Skip to main content
The BMJ logoLink to The BMJ
. 2002 Nov 2;325(7371):1038. doi: 10.1136/bmj.325.7371.1038/a

Supportive evidence is lacking for report on animal studies

Pandora Pound 1, Shah Ebrahim 1
PMCID: PMC1124512  PMID: 12411380

Editor—As Dobson notes, the House of Lords Select Committee on Animals in Scientific Procedures concluded that animal experiments were necessary but that more needs to be done to develop and promote alternative methods.1

Although it is clear that the committee sought the views and opinions of a wide range of experts, we were struck throughout by the lack of published, peer reviewed evidence to support one of its important conclusions: “On balance, we are convinced that experiments on animals have contributed greatly to scientific advances, both for human medicine and for animal health. Animal experimentation is a valuable research method which has proved itself over time” (page 22, para 4.8).2

We are not suggesting that the Lords did not seek out such evidence (it is clear from the transcripts published on the internet that on many occasions they asked witnesses to supply them with peer reviewed references and reviews to support their claims about the efficacy of animal experiments).3 Rather, we wish to draw attention to the poverty and paucity of this evidence. Hardly any systematic reviews, meta-analyses, or retrospective, historical evaluations either support or refute the practice of using animals as models of human disease. The Lords′ assertion of the value of animal experimentation rests on the increase in effective human treatments that have arisen at the same time as the expansion of animal experimentation. This correlation does not mean that animals were necessary for the development of these treatments.

The move in medicine to become more evidence based needs to be replicated in research. If uncertainty persists about a particular paradigm or method—in this case the efficacy of using animals as models of human disease—evidence needs to be gathered so that claims about its efficacy can be supported or refuted. If no evidence supports the use of a particular method and only custom and practice sustain it, then that method should be discarded. Currently animal tests are used as the gold standard by which so called alternatives are judged, yet virtually no evidence supports the use of the animal tests themselves. In the few cases where systematic reviews of animal experiments have been conducted, serious doubts have been raised about the methods used.4

Evaluating the practice of using animals as models of human disease is fairly straightforward and practicable when established animal models of diseases exist.5 The models should be evaluated retrospectively, the key criterion being the productivity of the animal model in terms of producing treatments for humans.

References

  • 1.Dobson R. Lords support animal experiments but call for alternatives. BMJ. 2002;325:238. doi: 10.1136/bmj.325.7358.238. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.House of Lords. Select Committee on Animals in Scientific Procedures. 16 July 2002. House of Lords. London: Stationery Office; 2002. [Google Scholar]
  • 3. House of Lords. Animals in scientific procedures. Available at: www.publications.parliament.uk/pa/ld/ldanimal.htm (accessed 9 October 2002).
  • 4.Horn J, De Haan RJ, Vermeulen M, Luiten PG, Limburg M. Nimodipine in animal model experiments of focal cerebral ischemia: a systematic review. Stroke. 2001;32:2433–2438. doi: 10.1161/hs1001.096009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Shapiro KJ. Animal models of human psychology. Seattle: Hogrefe and Huber; 1998. [Google Scholar]

Articles from BMJ : British Medical Journal are provided here courtesy of BMJ Publishing Group

RESOURCES