Abstract
Background
Urban-rural differences in schizophrenia risk have been widely evidenced across Western countries. However, explanation of these differences is lacking. We aimed to identify contextual risk factors for schizophrenia that explain urban-rural differences in schizophrenia risk.
Methods
Utilizing Danish population-based registers, we partitioned Denmark into 1885 geographic “neighborhoods” homogeneously sized in terms of population. Information on the entire Danish population from 1981 to 2016 was used to quantify a spectrum of neighborhood-level domains. We subsequently conducted multilevel survival analyses following persons born in Denmark from 1971 to 1982 for the development of schizophrenia allowing for clustering of people within neighborhoods. We used this method to tease apart the effects of individual, specific, and general contextual risk factors for schizophrenia.
Results
A significant general contextual effect in schizophrenia risk across neighborhoods was estimated (Medium Incidence Rate Ratio (MRR):1.41; 95% CI:1.35–1.48). Most of the specific contextual factors examined were associated with schizophrenia risk. For instance, neighborhood-level proportion of lone adult households (Incidence Rate Ratios (IRR):1.53; 95% CI:1.44–1.63) had largest risk estimate. Adjustment for all individual-level and specific contextual constructs reduced the IRR for urbanicity from 1.98 (95% CI:1.77–2.22) to 1.30 (95% CI:1.11–1.51).
Conclusions
In the largest prospective multilevel survival analyses of schizophrenia risk conducted to date, multiple neighborhood-level characteristics were associated with raised schizophrenia risk, with these contextual factors explaining most of the elevated risk linked with urbanicity. However, the unexplained heterogeneity that was evident in our multilevel models indicates that our understanding of the role of urbanicity in schizophrenia’s etiology remains incomplete.
Keywords: schizophrenia, multilevel survival analyses, urbanization, neighborhood-level domains, general contextual effect, specific contextual effect
Introduction
In 1939, Faris and Dunham published their classical ecological study of mental disorders in Chicago.1 They explored the potential relationship between social organization and the spatial distribution of mental disorders, including schizophrenia. They found that the lower the social organization of the municipality, the higher the incidence rate of schizophrenia. Although considered the first study to identify urban-rural differences in schizophrenia risk, fourteen years earlier Pollock and Nolan demonstrated urban-rural differences in schizophrenia risk in New York State.2 This phenomenon fostered numerous studies investigating urban-rural differences per se and studies investigating the impact of neighborhood-level constructs on schizophrenia risk.
People born or raised in urban areas have elevated risks of developing schizophrenia compared to their rural-dwelling counterparts.3 A meta-analysis found that the risk for schizophrenia linked with residing in a highly urbanized environment was at least twice as high as that associated with living in a very sparsely populated rural environment.4 Despite differences in methodology and methods to measure urbanization (density of inhabitants,5 the density of residences,6–8 city size,6,9–18 other methods19,20) almost all studies have reported significant urban-rural differences in schizophrenia incidence. Urbanization in itself likely does not causally influence the risk of developing schizophrenia; rather it is probably a marker for determinants that are more or less prevalent in urban areas.12,13,16,17 To date, the search for factors explaining these urban-rural differences have included both methodological issues of migration and selection,9,21 as well as potential individual-level explanations: sibling composition,18 maternal and paternal age,22 parental socioeconomic position,23 household crowding,24 family history of mental illness,12,13 ethnicity,25–28 toxoplasmosis,29,30 vitamin D,31,32 parental substance misuse,23 parental death,33 and air pollution.34 However, none of these factors have consistently explained all urban-rural differences in schizophrenia risk.12,13,18,21–28,33,34
There is also robust evidence that social-environmental characteristics of neighborhoods, including social deprivation,35–39 social disorganization,39 social fragmentation,36,39 social marginalization,39 social capital,40 residential mobility,39,41 income inequality,37 ethnic fragmentation,38,39 and physical illness42 are related to distribution of nonaffective psychotic disorders.35–41,43–49 Studies have also considered ethnic density,38,43,45 i.e., where the incidence of schizophrenia in ethnic minorities is greater when they compromise a smaller proportion of the local population. While Denmark has access to rich longitudinal population-based registers and have contributed with studies of urban-rural differences in schizophrenia risk, there exist no consistent small area local geography in Denmark, and thus the contribution from Denmark to identify potential social-environmental risk factors for schizophrenia is scarce; with the exceptance of a few studies investigating ethnic density and psychosis.46,47
In their national Swedish register-based study, Zammit et al.48 found that the effect of urbanicity was explained by neighborhood-level covariates including population density, although, the conceptual interchangeability of urbanicity and population density arguably compromises the interpretability of this finding.
We utilized rich interlinked Danish nationwide population-based registers50 to develop a new homogeneously population-sized geographic division of Denmark, which are hereafter referred to as “data zones”. By conducting a novel Danish population-based prospective multilevel survival analysis, we aimed to quantify the degree to which individual-level and neighborhood-level risk factors explain urban-rural differences in schizophrenia risk. Neighborhood-level indicators examined included domains of material deprivation, social fragmentation, social marginalization, and physical illness39,42,51
Methods
Since 1968, all Danish residents have been registered in the Danish Civil Registration System,52 which records personal identification number, gender, date and place of birth, parents’ personal identifiers, and continuously updated information on residential address, emigration, death, and disappearance. Unique personal identifiers are used in national registers enabling accurate linkage between registers and between cohort members and their first-degree relatives.
First, we describe the delineation of neighborhoods and neighborhood-level covariates, both of which was based on the entire Danish population, and subsequently, the study population followed for schizophrenia onset using multilevel survival analyses.
Small Area Geography
From 1971 to 2016, Denmark was divided into 2039–2194 parishes.53 From 1971 to 2006, the country was divided into 270–275 municipalities, and since 2007 it has been divided into 98 municipalities.53,54 Apart from the longitudinal variation in geographic boundaries, these geographic delineations also vary considerably in population size (Supplementary Methods 1). Both challenges may bias geospatial analyses.55 Due largely to the availability of the individual-level registers in Denmark, and the lack of a national census, no consistent and homogeneously sized small-area geographic delineation is available in Denmark. Thus, few previous Danish studies have investigated neighborhood-level influences on health.
Delineation of Data Zones
We adopted geographic zone design theory to create a new national small-area delineation.56,57 We utilized a nationwide database of regularly updated geographic front-door coordinates of residential address for all Danish residents from 1978 onwards52 combined with a sophisticated automated tessellation clustering procedure to create a homogeneously population-sized small-area division of Denmark. This resulted in 1885 delineated geographic areas, hereafter referred to as “data zones”. Data zones were nested within the 98 municipalities. We aimed for a mean data zone size of 2500 residents longitudinally from 1980 to 2016 (Supplementary Methods 1, Supplementary Figure 1). When we applied this delineation scheme nationally, the mean residents per data zone was 2820 people (IQR: 2311 to 3315, SD = 809, range: 768–6495), i.e., corresponding to a coefficient of variation of 28.7%.
Derivation of Neighborhood-Level Indicators/Specific Contextual Covariates
Data zone-level covariates were derived by aggregating individual-level covariates for the entire Danish population. These neighborhood-level indices included material deprivation (low income, short education, not employed, manual work, household overcrowding, no car owned), social fragmentation (lone adult household, residential transience, rented accommodation), social marginalization (violent offender, criminality, foreign-born) and neighborhood-level physical illness. For these factors we calculated the proportion of inhabitants (or households) in each data zone with the characteristic of interest (Supplementary Methods 2); for instance, the proportion of inhabitants in neighborhood convicted for a violent crime. We also calculated age distribution (proportion of residents aged 0–14 years, 15–29 years, 30–49 years, 50–64 years, and 65 age and above), urbanicity, and population density (as described below). These calculations were performed for each data zone annually from 1981 to 2016 using individual-level covariates for the entire Danish population on December 31 each year. We included the neighborhood-level indicators (specific contextual covariates) as data zone-level averages during years 1981–1984 (Supplementary Methods 2).
Delineation of Urbanicity and Population Density
When the causal factor(s) that explain urban-rural differences in incidence are unknown, a universal measure of urbanization cannot easily be conceptualized.15 We used two competing measures of urbanization; “population density” of the data zone and “urbanicity” categorized in five levels according to the number of inhabitants in the largest city or town in the 270-level municipality.13 The latter was referred to as “degree of urbanization” in previous Danish studies (e.g.12,15,16,58). Although, urbanicity and population density are both neighborhood-level covariates, both have been used as interchangeable proxies for the unknown mechanism driving the urban-rural differences in schizophrenia risk. Supplementary Figures 2A–2S show the geographic distribution of the neighborhood-level indicators examined.
Individual-Level Covariates
Parental sociodemographic indices were obtained from the Integrated Database for Labor Market Research59: gross income, highest level of completed education (primary school, high school/vocational training, higher education), and employment status (employed, unemployed, not working for other reasons). Similarly, parents were classified with a history of any secondary care diagnosed mental disorder using the following coding ranges: ICD10: F00–F99, ICD8: 290–315.60,61 We also included information on maternal and paternal age at cohort member’s birth,22 number of residential changes from the 5th to the 10th birthday,62 parental Charlson Comorbidity index, parental imprisonment, and parental death. All individual-level covariates were delineated at or shortly before the 10th birthday. Cohort members were classified as having schizophrenia (ICD10: F20; ICD8: 295.x9, excluding 295.79) if they had been admitted to a psychiatric hospital or had received outpatient care with this diagnosis.63 The date of onset was defined as the first day of the inpatient or outpatient episode during which the diagnosis was first assigned.
Statistical Analyses
People born in Denmark to Danish-born parents25,64 between 1st January 1972 and 31st December 1981 were followed for onset of schizophrenia. Follow-up was initiated at 10th birthday and was terminated at disease onset, emigration, death, or 31st January 2016, whichever came first. Individual-level variables examined included age, sex, number of residential moves during the past five years, maternal and paternal age at time of cohort member’s birth, parental imprisonment, parental Charlson disease, parental death, parental history of mental illness,61 parental income, parental level of education completed and parental employment status. Age was treated as time-scale. All other variables were analyzed as time-fixed variables measured at initiation of follow-up. Individual-level characteristics were delineated at or shortly before initiation of follow-up, thus ensuring the prospective nature of the study. To avoid potential heterogeneity and/or bias caused by differential effects sizes for foreign migrants and their descendants,26 by design the study population included people born in Denmark to Danish-born parents.
Data were analyzed using multilevel log-linear Poisson regression models that included both individual- and neighborhood-level variables, data zone as a random intercept,65 and with the logarithms of the aggregated person-years counts set as an offset variable.66 This is equivalent to the Cox proportional hazards model, assuming piecewise constant incidence rates67,68 and allowing for a random intercept for each data zone.69 Multilevel survival models enable researchers to make valid inferences when examining the effects of both individual- and neighborhood-level predictors of disease risk.69 All statistical analyses were conducted using R, version 3.5.0. The multilevel Poisson regression model was fitted using Markov Chain Monte Carlo with the brm function in the brms package (version 2.2.0). We used five chains with 2500 simulations each with a burn-in of 500 simulations to estimate parameters, i.e., 10 000 simulations in total were generated.
Interpretation of General and Specific Contextual Effects
The contextual influence on disease incidence (or magnitude of clustering) consists of the general contextual effect and the specific contextual effects. The specific contextual effects are the neighborhood-level indicators examined. The general contextual effect estimates effects of neighborhood context on disease incidence without reference to any of the specific neighborhood-level constructs other than the very boundaries defining neighborhoods.70 In multilevel models, the general contextual effects are random effects, and the specific contextual effects are fixed effects.
The median incidence rate ratio (MRR) quantifies the general contextual effect (degree of clustering) on the incidence rate ratio scale and are thus comparable with individual-level incidence rate ratios. MRRs were estimated following the exact calculation provided in Austin et al,65 this parameter being the median relative change in the incidence rate of the event when comparing identical individuals from two randomly selected different clusters that are ordered by incidence rate.
Statistical Adjustment Scenarios
Using multilevel survival analyses, we first considered the effect of the individual-level covariates on schizophrenia risk and estimated the general contextual effect across data zones for different individual-level adjustments. The specific contextual effects were estimated in basic adjustment (individual-level age and sex and neighborhood-level age structure) and also in full individual-level adjustment (all individual-level variables examined and neighborhood-level age structure). All estimates of neighborhood-level characteristics were also adjusted for the potential confounding influence of neighborhood-level age structure. We used principal component analyses to generate a combined adjustment for specific neighborhood-level indicators (Supplementary Methods 2). Finally, we estimated the effect of urbanicity and population density with basic and with full individual-level adjustment.
Sensitivity Analyses
As a sensitivity analyses, we repeated the equivalent multilevel survival analyses a) with parish boundaries as the geographic delineators (Supplementary Methods 3, Supplementary Table 2, Supplementary Table 3); b) using schizophrenia and related disorders (ICD10: F20-F29 and eq: ICD8)60 as the outcome of interest (Supplementary Table 4, Supplementary Table 5); c) using explanatory factor analyses to generate a combined adjustment for all specific neighborhood-level indicators (Supplementary Methods 4, Supplementary Table 6).
Ethics
The Danish Data Protection Agency approved the study, with data access agreed by the Danish Health Data Authority and Statistics Denmark. Because it was based exclusively on registry data, informed consent from cohort members was not required in accordance with Danish legislation.
Results
Among the 579 039 people born in Denmark 1972–1981 by Danish-born parents, a total of 5103 people developed schizophrenia during the 17 191 889 person-years of follow-up 1982–2016; a crude incidence rate of 2.97 per 10 000 person-years at risk.
Associations pertaining to the individual-level risk factors investigated are shown in Table 1, along with descriptive information on number of cases and crude incidence rates per characteristic. For instance, each residential change was associated with a 1.44 (95% CI: 1.38–1.51) fold increased risk of schizophrenia. Most individual-level risk factors were attenuated after adjustment for all other individual-level risk factors examined. For example, the effect of a parental history of any mental disorder was reduced from 3.11 (95% CI: 2.90–3.35) to 2.47 (95% CI: 2.29–2.65). Risk estimates in Table 1 concur with those reported previously in the literature.22,23,61,71–73
Table 1.
Individual-Level Risk Factors for Schizophreniaa
Basic adjustmentc | Full individual-level adjustmentd | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Individual-level risk factor | No of cases | Incidence Rateb | Incidence Rate Ratio (95% CI) | Incidence Rate Ratio (95% CI) |
Residential changese | ||||
None | 4013 | 2.73 | 1.00 (ref) | 1.00 (ref) |
1 or more | 1090 | 4.42 | 1.44 (1.38–1.51) | 1.26 (1.20–1.32) |
Parental history of mental illness | ||||
No | 4133 | 2.58 | 1.00 (ref) | 1.00 (ref) |
Any | 970 | 8.23 | 3.11 (2.90–3.35) | 2.47 (2.29–2.65) |
Paternal age (years) | ||||
12–19 | 98 | 4.21 | 1.53 (1.25–1.87) | 1.06 (0.85–1.32) |
20–24 | 1074 | 3.27 | 1.20 (1.11–1.29) | 1.06 (0.98–1.16) |
25–29 | 1873 | 2.69 | 1.00 (ref) | 1.00 (ref) |
30–34 | 1271 | 2.90 | 1.07 (1.00–1.14) | 1.09 (1.01–1.18) |
35–39 | 479 | 3.07 | 1.14 (1.03–1.25) | 1.10 (0.99–1.23) |
40 and older | 308 | 4.03 | 1.50 (1.33–1.69) | 1.29 (1.12–1.49) |
Maternal age (years) | ||||
12–19 | 445 | 4.35 | 1.58 (1.42–1.75) | 1.27 (1.13–1.43) |
20–24 | 1747 | 2.99 | 1.11 (1.03–1.18) | 1.06 (0.99–1.14) |
25–29 | 1774 | 2.69 | 1.00 (ref) | 1.00 (ref) |
30–34 | 823 | 2.86 | 1.05 (0.97–1.14) | 0.99 (0.91–1.08) |
35–39 | 268 | 3.57 | 1.32 (1.14–1.51) | 1.11 (0.96–1.29) |
40 and older | 46 | 3.92 | 1.46 (1.07–1.96) | 1.06 (0.76–1.44) |
Parental Charlson Comobidity | ||||
Yes | 417 | 3.87 | 1.31 (1.19–1.44) | 1.04 (0.93–1.15) |
No | 4686 | 2.91 | 1.00 (ref) | 1.00 (ref) |
Parental imprisonment | ||||
Yes | 395 | 6.41 | 2.17 (1.95–2.39) | 1.36 (1.22–1.52) |
No | 4708 | 2.84 | 1.00 (ref) | 1.00 (ref) |
Parental death | ||||
Yes | 177 | 2.91 | 2.08 (1.79–2.40) | 1.23 (0.87–1.73) |
No | 4926 | 6.18 | 1.00 (ref) | 1.00 (ref) |
Parental income quartilef | ||||
Q1 (lowest) | 547 | 4.83 | 1.90 (1.73–2.10) | 1.34 (1.19–1.50) |
Q2 | 1060 | 3.27 | 1.30 (1.20–1.41) | 1.21 (1.11–1.31) |
Q3 | 1500 | 2.80 | 1.11 (1.04–1.19) | 1.10 (1.02–1.18) |
Q4 (highest) | 1851 | 2.56 | 1.00 (ref) | 1.00 (ref) |
Parental educationf | ||||
Primary school | 527 | 3.48 | 1.31 (1.18–1.44) | 0.96 (0.86–1.06) |
High school/vocational training | 2854 | 2.88 | 1.06 (0.99–1.13) | 0.91 (0.84–0.97) |
Short cycle higher education | 149 | 2.40 | 0.88 (0.74–1.04) | 0.84 (0.71–1.00) |
Higher education | 1222 | 2.74 | 1.00 (ref) | 1.00 (ref) |
Parental employment statusf | ||||
Outside workforce | 169 | 9.60 | 3.34 (2.85–3.89) | 1.65 (1.39–1.95) |
Unemployed | 252 | 7.07 | 2.52 (2.20–2.84) | 1.55 (1.35–1.78) |
Employed | 4475 | 2.75 | 1.00 (ref) | 1.00 (ref) |
a Estimates were based on multilevel survival analyses, neighborhoods are 1885 novel data zones nested in Denmark’s 98 municipalities. The 579 039 people born in Denmark 1972–1981 were followed for development of schizophrenia. During the follow-up period from 1982 to 2016, a total of 5103 developed schizophrenia during the 17 191 889 person-years at risk
b The incidence rate measure the number of new people who developed schizophrenia per 10,000 person-years at risk.
c Estimates were adjusted for age and sex.
d Estimates were adjusted for age and sex and all other covariates.
e Incidence rate ratio for each residential change from 5th to 10th birthday.
f Parental income quartile was delineated as the maximum of maternal and paternal quartiles. Parental educational attainment was delineated as the parent with highest level of completed education. Parental employment status was delineated hierarchically as either parent employed, either parent not employed, and either parent outside workforce. Categories with missing parental socioeconomic position information are not shown.
Table 2 shows the general contextual effect (unexplained heterogeneity) on schizophrenia risk across data zones in different individual-level adjustment scenarios. In the model without covariates, the estimated standard deviation of the random intercept was 0.36 and its estimated standard error was 0.02, which equates to an MRR of 1.41 (95% CI: 1.34–1.47). Further adjustment for individual-level age and sex did not reduce the general contextual effect. When adjusting for all individual-level factors, the MRR was attenuated to 1.32 (95% CI: 1.26–1.39). Further adjustment for neighborhood-level age structure slightly reduced the unexplained heterogeneity between data zones.
Table 2.
Heterogeneity in Schizophrenia Risk Across Data Zones Adjusting for Individual-Level Risk Factors for Schizophrenia
General contextual effect in schizophrenia risk across data zones, two competing measuresa | ||
---|---|---|
Adjustment scenario | Between data zone standard deviation; Random intercept (Standard error of Random Intercept) | Median Incidence Rate Ratio (95% Probability Interval)b |
None | 0.36 (0.02) | 1.41 (1.34–1.47) |
Basic individual-level adjustment, i.e., age and its interaction with sex | 0.36 (0.02) | 1.41 (1.35–1.48) |
Basic individual-level adjustment, childhood residential transience, parental Charlson, parental death and parental imprisonment | 0.33 (0.02) | 1.38 (1.31–1.44) |
Basic individual-level adjustment, parental age and parental history of mental illness | 0.31 (0.03) | 1.34 (1.27–1.41) |
Basic individual-level adjustment, parental income, parental education and parental employment status | 0.34 (0.02) | 1.38 (1.31–1.45) |
Comprehensive individual-level adjustment, all factors above | 0.29 (0.02) | 1.32 (1.26–1.39) |
Full individual-level adjustment and neighborhood-level age structure | 0.27 (0.02) | 1.30 (1.24–1.36) |
a Estimates were based on multilevel survival analyses, neighborhoods are 1885 novel data zones nested in Denmark’s 98 municipalities. The 579 039 people born in Denmark 1972–1981 were followed for development of schizophrenia. During the follow-up period from 1982 to 2016, a total of 5103 developed schizophrenia during the 17 191 889 person-years at risk
b The Median Incidence Rate Ratio (MRR) quantified the variation between data zones (clusters) by comparing two identical individuals from two randomly chosen data zones. Consider two people with the same covariates chosen randomly from different data zones, the Median Incidence Rate Ratio is the median incidence rate ratio between the person of higher incidence rate and the person of lower incidence rate.
Table 3 shows the effects of the array of specific contextual effects investigated. In the basic adjustment models each specific contextual effect was associated with an increased risk of schizophrenia, except for proportion of individuals with low income, proportion of individuals with short education, and proportion of individuals who are manual workers. Each specific contextual effect slightly attenuated the general contextual effect. For instance, a one standard deviation increase in the proportion of households with lone adults increased the incidence rate of schizophrenia 1.53 (95% CI: 1.44–1.63)-fold for all other factors held fixed except data zone and, on the MRR scale, this neighborhood-level indicator reduced the general contextual effect to 1.27 (95% CI: 1.21–1.34). Further adjustment for all individual-level variables slightly attenuated all risk estimates. Following full individual- and neighborhood-level adjustment statistically significant unexplained heterogeneity remained between data zones (MRR 1.22; 95% CI: 1.14–1.29). The scatterplots presented in Figure 1 reveal the correlation between the neighborhood-level covariates as well as their distributions. Proportion of households without car and proportion of households that rents their home had highest Pearson’s correlation coefficient (r = 0.92), while proportion of individuals with a severe physical illness and proportion of individuals convicted with any criminality had lowest absolute correlation coefficient (r = 0.002).
Table 3.
General and Specific Contextual Effects on Schizophrenia Risk
Basic adjustmenta,b | Full individual-level adjustmenta,c | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Specific contextual effect | General contextual effect | Specific contextual effect | General contextual effect | |
Neighborhood-level socioeconomic indicatord | Incidence Rate Ratio of neighborhood per 1 sd increase | Median Incidence Rate ratio (95% Probability Interval | Incidence Rate Ratio of neighborhood per 1 sd increase | Median Incidence Rate Ratio (95% Probability Interval) |
None | - | 1.37 (1.31–1.43) | 1.30 (1.23–1.36) | |
Material deprivation | ||||
PI low Income | 0.96 (0.92–1.00) | 1.36 (1.30–1.42) | 0.93 (0.89–0.97) | 1.29 (1.22–1.35) |
PI short education | 0.89 (0.85–0.93) | 1.35 (1.29–1.41) | 0.87 (0.84–0.91) | 1.27 (1.21–1.34) |
PI not employed | 1.38 (1.28–1.49) | 1.33 (1.27–1.39) | 1.21 (1.13–1.30) | 1.28 (1.22–1.35) |
PI manual workers | 1.02 (0.98–1.06) | 1.37 (1.31–1.43) | 0.97 (0.94–1.02) | 1.30 (1.23–1.36) |
PH overcrowded | 1.16 (1.12–1.20) | 1.34 (1.27–1.40) | 1.11 (1.07–1.15) | 1.28 (1.21–1.34) |
PH no car owned | 1.44 (1.38–1.51) | 1.24 (1.16–1.30) | 1.32 (1.26–1.38) | 1.22 (1.14–1.29) |
Social fragmentation | ||||
PH lone adult | 1.53 (1.44–1.63) | 1.27 (1.21–1.34) | 1.36 (1.28–1.45) | 1.24 (1.17–1.31) |
PH rents home | 1.29 (1.25–1.34) | 1.26 (1.19–1.32) | 1.21 (1.17–1.26) | 1.23 (1.16–1.30) |
PI residential transience | 1.21 (1.16–1.26) | 1.32 (1.26–1.38) | 1.14 (1.10–1.19) | 1.27 (1.21–1.34) |
Social marginalization | ||||
PI violent offending | 1.21 (1.18–1.25) | 1.29 (1.23–1.36) | 1.13 (1.10–1.17) | 1.26 (1.20–1.33) |
PI any criminality | 1.15 (1.11–1.19) | 1.33 (1.27–1.39) | 1.08 (1.05–1.12) | 1.28 (1.22–1.35) |
PI born abroad | 1.22 (1.18–1.26) | 1.30 (1.24–1.36) | 1.16 (1.12–1.19) | 1.26 (1.19–1.32) |
Physical illness | ||||
PI Physical illness | 1.16 (1.10–1.22) | 1.35 (1.29–1.41) | 1.12 (1.07–1.18) | 1.28 (1.22–1.35) |
All area-level indicese | – | 1.23 (1.16–1.30) | – | 1.22 (1.14–1.29) |
The specific contextual effects are the neighborhood-level indicators examined. The general contextual effect estimates effects of neighborhood context on disease incidence without reference to any of the specific neighborhood-level constructs other than the very boundaries defining neighborhoods.
PI: Proportion of individuals; PH: Proportion of households
a Estimates were based on multilevel survival analyses, neighborhoods are 1885 novel data zones nested in Denmark’s 98 municipalities. The 579 039 people born in Denmark 1972–1981 were followed for development of schizophrenia. During the follow-up period from 1982 to 2016, a total of 5103 developed schizophrenia during the 17 191 889 person-years at risk
b Estimates were adjusted for individual-level age and its interaction with sex and neighborhood-level age distribution (Basic adjustment).
c Estimates were adjusted for Basic adjustment and individual-level residential instability, parental Charlson, parental death, parental imprisonment, parental age, parental history of mental disorders, parental income, parental employment status, and parental education.
d Neighborhood-level covariates measure the effect of a one standard deviation increase in the proportion of each covariate. Neighborhood-level covariates were data zone-level averages 1981–1984 (details in Supplementary Methods 2). The effects of each neighborhood-level covariate were modeled separately.
e Neighborhood-level covariates were summarized using the first 3 Principal Components of all neighborhood-level covariates (excl. urbanicity and population density). The fixed effect (specific contextual effect) estimate is not comparable to the other neighborhood-level fixed effects and is therefore not shown.
Fig. 1.
Scatterplot of neighborhood-level covariates. The scatterplots reveal the correlation between the neighborhood-level covariates across the 1885 data zones as well as their distributions (details in Supplementary Methods 1 and 2, Supplementary Figure 1).
Table 4 shows the associations for the two competing measures of urbanization: urbanicity and population density. The former measured the effect of residence in the most urban environment compared to the most rural environment, whereas the latter measured the effect arising from a one standard deviation increase in the log population density. With full individual-level adjustment, higher population density and higher urbanicity were both associated with elevated schizophrenia risks. Further adjusting for all neighborhood-level indices (Table 3) reduced the effect of both measures of urbanization (Table 4). Further adjusting the two competing measures of urbanization mutually, neighborhood-level population density had marginal significant impact, whereas the relationship with urbanicity remained essentially unchanged.
Table 4.
Urbanicity, Population Density and Data Zone-Level Heterogeneity by Individual- and Neighborhood-Level Adjustment Scenarios
Basic adjustmenta,b | Full individual-level adjustmenta,c | |||
---|---|---|---|---|
Specific contextual effect | General contextual effect | Specific contextual effect | General contextual effect | |
Measure of urbanizationd | Incidence Rate Ratio of neighborhood per 1 sd increase | Median Incidence Rate ratio (95% Probability Interval) | Incidence Rate Ratio of neighborhood per 1 sd increase | Median Incidence Rate Ratio (95% Probability Interval) |
No adjustment for neighborhood-level covariates | ||||
Population Density | 1.24 (1.20–1.29) | 1.30 (1.24–1.36) | 1.20 (1.16–1.25) | 1.24 (1.17–1.31) |
Urbanicity | 1.98 (1.77–2.22) | 1.29 (1.22–1.35) | 1.78 (1.59–1.98) | 1.23 (1.16–1.30) |
Adjusted for neighborhood-level covariatese | ||||
Population Density | 1.08 (1.02–1.14) | 1.23 (1.15–1.29) | 1.08 (1.02–1.15) | 1.22 (1.14–1.28) |
Urbanicity | 1.29 (1.10–1.50) | 1.22 (1.15–1.29) | 1.30 (1.11–1.51) | 1.21 (1.13–1.28) |
Adjusted for neighborhood-level covariatese and other urbanization proxy | ||||
Population Density | 1.06 (0.99–1.12) | 1.22 (1.15–1.29) | 1.06 (1.00–1.12) | 1.21 (1.13–1.28) |
Urbanicity | 1.24 (1.06–1.45) | 1.22 (1.15–1.29) | 1.24 (1.06–1.46) | 1.21 (1.13–1.28) |
a Estimates were based on multilevel survival analyses, neighborhoods are 1885 novel data zones nested in Denmark’s 98 municipalities. The 579 039 people born in Denmark 1972–1981 were followed for development of schizophrenia. During the follow-up period from 1982 to 2016, a total of 5103 developed schizophrenia during the 17 191 889 person-years at risk
b Estimates were adjusted for individual-level age and its interaction with sex and neighborhood-level age distribution (Basic adjustment).
c Estimates were adjusted for Basic adjustment and individual-level residential instability, parental Charlson, parental death, parental imprisonment, parental age, parental history of mental disorders, parental income, parental employment status and parental education.
d Proxy measure for urbanization. For each data zone, population density was calculated as the number of inhabitants divided by the area, log transformed and thereafter standardized to unit standard deviation. The estimate for urbanicity measure the effect of residence in the most urban environment compared to residence in the most rural environment. It was included as a trend variable scored as Capital 1, capital suburb 0.75, provincial city 0.5, provincial towns 0.25, and rural area 0. Both variables were delineated at initiation of follow-up. Due to the different scales, effect sizes are not directly comparable in size.
e Neighborhood-level indicators were summarized using the first 3 Principal Components of all neighborhood-level covariates (excl. urbanicity and population density).
Sensitivity Analyses
Using parish as the neighborhood boundaries of interest nearly identical results were obtained except neighborhood-level population density was more important than urbanicity (Supplementary Methods 3, Supplementary Tables 2 and 3). Identical results were obtained considering schizophrenia spectrum disorders as the outcome of interest (9288 developed the disorder during 17 140 418 person-year at risk, Supplementary Tables 4 and 5). Identical results were obtained using explanatory factor analyses to generate a combined adjustment for all neighborhood-level constructs (Supplementary Methods 4, Supplementary Table 6).
Discussion
Previous studies have identified consistent but unexplained urban-rural differences in schizophrenia risk.3,4 Utilizing Denmark’s rich interlinked population-based registers,50 we divided the country into 1885 homogeneously sized data zones, derived a comprehensive spectrum of neighborhood-level characteristics, and used multilevel survival analyses to tease out the effect of neighborhood independently from the characteristics of individuals living in these neighborhoods. We observed unexplained heterogeneity on schizophrenia risk across data zones, which was not explained after adjustment for all measured individual-level covariates. Ten of the thirteen specific neighborhood-level characteristics examined were associated with increased schizophrenia risk. The neighborhood-level associations were attenuated slightly by adjustment for multiple individual-level factors, but the general contextual effect persisted. Finally, following comprehensive individual- and neighborhood-level adjustment, the effects of urbanicity and population density were both greatly attenuated. Further adjusting population density for urbanicity attenuated that relationship marginally around statistical significance, whilst the effect of urbanicity was slightly attenuated. In this large population-based study, multiple neighborhood-level characteristics across a broad array were associated with schizophrenia risk; there was unexplained heterogeneity in schizophrenia risk across data zones, and the effect of urbanicity was attenuated but remained significant.
The most directly comparable published findings thus far have been reported by Zammit et al.48 who showed that the association with urbanicity was attenuated after adjustment for social fragmentation (immigration, residential mobility, single-parent household) and was eliminated when further adjusted for municipality-level population density. It was based on 203 829 Swedish individuals, 328 of whom were diagnosed with schizophrenia; i.e. a much lower level of statistical power compared to the study that we conducted. Using our data zones with homogenously sized populations, urbanicity tended to be more important than population density. Conversely, using parish boundaries as the geographic delineator, population density tended to be more important than urbanicity (Supplementary Methods 3).55
We opted not to generate composite area-level measures of material deprivation74 or social fragmentation.75 Instead we examined multiple discrete area-level and individual-level indices across a number of sociodemographic and health domains. This constituted a broad array of explanatory variables but, as with any investigation conducted using routinely collected administrative data, we were constrained by the information that was available. Thus, the array of variables examined was not comprehensive, and a considerable degree of residual confounding in the multivariable modeling that we performed is plausible. For instance, following adjustment for all identified individual- and neighborhood-level covariates there remained statistically significant unexplained heterogeneity on schizophrenia risk across neighborhoods. There are numerous unmeasured individual-level genetic and environmental factors as well as area-level contextual factors that could explain this variability.
Among the neighborhood-level indices that we examined, the proportion of households without a car and the proportion of lone adult households were the strongest predictors of schizophrenia risk. Not owning a car has been used extensively in the literature as a proxy for poverty, but older age, living in an urban neighborhood with high quality local public transportation infrastructure, taking a strong political position on environmental pollution caused by vehicle exhaust fumes, and major life events such as marital break-up can all deter people from owning a car.76 Conversely, in rural areas, car ownership is often considered a necessity, regardless of socioeconomic status. Thus, indices such as households without a car and those in which single adults live without other residing adults may act as crude markers for other unmeasured complex neighborhood characteristics. Localities, where not owning a car and not living with other adults, are much more common than in the average neighborhood are probably atypical in other influential ways that are unmeasured. Therefore, the observed associations might not be causal. Such variables are also strongly correlated at both area-level and individual-level (Figure 1). Thus, adults living in large cities with high population density are more likely to live in single-occupancy dwellings and not own a car.
Interestingly, studies have reported that the incidence of schizophrenia in ethnic minorities is greater when they compromise a smaller proportion of the population.43,47,77 In this study including people born in Denmark to Danish-born parents, for each standard deviation increase in the proportion of neighborhood’s residents who are foreign-born, schizophrenia risk increased 1.22 fold (Table 3). Thus, in neighborhoods where ethnic minority groups are relatively large in size, this “ethnic density” (or ethnic distinctness) influence may also operate among residents who do not belong to ethnic minorities (people born in Denmark to Danish-born parents in this study). Further studies are needed to investigate the potential generalizability of this novel finding, both in terms of setting and mental health outcome.
In summary, multiple neighborhood-level characteristics across a broad array were associated with schizophrenia risk and collectively these neighborhood-level constructs reduced the effect of urbanicity from 1.98 (95% CI: 1.77–2.22) to 1.30 (95% CI: 1.11–1.51), i.e., contextual factors explained the majority of the urbanicity effect.
Strengths and Limitations
The use of interlinked Danish national registers yielded abundant statistical power and precision for conducting this study. We had access to complete residential address information for all Danish residents. Ascertainment of schizophrenia cases was based on contacts with inpatient and outpatient psychiatric departments and visits to psychiatric emergency care units in a country where barriers to treatment are low, and where treatment is provided through a public healthcare system that is free of charge, and where there is virtually no private mental healthcare provision. Financial factors are thus far less likely to influence pathways to healthcare in Denmark compared to other nations. Our study is thus representative of the entire population irrespective of recall bias, health status, and socioeconomic position.18 The prospective nature of this study ensured that all covariates were measured before illness onset. The results of the study are therefore not biased by temporality nor selective migration of people prior to disease onset.9,21
In addition, although our results were based on longitudinal prospective survival analyses and have adjusted for some key confounders including history of parental mental illness, urbanicity, and socioeconomic position, as with all observational studies, causality cannot be inferred.78
Internationally, this is the largest cohort study to disentangle individual-level and neighborhood-level risk factors on schizophrenia risk. Nationally, it is the first Danish study to utilize a nationwide geographic division of homogeneously sized small area populations on any health outcome. The individual- and area-level measures included in this study may not constitute a comprehensive array of all risk factors. As with all register-based studies, we can only examine the information that’s been collected for administrative purposes.
Conclusion
From the largest prospective multilevel survival analyses of schizophrenia risk conducted to date, multiple specific contextual constructs were associated with schizophrenia risk. There, however, remained a substantial level of unexplained heterogeneity in schizophrenia risk across neighborhood boundaries, and the effect of urbanicity was attenuated but remained significant. Although our analyses and findings are novel, we conclude that our understanding of the role of urbanicity in schizophrenia’s etiology remains incomplete, and further population-based multilevel modeling studies are needed.
Supplementary Material
Acknowledgments
This study was funded by the Lundbeck Foundation Initiative for Integrative Psychiatric Research (iPSYCH) (Grant number: R155-2014-1724), and by BERTHA - the Danish Big Data Centre for Environment and Health funded by the Novo Nordisk Foundation Challenge Programme (grant NNF17OC0027864). The National Institute for Health Research (NIHR) Greater Manchester Patient Safety Translational Research Centre funds a portion of Roger Webb’s salary costs. Lundbeck Foundation grant 20200535 (Precision Psychiatry Inititative) funds a portion of Chun Fan’s and Wesley Thompson’s salary. The funders of the study had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Carsten Bøcker Pedersen, Allan Timmermann, Marianne Giørtz Pedersen and Sussie Antonsen had full access to all the data in the study and take full responsibility for the integrity of the data and the accuracy of the data analysis.
Conflict of interests
All authors declare no conflict of interest.
References
- 1.Faris REI, Dunham HW.. Mental Disorders in Urban Areas: An Ecological Study of Schizophrenia and Other Psychoses. Chicago: Chicago University Press; 1939. [Google Scholar]
- 2.Pollock HM, Nolan WJ. Mental disease in cities, villages and rural districts of New York State, 1915-1920. State Hospital. 1921; 7:38–65. [Google Scholar]
- 3.March D, Hatch SL, Morgan C, et al. Psychosis and place. Epidemiol Rev. 2008;30:84–100. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Vassos E, Pedersen CB, Murray RM, Collier DA, Lewis CM. Meta-analysis of the association of urbanicity with schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38(6):1118–1123. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Sundquist K, Frank G, Sundquist J. Urbanisation and incidence of psychosis and depression: follow-up study of 4.4 million women and men in Sweden. Br J Psychiatry. 2004;184:293–298. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Marcelis M, Navarro-Mateu F, Murray R, Selten JP, Van Os J. Urbanization and psychosis: a study of 1942–1978 birth cohorts in The Netherlands. Psychol Med. 1998;28(4):871–879. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.van Os J, Hanssen M, Bijl RV, Vollebergh W. Prevalence of psychotic disorder and community level of psychotic symptoms: an urban-rural comparison. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58(7):663–668. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.van Os J, Hanssen M, Bak M, Bijl RV, Vollebergh W. Do urbanicity and familial liability coparticipate in causing psychosis? Am J Psychiatry. 2003;160(3):477–482. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Lewis G, David A, Andréasson S, Allebeck P. Schizophrenia and city life. Lancet. 1992;340(8812):137–140. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Marcelis M, Takei N, van Os J. Urbanization and risk for schizophrenia: does the effect operate before or around the time of illness onset? Psychol Med. 1999;29(5):1197–1203. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Suvisaari JM, Haukka JK, Tanskanen AJ, Lönnqvist JK. Decreasing seasonal variation of births in schizophrenia. Psychol Med. 2000;30(2):315–324. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Mortensen PB, Pedersen CB, Westergaard T, et al. Effects of family history and place and season of birth on the risk of schizophrenia. N Engl J Med. 1999;340(8):603–608. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Pedersen CB, Mortensen PB. Family history, place and season of birth as risk factors for schizophrenia in Denmark: a replication and reanalysis. Br J Psychiatry. 2001;179:46–52. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Pedersen CB, Mortensen PB. Are the cause(s) responsible for urban-rural differences in schizophrenia risk rooted in families or in individuals? Am J Epidemiol. 2006;163(11):971–978. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Pedersen CB. No evidence of time trends in the urban-rural differences in schizophrenia risk among five million people born in Denmark from 1910 to 1986. Psychol Med. 2006;36(2):211–219. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Pedersen CB, Mortensen PB. Evidence of a dose-response relationship between urbanicity during upbringing and schizophrenia risk. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2001;58(11):1039–1046. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Pedersen CB, Mortensen PB. Why factors rooted in the family may solely explain the urban-rural differences in schizophrenia risk estimates. Epidemiol Psichiatr Soc. 2006;15(4):247–251. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Pedersen CB, Mortensen PB. Sibship characteristics during upbringing and schizophrenia risk. Am J Epidemiol. 2004;160(7):652–660. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Haukka J, Suvisaari J, Varilo T, Lönnqvist J. Regional variation in the incidence of schizophrenia in Finland: a study of birth cohorts born from 1950 to 1969. Psychol Med. 2001;31(6):1045–1053. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Torrey EF, Bowler AE, Clark K. Urban birth and residence as risk factors for psychoses: an analysis of 1880 data. Schizophr Res. 1997;25(3):169–176. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Pedersen CB. Persons with schizophrenia migrate towards urban areas due to the development of their disorder or its prodromata. Schizophr Res. 2015;168(1-2):204–208. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.McGrath JJ, Petersen L, Agerbo E, Mors O, Mortensen PB, Pedersen CB. A comprehensive assessment of parental age and psychiatric disorders. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71(3):301–309. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Byrne M, Agerbo E, Eaton WW, Mortensen PB. Parental socio-economic status and risk of first admission with schizophrenia- a Danish national register based study. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2004;39(2):87–96. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Agerbo E, Torrey EF, Mortensen PB. Household crowding in early adulthood and schizophrenia are unrelated in Denmark: a nested case-control study. Schizophr Res. 2001;47(2-3):243–246. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Cantor-Graae E, Pedersen CB. Risk of schizophrenia in second-generation immigrants: a Danish population-based cohort study. Psychol Med. 2007;37(4):485–494. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Cantor-Graae E, Pedersen CB. Full spectrum of psychiatric disorders related to foreign migration: a Danish population-based cohort study. JAMA Psychiatry. 2013;70(4):427–435. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Allardyce J, Boydell J, Van Os J, et al. Comparison of the incidence of schizophrenia in rural Dumfries and Galloway and urban Camberwell. Br J Psychiatry. 2001;179:335–339. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Kirkbride JB, Fearon P, Morgan C, et al. Heterogeneity in incidence rates of schizophrenia and other psychotic syndromes: findings from the 3-center AeSOP study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2006;63(3):250–258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Mortensen PB, Nørgaard-Pedersen B, Waltoft BL, Sørensen TL, Hougaard D, Yolken RH. Early infections of Toxoplasma gondii and the later development of schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2007;33(3):741–744. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Brown AS, Schaefer CA, Quesenberry CP Jr, Liu L, Babulas VP, Susser ES. Maternal exposure to toxoplasmosis and risk of schizophrenia in adult offspring. Am J Psychiatry. 2005;162(4):767–773. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.McGrath J. Hypothesis: is low prenatal vitamin D a risk-modifying factor for schizophrenia? Schizophr Res. 1999;40(3):173–177. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.McGrath JJ, Eyles DW, Pedersen CB, et al. Neonatal vitamin D status and risk of schizophrenia: a population-based case-control study. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(9):889–894. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Laursen TM, Munk-Olsen T, Nordentoft M, Bo Mortensen P. A comparison of selected risk factors for unipolar depressive disorder, bipolar affective disorder, schizoaffective disorder, and schizophrenia from a danish population-based cohort. J Clin Psychiatry. 2007;68(11):1673–1681. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Antonsen S, Mok PLH, Webb RT, et al. Exposure to air pollution during childhood and risk of developing schizophrenia: a national cohort study. Lancet Planet Health. 2020;4(2):e64–e73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Croudace TJ, Kayne R, Jones PB, Harrison GL. Non-linear relationship between an index of social deprivation, psychiatric admission prevalence and the incidence of psychosis. Psychol Med. 2000;30(1):177–185. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Allardyce J, Gilmour H, Atkinson J, Rapson T, Bishop J, McCreadie RG. Social fragmentation, deprivation and urbanicity: relation to first-admission rates for psychoses. Br J Psychiatry. 2005;187:401–406. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Kirkbride JB, Jones PB, Ullrich S, Coid JW. Social deprivation, inequality, and the neighborhood-level incidence of psychotic syndromes in East London. Schizophr Bull. 2014;40(1):169–180. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Kirkbride JB, Morgan C, Fearon P, Dazzan P, Murray RM, Jones PB. Neighbourhood-level effects on psychoses: re-examining the role of context. Psychol Med. 2007;37(10):1413–1425. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Veling W, Susser E, Selten JP, Hoek HW. Social disorganization of neighborhoods and incidence of psychotic disorders: a 7-year first-contact incidence study. Psychol Med. 2015;45(9):1789–1798. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Kirkbride JB, Boydell J, Ploubidis GB, et al. Testing the association between the incidence of schizophrenia and social capital in an urban area. Psychol Med. 2008;38(8):1083–1094. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Silver E, Mulvey EP, Swanson JW. Neighborhood structural characteristics and mental disorder: Faris and Dunham revisited. Soc Sci Med. 2002;55(8):1457–1470. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Allardyce J, Boydell J. Review: the wider social environment and schizophrenia. Schizophr Bull. 2006;32(4):592–598. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Boydell J, van Os J, McKenzie K, et al. Incidence of schizophrenia in ethnic minorities in London: ecological study into interactions with environment. BMJ. 2001;323(7325):1336–1338. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Kirkbride JB, Fearon P, Morgan C, et al. Neighbourhood variation in the incidence of psychotic disorders in Southeast London. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2007;42(6):438–445. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Veling W, Susser E, van Os J, Mackenbach JP, Selten JP, Hoek HW. Ethnic density of neighborhoods and incidence of psychotic disorders among immigrants. Am J Psychiatry. 2008;165(1):66–73. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Schofield P, Thygesen M, Das-Munshi J, et al. Neighbourhood ethnic density and psychosis - Is there a difference according to generation? Schizophr Res. 2018;195:501–505. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Schofield P, Thygesen M, Das-Munshi J, et al. Ethnic density, urbanicity and psychosis risk for migrant groups—a population cohort study. Schizophr Res. 2017;190:82–87. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Zammit S, Lewis G, Rasbash J, Dalman C, Gustafsson JE, Allebeck P. Individuals, schools, and neighborhood: a multilevel longitudinal study of variation in incidence of psychotic disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(9):914–922. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Richardson L, Hameed Y, Perez J, Jones PB, Kirkbride JB. Association of environment with the risk of developing psychotic disorders in rural populations: findings from the social epidemiology of psychoses in East Anglia Study. JAMA Psychiatry. 2018;75(1):75–83. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Frank L. Epidemiology. The epidemiologist’s dream: Denmark. Science. 2003;301(5630):163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Meijer M, Engholm G, Grittner U, Gritter U, Bloomfield K. A socioeconomic deprivation index for small areas in Denmark. Scand J Public Health. 2013;41(6):560–569. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Pedersen CB, Gøtzsche H, Møller JO, Mortensen PB. The Danish Civil Registration System. A cohort of eight million persons. Dan Med Bull. 2006;53(4):441–449. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Ministeriet for Videnskab -Teknologi og Udvikling [Ministry of science - technology and development]. Digitalt atlas over Danmarks historisk-administrative geografi [Digital map of historical administrative boarders in Denmark]. Ministeriet for Videnskab, Teknologi og Udvikling [Ministry of science, technology and development]. www.digdag.dk. Published 2021. Accessed 2021.
- 54.The Danish Civil Registration Office. Kommunesammenlægninger/ opdeling [Danish municipality reforms]. Published 2007. Accessed 2021.
- 55.Flowerdew R, Manley DJ, Sabel CE. Neighbourhood effects on health: does it matter where you draw the boundaries? Soc Sci Med. 2008;66(6):1241–1255. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Martin D, Nolan A, Tranmer M. The Application of Zone-Design Methodology in the 2001 UK Census. Environ Plan A: Econ Space. 2001;33(11):1949–1962. [Google Scholar]
- 57.Sabel CE, Kihal W, Bard D, Weber C. Creation of synthetic homogeneous neighbourhoods using zone design algorithms to explore relationships between asthma and deprivation in Strasbourg, France. Soc Sci Med. 2013;91:110–121. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Fan CC, McGrath JJ, Appadurai V, et al. Spatial fine-mapping for gene-by-environment effects identifies risk hot spots for schizophrenia. Nat Commun. 2018;9(1):5296. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Petersson F, Baadsgaard M, Thygesen LC. Danish registers on personal labour market affiliation. Scand J Public Health. 2011;39(7 Suppl):95–98. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Pedersen CB, Mors O, Bertelsen A, et al. A comprehensive nationwide study of the incidence rate and lifetime risk for treated mental disorders. JAMA Psychiatry. 2014;71(5):573–581. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Dean K, Stevens H, Mortensen PB, Murray RM, Walsh E, Pedersen CB. Full spectrum of psychiatric outcomes among offspring with parental history of mental disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2010;67(8):822–829. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Mok PL, Webb RT, Appleby L, Pedersen CB. Full spectrum of mental disorders linked with childhood residential mobility. J Psychiatr Res. 2016;78:57–64. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Mors O, Perto GP, Mortensen PB. The Danish Psychiatric Central Research Register. Scand J Public Health. 2011;39(7 Suppl):54–57. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Cantor-Graae E, Pedersen CB, McNeil TF, Mortensen PB. Migration as a risk factor for schizophrenia: a Danish population-based cohort study. Br J Psychiatry. 2003;182:117–122. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Austin PC, Stryhn H, Leckie G, Merlo J. Measures of clustering and heterogeneity in multilevel Poisson regression analyses of rates/count data. Stat Med. 2018;37(4):572–589. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Carstensen B.Example and programs for splitting follow-up time in cohort studies. Copenhagen: Institute of Public Health, University of Copenhagen. 2007. http://staff.pubhealth.ku.dk/~bxc/Lexis/. Published 2007. [Google Scholar]
- 67.Andersen PK, Borgen O, Gill RD, Keiding N.. Statistical Models Based on Counting Processes. New York: Springer-Verlag; 1993. [Google Scholar]
- 68.Laird N, Olivier D. Covariance analysis of censored survival data using log-linear analysis techniques. J Am Stat Assoc. 1981;76:231–240. [Google Scholar]
- 69.Austin PC. A tutorial on multilevel survival analysis: methods, models and applications. Int Stat Rev. 2017;85(2):185–203. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Merlo J, Wagner P, Austin PC, Subramanian SV, Leckie G. General and specific contextual effects in multilevel regression analyses and their paradoxical relationship: a conceptual tutorial. SSM Popul Health. 2018;5:33–37. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Varese F, Smeets F, Drukker M, et al. Childhood adversities increase the risk of psychosis: a meta-analysis of patient-control, prospective- and cross-sectional cohort studies. Schizophr Bull. 2012;38(4):661–671. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Hakulinen C, Webb RT, Pedersen CB, Agerbo E, Mok PLH. Association between parental income during childhood and risk of schizophrenia later in life. JAMA Psychiatry. 2020;77(1):17–24. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Agerbo E, Sullivan PF, Vilhjálmsson BJ, et al. Polygenic risk score, parental socioeconomic status, family history of psychiatric disorders, and the risk for schizophrenia: a Danish Population-Based Study and Meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry. 2015;72(7):635–641. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Ellaway A. Are single indicators of deprivation as useful as composite indicators in predicting morbidity and mortality: results from the Central Clydeside Conurbation. Health Bull (Edinb). 1997;55(5):283–284. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Bagheri N, Batterham PJ, Salvador-Carulla L, et al. Development of the Australian neighborhood social fragmentation index and its association with spatial variation in depression across communities. Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol. 2019;54(10):1189–1198. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Klein NJ, Smart MJ. Life events, poverty, and car ownership in the United States: A mobility biography approach. J Transport Land Use. 2019;12(1):395–418. [Google Scholar]
- 77.Dykxhoorn J, Lewis G, Hollander AC, Kirkbride JB, Dalman C. Association of neighbourhood migrant density and risk of non-affective psychosis: a national, longitudinal cohort study. Lancet Psychiatry. 2020;7(4):327–336. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Rothman J, Greenland S, Lash TL.. Modern Epidemiology, 3.rd edition. Philadelphia: Lippinicott-Raven Publishers, 1998.; 1998. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.