Abstract
The lateral amygdala (LA) encodes fear memories by potentiating sensory inputs associated with threats and, in the process, recruits 10–30% of its neurons per fear memory engram. However, how the local network within the LA processes this information and whether it also plays a role in storing it are still largely unknown. Here, using ex vivo 12-patch-clamp and in vivo 32-electrode electrophysiological recordings in the LA of fear-conditioned rats, in combination with activity-dependent fluorescent and optogenetic tagging and recall, we identified a sparsely connected network between principal LA neurons that is organized in clusters. Fear conditioning specifically causes potentiation of synaptic connections between learning-recruited neurons. These findings of synaptic plasticity in an autoassociative excitatory network of the LA may suggest a basic principle through which a small number of pyramidal neurons could encode a large number of memories.
Subject terms: Neural circuits, Fear conditioning, Amygdala
A sparsely connected network, organized in clusters, identified in the rat lateral amygdala shows potentiation between recruited neurons after fear conditioning. This implies a mechanism for encoding multiple memories with a small number of neurons.
Main
Since the seminal publications of LeDoux and Shinnick-Gallagher1,2, the potentiation of converging sensory inputs into the lateral amygdala (LA) has become the principal working model for how synaptic plasticity in sensory afferents underlies fear learning3. More recently, it was shown how fear learning recruits a subset of 10–30% of LA neurons into the fear memory engram by competitive selection based on their intrinsic excitability4–6. Hence, this has become a framework for studying how memories could be locally encoded within the LA7. However, little is known about the local connections between these neurons and whether and how they are affected by fear learning.
Recently, neuronal ensembles, that is, groups of neurons with simultaneous activity emerging from local excitatory connections, have started to receive increasing attention for their possible role in information processing and memory encoding (for a summary, see refs. 8,9). Thus, Jonas’ group characterized in vitro excitatory connections in the hippocampus CA3 (ref. 10) as well as their capability for local plasticity11. Further, in the developing neocortex, neurons with similar developmental origin appear to exhibit higher connectivity12, and in the mature visual cortex, cortical neurons with similar stimulus feature selectivity are clonally related13 and show higher interconnectivity14. However, it is not known whether this higher interconnectivity is induced by learning12–14. Indeed, none of the present studies seem to have directly tested the hypothesis that learning itself can induce synaptic changes in local connections within neuronal ensembles9. This gap in knowledge may be inherent to the limitations of the currently available chemo/opto/fluorescence viral tools that either require different neuronal genotypes or sufficient distance between pre- and postsynaptic elements to spatially tag these separately. Also, optical imaging methods provide limited temporal resolution.
In the present study, we used 12-patch-clamp single-cell electrophysiological recordings in vitro and ex vivo and 32 fine wire electrodes mounted on microdrives for single-unit recordings in vivo, together with activity-dependent expression of fluorescent and optogenetic markers to selectively study changes in synaptic connections between fear memory-recruited neurons. We found that, (1) in vitro, Hebbian stimulation can induce synaptic plasticity between local LA neurons, (2) ex vivo, after fear learning, connections between fluorescently labeled neurons are increased in synaptic strength, and, (3) in vivo, fear learning induces increases in functional connectivity between optogenetically labeled neurons. These changes in synaptic connectivity that occur specifically between engram neurons in the LA suggest that encoding of fear memories may also take place by changes within local excitatory circuitry of the LA.
Results
Sparse connectivity within the LA
To expose and characterize excitatory synapses between putative principal LA neurons in vitro, we probed with 12-pipette whole-cell patch-clamp recordings in rat horizontal brain slices for unitary excitatory postsynaptic potentials (uEPSPs) evoked by presynaptically induced action potentials in a single connection (APs; Fig. 1). This configuration allowed us to identify, per experiment, up to 132 neuronal pairs (n(n – 1)) and revealed, between 637 recorded excitatory neurons, 89 excitatory connections distributed evenly across left and right hemispheres (n = 47 and 43 slices, respectively; N = 34 rats; Fig. 2a,b, Extended Data Fig. 1a–d and Supplementary Note 1). The calculated 2.1% connectivity level (89/4,157 tested connections; Methods) places the LA between rat hippocampal CA3 (0.9%), the piriform cortex (1.0%) and primary sensory cortices (>10%)10,15,16. Quantal size and number of release sites (460 ± 320 µV; 5 ± 3 (mean ± s.d.); n = 15 trials × 17 connections; Fig. 2c,d and Extended Data Fig. 1e) were similar to CA3 (500 ± 300 µV; 3.2 ± 0.8)10 and the primary sensory cortex (211 ± 65 µV; 3.4 ± 2.2)17.
By further analysis of intersomatic distances, we uncovered that the highest connection probability and synaptic strength occurred within a 100-µm radius. Both rapidly declined with increasing distance (Fig. 3a and Extended Data Fig. 2a), as also reported in rat somatosensory cortices16, although not in the CA3 (ref. 10) and piriform cortex15. From this distribution, we computed (Supplementary Note 2) an average of up to 230 local inputs per neuron. We found connections occurring for >75% in complex motifs, particularly and significantly more in double-divergent (14 × 2/89), triple-convergent (4 × 3/89), reciprocal (4/89) and feed-forward (16/89) motifs than expected from a random connectivity model (Fig. 3b, Extended Data Fig. 2b–g and Supplementary Note 3). Double-convergent motifs remained within a 300-µm radius around the receiving neuron, and divergent motifs extended further out (Extended Data Fig. 2e,f). Together, this suggests an organization in local clusters that are connected by longer projections18.
Signal processing across the LA
We further characterized the functional organization of this LA network by inducing glutamatergic epileptiform bursts with bicuculline perfusion (Methods)19,20. The variability in onset and spreading of bursts (measured with 3 × 4 groups of patch pipettes) indicated a local buildup propagating in caudal to medial and rostral directions, that is, from the LA to its output regions in the central and basal amygdala, respectively. Furthermore, this was accompanied by a decrease in APs, suggestive of progressive filtering (Fig. 3c and Supplementary Note 4). To further study the filtering characteristics of this network, we exposed individual connections to a series of repetitive stimulations (eight presynaptic APs at 20 Hz). This revealed 33% facilitating, 46% stable, 13% depressing and 8% uncategorized types of synaptic contacts. All exhibited a full recovery uEPSP (uEPSPR) 500 ms later, leading to an overall temporal summation of 183% (n = 89; Fig. 3d and Extended Data Fig. 3a). Based on an arithmetic spatial summation within this network (Fig. 3e, Extended Data Fig. 3b and Supplementary Note 5), this would imply the requirement of synaptic inputs of 100 ± 23 to trigger a postsynaptic AP which, as a result of temporal summation during 20-Hz stimulation, would reduce to a total of 34 ± 8. The local LA network can thus act as a high-pass filter that propagates APs during heightened activation of a sufficient number of synapses. Based on previous findings that cued fear conditioning (CFC) recruits 10–30% of LA neurons6 (which corresponds to activation of 23–69 of a total of 230 neuronal inputs; see above and Supplementary Note 2) and on the premise that CFC can indeed induce a local synaptic potentiation that reaches a similar level as temporal summation, this should allow for a reliable signal propagation across the LA21,22.
In vitro potentiation of connections
To test this premise, we first assessed whether the LA network is indeed capable of local synaptic plasticity. For this purpose, we used a standard Hebbian association protocol23 in vitro, to associate 15 × 10 presynaptic APs (at 30 Hz) with postsynaptically evoked APs (at a delay of 10 ms). This indeed potentiated LA–LA connections by 140 ± 16% but only in intrinsically higher-excitable, nonaccommodating neurons (normalized on baseline; Fig. 4a and Extended Data Fig. 4a,b). Connections from accommodating neurons did not show any potentiation (Fig. 4b) nor did connections that were not exposed to this protocol (Extended Data Figs. 4a,b and 5a–d). The synaptic strengths of nonaccommodating and accommodating neurons were similar before exposure to the Hebbian protocol (baselines of first uEPSP from nonaccommodating (Fig. 4a) and accommodating (Fig. 4b) neurons were not significantly different; Mann–Whitney, U = 32, P = 0.7) and did not correlate with intrinsic excitability (Fig. 4c), indicating that synaptic strength does not predict potentiation. However, the excitability-based requirement for the induction of potentiation narrowly follows previous observations that intrinsic excitability favors recruitment in the fear memory engram6.
Potentiation between neurons within the LA thereby provides a first basis of how local increases in connection strength could bind intrinsically higher-excitable neurons together for stronger coactivation. This potentiation seems to involve a presynaptic redistribution of synaptic efficacy toward the first stimulus24 without any postsynaptic changes in miniature EPSP (mEPSP) amplitude nor AMPA receptor conductance (Extended Data Figs. 4c and 6; of note, however, mEPSPs may also originate from projections outside the LA). Consistent with this redistribution of efficacy, in a double-divergent motif, we found that potentiation caused an increased co-occurrence of successful transmissions at the beginning of the train (Extended Data Fig. 4d). Taken together, this synaptic potentiation leads to a redistribution of presynaptic efficacy binding CFC-recruited LA neurons into a network that exhibits instantaneously higher response characteristics (as has also been observed in vivo after fear conditioning21) and a heightened sensitivity to propagate single, nonrepetitive stimuli, such as evoked by fear memory recall.
Ex vivo connectivity after CFC
To test whether these in vitro-induced increases in strength of local connectivity in the LA could also be found ex vivo, after fear learning, we used an adeno-associated virus (AAV) expressing a destabilized form of green fluorescent protein (dGFP; t1/2 = 2 h) under the Arc promoter enhanced synaptic activity responsive element (E-SARE)25 as a reporter of recruited LA neurons after CFC (Fig. 5a and Extended Data Fig. 7a). Fear recall the next day was indeed able to efficiently induce dGFP expression 90 min later in 24% of the total neuronal LA population (Fig. 5b and Extended Data Fig. 8a,b), similar to well-known reported percentages in mice4 and reflecting successful reactivation of neurons using the E-SARE promoter. Subsequent ex vivo 12-patch-clamp recordings of the recruited (GFP+red fluorescent protein+ (GFP+RPF+)) LA neurons showed (compared to nonrecruited, only RFP+ neurons) more connections (7.2% between recruited neurons and 1.4% between nonrecruited neurons; Extended Data Fig. 7b) with higher uEPSP responses (1.67 ± 0.37 versus 0.24 ± 0.08 mV; Fig. 5c). Again, as in our in vitro findings, no changes occurred in postsynaptic efficacy (Extended Data Fig. 6a,d). Taken together, these recordings show that, after fear learning, recruited neurons in the LA are more likely to be tied together and exhibit stronger connections than nonrecruited neurons.
Hypothetically, it is possible that the increased synaptic strength that we found ex vivo between recruited neurons is, in fact, not the result of fear learning but rather reflects selective recruitment of neurons into the fear memory engram that already had stronger synaptic connections before fear learning, that is, that stronger connections between neurons serve as a selection criterion for inclusion in the fear memory engram4,26,27. However, this reasoning may not apply because we found no correlation between synaptic strength and excitability in naive slices, demonstrating that there is no stronger local synaptic connectivity before recruitment between neurons that exhibit higher intrinsic excitability (that is, are more likely to become potentiated in our in vitro induction protocol; Fig. 4c). Thus, it is most likely that the stronger connections between recruited neurons must have resulted from potentiation induced by CFC.
To discriminate further between these two possibilities, in brain slices prepared after the CFC protocol, we also experimentally tested whether the in vitro Hebbian-induced synaptic plasticity was occluded in recruited connections. Therefore, we compared the ex vivo CFC-induced potentiation with Hebbian-induced in vitro potentiation by applying the Hebbian in vitro protocol (compare Fig. 4a–c) on CFC-recruited and nonrecruited neurons. Whereas further potentiation was indeed occluded in the recruited neurons (uEPSP1: 1.44 ± 0.39 → 1.47 ± 0.36 mV), it was still possible between nonrecruited neurons (0.24 ± 0.08 mV → 0.47 ± 0.13 mV; Fig. 5d and Extended Data Fig. 5e,f). This suggests that, through a Hebbian mechanism, CFC leads to synaptic strengthening between recruited LA neurons and may thereby ensure, after fear conditioning, a strengthening of local synaptic connections that can promote reliable signal propagation across the LA (compare Fig. 3d,e).
CFC-driven potentiation of connections
Contrary to the above ex vivo measurements that can only make cross-comparison changes in synaptic strength between recruited and nonrecruited connections after CFC, in vivo measurements can directly assess any changes induced by CFC longitudinally within the same animal. We thus conducted a series of experiments in behaving animals in which we measured multi-single-unit activity from conditioned stimulus (CS+)-responsive LA neurons throughout a 6-h period before and after CFC (Extended Data Fig. 9). After identifying recruited neurons (by their enhanced firing to the CS+ after CFC; Fig. 6a), we retrospectively traced back their connectivity levels before CFC. We then used Granger causality analysis of baseline spiking levels to statistically assess changes in functional connectivity between principal neurons (Methods28,29). Across all neurons, we found that functional connectivity levels (2.8 ± 4.7% (±s.d.), 78 connections, n = 7 rats, n = 6,929 possible connections) and the distribution of connectivity motifs both corresponded with the in vitro findings (compare Figs. 6b and 3b), corroborating the comparability between our in vitro and in vivo measurements. Before CFC, functional connectivity levels did not differ between future CFC-recruited and nonrecruited neurons, already suggesting no functional connectivity bias in recruitment (Fig. 6c). Moreover, only after CFC did we find that recruited neurons, in contrast to nonrecruited neurons, exhibit significant increases in functional connectivity (from 0.6 ± 0.3 to 3.7 ± 1.0 artificial units (AU) versus 0.5 ± 0.5 AU to 0.3 ± 0.1; Fig. 6c). We have to acknowledge that the Granger causality approach does not allow us to definitely determine in vivo that local recurrent synaptic connections between recruited/engram neurons are strengthened as a result of fear conditioning, and it is still possible that in vivo coupling could be driven by other factors (for example, distal inputs to LA) rather than local synaptic changes. However, given the correspondence between our ex vivo and in vivo findings, there is compelling evidence that does suggest the later interpretation. Taken together, these findings point to an enhanced local functional connectivity that is caused by, rather than at the origin of, recruitment in the fear memory engram.
Further supporting our previous observations, these in vivo findings also showed that future to-be recruited neurons exhibited significantly higher baseline firing rates before CFC than nonrecruited neurons (Fig. 6d). This finding confirms the notion that intrinsic excitability can predict recruitment as shown previously ex vivo by others6 and as we had found in vitro before Hebbian induction (Fig. 4 and Extended Data Fig. 4).
Optogenetic reactivation of recruited neurons
Our ex vivo and in vivo findings are thus consistent with an organization of neuronal ensembles in the LA through which signal propagation is enhanced after fear learning as a result of potentiation of local connections between recruited neurons. However, ex vivo, we used an activity reporter gene, whereas in vivo, we used CS+ responsiveness to identify neuronal recruitment. To confirm that these methods identify the same populations of neuronal ensembles, we developed an in vivo protocol that more closely matched our ex vivo approach. We therefore replaced dGFP with a stable tamoxifen-inducible optogenetic Channelrhodopsin-2 (ChR2) tag under the same Arc promoter E-SARE25 that we used in the ex vivo experiments. With tamoxifen, 24 h after CFC, we opened a time window to initiate ChR2 expression by CS+ recall (Fig. 7a and Methods). After 1 week of ChR2 expression (Extended Data Fig. 8c) and LA optrode implantation (Extended Data Fig. 9a–e), we found that most identified principal LA neurons (Extended Data Fig. 10e) that responded to the CS+ also responded to the blue light (BL) (65 out of 82; Fig. 7b,c, Extended Data Fig. 9f and Supplementary Note 6). This 79% overlap is close to the percentage of activated neurons that are typically labeled with our E-SARE approach25. Having established this overlap, we used Granger causality analysis and found significantly stronger causal connectivity levels between recruited neurons (BL responsive, 2.3 ± 0.4 AU, n = 4) than between nonrecruited neurons (0.8 ± 0.1 AU, n = 67; Fig. 7d) also in BL-responsive neuronal ensembles. Moreover, behaviorally, reactivation of these neurons, which had been active previously during fear memory recall, by BL fully recapitulated the freezing levels as seen after presentation of the CS+ (75% for BL and 79% for CS+; Fig. 7e). Together, these findings indicate that these tagged neurons (and by inference dGFP-expressing neurons) identify the same recruited ensembles in ex vivo and in vivo measurements, further supporting the comparability between these different approaches.
Discussion
Here, we examined the extent of local connectivity among LA neurons and how these synaptic connections are modulated by fear learning. We found that the connectivity levels throughout the in vitro (2.1%), ex vivo (1.9%) and in vivo (2.8%) preparations, together with the in vitro- and in vivo-identified connectivity motifs, all converge onto the identification of an autoassociative, sparsely connected excitatory network in the rat LA with self-propagating and filtering characteristics that, in basic configuration, permit transmission across the LA of repetitive stimulations and, after fear learning, may allow for propagation of single stimuli through synaptic potentiation between locally recruited LA neurons. Although based on a limited number of observations, due to sparse connectivity levels, together, these findings reposition the LA from a passive relay station into an active hub where synaptic plasticity strengthens LA–LA connections within neuronal ensembles following fear learning. Thus, fear memory encoding involves not only the recruitment of intrinsically more highly excitable LA neurons and the potentiation of their external afferents but also stronger binding of these neurons together in a local network that, after fear learning, can promote signal processing across the LA.
Recent in vivo findings in the visual cortex have shown that Hebbian paradigms can artificially imprint neuronal ensembles for coactivation30, and this was shown to affect high-performance behavioral discrimination tasks31. Meanwhile, the synaptic and circuit mechanisms by which this happens are still unclear and remain an area of intense investigation9. In the mouse basolateral amygdala, different neuronal ensembles can encode distinct behavioral states32,33 or learned associations with distinct aversive events34 or separated by different time intervals35. However, following coactivated recall, integration between individual memories may occur together with a coallocation of partially overlapping neuronal ensembles. The sparse connectivity that we found in the LA would allow storing such multiple memories within and between neuronal ensembles by strengthening of existing connections or the formation of additional connections (see also Lisman et al.7). Furthermore, the autoassociative features of this network could permit single neurons to trigger reactivation of connected neurons all or not part of multiple ensembles, leading to pattern completion9,36, and contribute to the stabilization of memories7 and/or the integration of individually formed memories, for example, such as occurring during second-order fear conditioning37.
Recently, in the primate amygdala, the importance of timing and the order of spiking activity for memory encoding was shown, suggesting that even different sequences could encode stimuli of opposite valence38. The circuit mechanisms that can generate such reliable sequences, however, are yet to be demonstrated and will require techniques of high spatiotemporal resolution. The present combination of electrophysiological recordings allows such high-resolution measurements and provides functional insights in signal processing and memory formation by showing intranuclear plasticity within the LA, a most primitive cortex. It brings forth a mechanism that places plasticity within neuronal ensembles at the heart of fear memory encoding. The LA could therefore serve as an important model system to study learning and memory through local plasticity within other autoassociative cortical networks.
Methods
Animals
We used in-house-bred Wistar (14–19 days old; Ecole Polytechnique Fédérale de Lausanne) and Sprague–Dawley (4–6 weeks old; Center for Psychiatric Neuroscience) rats of both sexes. We found no differences in electrophysiological measurements between different ages, strains (see Figs. 2–4 versus Fig. 5) or sexes, so all animals were pooled together (see also Supplementary Note 1). Animals were housed at room temperature (~20 °C) and placed under a 12-h light/12-h dark cycle, with behavioral experiments performed during the light cycle. All animal handling procedures were approved by the Veterinary Service of the Canton of Vaud (authorizations VD2745 and VD3205).
Whole-cell recordings on acute brain slices
Animals were decapitated, and their brains were swiftly extracted and placed in chilled artificial cerebrospinal fluid (ACSF). The ACSF slicing solution was saturated with oxycarbon (95% O2 and 5% CO2) at pH 7.4 and contained 110 mM sucrose, 60 mM NaCl, 28 mM NaHCO3, 3 mM KCl, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4, 7 mM MgSO4, 0.5 mM CaCl2 and 5 mM d-glucose (Sigma-Aldrich). Acute, horizontal 400-μm-thick rat brain slices were cut between −8.6 mm and −7.6 mm depth from bregma39 using a vibratome (Compresstome VF-200, Precisionary Instruments); the presence of external capsule fibers and the beginning of the lateral ventricle were used as landmarks. After slicing, each of the usually obtained four slices was transferred on a nylon grid in a beaker filled with extracellular oxygenated ACSF solution (described below), with a recovery period of at least 1 h at room temperature before being transferred to the recording chamber. Under hyperexcitable conditions, to study epileptiform bursting activity, the KCl concentration was increased to 5 mM, and bicuculline-methiodide (20 µM; Sigma-Aldrich) was added to block GABAA receptors.
A semiautomated 12-patch-clamp setup40 (Fig. 1) was used to allow multiple-patch-clamp recordings. Cells were visualized by infrared differential interference contrast video microscopy using a VX55 camera (TILL Photonics) mounted on an upright BX51WI microscope equipped with an Olympus U-RFL-T lamp housing a 100-W mercury burner (Olympus Corporation). A group of up to 12 cells were selected for the electrophysiological recordings based on their morphology (pyramidal shaped) and, where applicable, their fluorescence. The identity of these cells was further confirmed following the injection of square pulses of hyperpolarizing and depolarizing 400-ms currents in 50-pA steps41,42 to assess accommodating and nonaccommodating subtypes or interneurons based on their high-frequency (that is, >30 Hz) firing rates. Besides the number of APs resulting from sustained current injection, accommodating and nonaccommodating neurons could be separated further by the delay required to observe an AP (that is, time-to-spike) following minimal stimulation (400 ms, 20-pA steps) with 111 ± 12 ms for nonaccommodating neurons and 82 ± 8 ms for accommodating neurons. In this manner, we classified nonaccommodating neurons with a time-to-spike of >100 ms. Interneurons were excluded from further experimentation and analyses (see also Supplementary Note 1 for more details).
Electrophysiological data were acquired with a Multiclamp 700B (Molecular Devices) in either current clamp or voltage clamp mode. Data acquisition was performed through an ITC-1600 board (Instrutech) connected to a PC running a custom-written routine (Pulse-Q) under IGOR Pro (Wavemetrics, version 7). Recordings were sampled at 10 kHz, and the recorded signal was filtered with a 5-kHz Bessel filter.
Recording pipettes of 4–10 MΩ were pulled from borosilicate capillary glass (Sutter Instrument; outer diameter: 1.5 mm; inner diameter: 0.86 mm; 7.5 cm length) by a P-97 Flame-Brown Micropipette Puller (Sutter Instrument). The pipettes were filled with an internal solution composed of 135 mM KMeSO4, 8 mM NaCl, 10 mM HEPES, 2 mM Mg2ATP, 0.3 mM Na3-GTP and 1 mg ml−1 biocytin (Sigma-Aldrich) with a pH of 7.3 and an osmolarity of 300 mOsm. The ACSF in the recording bath was composed of 118 mM NaCl, 25 mM NaHCO3, 10 mM d-glucose, 2.5 mM KCl, 1 mM MgCl2, 1.25 mM NaH2PO4 and 2 mM CaCl2 (Sigma-Aldrich) dissolved in deionized water of 18.2 MΩ cm resistivity. ACSF was supplemented with 1 mM l-glutamine (Sigma-Aldrich) to avoid homosynaptic depression43,44. Recorded neurons were considered stable in current clamp configuration if their membrane potential was lower than −55 mV and in voltage clamp configuration if less than 200 pA was required to maintain the membrane potential at −70 mV.
EPSP and EPSC analyses
Evoked EPSPs and EPSCs were analyzed using Mini Analysis software (Synaptosoft). Synaptic delay was measured as the time difference between the peak of the presynaptic AP and the onset of the postsynaptic response. Other criteria used for selecting EPSPs were 100 μV for minimal amplitude and 1 mV2 for the minimal area under the EPSP. A duration of 5 ms was used as a baseline, sampled 20 ms before the peak. For EPSCs, the minimal amplitude was 5 pA. Finally, the root mean square of the noise was measured over 0.5 ms at the beginning of each trial, outside of spontaneous or evoked responses, and was used for estimating the parameters of quantal analysis.
Assessment of connectivity
Once a patch clamp was obtained, a 3-ms, 1- to 4-nA square pulse was injected in each neuron to determine the AP firing threshold. Nine suprathreshold pulses were then delivered with eight pulses at 20 Hz, followed by a recovery pulse 550 ms later. This stimulation pattern was delivered successively to each of the patched neurons and repeated 15 times (Fig. 1c). Following this, an average of the responses to each neuronal activation was plotted, and the traces were assessed for time-locked EPSPs occurring within <5 ms with <2.5-ms jitter16,45. The presence of such EPSPs indicated a connection between neurons, which was further subjected to visual inspection that could readily and unequivocally confirm the actual presence of a connection (see examples in Extended Data Fig. 1b). Confirmed connections were then further subjected to quantal analysis or plasticity. LA network connectivity was calculated as the proportion of connections found in a given slice to all possible connections. To calculate the number of possible connections, while excluding autoconnections, we used n(n – 1), with n equal to the number of patched neurons for a given slice.
Quantal analysis experiments
The Ca2+:Mg2+ ratio was modified to isolate the synaptic quantum. Lower Ca2+ concentrations are characterized by a lower probability to observe an EPSC, thereby facilitating the extraction of the quantal size46. The following concentrations were used:
[Ca2+] = 2 mM and [Mg2+] = 1 mM (initial ACSF solution, as described above),
[Ca2+] = 1 mM and [Mg2+] = 2 mM and
[Ca2+] = 0.5 mM and [Mg2+] = 2.5 mM.
EPSCs were measured following the same stimulation pattern (for one trial, 8 + 1 APs delivered at 20 Hz), as described above. At least 30 trials were recorded at 2 mM Ca2+, and at least 100 trials were recorded at lower concentrations, and the first response of each trial was used for extracting the quantal parameters. The intertrial interval was at least 8 s. Stability of the response was tested by comparing the average response value for the first trial with the last ten trials. Connections that had over 30% variability were excluded from the analyses.
To estimate quantal size, we then identified EPSC and EPSP peaks from individual traces and fed these into a simple binomial model based on earlier observations that multiple release sites on cortical synapses share similar release probabilities47,48. Therefore, EPSC and EPSP amplitudes were drawn from a simple binomial distribution with a given number of release sites (n), release probability (p) and quantal size (q)49. The mean and standard deviation of this simple binomial distribution are given by
Extraction of quantal parameters was performed using a custom MATLAB script developed by Hardingham et al. to successfully describe the quantal content of the cortical layer 2/3 synapse using the above simple binomial model48,50. This model also uses the value for the recorded noise to better estimate n, p and q. In addition, the maximum likelihood method51 was used to fit the acquired data (fminsearch function from MATLAB’s Optimization Toolbox), starting with the lowest estimate for n and increasing until a maximum of arbitrarily defined 14 release sites was reached (if the model reached the maximum number of release sites, the resulting fit was discarded). For increased accuracy, the data were fitted against ten different starting points in the parameter space. Finally, the resulting fit was tested against simulated datasets sharing the same parameters using the Monte Carlo simulation and the χ2 test.
The binomial model has the following parameters:
v = amplitude of EPSC or EPSP;
v0 = offset, assumed to be added to all EPSCs or EPSPs;
σnoise = standard deviation of noise, assumed Gaussian;
n = number of release sites;
q = quantal size;
p = release probability at each release site;
pstim = probability that stimulation results in an AP that reaches the release sites (one in our case);
mσq = quantal variance, equals standard deviation on first peak in absence of noise; and
σm = variance affecting the mth peak, where m ranges from 0 to n.
For type I quantal variance, ,
for ‘flat’ quantal variance, σm = σnoise for m = 0, and for m > 0.
The probability density function f(v) for estimating the EPSP or EPSC amplitude (v), as it was used in the original MATLAB script48,50, was the following:
Plasticity protocol
To trigger pre- and postsynaptic APs, a square pulse stimulus was used (1–2 nA, 3 ms) in a train of ten stimuli at 30 Hz repeated 15 times (intertrial interval of 10 s), with the presynaptic potential leading the postsynaptic potential by 5–10 ms (refs. 24,52). For testing whether the long-term potentiation-inducing protocol led to changes in EPSP amplitude, the same protocol was used as for assessing connectivity (eight pulses at 20 Hz, followed by a recovery pulse 550 ms later), which was applied every 30 s for up to 1 h, with potentiation typically lasting 20–30 min, that is, until deterioration of the multiple patched cells.
Surgery
All surgeries were performed under aseptic conditions with isoflurane anesthesia (5% initially and then 2% for maintenance) on a stereotaxic frame (David Kopf Instruments). Animals were kept on a heating pad throughout the duration and recovery from surgery. The animal’s scalp was opened to expose the skull, which was then cleaned with 3% H2O2. For both virus injections and in vivo electrophysiology, bilateral holes were drilled at −3.00 mm (anterior–posterior) and ±5.15 mm (medial–lateral) relative to bregma. If the bregma-to-lambda distance was less than 8.72 mm (reference for adult39), these coordinates were proportionally adjusted.
Viral vector and virus injection and infection
To fluorescently tag recently activated memory-participating neurons in the LA, we expressed dGFP (a fusion of Venus with an mODC PEST sequence with a half-life of 2 h (ref. 53); excitation peak: 515 nm; emission peak: 528 nm) under a modified minimal Arc promoter downstream of a synthetic E-SARE25.
Fluorescent tagging of recruited neurons ex vivo
We used an AAV 2/1 vector to express the E-SARE-driven dGFP. In addition to E-SARE–dGFP, the AAV contained a second cassette that expressed an RFP as an infection marker (RFP635; excitation peak: 588 nm; emission peak: 635 nm) under the constitutive enhanced phosphoglycerate kinase-1 (Pgk1) promoter, with a woodchuck hepatitis post-transcriptional regulatory element54.
Rats were infused bilaterally in the LA with the AAV at 4 weeks of age under isoflurane anesthesia and semisterile conditions. The bregma coordinates used were anterior–posterior −3.00 mm, medial–lateral ±5.15 mm and dorsal–ventral −7.8 mm. The injector consisted of a glass pipette containing 1.2 μl of AAV at a titer of 1.4 × 1013 genomic copies per ml. One microliter was lowered to a depth of 7.8 mm, and virus was infused over 10 min. The pipette was left in place for an additional 3 min to allow viral diffusion55.
Optogenetic tagging of recruited neurons in vivo
For activity-dependent optogenetic tagging following fear memory recall, we used a dual AAV system, in which the expression of double-floxed E-SARE-ChR2, delivered by one AAV, was controlled by tamoxifen-inducible recombinase ERT2CreERT2 delivered by another AAV56. As previously shown for tamoxifen-based gene activation25, to induce ChR2 expression, tamoxifen (10 mg) was administered by gavage 8 h before fear testing, as for mice25. This time window coincides with peak tamoxifen metabolism into 4-hydroxytamoxifen in rats57. Thus, 1 day after CFC, rats were administered tamoxifen and exposed only to the CS+ 8 h later to induce stable expression of ChR2 in recruited neurons following CFC recall. ChR2 expression was assessed by BL responses 1 week after CFC recall.
Microdrive implantation
Microdrives were built in-house and were implanted according to the same coordinates as used for the virus injections. They included eight tetrodes for a total of 32 channels. The tetrodes were assembled from nichrome wire of 25 μm in diameter (STABLOHM 675 California fine wire), which was insulated with heavy Formvar58. Tetrodes were mounted on the microdrive on a copper screw with a 270-μm step. For the optogenetic experiments, an optical fiber was mounted 200 μm away from the tetrode bundle.
To stably anchor the implantation, four fixation points surrounding the microdrive were created to each harbor a small bone screw, two of which were attached to a ground wire. A dental cement layer was used to secure the screws to the skull. The microdrive was lowered to a depth of 6.8 mm. The space between the electrodes and the skull was filled with softened paraffin. An additional layer of dental cement firmly attached the microdrive to the skull. Finally, a copper screen was fitted around the implanted microdrive as a partial Faraday cage to reduce noise during the recordings.
Electrophysiology in vivo
Electrodes were connected to a headstage (Plexon) containing 32 unity-gain operational amplifiers. The headstage was connected to a 32-channel computer-controlled preamplifier (with a gain of 1,000 and bandpass filter from 400 Hz to 7 kHz, Plexon). Neuronal activity was digitized at 40 kHz bandpass filtered from 250 Hz to 8 kHz and isolated by time–amplitude window discrimination and template matching using a multichannel acquisition processor system (Plexon).
During fear conditioning, spike waveforms and associated time stamps were recorded. For analysis, the artifact waveforms were removed, and the spike waveform minima were aligned using Offline Sorter 4.0 software (Plexon). Principal component scores were calculated for unsorted waveforms and plotted on three-dimensional principal component space, and clusters containing similar valid waveforms were manually defined (based on principal component and waveform feature spaces; Extended Data Fig. 10). A group of waveforms was considered to originate from a single neuron if it was defined as a discrete cluster in principal component space that was distinct from clusters for other units and if it displayed a clear refractory period (1.2 ms) in autocorrelograms59,60. Template waveforms were then calculated for well-separated clusters and stored for further analysis in MATLAB and to track neurons over time. To ensure that the same neuron was recorded over multiple sessions (6 h or more), we quantified the squared Mahalanobis distance, discarding neurons with unstable values. For further confirmation, we also measured cluster stability across recording sessions using J3 and Davies–Bouldin statistics59,60.
For each isolated unit and for each experiment, neuronal spikes were plotted as a raster plot of time stamps relative to stimulus exposure (CS+, CS– and BL; t = 0). Spike counts were binned in 50-ms bins (Fig. 6) or 0.5-s bins (Fig. 7) and normalized to a 500-ms (Fig. 6) or 5-s (Fig. 7) baseline average to obtain a z-score. Neurons were considered responsive to a stimulus if the z-score value of their activity crossed the significance level (3 s.d. compared to prestimulus baseline)59. In particular, for optogenetic experiments, neurons were considered BL sensitive if they responded with time-locked (<5-ms jitter) millisecond precision61 (Extended Data Fig. 9f).
Behavior: auditory CFC
To fear condition the rats to a tone (the CS), animals were placed in a fear conditioning box, which included a metal grid for scrambled shock delivery (0.45 mA over 2 s) to the feet and a clear Plexiglass top for camera recording. The delivery of the tone (12 kHz, 250-ms blips presented at 1 Hz over 20 s) and the shock as the unconditioned stimulus (US) were controlled by MedPC IV software. The stimuli (CS and/or US) were presented at random intervals (1–3 min). Auditory fear memory recall was tested in a different context, the testing cage, which was a hexagonal cardboard box with wooden bedding. The conditioning and test boxes were cleaned with a solution containing 70% ethanol after each session.
Animals were randomly assigned to three independent groups: homecage, CS/US paired and CS/US unpaired. The homecage group was not exposed to any aspect of the behavioral experiment (including surgery, habituation and handling). Following a 3-day recovery period from surgery, the rats from the unpaired and paired CS/US groups were handled and habituated to the conditioning cage and the testing cage in 5-min sessions once per day for 3 days. One day after the last habituation session, the animals were placed in the conditioning chamber. After a delay of 5 min, the paired group received three paired co-terminating presentations of the CS–US over 3–9 min. The unpaired control group received three consecutive US presentations, followed by three CS presentations. Twenty-four hours after fear memory acquisition, rats from the paired and unpaired groups received three presentations of the CS in the testing cage and were then returned to their home cages. After a delay of 90 min to achieve optimal dGFP expression, the animals were killed either by decapitation (for use for electrophysiology) or were deeply anesthetized with 4% isoflurane and perfused with phosphate buffer (PB; pH 7.4) and formaldehyde (4% in PB; fixation for confocal microscopy). Animals from the homecage group were killed following the same procedures. For the in vivo optogenetics experiments (Fig. 7b–d), a second tone (CS−; 5 kHz, continuous tone over 20 s) was presented that was never paired to the US, and we infused AAVs to make tamoxifen-inducible expression of ChR possible (see above). One week later, animals were subjected to CFC, and fear memory was recalled 24 h after learning in the presence of tamoxifen. After one additional week to allow ChR to be expressed, fear was recalled again, and neuronal responses to the CS+, CS– and BL were recorded. An additional group of animals (n = 4) was used for visualizing the fluorescent reporter ChR2 by confocal microscopy (Extended Data Fig. 8c). For the short-term memory in vivo experiments (Fig. 6), CS and US association and subsequent CS memory testing were separated by a 6-h interval21. We chose this shorter time interval to maximize the probability of recording from the same neurons reliably at the beginning and end of the experiment.
A FT200EMT optical fiber (Thorlabs) was mounted 200 μm adjacent to the tetrode bundle, with a laser (Dream Lasers) ensuring BL delivery at 473 nm, with a power at source of 50 mW and ~15 mW at the tip of the optical fiber. BL was delivered with a pulse width of 2 ms, either as a single pulse or at 20-Hz frequency trains.
Rats were considered as freezing to a stimulus (CS or BL) if no movement was detected for at least 2 s; the sum of the freezing bouts was then expressed as a percentage of the stimulus presentation. Freezing was assessed by an experimenter blind to the experimental conditions.
Immunohistochemistry and confocal image acquisition
For confocal microscopy experiments, following perfusion, brains were extracted and postfixed for 2 days in 4% formaldehyde and cryoprotected in 30% sucrose for an addition 2 days. Horizontal sections (50 µm thick) were cut on a MICROM HM 440E microtome (GMI).
To visualize inhibitory neurons, nonspecific binding sites on free-floating sections were blocked with 2% normal horse serum (Jackson Immuno Research Laboratories) in 0.1 M PB (pH 7.4) supplemented with 0.3% Triton X-100 (Sigma-Aldrich) and sodium azide (1 g l–1; Sigma-Aldrich) for 1 h at room temperature. Sections were then incubated with mouse primary anti-GAD67 (1:2,500; MAB5406, Merck Millipore) in blocking buffer for 48 h at 4 °C. To visualize antibody–antigen complexes, an AlexaFluor 405-conjugated goat anti-mouse antibody (1:300; A31553, Life Technologies) was applied in PB (with 0.3% Triton X-100 and sodium azide; Sigma-Aldrich) for 1 h at room temperature. Sections were then mounted with Vectashield (Vectorlabs) and stored at 4 °C until assessment by microscopy.
Fluorescence of both dGFP and mCherry following viral expression was sufficiently strong to be visualized directly.
Images were acquired using a Zeiss LSM 780 Quasar Confocal Microscope (Zeiss). Three lasers were used to excite at 405, 488 and 561 nm (diode, argon and diode-pumped solid-state lasers, respectively) at 2–3% power. Zen 2012 software was used to control the acquisition parameters of the LSM 780. Constant parameters included a pixel depth at 16 bit, filtering the average of two values and scanning unidirectionally and in ‘Line’ mode.
An ImageJ script was used to automatically detect fluorescence thresholds and count cell bodies. Cutoff parameters were used to minimize the inclusion of false positives. Exclusion criteria included cell size (minimal cutoff of 45 μm2 neuron body area) and fluorescence intensity (minimal cutoff of 15,000 average pixel value for a given channel out of a maximal 2 (ref. 16) or 65,536 for saturated pixels).
Statistics
All statistical analyses were conducted with GraphPad Prism 9 and R4.2 (ref. 62). Sample sizes were determined online based on mean difference and standard deviation (http://www.biomath.info; Center for Biomathematics, Department of Pediatrics at Columbia University Medical Center). When comparing populations, data were first tested for normality with either the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test or, when the sample data size was less than 50, with the Shapiro–Wilk test and for homogeneity of variance using Bartlett’s test. If the data did not deviate significantly from normal distribution and the variances were homogenous, independent samples t-tests, paired t-tests or ANOVAs were used. Results that were found to deviate significantly from the normal distributions and/or whose variance was not homogenous were analyzed with appropriate nonparametric tests (see below for a list of tests used). When multiple comparisons occurred, the tests were Bonferroni corrected.
When the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test was used to compare differences between two distributions, the cumulative frequency of each distribution was normalized to its maximal value.
For the RM-ANOVA, data were tested for sphericity using the Mauchly test, and to correct for departures from sphericity, the Greenhouse–Geisser and Huynh–Feldt corrections were applied63–65. Where applicable, statistical tests were two tailed. For cross-correlations, the corrplot function in MATLAB (MathWorks, 9.6) was used, with a Pearson test for linear correlations and a Spearman test for nonlinear correlations (resulting in their respective correlation coefficients, r and ρ).
The frequency of observed connectivity motifs was compared to their respective frequency in a simulated network (100,000 Monte Carlo simulations taking into account real cell positions and random distance-dependent connectivity based on Fig. 3a; see a detailed explanation in Supplementary Note 3).
The following statistical tests and variables (degrees of freedom are shown as underscore numbers) were used:
Student’s t-test uses the t variable.
ANOVA uses the F variable.
Wilcoxon signed-rank test uses the W variable.
Mann–Whitney U-test uses the U variable.
Kolmogorov–Smirnov test uses the D variable.
Monte Carlo simulation for network connectivity: n = 100,000 simulations.
For neuronal activity represented as z-scores, when assessing stimulus response, a poststimulus response was considered significant when its value was greater than 3 s.d. of the prestimulus baseline (see above).
Granger causality analysis
To determine connectivity (positive relationship with P < 0.001) and connection strength (Granger causality statistic) between two recorded neurons in vivo, we used Weiner–Granger vector autoregressive causality analysis as implemented in the multivariate Granger causality toolbox28,29. Time stamps recorded from each neuron were converted into a continuous signal by binning in 1-ms increments and convolving the resulting signal with a half-Gaussian filter (5-ms width). This analysis was performed on spike train data gathered over a period of 20 min outside of stimulus exposure. Stationarity was checked and confirmed for all models by determining whether the spectral radius of the estimated full model was less than 1 (ref. 29). Model order was derived by using Bayesian information criteria, and the vector autoregressive model parameters were determined accordingly. Subsequently, time domain-conditional Granger causality values were calculated for each neuron pair. Causal density was taken as the mean pairwise-conditional causality and was subsequently normalized to the entire dataset29. Because the current model assessed Granger causality between presynaptic and postsynaptic spikes, it could only infer excitatory but not inhibitory connections (Granger causality of presynaptic spike to postsynaptic silence).
Granger causality analysis was always performed on baseline data recorded from the animal at rest (either before or after fear conditioning) to prevent false-positive connections resulting from stimulus-evoked activity. Furthermore, we did not estimate connectivity (only connection strength) after fear conditioning because CFC increased the number of positive connections (P < 0.001). However, CFC did not affect connection strength (Granger causality statistics), as assessed in Fig. 6c where connection strength increased only in the recruited connections but not in the nonrecruited connections. Whether neurons were recruited or not was assessed after fear conditioning.
Reporting summary
Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.
Online content
Any methods, additional references, Nature Portfolio reporting summaries, source data, extended data, supplementary information, acknowledgements, peer review information; details of author contributions and competing interests; and statements of data and code availability are available at 10.1038/s41593-024-01676-6.
Supplementary information
Acknowledgements
We thank the Centre d’études du comportement and in particular its staff (B. Boury-Jamot and F. Magara) for animal housing and assistance with behavioral procedures and the Cellular Imaging Facility (University of Lausanne and University-Hospital of Vaud) for its centralized access to confocal microscopy. We further thank G. Giobellina for assistance with virus injections and R. Setareh for statistical evaluation of our data. This work was funded by KAKENHI (17H06312 to H.B., R.K. and M.O.; 17H14930 to H.B., R.K. and M.O.), JSPS, Brain/MINDS and AMED grants (to H.B.), the Marie–Heim Vögtlin Foundation (to C.H.), the Swiss National Science Foundation (31003A_138526 to R.S.; 310030_192463 to R.S.; IZLIZ3_200297 to R.S.; IZLCZ0_206045 to R.S.; IZLSZ3_148803 to R.S.) and the Synapsis Foundation (2020-PI02 to E.v.d.B. and R.S.). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish or preparation of the paper.
Extended data
Author contributions
Conceptualization: R.S. Methodology: C.H., R.K. and M.O. Investigation: M.A., R.P. and R.N. Visualization: M.A., R.N. and R.S. Funding acquisition: H.B. and R.S. Project administration: R.S. Supervision: H.B. and R.S. Writing the original draft: M.A., H.B., E.v.d.B., H.M. and R.S. Writing, reviewing and editing: M.A., E.v.d.B. and R.S.
Peer review
Peer review information
Nature Neuroscience thanks the anonymous reviewers for their contribution to the peer review of this work.
Data availability
All data are available in the main text or the supplementary information available at Zenodo at 10.5281/zenodo.10890959 (ref. 66).
Code availability
No custom software code was used to analyze the data.
Competing interests
The authors declare no competing interests.
Footnotes
Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
Extended data
is available for this paper at 10.1038/s41593-024-01676-6.
Supplementary information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41593-024-01676-6.
References
- 1.Rogan MT, Stäubli UV, LeDoux JE. Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Nature. 1997;390:604–607. doi: 10.1038/37601. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.McKernan MG, Shinnick-Gallagher P. Fear conditioning induces a lasting potentiation of synaptic currents in vitro. Nature. 1997;390:607–611. doi: 10.1038/37605. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Cho J-H, et al. Coactivation of thalamic and cortical pathways induces input timing-dependent plasticity in amygdala. Nat. Neurosci. 2012;15:113–122. doi: 10.1038/nn.2993. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Han J-H, et al. Neuronal competition and selection during memory formation. Science. 2007;316:457–460. doi: 10.1126/science.1139438. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Han J-H, et al. Selective erasure of a fear memory. Science. 2009;323:1492–1496. doi: 10.1126/science.1164139. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Yiu AP, et al. Neurons are recruited to a memory trace based on relative neuronal excitability immediately before training. Neuron. 2014;83:722–735. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.07.017. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Lisman J, Cooper K, Sehgal M, Silva AJ. Memory formation depends on both synapse-specific modifications of synaptic strength and cell-specific increases in excitability. Nat. Neurosci. 2018;21:309–314. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0076-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Ryan TJ, Roy DS, Pignatelli M, Arons A, Tonegawa S. Engram cells retain memory under retrograde amnesia. Science. 2015;348:1007–1013. doi: 10.1126/science.aaa5542. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Carrillo-Reid L, Yuste R. Playing the piano with the cortex: role of neuronal ensembles and pattern completion in perception and behavior. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2020;64:89–95. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2020.03.014. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Guzman SJ, Schlögl A, Frotscher M, Jonas P. Synaptic mechanisms of pattern completion in the hippocampal CA3 network. Science. 2016;353:1117–1123. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf1836. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Mishra RK, Kim S, Guzman SJ, Jonas P. Symmetric spike timing-dependent plasticity at CA3–CA3 synapses optimizes storage and recall in autoassociative networks. Nat. Commun. 2016;7:11552. doi: 10.1038/ncomms11552. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Yu Y-C, Bultje RS, Wang X, Shi S-H. Specific synapses develop preferentially among sister excitatory neurons in the neocortex. Nature. 2009;458:501–504. doi: 10.1038/nature07722. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Li Y, et al. Clonally related visual cortical neurons show similar stimulus feature selectivity. Nature. 2012;486:118–121. doi: 10.1038/nature11110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Ko H, et al. Functional specificity of local synaptic connections in neocortical networks. Nature. 2011;473:87–91. doi: 10.1038/nature09880. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Franks KM, et al. Recurrent circuitry dynamically shapes the activation of piriform cortex. Neuron. 2011;72:49–56. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2011.08.020. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Perin R, Berger TK, Markram H. A synaptic organizing principle for cortical neuronal groups. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2011;108:5419–5424. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1016051108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Hardingham NR, et al. Quantal analysis reveals a functional correlation between presynaptic and postsynaptic efficacy in excitatory connections from rat neocortex. J. Neurosci. 2010;30:1441–1451. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3244-09.2010. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Song S, Sjöström PJ, Reigl M, Nelson S, Chklovskii DB. Highly nonrandom features of synaptic connectivity in local cortical circuits. PLoS Biol. 2005;3:e68. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.0030068. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Stoop R, Pralong E. Functional connections and epileptic spread between hippocampus, entorhinal cortex and amygdala in a modified horizontal slice preparation of the rat brain. Eur. J. Neurosci. 2000;12:3651–3663. doi: 10.1046/j.1460-9568.2000.00253.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Stoop R, Conquet F, Zuber B, Voronin LL, Pralong E. Activation of metabotropic glutamate 5 and NMDA receptors underlies the induction of persistent bursting and associated long-lasting changes in CA3 recurrent connections. J. Neurosci. 2003;23:5634–5644. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.23-13-05634.2003. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Quirk GJ, Repa C, LeDoux JE. Fear conditioning enhances short-latency auditory responses of lateral amygdala neurons: parallel recordings in the freely behaving rat. Neuron. 1995;15:1029–1039. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90092-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Pape H-C, Narayanan RT, Smid J, Stork O, Seidenbecher T. Theta activity in neurons and networks of the amygdala related to long-term fear memory. Hippocampus. 2005;15:874–880. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20120. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Weisskopf MG, Bauer EP, LeDoux JE. L-Type voltage-gated calcium channels mediate NMDA-independent associative long-term potentiation at thalamic input synapses to the amygdala. J. Neurosci. 1999;19:10512–10519. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-23-10512.1999. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Markram H, Tsodyks M. Redistribution of synaptic efficacy between neocortical pyramidal neurons. Nature. 1996;382:807–810. doi: 10.1038/382807a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Kawashima T, et al. Functional labeling of neurons and their projections using the synthetic activity-dependent promoter E-SARE. Nat. Methods. 2013;10:889–895. doi: 10.1038/nmeth.2559. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Zhou Y, et al. CREB regulates excitability and the allocation of memory to subsets of neurons in the amygdala. Nat. Neurosci. 2009;12:1438–1443. doi: 10.1038/nn.2405. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Rao-Ruiz P, Yu J, Kushner SA, Josselyn SA. Neuronal competition: microcircuit mechanisms define the sparsity of the engram. Curr. Opin. Neurobiol. 2019;54:163–170. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2018.10.013. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Schmitt, L. I. et al. Thalamic amplification of cortical connectivity sustains attentional control. Nature545, 219–223 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 29.Barnett L, Seth AK. The MVGC multivariate Granger causality toolbox: a new approach to Granger-causal inference. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2014;223:50–68. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Carrillo-Reid L, Yang W, Bando Y, Peterka DS, Yuste R. Imprinting and recalling cortical ensembles. Science. 2016;353:691–694. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf7560. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Marshel JH, et al. Cortical layer-specific critical dynamics triggering perception. Science. 2019;365:eaaw5202. doi: 10.1126/science.aaw5202. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Gründemann J, et al. Amygdala ensembles encode behavioral states. Science. 2019;364:eaav8736. doi: 10.1126/science.aav8736. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Fustiñana MS, Eichlisberger T, Bouwmeester T, Bitterman Y, Lüthi A. State-dependent encoding of exploratory behaviour in the amygdala. Nature. 2021;592:267–271. doi: 10.1038/s41586-021-03301-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Yokose J, et al. Overlapping memory trace indispensable for linking, but not recalling, individual memories. Science. 2017;355:398–403. doi: 10.1126/science.aal2690. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Rashid AJ, et al. Competition between engrams influences fear memory formation and recall. Science. 2016;353:383–387. doi: 10.1126/science.aaf0594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hopfield JJ. Neural networks and physical systems with emergent collective computational abilities. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 1982;79:2554–2558. doi: 10.1073/pnas.79.8.2554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Johnson, L. R. A recurrent network in the lateral amygdala: a mechanism for coincidence detection. Front. Neural Circuits2, 3 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 38.Reitich-Stolero T, Paz R. Affective memory rehearsal with temporal sequences in amygdala neurons. Nat. Neurosci. 2019;22:2050–2059. doi: 10.1038/s41593-019-0542-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Paxinos, G. & Watson, C. The Rat Brain in Stereotaxic Coordinates (Academic Press, 1998).
- 40.Perin, R. & Markram, H. A computer-assisted multi-electrode patch-clamp system. J. Vis. Exp.2013, e50630 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 41.Faber ES, Callister RJ, Sah P. Morphological and electrophysiological properties of principal neurons in the rat lateral amygdala in vitro. J. Neurophysiol. 2001;85:714–723. doi: 10.1152/jn.2001.85.2.714. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Duvarci S, Pare D. Amygdala microcircuits controlling learned fear. Neuron. 2014;82:966–980. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.04.042. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Larkman A, Stratford K, Jack J. Quantal analysis of excitatory synaptic action and depression in hippocampal slices. Nature. 1991;350:344–347. doi: 10.1038/350344a0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.O’Connor DH, Wittenberg GM, Wang SS-H. Timing and contributions of pre-synaptic and post-synaptic parameter changes during unitary plasticity events at CA3–CA1 synapses. Synapse. 2007;61:664–678. doi: 10.1002/syn.20403. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Petreanu L, Huber D, Sobczyk A, Svoboda K. Channelrhodopsin-2-assisted circuit mapping of long-range callosal projections. Nat. Neurosci. 2007;10:663–668. doi: 10.1038/nn1891. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Kraushaar U, Jonas P. Efficacy and stability of quantal GABA release at a hippocampal interneuron-principal neuron synapse. J. Neurosci. 2000;20:5594–5607. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.20-15-05594.2000. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Koester HJ, Johnston D. Target cell-dependent normalization of transmitter release at neocortical synapses. Science. 2005;308:863–866. doi: 10.1126/science.1100815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Hardingham NR, et al. Extracellular calcium regulates postsynaptic efficacy through group 1 metabotropic glutamate receptors. J. Neurosci. 2006;26:6337–6345. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5128-05.2006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Larkman AU, Jack JJ, Stratford KJ. Quantal analysis of excitatory synapses in rat hippocampal CA1 in vitro during low-frequency depression. J. Physiol. 1997;505:457–471. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1997.457bb.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Quantalanalysis2 (Jenny Read, 2018).
- 51.Press, W. et al. Numerical recipes: the art of scientific computing. Technometrics29, 501–502 (1987).
- 52.Weisskopf MG, LeDoux JE. Distinct populations of NMDA receptors at subcortical and cortical inputs to principal cells of the lateral amygdala. J. Neurophysiol. 1999;81:930–934. doi: 10.1152/jn.1999.81.2.930. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Li X, et al. Generation of destabilized green fluorescent protein as a transcription reporter. J. Biol. Chem. 1998;273:34970–34975. doi: 10.1074/jbc.273.52.34970. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Klein R, et al. WPRE-mediated enhancement of gene expression is promoter and cell line specific. Gene. 2006;372:153–161. doi: 10.1016/j.gene.2005.12.018. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Cetin A, Komai S, Eliava M, Seeburg PH, Osten P. Stereotaxic gene delivery in the rodent brain. Nat. Protoc. 2006;1:3166–3173. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2006.450. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Matsuda T, Cepko CL. Controlled expression of transgenes introduced by in vivo electroporation. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2007;104:1027–1032. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0610155104. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Buchanan CM, et al. Pharmacokinetics of tamoxifen after intravenous and oral dosing of tamoxifen–hydroxybutenyl-β-cyclodextrin formulations. J. Pharm. Sci. 2007;96:644–660. doi: 10.1002/jps.20709. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Lin L, et al. Large-scale neural ensemble recording in the brains of freely behaving mice. J. Neurosci. Methods. 2006;155:28–38. doi: 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2005.12.032. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Ghosh S, Chattarji S. Neuronal encoding of the switch from specific to generalized fear. Nat. Neurosci. 2015;18:112–120. doi: 10.1038/nn.3888. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Nicolelis MAL, et al. Chronic, multisite, multielectrode recordings in macaque monkeys. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA. 2003;100:11041–11046. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1934665100. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Cardin JA, et al. Targeted optogenetic stimulation and recording of neurons in vivo using cell-type-specific expression of channelrhodopsin-2. Nat. Protoc. 2010;5:247–254. doi: 10.1038/nprot.2009.228. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing.http://www.R-project.org/R (Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2018).
- 63.Huynh H, Feldt LS. Estimation of the Box correction for degrees of freedom from sample data in randomized block and split-plot designs. J. Educ. Stat. 1976;1:69. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Geisser S, Greenhouse SW. An extension of Box’s results on the use of the F distribution in multivariate analysis. Ann. Math. Stat. 1958;29:885–891. [Google Scholar]
- 65.Greenhouse S, Geisser S. On methods in the analysis of profile data. Psychometrika. 1959;24:95–112. [Google Scholar]
- 66.Abatis, M., et al. Fear learning induces synaptic potentiation between engram neurons in the rat lateral amygdala. Zenodo10.5281/zenodo.10890959 (2024). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All data are available in the main text or the supplementary information available at Zenodo at 10.5281/zenodo.10890959 (ref. 66).
No custom software code was used to analyze the data.