Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2025 Jul 1.
Published in final edited form as: Epilepsia. 2024 May 22;65(7):e104–e112. doi: 10.1111/epi.18019

How accurate do self-reported seizures need to be for effective medication management in epilepsy?

Daniel Goldenholz 1,2, Benjamin H Brinkmann 3,*, M Brandon Westover 1,2,4,5,*
PMCID: PMC11251847  NIHMSID: NIHMS1992859  PMID: 38776216

Abstract

Studies suggest that self-reported seizure diaries suffer from 50% under-reporting on average. It is unknown to what extent this impacts medication management. This study used simulation to predict the seizure outcomes of a large heterogeneous clinic population treated with a standardized algorithm based on self-reported seizures. Using CHOCOLATES, a state-of-the-art realistic seizure diary simulator, 100,000 patients were simulated over 10 years. A standard algorithm for medication management was employed at 3-month intervals for all patients. The impact on true seizure rates, expected seizure rates and time-to-steady-dose were computed for self-reporting sensitivities 0-100%. Time-to-steady-dose and medication usage mostly did not depend on sensitivity. True seizure rate decreased minimally with increasing self-reporting in a non-linear fashion, with the largest decreases at low sensitivity rates (0-10%). This study suggests that an extremely wide range of sensitivity will have similar seizure outcomes when clinically treated using an algorithm similar to the one presented. Conversely, patients with sensitivity less than or equal to 10% would be expected to benefit (via lower seizure rates) from objective devices that provide even small improvements in seizure sensitivity.

Keywords: seizures, patient reported outcomes, simulation, clinical

BACKGROUND:

The most readily available metric for degree of illness in epilepsy is self-reported seizures (SRS)1. One common use of SRS is clinical treatment with anti-seizure medications (ASMs). Investigators and clinicians worry that SRS has reliability concerns because of poor correlation to intracranial recordings2,3 or to scalp EEG recordings4,5. Some have suggested that objective metrics should replace SRS, such as subscalp EEG6 or wearable devices7. Nevertheless, any seizure detector has an imperfect sensitivity and a nonzero false alarm rate. Thus, the question arises: when would objective tools be better for ASM management, and when is SRS sufficient?

One approach to understand this situation is to use the framework of signal-to-noise ratio (SNR) used by engineers to quantify the utility of a measurement. In the present context, we will use sensitivity and false alarms from SRS to define SNR:

Sensitivity(%)=reportedtrueseizurestotaltrueseizures Eq. 1:
Falsealarmrate=reportednonseizureevents1month Eq. 2:
SNR=sensitivityfalsealarmrate Eq. 3:

SNR can be thought of as overall SRS accuracy. Reports vary widely about sensitivity 2,4,5, likely because individuals vary widely, estimates as low as 13% and as high as 71% have been found in different subgroups, but a review across multiple studies and modalities suggested the typical sensitivity may be <50%4. As for false alarm rate, this quantity is unknown and challenging to study in the absence of a comprehensive gold standard (see Appendix). Here too, wide estimates are possible8,9, though without proper controls they are difficult to interpret.

With the availability of a statistically realistic seizure diary simulator10, it is possible to produce a simulation that would address the utility question under various possible scenarios. Our objective was to investigate the impact of different self-report accuracy (SNR) values for SRS on seizure outcomes when treating patients in clinic.

METHODS

The simulations were based on CHOCOLATES10, open-source software for generating seizure diaries that have characteristics observed in clinical studies. These features are: (1) heterogeneity in average seizure rates across subjects11, (2) a consistent relationship between average and standard deviation in seizure rate across patients (the “L-relationship”)12, (3) multiple coexisting seizure risk cycles13,14, (4) seizure clustering features15,16, and (5) limitations on minimum inter-seizure intervals11,17,18. The output of CHOCOLATES is a series of seizure counts, representing the number of seizures the synthetic patient reports within a user-specified time window (such as daily, or hourly). CHOCOLATES reproduces typical SRS diaries from a heterogeneous community of patients. Figure 1 illustrates the pipeline used to generate both ground truth clinical seizure diaries as well as self-reported diaries derived from these ground truth diaries. First, a realistic self-reported seizure frequency is chosen with the help of CHOCOLATES. This frequency is then modified to account for the self-report accuracy rate (increased due to sensitivity and decreased due to false alarm rate). This was input to CHOCOLATES to produce the ground truth clinical diary at the modified typical frequency. The ground truth diary was stochastically modified to account for the self-report accuracy (SNR) -seizures deleted based on sensitivity and added based on false alarm rate-, producing the self-reported diary. In this way, two parallel diaries were generated, one which represents ground truth clinical seizures, and the second representing SRS.

FIGURE 1:

FIGURE 1:

Flow diagram for simulation. The upper flow diagram shows how selecting a frequency and adjusting it based on self-report accuracy (signal to noise ratio (SNR)) allows the seizure diary simulator CHOCOLATES and medication effects to produce a “true diary”. That diary is then adjusted to account for the SNR (including under-reporting and over-reporting) to form a self-reported diary. Medications are adjusted based only on the self-reported diary data using the flow diagram on the lower half of the figure (gray box). The seizure rate from the previous 3-month clinic visit is compared to the current rate in the most recent 3 months. If the new rate is 50% reduced or more, then meds are not changed. Otherwise, meds are increased with a certain probability. If meds are unchanged for 2 years, they can be reduced.

Clinic visits were assumed to occur every 3 months for a total of 10 years in a set of 100,000 virtual patients. A standard clinical decision rule (Figure 1 in gray box) was applied at each visit as follows:

Let the current and prior 3-month seizure count be CCURRENT and CPRIOR and let probability of ASM increase be Pincrease:

  1. If CCURRENT > (½) CPRIOR then increase ASM dose, probability Pincrease.

  2. If CCURRENT = 0 or CCURRENT <= (½) CPRIOR then no change.

  3. If no change has been true for 2 years, then decrease ASM dose.

For rule #1, ASM adjustments were in half dose increments. The rule used (½) CPRIOR because often clinicians consider a 50% reduction a significant change19. Pincrease=0.3, consistent with clinical data20. A full dose of medicine was assumed to decrease 20% of true clinical seizures on average, and a half dose was assumed to decrease 10%. These values align with meta-analysis21. If an ASM was set to full dose and ASM increase was needed, a new drug at half dose was added to the existing regimen. Similarly, if a patient had a half dose of a medication and a reduction was needed, the final ASM was removed. Thus, patients taking more than 1 ASM would take full strength of all but the final ASM, which would either be full or half dose. ASM count was limited between 1 and 6 after any med was started.

What is the chance an added ASM results in seizure freedom? This has been previously reported20 in a large cohort. What follows is the probability of seizure freedom with the addition of that 1rst…,6th ASM, given that the previous medicines failed to produce seizure freedom: 46%,28%,24%,15%,14%,14%. Each patient’s form of seizure-freedom is also subject to a probabilistic pattern22 reported in a large cohort of outpatients: (1) lasting early (37%), (2) delayed last (22%), (3) fluctuating 1-year seizure freedom (16%), (4) no extended seizure freedom (25%). Both probability of seizure-freedom and pattern were built into our medication change model, so that if any new ASM was added, there was a realistic chance of achieving various types of seizure freedom.

To account for different self-report accuracy (SNR) values, we set the false alarm rate to be fixed 1 seizure per month and varied the sensitivity values between 10%… 100%. In this way, SNR (expressed in terms of monthly false alarms) can be numerically equivalent to sensitivity. The number of months until a stable dose of ASM was computed by taking the median number of ASMs during the final one third of the 10 years (at the individual patient level) and determining how long until that individual started taking that number of ASMs. Additional values of false alarm rate were also explored (Appendix).

DATA AVAILABILITY

All data used in this study was synthetically generated using the open-source code.

CODE AVAILABILITY

Github code can be accessed here: https://github.com/GoldenholzLab/WEARsimulator/.

RESULTS

The median number of ASMs needed per patient ranged between 1.5 and 2.1 across all values of self-report accuracy except SNR=0 (0 meds). Median seizure rate ranged 2.3 to 1.9 for all self-report accuracy values (at SNR=0, rate=6.5). The duration until stable ASM dose was 20-24 months for all SNR other than SNR=0, which was 0 months. Results are summarized in Figure 2. Additional simulations for other values of false alarm rates between 2 per day down to 1 per year are included in the Appendix, but the results are approximately the same throughout.

Figure 2:

Figure 2:

Result of simulating 100,000 patients for 10 years. Upper left – the daily number of medications are shown as a function of sensitivity (SNR). A red curve is plotted through the median values. Upper right – the average seizure rate per patient as a function of SNR. A red curve is plotted through the median values. Lower left – the months until stable medication dose versus SNR is shown, with a red curve through the median values. There is no dramatic difference for any SNR value. Lower right – the median values from the average seizure rate per patient is plotted (x axis) vs. daily number of medications. These figures indicate that months until stable dose, and typical number of medications does not depend on SNR. Additionally, with the exception of the lowest SNR values, the typical seizure rate decreased only modestly in response to higher SNR values.

DISCUSSION

This study evaluated the impact of different values of self-report accuracy (SNR) values from SRS diaries on simulated patients in clinic. It was shown that virtually any SNR value resulted in similar distribution of patients with few or many ASMs, and similarly the time to achieve a stable dose was roughly 2 years for the typical patient. Conversely, the typical number of seizures seen with different SNR values changed dramatically only for the lowest SNR values. These findings suggest that even if self-reported sensitivity is as low as 10%, a more objective detection device would be unlikely to have a dramatic impact on a large clinic population. Only patients with extremely poor SNR (less than 10% or worse) would be expected to see a significant seizure burden reduction with a high sensitivity wearable. Importantly, the simulator (CHOCOLATES) 10 accounts for a heterogeneous seizure frequency, so patients of low rates, high rates and everything in between were included, at the rates previously found in outpatients11.

Does this mean that clinicians should “trust” self-reported data? We do not think this question is the correct approach. Rather, we ask: “in the setting of outpatient ASM management, how much confidence can be assigned to SRS?” For most values of self-report accuracy, high confidence can be assigned to SRS when managing ASMs. In other contexts, SRS with low or moderate SNR would pose unacceptable risk, (e.g. SUDEP and injury prevention).

It is worthwhile considering the observation that patients may under-report more at night than during the day4,23,24. Our present study did not account for day or night, rather simply accounting for total daily counts of seizures. There are several cases to consider: patient A, who only has daytime seizures, patient B, who has only night time seizures, and patient C, who has both day and night events. Statistically, one would assume patient A would have higher SNR than patient B, and one could use the results shown here to derive expectations given their SNR value. In the case of patient C, things are more complex, because the overall SNR reflects a weighted SNR from the daytime and nighttime. From the perspective of treatment in clinic, knowing this overall SNR ultimately is the only value needed, so such details are not required. Conversely, if one wanted to target therapy for a specific time period, (e.g. nighttime), would could consider the SNR of each time period independently. This might mean that nocturnal devices would be recommended to boost the night-time SNR for a patient that had very low nocturnal SNR.

The results here suggest that exact numbers of seizures are not necessary for adequate clinical treatment. Nevertheless, some patients or caregivers would feel greater comfort with apparent increased certainty afforded by objective device monitoring. Due to the many psychological factors involved here, we recommend an individualized approach to patients and caregivers, accounting for perceptions of certainty, false alarms, and general anxiety levels.

These results must be taken in context. The treatment model presented here represents a realistic guess at one possible treatment strategy in a space of nearly infinite options available to clinicians. CHOCOLATES was developed to account for many known statistical features of self-reported seizure diaries10. That tool represents the current state-of-the-art in seizure-diary simulation, yet many details remain unknown. For example, does the tool equally represent all subtypes of epilepsy25 and all subtypes seizures26? The data used to accommodate the possibility of seizure-freedom was derived from the experience of one center in Scotland20 with 1,795 patients. The data to understand typical medication efficacy comes from meta-analysis of 63 RCTs27 from 11 ASMs. Although a wide variety of data collection techniques and methodologies were used to accumulate the constraints needed to build both CHOCOLATES and the present simulator, could these be insufficient for a more nuanced specific situation or specific treatment style? These questions are currently open for further study. Additionally, assumptions were made for the method to approximate a “true” seizure rate (Figure 1), which cannot be truly known with present technology and therefore represent a best guess. Similarly, seizure-reporting accuracy was by necessity assumed to be constant over time on average but may not be true in many real-world situations. Moreover, the seizure-freedom of patients in clinic was simulated to reflect the rates seen in a large-scale single-center study of 1795 patients over a 30-year period20. When larger multi-center datasets become available, the assumption about seizure freedom may require adjustment. It is therefore fortunate that when new data becomes available, the open-source code can be modified by the research community.

In conclusion, the present study finds that SRS may be sufficient in many cases for clinical ASM management. A surprisingly wide range of SNR values and a standard treatment algorithm result in similar clinical outcomes. Conversely, patients with extremely low reporting SNR tend to suffer dramatically more seizures than those with even modest SNR. Such patients would be expected to clinically benefit from objective seizure detections devices due to the higher SNR.

Acknowledgements

DMG was supported by NIH K23NS124656. MBW was supported by grants from the NIH (R01NS102190, R01NS102574, R01NS107291, RF1AG064312, RF1NS120947, R01AG073410, R01HL161253, R01NS126282, R01AG073598) and NSF (2014431). BHB was supported by the Epilepsy Foundation of America’s My Seizure Gauge grant, and NIH UG3NS123066

Disclosure of Potential Conflicts of interest:

DMG is an unpaid advisor for Epilepsy AI and Eysz. He has been a paid advisor for Magic Leap. He has been provided speaker fees from AAN, AES, ACNS and AI in Epilepsy and Neurology. He also previously has been a paid consultant for Neuro Event Labs, IDR, LivaNova and Health Advances. He has received grants from NIH and BIDMC. None of the above relationships pose a financial conflict of interest.

B.H.B. has received research support from UNEEG Medical and Seer Medical, and has received device do- nations for research from Medtronic. He has licensed intellectual property to Cadence Neurosciences, and has consulted for Otsuka Pharmaceuticals. None of the above relationships pose a financial conflict of interest.

M.B.W. is a cofounder of Beacon Biosignals. This relationship does not pose a financial conflict of interest.

APPENDIX

Challenges with False Alarm Rate

False alarm rates may also influence the effects described in the main manuscript. However, in order to know the false alarm rate for self-reported seizures, it is necessary to have a gold standard upon which to compare. This has proven extremely challenging. In the setting of the epilepsy monitoring unit, reports exist for how often patients report seizures which turn out to have no EEG correlate. These non-correlated events cannot be automatically presumed to be “false alarms” because many seizures can occur without scalp EEG correlate. These events are better detected with intracranial EEG. On the other hand, studies employing intracranial EEG also cannot be the gold-standard for true seizures, unless there is absolute certainty that all seizure foci are represented by the specific intracranial electrodes. This certainty is extremely challenging to have, and often comes retrospectively after a surgery results in long term complete seizure freedom (several years) which requires no anti-seizure medication. At present, no large-scale study has been conducted with a cohort of these types of patients to look at the false alarm rate of self-reported events compared to a true seizure log derived from intracranial electrodes. Other studies, employing intracranial electrodes either during short term or long term recordings have made anecdotal observations about the discordance of some self-reported events and intracranial recordings, however the interpretation of such reports has the limitations mentioned above.

In the absence of clear data to guide our simulations, we have resorted to a simplifying assumption of a low self-reported false alarm rate of 1 per month. This assumption may be too low or too high, and future studies will help inform that. For now, we have also explored several additional false alarm rates (below) to see what effect this selection has on the overall results.

One speculative false alarm rate model

Currently, it is unknown how frequent patients will self-report events that are not seizures (see above). However, it is logical to assume that perhaps the rate of self-reported false alarm events has two components: a fixed rate, as well as a rate that depends on the true rate of seizures. Thus, a model can be constructed as follows (FPR = false positive rate):

Total FPR=Static FPR+Proportional FPR*(Seizure Frequency)

Further studies are needed to determine if such a hybrid model reflects the clinical reality of most patients. For the purposes of the present study, we did not explore the above speculative model, because it adds additional degrees of freedom without data driven constraints.

Influence of different false alarm rates

graphic file with name nihms-1992859-f0003.jpg

Shown in the figure, higher sensitivity values result in higher drugs/day typically, and fewer seizures per day. However, these trends are relatively small and stable after the lowest sensitivities. Oddly, the number of seizures/day and drugs/day do not follow a linear relationship with false alarm rate. This is due to the impact of having enough self-reported seizures (due to false alarms) that drugs are started. When that happens, the number of true seizures becomes dramatically lower. As seen more clearly in the data table below, when the false alarm rate is 1/180 days then some patients start getting meds, seizure freedom rises from zero and the number of seizures/month falls.

An important note is made here. In the present simulations, the FAR is considered a known value. In that case, the treating physician performs a discounting prior to considering a reported seizure rate. For example, if FAR = 1 per month, and at 3 months a patient reports 4 seizures per month, the physican would consider this number to be 3 = 4 – 1.

If the FAR is not known, this discounting procedure is not possible, and that would increase the number of drugs and decrease the number of seizures for all patients with a FAR>0.

Sens FAR szfree meanDrug meanSz how_long
0.0 0.0 0.00000 0.000000 6.495726 0.0
0.1 0.0 0.52847 1.461538 2.341880 20.0
0.2 0.0 0.55001 1.794872 2.162393 22.0
0.3 0.0 0.55838 1.897436 2.068376 23.0
0.4 0.0 0.56783 1.948718 2.017094 23.0
0.5 0.0 0.57000 2.000000 1.982906 24.0
0.6 0.0 0.57301 2.025641 1.991453 24.0
0.7 0.0 0.57604 2.051282 1.957265 24.0
0.8 0.0 0.57949 2.076923 1.982906 24.0
0.9 0.0 0.57948 2.102564 1.948718 24.0
1.0 0.0 0.57850 2.128205 1.940171 24.0
0.0 0.002778 0.00000 0.000000 6.470085 0.0
0.1 0.002778 0.53274 1.538462 2.256410 20.0
0.2 0.002778 0.55719 1.871795 2.068376 22.0
0.3 0.002778 0.56718 1.974359 1.982906 23.0
0.4 0.002778 0.57394 2.025641 1.982906 24.0
0.5 0.002778 0.57508 2.051282 1.940171 24.0
0.6 0.002778 0.57777 2.102564 1.923077 24.0
0.7 0.002778 0.58085 2.128205 1.923077 24.0
0.8 0.002778 0.58298 2.153846 1.923077 24.0
0.9 0.002778 0.58342 2.179487 1.905983 24.0
1.0 0.002778 0.58289 2.179487 1.923077 25.0
0.0 0.005556 0.53923 2.051282 2.427350 28.0
0.1 0.005556 0.56474 2.358974 2.145299 29.0
0.2 0.005556 0.57348 2.461538 2.068376 29.0
0.3 0.005556 0.57592 2.512821 2.025641 29.0
0.4 0.005556 0.58062 2.538462 1.974359 29.0
0.5 0.005556 0.58203 2.564103 1.965812 29.0
0.6 0.005556 0.58409 2.589744 1.940171 29.0
0.7 0.005556 0.58390 2.615385 1.923077 29.0
0.8 0.005556 0.58380 2.641026 1.931624 29.0
0.9 0.005556 0.58471 2.641026 1.914530 29.0
1.0 0.005556 0.58222 2.641026 1.888889 29.0
0.0 0.011111 0.00000 0.000000 6.521368 0.0
0.1 0.011111 0.52441 1.487179 2.341880 20.0
0.2 0.011111 0.54833 1.794872 2.153846 22.0
0.3 0.011111 0.55681 1.923077 2.094017 23.0
0.4 0.011111 0.56564 1.974359 2.051282 24.0
0.5 0.011111 0.57234 2.000000 1.982906 24.0
0.6 0.011111 0.57390 2.025641 1.974359 24.0
0.7 0.011111 0.57763 2.051282 1.957265 24.0
0.8 0.011111 0.57928 2.076923 1.957265 24.0
0.9 0.011111 0.57992 2.102564 1.948718 24.0
1.0 0.011111 0.58030 2.102564 1.905983 24.0
0.0 0.033333 0.00000 0.000000 6.470085 0.0
0.1 0.033333 0.52740 1.461538 2.333333 20.0
0.2 0.033333 0.54722 1.769231 2.128205 22.0
0.3 0.033333 0.55990 1.897436 2.085470 23.0
0.4 0.033333 0.56259 1.974359 2.059829 24.0
0.5 0.033333 0.56873 2.000000 2.008547 24.0
0.6 0.033333 0.57646 2.025641 1.974359 24.0
0.7 0.033333 0.57528 2.051282 1.987179 24.0
0.8 0.033333 0.57969 2.076923 1.931624 24.0
0.9 0.033333 0.57969 2.102564 1.931624 24.0
1.0 0.033333 0.58237 2.102564 1.905983 24.0
0.0 0.142857 0.59477 3.282051 1.846154 30.0
0.1 0.142857 0.58206 2.948718 2.017094 30.0
0.2 0.142857 0.58335 2.974359 2.008547 30.0
0.3 0.142857 0.58248 3.000000 1.982906 30.0
0.4 0.142857 0.58436 3.025641 1.948718 30.0
0.5 0.142857 0.58584 3.051282 1.931624 30.0
0.6 0.142857 0.58539 3.051282 1.914530 30.0
0.7 0.142857 0.58494 3.076923 1.914530 30.0
0.8 0.142857 0.59089 3.076923 1.897436 30.0
0.9 0.142857 0.58886 3.076923 1.914530 30.0
1.0 0.142857 0.58949 3.102564 1.863248 30.0
0.0 1.0 0.49224 1.589744 2.811966 26.0
0.1 1.0 0.54152 1.974359 2.350427 28.0
0.2 1.0 0.55414 2.102564 2.230769 28.0
0.3 1.0 0.56160 2.205128 2.145299 28.0
0.4 1.0 0.56860 2.256410 2.076923 28.0
0.5 1.0 0.57285 2.307692 2.042735 28.0
0.6 1.0 0.57461 2.333333 2.051282 28.0
0.7 1.0 0.57237 2.358974 2.008547 28.0
0.8 1.0 0.57889 2.384615 1.974359 28.0
0.9 1.0 0.57800 2.410256 1.965812 29.0
1.0 1.0 0.58351 2.410256 1.940171 28.0
0.0 2.0 0.50437 1.692308 2.717949 27.0
0.1 2.0 0.53998 1.974359 2.418803 28.0
0.2 2.0 0.55079 2.102564 2.316239 28.0
0.3 2.0 0.55857 2.179487 2.213675 28.0
0.4 2.0 0.56335 2.230769 2.145299 28.0
0.5 2.0 0.56164 2.282051 2.128205 28.0
0.6 2.0 0.56653 2.307692 2.094017 28.0
0.7 2.0 0.56920 2.333333 2.051282 29.0
0.8 2.0 0.57247 2.358974 2.034188 29.0
0.9 2.0 0.57365 2.384615 2.017094 29.0
1.0 2.0 0.57711 2.384615 1.991453 29.0

Footnotes

Ethical publication statement:

We confirm that we have read the Journal’s position on issues involved in ethical publication and affirm that this report is consistent with those guidelines.

REFERENCES

  • 1.Karoly P, Goldenholz DM, Cook M. Are the days of counting seizures numbered? Curr Opin Neurol. 2018;31. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Cook MJ, O’Brien TJ, Berkovic SF, et al. Prediction of seizure likelihood with a long-term, implanted seizure advisory system in patients with drug-resistant epilepsy: A first-in-man study. Lancet Neurol. 2013;12:563–571. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Cui J, Balzekas I, Nurse E, et al. Perceived seizure risk in epilepsy: Chronic electronic surveys with and without concurrent electroencephalography. Epilepsia. 2023;64:2421–2433. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Elger CE, Hoppe C. Diagnostic challenges in epilepsy: seizure under-reporting and seizure detection. Lancet Neurol [online serial]. Lancet Publishing Group; 2018;17:279–288. Accessed at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/29452687. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Schulze-Bonhage A, Richardson MP, Brandt A, Zabler N, Dümpelmann M, San Antonio-Arce V. Cyclical underreporting of seizures in patient-based seizure documentation. Ann Clin Transl Neurol. 2023;10:1863–1872. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Haneef Z, Yang K, Sheth SA, et al. Sub-scalp electroencephalography: A next-generation technique to study human neurophysiology. Clin Neurophysiol [online serial ]. Elsevier Ireland Ltd; 2022;141:77–87. Accessed at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/35907381. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Meritam Larsen P, Beniczky S. Non-electroencephalogram-based seizure detection devices: State of the art and future perspectives. Epilepsy Behav. 2023;148:109486. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Tatum WO, Winters L, Gieron M, et al. Outpatient Seizure Identification. Journal of Clinical Neurophysiology. 2001;18:14–19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.DuBois JM, Boylan LS, Shiyko M, Barr WB, Devinsky O. Seizure prediction and recall. Epilepsy and Behavior [online serial]. Elsevier Inc.; 2010;18:106–109. Accessed at: 10.1016/j.yebeh.2010.03.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Goldenholz DM, Westover MB. Flexible realistic simulation of seizure occurrence recapitulating statistical properties of seizure diaries. Epilepsia [online serial]. 2023;64:396–405. Accessed at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/36401798. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ferastraoaru V, Goldenholz DM, Chiang S, Moss R, Theodore WH, Haut SR. Characteristics of large patient-reported outcomes: Where can one million seizures get us? Epilepsia Open [online serial]. 2018;3:364–373. Accessed at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30187007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Goldenholz DM, Goldenholz SR, Moss R, et al. Is seizure frequency variance a predictable quantity? Ann Clin Transl Neurol. 2018;5:201–207. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Baud MO, Kleen JK, Mirro EA, et al. Multi-day rhythms modulate seizure risk in epilepsy. Nat Commun [online serial]. Springer US; 2018;9:1–10. Accessed at: 10.1038/s41467-017-02577-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Karoly PJ, Goldenholz DM, Freestone DR, et al. Circadian and circaseptan rhythms in human epilepsy: a retrospective cohort study. Lancet Neurol [online serial]. 2018;17:977–985. Accessed at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/30219655 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Chiang S, Haut SR, Ferastraoaru V, et al. Individualizing the definition of seizure clusters based on temporal clustering analysis. Epilepsy Res. 2020;163. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Haut SR. Seizure clusters: characteristics and treatment. Curr Opin Neurol [online serial]. 2015;28:143–150. Accessed at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/25695133. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Trinka E, Cock H, Hesdorffer D, et al. A definition and classification of status epilepticus - Report of the ILAE Task Force on Classification of Status Epilepticus. Epilepsia. 2015;56:1515–1523. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Theodore WH, Porter RJ, Albert P, et al. The secondarily generalized tonic-clonic seizure: a videotape analysis. Neurology [online serial]. 1994;44:1403–1407. Accessed at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/8058138. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Karoly PJ, Romero J, Cook MJ, Freestone DR, Goldenholz DM. When can we trust responders? Serious concerns when using 50% response rate to assess clinical trials. Epilepsia. 2019;60. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Chen Z, Brodie MJ, Liew D, Kwan P. Treatment outcomes in patients with newly diagnosed epilepsy treated with established and new antiepileptic drugs a 30-year longitudinal cohort study. JAMA Neurol. 2018;75:279–286. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Rheims S, Cucherat M, Arzimanoglou A, Ryvlin P. Greater response to placebo in children than in adults: A systematic review and meta-analysis in drug-resistant partial epilepsy. PLoS Med. 2008;5:1223–1237. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Brodie MJ, Barry SJE, Bamagous GA, Norrie JD, Kwan P. Patterns of treatment response in newly diagnosed epilepsy. Neurology. 2012;78:1548–1554. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.LaGrant B, Goldenholz DM, Braun M, Moss RE, Grinspan ZM. Patterns of Recording Epileptic Spasms in an Electronic Seizure Diary Compared With Video-EEG and Historical Cohorts. Pediatr Neurol [online serial]. Elsevier Ltd; 2021;122:27–34. Accessed at: 10.1016/j.pediatrneurol.2021.04.008. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Hannon T, Fernandes KM, Wong V, Nurse ES, Cook MJ. Over- and underreporting of seizures: How big is the problem? Epilepsia. Epub 2024. Mar 19. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Scheffer IE, Berkovic S, Capovilla G, et al. ILAE classification of the epilepsies: Position paper of the ILAE Commission for Classification and Terminology. Epilepsia [online serial]. 2017;58:512–521. Accessed at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28276062. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Fisher RS, Cross JH, French JA, et al. Operational classification of seizure types by the International League Against Epilepsy: Position Paper of the ILAE Commission for Classification and Terminology. Epilepsia [online serial]. 2017;58:522–530. Accessed at: http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/28276060. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Rheims S, Perucca E, Cucherat M, Ryvlin P. Factors determining response to antiepileptic drugs in randomized controlled trials. A systematic review and meta-analysis. Epilepsia. 2011;52:219–233. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

All data used in this study was synthetically generated using the open-source code.

RESOURCES