical abuse, sexual abuse, emotional abuse and neglect, “other” [ie, verbal abuse, domestic violence, witnessing a shooting, witnessing a suicide, death of a close relative/friend, car accident, and severe physical injury]). The AT assessed childhood trauma using multiple items related to the abovementioned subcategories. The results showed a small but significant association between childhood physical abuse or “other” trauma and higher-intensity perceptual abnormalities; however, the association with physical abuse should be considered with caution. Furthermore, relative to auditory hallucinations, visual hallucinations were found to be more strongly correlated with physical abuse. Finally, perceptual abnormalities with abusive content emerged as associated with sexual trauma.31

Reliability and Quality of Included Studies

Four studies were classified as having medium levels of risk of bias while only one was considered with a low presence of bias. A table with a summary of the risk of bias assessment for each study is reported in the Online supplementary materials (Online supplementary table 2). The main possible biases reported were about the selection and the detection bias. In particular, most of the studies have a medium risk of bias level because of the retrospective study design. Albeit these retrospective studies25,29–31 satisfied majority of the criteria for risk of bias assessment. Main concerns about their methodology were about the not complete blindness of research assistants with respect to conversion status and the retrospective extraction of data from the initial clinical assessment. Moreover, in these studies, the auditing tool developed by the authors was considered as a possible source of bias because it is not a tool validated by scientific literature. However, this tool was developed by a research group of authors expert in UHR subjects evaluation, and that significantly contributed to the definition of UHR criteria.1,32 The only one study classified with a low risk of bias24 shows a good satisfaction of risk of bias criteria. Nevertheless, we point out a possible bias in selecting only UHR subjects with attenuated psychotic syndrome (APS) and the retrospective collection of severity scores for perceptual modalities. In general, the review shows a medium quality of evidence. However main limitations to quality evidence come from the small number of included studies and the overlap between the assessed populations, rather than the risk of bias of examined studies.

Discussion

Hallucinatory Symptoms

The present research found that the relationship between hallucinations and the risk of conversion to psychosis in UHR children and adolescents is not unique. In general, hallucinations do not appear to be significantly correlated with the risk of developing psychosis, even when hallucinatory subgroups are considered (ie, auditory, visual, tactile, olfactory, gustative, and somatic).24,25,29 Rather, significant correlations with the risk of transitioning to psychosis only emerge with more complex characterizations of symptomology. Specifically, it appears that hallucinations without an associated thought disorder or the presence of nonverbal auditory hallucinations are less suggestive of the transition to psychosis.24,25 Furthermore, only auditory hallucinations with verbal content appear to be associated with a higher risk of psychosis development.24 In examining these studies it must be said that the samples have a medium size and the time of follow-up may be a limitation. However, these issues are common problems in studies about the risk of conversion. For example, the time of follow-up is aligned with similar studies on this topic.3 The studies with a retrospective design have many limitations, as pointed out in the risk of bias assessment, but show a longer observational time of follow-up. The “others” perceptual abnormalities as the tactile/somatic or the olfactory appear to be less reported. In particular when there is no association with auditory or visual abnormalities. However, these kinds of hallucinations have to be deepened in the UHR young population as they seem to be relevant in psychotic disorders. The somatic/tactile ones especially.32,33 Nevertheless the findings are partly aligned with the results of studies involving older samples (mean age 19–21 years), which have found auditory—rather than visual—hallucinations to be more predictive of conversion to psychosis.14,34 Additionally in two included studies24,25 emerges an interesting data: Some types of hallucinations could be protective against conversion to psychosis. This result is understandable if we reflect on what will be said later about hallucinations as alterations of thought. In fact, hallucinations could be significant for a risk of psychosis only if present as an alteration of thought (eg, auditory with a verbal content) and therefore associated with other thought-disorder symptoms. The literature on psychotic disorders (especially schizophrenia) shows that the most characteristic hallucinations are auditory verbal (eg, voices dialoguing, commenting, or presenting imperatives), which are more associated with thought disorders (eg, disorganization or delusions), rather than visual. Consequently, changes in perception in the absence of thought disturbances—or with visual or auditory but non-verbal hallucinations—do not appear to be typical manifestations of a schizophrenia spectrum disorder.35 Therefore, the classification of hallucinations could be improved by differentiating perceptual abnormalities according to form and content, and thereby identifying some as more significant (eg, verbal auditory hallucinations)—even if attenuated or brief—in predicting conversion to psychosis. In addition, as the (non-)association between hallucinations and other psychotic-like symptoms seems important,25 diagnostic weight should be given to not only specific hallucinatory symptoms, but also the cooccurrence of symptoms. In fact, the delineation of detailed “constellations” of symptoms may increase our ability to recognize at-risk children and adolescents and provide more targeted psychiatric care.13,36

The investigated studies also examined the correlation between positive psychotic symptoms (ie, hallucinations) and childhood trauma. While the findings were heterogeneous, 2 studies suggest an association between perceptual abnormalities and trauma.30,31 However these results are gathered on a small population and need to be confirmed on larger samples. Indeed no one of the trauma sub-groups overcome the number of 52 subjects. In particular, in Velthorst31 the group with sexual trauma was composed of 26 individuals, and the one with physical abuse of 35. While in O’Connor30 the group with a history of trauma was composed of 52 individuals but there was no further sub-group classification of trauma and the characterization of perceptual abnormalities is not deepened.

Although our results about trauma are poor, there is extensive literature associating childhood trauma with the development of psychotic disorders,37–39 as well as with APS and the UHR state,40–42 for a meta-analysis see: Kraan, T. et al..43 Specifically some studies confirmed the association between a history of childhood trauma and higher perceptual abnormalities on psychotic patients,44,45 while others do not clearly delineate this relationship in UHR young adults.41,42 Moreover results remain nonunique also focusing on form and content of perceptual abnormalities in presence of history of trauma. In some studies noted as the history of trauma, in early psychotic patients, could promote the onset of visual hallucinations46rather than verbal auditory hallucinations47 or specific contents in auditory hallucinations.30 However a study by Begemann and colleagues48 shows a great presence of malignant/omnipotence AVH in psychotic patients with multiple traumas. This latter interesting data, beyond their heterogeneity, could suggest the cooccurence of PTSD and psychotic symptoms in patients with childhood trauma. The comorbidity could lead to an increase in “visual symptoms” as a flashback or in abusive egodystonic thoughts that can be confused with hallucinations or could manifest as mixed symptomatology. Only one study has explored comorbidities with hallucinations in a UHR population, finding that visual hallucinations are associated with depressive disorder, while auditory (and specifically verbal) hallucinations in the second person are associated with borderline personality disorder.30

A deeper understanding of the relationship between childhood trauma and hallucinations of varying types (with respect to form and content) could improve our characterization and differentiation of UHR individuals. The present results suggest that with further studies it may be possible to discriminate between hallucinations that emerge from a history of childhood trauma versus those that arise from the progressive evolution of a psychotic disorder, according to their form and content. Specifically, we hypothesize that auditory verbal hallucinations in conjunction with thought disorders may suggest progression toward a psychotic disorder, while isolated visual hallucinations could suggest other conditions. Further studies need to define if visual hallucinations could be related to a history of childhood trauma and if specific contents are linked with a specific type of trauma. However, this framework should be applied with caution, as a history of trauma and the transition to psychosis appear to be correlated, as previously described.

Auditory hallucinations

In recent years, much work has been done to define the at-risk condition for psychosis among children and adolescents.1–3 However, some of the inclusion criteria that have been developed seem insufficiently specific (eg, perceptual abnormalities, in general). For this reason, it is critical to improving our understanding of the specific forms and contents of hallucinations that are most indicative of a risk of developing psychosis. Phenomenologically, hallucinations may differ significantly, and their forms and contents may shed important light on patients’ altered states of consciousness, thought processes, and perceptions of self versus others. Hallucinations—and especially verbal auditory hallucinations—suggest an altered experience as a first-person subject and disturbed thought agency and ownership, leading to the objectification and externalization of thoughts in the form of “audible” voices.49,50 For this reason, verbal auditory hallucinations may be more significantly associated with schizophrenia and qualitatively different from other types of perceptual abnormalities. To confirm these hypotheses, further research is needed to investigate whether UHR individuals who develop psychosis have more visual, auditory, or other hallucinations. With respect to auditory hallucinations, specifically, research should explore whether these hallucinations tend to include verbal commenting or dialoguing content; whether the voices address the subject in the first, second, or third person; whether they have abusive or grandiose content; and whether these characteristics are associated with other factors that predict psychosis (eg, an altered state of consciousness, an altered sense of self and agency, disorganized thought, bizarre thought, ideas of reference or influence, blocked thought, etc.). Finally, research should attempt to uncover how such prodrome symptoms interact with comorbid conditions, as well as with trauma. In particular, such research should be directed at the younger population, which experiences a higher frequency of perceptual abnormalities than the adult population, though not always with pathological significance.12,51 In fact, the population of children and adolescents with UHR status shows particular characteristics compared to adults. There are few studies on this issue but some have pointed out that in general, psychotic symptomatology in juvenile age shows a low specificity in predicting conversion into full-blown psychosis.19,20

Limitations and Strengths

To the best of our knowledge, the present study was the first to review the literature on hallucinations among UHR children and adolescents and summarize the results in a systematic way. However, there are several limitations of the research. First, only a small number of studies meet the inclusion criteria of our review and none of the studies included focused on only the child population. Indeed, the age of individuals was heterogeneous including young adults. Second, the included studies did not assess the presence of neurodevelopment disorders that could represent a trait of vulnerability, especially in the child UHR population. Third, the assessment of hallucinations was quite homogeneous across the investigated studies: almost all of the studies utilized the CAARMS as a diagnostic tool, while only one assessed subjects using the SIPS. Of note, almost all of the investigated studies classified perceptual disturbances using the same assessment tool, which characterized the form and content of participants’ hallucinations. However, all of the studies utilized the same theoretical basis and approach to the subject, which generated coherence in the results even if there is an overlapping in assessed populations. In fact, the investigated studies focused on only two variables linked to hallucinations in young UHR: the presence of childhood trauma and the risk of transition to psychosis.

Conclusions

The present review contributes to clarifying and discriminating between the multiform manifestations of hallucinations in children and adolescents at ultrahigh risk of developing a psychotic spectrum disorder. While the number of relevant studies was limited and none was found specifically focused on the child population, the results suggest a preliminary framework for further research, with the aim of defining a more precise UHR profile and thereby improving diagnostic tools—particularly for the younger population. Briefly, we have a main message emerging from wanting to emphasize this paper. The actual assessment of hallucinations in juvenile UHR state is not enough reliable, a more accurate clinical differentiation of perceptual abnormalities is necessary. Such research is critically needed to help clinicians develop effective treatment plans for UHR children and adolescents, tailored to their specific symptomatology.

Supplementary Material

sgac068_suppl_Supplementary_Material

Acknowledgments

M.d.L., M.P., and S.V. conceived the review. M.d.L., C.d.V., F.D., and D.B. carried out the literature search and, with M.P., defined the inclusion/exclusion criteria; M.d.L., C.d.V., and D.B. considered the eligibility of all studies and, where relevant, participated in Delphi rounds to achieve complete consensus; M.d.L., D. B. and C.d.V. rated the risk of bias. M.P. and M.d.L. wrote the “Introduction” section; M.d.L., C.d.V., and D.B. wrote the “Results” and “Methods” sections; M.d.L., M.P., F.D., and S.V. wrote the “Discussion” and “Conclusions” sections. M.d.L., M.P., C.d.V., D.B., and S.V. wrote the first draft; M.d.L., M.P., F.D., and S.V. supervised the writing of the manuscript and wrote the final draft. All authors endorsed the final form of the manuscript.

Contributor Information

Michelangelo Di Luzio, Child and Adolescence Neuropsychiatry Unit, Department of Neuroscience, IRCCS “Bambino Gesù” Children’s Hospital, Rome, Italy.

Maria Pontillo, Child and Adolescence Neuropsychiatry Unit, Department of Neuroscience, IRCCS “Bambino Gesù” Children’s Hospital, Rome, Italy.

Cristina Di Vincenzo, Child and Adolescence Neuropsychiatry Unit, Department of Neuroscience, IRCCS “Bambino Gesù” Children’s Hospital, Rome, Italy.

Domenica Bellantoni, Child and Adolescence Neuropsychiatry Unit, Department of Neuroscience, IRCCS “Bambino Gesù” Children’s Hospital, Rome, Italy.

Francesco Demaria, Child and Adolescence Neuropsychiatry Unit, Department of Neuroscience, IRCCS “Bambino Gesù” Children’s Hospital, Rome, Italy.

Stefano Vicari, Child and Adolescence Neuropsychiatry Unit, Department of Neuroscience, IRCCS “Bambino Gesù” Children’s Hospital, Rome, Italy; Department of Life Sciences and Public Health, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, 00168 Rome, Italy.

Funding

This work has not received grants or funds.

Disclosures

All authors declare they have no conflict of interest.

References

  • 1.Yung AR, Yuen HP, McGorry PD, et al. Mapping the onset of psychosis: the comprehensive assessment of at-risk mental states. Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2005;39:964–971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Fusar-Poli P., Borgwardt S., McGuire P. Vulnerability to Psychosis: From Neurosciences to Psychopathology. London, UK: Psychology Press; 2011. [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Fusar-Poli P, Borgwardt S, Bechdolf A, et al. The psychosis high-risk state: a comprehensive state-of-the-art review. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70:107–120. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Fusar-Poli P, Smieskova R, Serafini G, Politi P, Borgwardt S. Neuroanatomical markers of genetic liability to psychosis and first episode psychosis: a voxelwise meta-analytical comparison. World J Biol Psychiatry. 2014;15:219–228. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Miller TJ, McGlashan TH, Rosen JL, et al. Prodromal assessment with the structured interview for prodromal syndromes and the scale of prodromal symptoms: predictive validity, interrater reliability, and training to reliability [published correction appears in Schizophr Bull. 2004;30:following 217]. Schizophr Bull. 2003;29:703–715. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Cannon TD, Cornblatt B, McGorry P. The empirical status of the ultra high-risk (prodromal) research paradigm. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33:661–664. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Fusar-Poli P, Bonoldi I, Yung AR, et al. Predicting psychosis: meta-analysis of transition outcomes in individuals at high clinical risk. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2012;69:220–229. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.American Psychiatric Association (APA). Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 5th ed. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Association (APA); 2013. [Google Scholar]
  • 9.aSommer IE, Derwort AM, Daalman K, de Weijer AD, Liddle PF, Boks MP. Formal thought disorder in non-clinical individuals with auditory verbal hallucinations. Schizophr Res. 2010;118:140–145. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.bSommer IE, Daalman K, Rietkerk T, et al. Healthy individuals with auditory verbal hallucinations; who are they? Psychiatric assessments of a selected sample of 103 subjects [published correction appears in Schizophr Bull. 2014 Sep;40:1182-3]. Schizophr Bull. 2010;36:633–641. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Daalman K, van Zandvoort M, Bootsman F, Boks M, Kahn R, Sommer I. Auditory verbal hallucinations and cognitive functioning in healthy individuals. Schizophr Res. 2011;132:203–207. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Maijer K, Hayward M, Fernyhough C, et al. Hallucinations in children and adolescents: an updated review and practical recommendations for clinicians. Schizophr Bull. 2019;45:S5–S23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Zhang T, Xu L, Tang Y, et al. Isolated hallucination is less predictive than thought disorder in psychosis: Insight from a longitudinal study in a clinical population at high risk for psychosis. Sci Rep. 2018;8:13962. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lehembre-Shiah E, Leong W, Brucato G, et al. Distinct relationships between visual and auditory perceptual abnormalities and conversion to psychosis in a clinical high-risk population. JAMA Psychiatry. 2017;74:104–106. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Addington J, Liu L, Buchy L, et al. North american prodrome longitudinal study (NAPLS 2): the prodromal symptoms. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2015;203:328–335. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Pontillo M, Tata MC, Averna R, et al. Clinical profile, conversion rate, and suicidal thinking and behaviour in children and adolescents at ultra-high risk for psychosis: a theoretical perspective. Res Psychother. 2020;23:16–23. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Dolz M, Tor J, De la Serna E, et al. Characterization of children and adolescents with psychosis risk syndrome: the children and adolescents psychosis risk syndrome (CAPRIS) study. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2019;13:1062–1072. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Poletti M, Pelizza L, Azzali S, et al. Clinical high risk for psychosis in childhood and adolescence: findings from the 2-year follow-up of the ReARMS project. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2019;28:957–971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Schultze-Lutter F, Michel C, Schmidt SJ, et al. EPA guidance on the early detection of clinical high risk states of psychoses. Eur Psychiatry. 2015;30:405–416. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Armando M, Pontillo M, De Crescenzo F, et al. Twelve-month psychosis-predictive value of the ultra-high-risk criteria in children and adolescents. Schizophr Res. 2015;169:186–192. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Pelizza L, Azzali S, Garlassi S, et al. Adolescents at ultra-high risk of psychosis in Italian neuropsychiatry services: prevalence, psychopathology and transition rate. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2018;27:725–737. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Page MJ, McKenzie JE, Bossuyt PM, et al. The PRISMA 2020 statement: an updated guideline for reporting systematic reviews. BMJ. 2021;372:71. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Methods Guide for Effectiveness and Comparative Effectiveness Reviews [Internet]. Rockville (MD): Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality (US); 2008. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Niles HF, Walsh BC, Woods SW, Powers AR 3rd. Does hallucination perceptual modality impact psychosis risk?. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2019;140:360–370. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.O’Connor K, Nelson B, Lin A, Wood SJ, Yung A, Thompson A. Are UHR patients who present with hallucinations alone at lower risk of transition to psychosis? Psychiatry Res. 2016;235:177–196. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.McGuffin P, Farmer A, Harvey I. A polydiagnostic application of operational criteria in studies of psychotic illness. Development and reliability of the OPCRIT system. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 1991;48:764–770. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Yung AR, Phillips LJ, Yuen HP, et al. Psychosis prediction: 12-month follow up of a high-risk (“prodromal”) group. Schizophr Res. 2003;60:21–32. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Yung AR, Phillips LJ, Yuen HP, McGorry PD. Risk factors for psychosis in an ultra high-risk group: psychopathology and clinical features. Schizophr Res. 2004;67:131–142. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Thompson A, Nelson B, Bruxner A, et al. Does specific psychopathology predict development of psychosis in ultra high-risk (UHR) patients? Aust N Z J Psychiatry. 2013;47:380–390. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.O’ Connor K, Nelson B, Cannon M, Yung A, Thompson A. Perceptual abnormalities in an ultra-high risk for psychosis population relationship to trauma and co-morbid disorder. Early Interv Psychiatry. 2019;13:231–240. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Velthorst E, Nelson B, O’Connor K, et al. History of trauma and the association with baseline symptoms in an Ultra-High Risk for psychosis cohort. Psychiatry Res. 2013;210:75–81. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Schutte MJL, Linszen MMJ, Marschall TM, et al. Hallucinations and other psychotic experiences across diagnoses: a comparison of phenomenological features. Psychiatry Res. 2020;292:113314. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Lim A, Blom JD. Tactile and somatic hallucinations in a muslim population of psychotic patients. Front Psychiatry. 2021;12:728397. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Ciarleglio AJ, Brucato G, Masucci MD, et al. A predictive model for conversion to psychosis in clinical high-risk patients. Psychol Med. 2019;49:1128–1137. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Waters F, Fernyhough C. Hallucinations: a systematic review of points of similarity and difference across diagnostic classes. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43:32–43. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Hermans K, van der Steen Y, Kasanova Z, et al. Temporal dynamics of suspiciousness and hallucinations in clinical high risk and first episode psychosis. Psychiatry Res. 2020;290:113039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Janssen I, Krabbendam L, Bak M, et al. Childhood abuse as a risk factor for psychotic experiences. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2004;109:38–45. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Shevlin M, Dorahy MJ, Adamson G. Trauma and psychosis: an analysis of the National Comorbidity Survey. Am J Psychiatry. 2007;164:166–169. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Bechdolf A, Thompson A, Nelson B, et al. Experience of trauma and conversion to psychosis in an ultra-high-risk (prodromal) group. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2010;121:377–384. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Thompson A, Nelson B, McNab C, et al. Psychotic symptoms with sexual content in the “ultra high risk” for psychosis population: frequency and association with sexual trauma. Psychiatry Res. 2010;177:84–91. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Falukozi E, Addington J. Impact of trauma on attenuated psychotic symptoms. Psychosis. 2012;4:203–212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Thompson JL, Kelly M, Kimhy D, et al. Childhood trauma and prodromal symptoms among individuals at clinical high risk for psychosis. Schizophr Res. 2009;108:176–181. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Kraan T, Velthorst E, Smit F, de Haan L, van der Gaag M. Trauma and recent life events in individuals at ultra high risk for psychosis: review and meta-analysis. Schizophr Res. 2015;161:143–149. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Kilcommons AM, Morrison AP. Relationships between trauma and psychosis: an exploration of cognitive and dissociative factors. Acta Psychiatr Scand. 2005;112:351–359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Bentall RP, Wickham S, Shevlin M, Varese F. Do specific early-life adversities lead to specific symptoms of psychosis? A study from the 2007 the Adult Psychiatric Morbidity Survey. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38:734–740. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Solesvik M, Joa I, Larsen TK, et al. Visual hallucinations in first-episode psychosis: association with childhood trauma. PLoS One. 2016;11:e0153458. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Alderson-Day B, Woods A, Moseley P, et al. Voice-hearing and personification: characterizing social qualities of auditory verbal hallucinations in early psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2021;47:228–236. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Begemann MJH, Sommer IE, Brand RM, et al. Auditory verbal hallucinations and childhood trauma subtypes across the psychosis continuum: a cluster analysis. Cogn Neuropsychiatry. 2022;27:150–168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Pienkos E, Giersch A, Hansen M, et al. Hallucinations beyond voices: a conceptual review of the phenomenology of altered perception in psychosis. Schizophr Bull. 2019;45:S67–S77. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Raballo A. From perception to thought: a phenomenological approach to hallucinatory experience. Schizophr Bull. 2017;43:18–20. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Maijer K, Begemann MJH, Palmen SJMC, Leucht S, Sommer IEC. Auditory hallucinations across the lifespan: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Psychol Med. 2018;48:879–888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

sgac068_suppl_Supplementary_Material

Articles from Schizophrenia Bulletin Open are provided here courtesy of Oxford University Press on behalf of the University of Maryland's School of Medicine, Maryland Psychiatric Research Center

RESOURCES