Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2024 Jul 22;13:RP97256. doi: 10.7554/eLife.97256

Energetic demands regulate sleep-wake rhythm circuit development

Amy R Poe 1, Lucy Zhu 1, Si Hao Tang 1, Ella Valencia 1, Matthew S Kayser 1,2,3,
Editors: Gáspár Jékely4, Albert Cardona5
PMCID: PMC11262794  PMID: 39037919

Abstract

Sleep and feeding patterns lack strong daily rhythms during early life. As diurnal animals mature, feeding is consolidated to the day and sleep to the night. In Drosophila, circadian sleep patterns are initiated with formation of a circuit connecting the central clock to arousal output neurons; emergence of circadian sleep also enables long-term memory (LTM). However, the cues that trigger the development of this clock-arousal circuit are unknown. Here, we identify a role for nutritional status in driving sleep-wake rhythm development in Drosophila larvae. We find that in the 2nd instar larval period (L2), sleep and feeding are spread across the day; these behaviors become organized into daily patterns by the 3rd instar larval stage (L3). Forcing mature (L3) animals to adopt immature (L2) feeding strategies disrupts sleep-wake rhythms and the ability to exhibit LTM. In addition, the development of the clock (DN1a)-arousal (Dh44) circuit itself is influenced by the larval nutritional environment. Finally, we demonstrate that larval arousal Dh44 neurons act through glucose metabolic genes to drive onset of daily sleep-wake rhythms. Together, our data suggest that changes to energetic demands in developing organisms trigger the formation of sleep-circadian circuits and behaviors.

Research organism: D. melanogaster

eLife digest

Like most young animals, babies must obtain enough nutrients and energy to grow, yet they also need to rest for their brains to mature properly. As many exhausted new parents know first-hand, balancing these conflicting needs results in frequent, rapid switches between eating and sleeping. Eventually, new-borns’ internal biological clock system, which is aligned with the 24-hour light cycle, becomes fully operational. Exactly how this then translates into allowing them to stay alert during the day and be sleepy at night is still unclear.

Like humans, the larvae of fruit flies first sleep haphazardly before developing a circadian pattern whereby they sleep at night and eat during the day. This shift occurs when a group of nerve cells called DN1a, whose job is to ‘keep time’, connects with Dh44, a subset of neurons which, when active, promote wakefulness. The trigger for these changes, however, has remained elusive.

In response, Poe et al. hypothesized that feeding behaviour and nutrient availability coordinated the emergence of sleep rhythms in fruit flies. Forcing fruit fly larvae to keep feeding in an ‘immature’ pattern – by either genetic manipulations or reducing the sugar content of their food – not only prevented them from developing ‘mature’ sleeping rhythms but also resulted in memory problems.

These experiments also showed that the DN1a-Dh44 connection depends on nutrient availability, as it did not form in larvae raised on the low-sugar food. Further genetic experiments showed that the Dh44 cells themselves act like nutrient sensors during the emergence of sleeping patterns.

These results shed new light on the factors triggering sleep rhythm development. Poe et al. hope that the understanding gained can be extended to humans and eventually help manage nervous system disorders and health problems associated with disrupted sleep during early life.

Introduction

The development of behavioral rhythms such as sleep-wake patterns is critical for brain development (Vallone et al., 2007). Indeed, early life circadian disruptions in rodents negatively impacts adult behaviors, neuronal morphology, and circadian physiology (Varcoe et al., 2016; Mendez et al., 2016; Ameen et al., 2022). Likewise, in humans, disruptions in sleep and rhythms during development are a common co-morbidity in neurodevelopmental disorders including ADHD and autism (Hoffmire et al., 2014; Krakowiak et al., 2008; Kotagal, 2015; Ednick et al., 2009). Although mechanisms encoding the molecular clock are well understood, little is known about how rhythmic behaviors first emerge (Vallone et al., 2007; Carmona-Alcocer et al., 2018; Sehgal et al., 1992; Poe et al., 2022). In particular, cues that trigger the consolidation of sleep and waking behaviors as development proceeds are unclear (Blumberg et al., 2005; Blumberg et al., 2014; Frank et al., 2017; Frank, 2020).

A key potential factor in the maturation of sleep patterns is the coincident change in feeding and metabolism during development. Early in development, most young animals must obtain enough nutrients to ensure proper growth (Ghosh et al., 2023). Yet, developing organisms must also sleep to support nervous system development (Vallone et al., 2007). These conflicting needs (feeding vs. quiescence) result in rapid transitions between sleeping and feeding states early in life (Adair and Bauchner, 1993; Levin and Stern, 1975). As development proceeds, nutritional intake and storage capacity increase, allowing for the consolidation of feeding and sleep to specific times of day (Li et al., 2018). These changes in nutritional storage capacity are likely conserved as mammalian body composition and nutritional capacity change over infant development (Toro-Ramos et al., 2015) and Drosophila show rapid increases in overall larval body size including the size of the fat body (used for nutrient storage) across development (Butterworth et al., 1988; Jacobs et al., 2020). However, the role that developmental change in metabolic drive plays in regulating the consolidation of behavioral rhythms is not known.

In adult Drosophila, sleep and feeding behaviors are consolidated to specific times of day with flies eating more in the day than the night (Barber et al., 2016). Yet, early in development, sleep in 2nd instar Drosophila larvae (L2) lacks a circadian pattern (Poe et al., 2023). We previously determined that sleep-wake rhythms are initiated in early 3rd instar Drosophila larvae (L3) (72 hr AEL). DN1a clock neurons anatomically and functionally connect to Dh44 arousal output neurons to drive the consolidation of sleep in L3. Development of this circuit promotes deeper sleep in L3 resulting in the emergence of long-term memory (LTM) capabilities at the L3 stage but not before (Poe et al., 2023).

Here, we identify the cues that trigger the emergence of the DN1a-Dh44 circuit and the consolidation of sleep-wake rhythms in Drosophila larvae. We demonstrate that developmental changes to energetic demands drive consolidation of periods of sleep and feeding across the day as animals mature. While endogenous deeper sleep in L3 facilitates LTM, we find that experimentally inducing deep sleep prematurely in L2 is detrimental to development and does not improve LTM performance. Additionally, we demonstrate that DN1a-Dh44 circuit formation is developmentally plastic, as rearing on an insufficient nutritional environment prevents establishment of this neural connection. Finally, we find that Dh44 neurons require glucose metabolic genes to promote sleep-wake rhythm development, suggesting that these neurons sense the nutritional environment to promote circadian-sleep crosstalk.

Results

Energetic demands limit developmental onset of rhythmic behaviors

The emergence of circadian sleep-wake rhythms in Drosophila larvae is advantageous as it enables long-term memory capabilities at the L3 stage (Poe et al., 2023). Less mature larvae (L2) do not exhibit consolidated sleep-wake patterns, prompting us to ask why rhythmic behaviors do not emerge earlier in life. To determine if the absence of rhythmic sleep-wake patterns in L2 might be related to feeding patterns, we examined larval feeding rate (# mouth hook contractions in a 5-min period) under constant conditions at 4 times across the day (Circadian Time [CT] 1, CT7, CT13, and CT19) in developmentally age-matched 2nd (L2) and early 3rd instar (L3; 72 hr AEL) larvae. While we observed no differences in feeding rate across the day in L2, we found that L3 show diurnal differences in feeding with higher feeding rate during the subjective day compared to the subjective night (Figure 1A and B). Analysis of total food intake indicated that L2 consume the same amount at CT1 and CT13; however, L3 consume more at CT1 than at CT13 (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). To assess whether the daily pattern of feeding in early L3 is dependent on the canonical circadian biological clock, we examined feeding rate in null mutants for the clock gene tim (Konopka and Benzer, 1971). We observed no differences in feeding rate across the day in L3 tim mutants indicating that the daily feeding pattern requires a functioning molecular clock (Figure 1C). These findings underscore the tight relationship between sleep and feeding across development as diurnal differences in sleep emerge concurrently at the L3 stage (Poe et al., 2023).

Figure 1. Energetic drive limits sleep rhythm development.

(A–D) Feeding rate (# of mouth hook contractions per 5 min) of L2 controls (A) L3 raised on regular (ctrl) food (B) L3 clock mutants (C) and L3 raised on low sugar (L.S.) food (D) across the day. (E) Sleep duration at CT1 and CT13 in L3 raised on regular (ctrl) and L.S. food. (F) Arousal threshold at CT1 and CT13 in L3 raised on regular (ctrl) and L.S. food. A-D, n=18–20 larvae; E, n=29–34 larvae; (F) n=100–172 sleep episodes, 18 larvae per genotype. One-way ANOVAs followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons tests (A–D) Two-way ANOVAs followed by Sidak’s multiple comparison test (E–F). For this and all other figures unless otherwise specified, data are presented as mean ± SEM; n.s., not significant, *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001. Source data in Figure 1—source data 1.

Figure 1—source data 1. Raw values for sleep and feeding in genetic and nutritional manipulations.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Larvae reared on low sugar diet develop normally.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Feeding amount of L2 controls, L3 raised on regular (ctrl) food, and L3 raised on low sugar (L.S.) food at CT1 and CT13. (B, C) Sleep bout number (B) and bout length (C) at CT1 and CT13 in L3 raised on regular (ctrl) and L.S. food. (D) Total body weight of early L3 (in groups of 5) raised on ctrl or L.S. food. (E) Total body length of early L3 raised on ctrl or L.S. food. (F) Developmental analysis of time to pupal formation of animals raised on ctrl or L.S. food. (G–I) Naïve OCT, AM, and quinine preference in L3 raised on ctrl and L.S. food. (B-C) n=29–34 larvae; (D) n=30 larvae per food condition; (E) n=33–40 larvae; (F) n=100–170 larvae; (G-I) n=6 PREFs (180 larvae) per genotype. Two-way ANOVAs followed by Sidak’s multiple comparison test (B–C) Unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-tests (D–E) and (G–I). Source data in Figure 1—figure supplement 1—source data 1.
Figure 1—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Measures of all sleep and developmental parameters on L.S. food.

To further investigate how the emergence of circadian sleep is related to changes in feeding patterns during development, we asked whether enforcing a constant (immature) feeding pattern at the L3 stage affects sleep-wake rhythms. First, we devised a nutritional paradigm with reduced sugar content but otherwise normal food (low sugar, 1.2% glucose, L.S.). Critically, this paradigm did not affect any measures of larval growth or development (Figure 1—figure supplement 1D–F) in contrast to numerous other diets that were assessed. We found that the feeding pattern in L3 raised on L.S. food closely resembled that of L2 on normal food (8% glucose), with feeding spread out across the day (Figure 1D; Figure 1—figure supplement 1A). Compared to L3 raised on control food, this paradigm was also associated with loss of diurnal differences in sleep duration, sleep bout number, and arousal threshold (indicating less deep sleep) (Figure 1E and F; Figure 1—figure supplement 1B and C). Next, to avoid chronic effects of this dietary manipulation, we acutely stimulated feeding in L3 reared on normal food using thermogenetic activation of NPF +neurons (Figure 2A; Figure 2—figure supplement 1A; Wu et al., 2003; Chung et al., 2017). Like L.S. conditions, enforcing a constant feeding pattern through NPF +neuron activation led to loss of sleep rhythms and loss of deep sleep (higher arousal threshold) in L3 (Figure 2B–E; Figure 2—figure supplement 1B–E).

Figure 2. Immature feeding strategies limit LTM.

(A) Feeding rate of L3 expressing npf-Gal4 >UAS-TrpA1 and genetic controls at 30 °C at CT13. (B–D) Sleep duration (B), bout number (C), and bout length (D) of L3 expressing npf-Gal4 >UAS-TrpA1 and genetic controls at 30 °C. (E–G) Arousal threshold (E), long-term aversive memory performance (F), and short-term aversive memory performance (G) in L3 expressing npf-Gal4 >UAS-TrpA1 and genetic controls at 30 °C. (H,I) Short-and long-term term aversive memory performance in L3 raised on ctrl and L.S. food. A, n=18–20 larvae; B-D, n=24–61 larvae; E, n=125–160 sleep episodes, 18 larvae per genotype; F-I, n=8 PIs (240 larvae) per genotype. One-way ANOVAs followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons tests (A) and (E–G) Two-way ANOVAs followed by Sidak’s multiple comparison test (B–D) unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-test (H and I). Source data in Figure 2—source data 1.

Figure 2—source data 1. Sleep and LTM values for npf manipulations at 30c.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Baseline odor preferences, feeding, and sleep are not affected by npf-Gal4 manipulations.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Feeding rate of L3 expressing npf-Gal4 >UAS-TrpA1 and genetic controls at 22 °C at CT13. (B–E) Sleep duration (B), bout number (C), bout length (D), and arousal threshold (E) in L3 expressing npf-Gal4 >UAS-TrpA1 and genetic controls at CT1 and CT13 at 22 °C (temperature controls). (F) Long-term aversive memory performance in L3 expressing npf-Gal4 >UAS-TrpA1 and genetic controls at 22 °C (temperature controls). (G–I) Naïve OCT, AM, and quinine preference in L3 expressing npf-Gal4 >UAS-TrpA1 and genetic controls at 30 °C. (A) n=18–20 larvae; (B-D) n=22–27 larvae; (E) n=120–205 sleep episodes, 18 larvae per genotype; (F) n=8 PIs (240 larvae) per genotype; (G-I) n=6 PREFs (180 larvae) per genotype. One-way ANOVAs followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons tests (A), (E), and (F–I) Two-way ANOVAs followed by Sidak’s multiple comparison test (B–D). Source data in Figure 2—figure supplement 1—source data 1.
Figure 2—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Temperature control data for npf manipulations.

Disruption to circadian sleep and/or to deeper sleep stages during development is associated with impairments in long-term memory formation (Ameen et al., 2022; Konrad et al., 2016; Seehagen et al., 2015; Jones et al., 2019; Smarr et al., 2017). We next asked if the loss of deeper sleep observed in animals adopting an immature (constant) feeding strategy through either the dietary paradigm or NPF +neuron activation affects long-term memory (LTM). Consistent with deeper sleep stages being necessary for LTM, we observed a loss of LTM in either the NPF +neuron activation (Figure 2F; Figure 2—figure supplement 1F–I) or under L.S. conditions (Figure 2H; Figure 1—figure supplement 1G–I); short-term memory (STM) was intact in both manipulations (Figure 2G and I). These findings suggest that immature feeding strategies preclude the emergence of sleep rhythms and LTM. Together, our data indicate that consolidated periods of sleep and feeding emerge due to developmentally dynamic changes in energetic demands.

Deeper sleep stages are energetically disadvantageous in L2

To investigate if promoting deep sleep at night in L2 can enable precocious LTM abilities, we fed L2 larvae the GABA-A agonist gaboxadol (Dissel et al., 2015; Dijk et al., 2012). Gaboxadol feeding induced deeper sleep in L2 (as reflected by an increase in arousal threshold) although sleep duration was unchanged. Despite achieving deeper sleep, LTM was still not evident in L2 (Figure 3A, B and E; Figure 3—figure supplement 1A and B); however, in contrast to L3, L2 on gaboxadol failed to develop normally (Figure 3C and D). Next, to avoid chronic pharmacological manipulations altogether, we acutely stimulated sleep-inducing neurons using thermogenetic approaches. We found that acute activation of these neurons caused an increase in sleep duration and bout length with a decrease in arousal threshold (Figure 3F and G; Figure 3—figure supplement 1C–H). However, inducing deeper sleep in L2 via genetic approaches likewise did not improve LTM performance (Figure 3H; Figure 3—figure supplement 1I–K) despite STM being intact (Figure 3—figure supplement 1L). Moreover, as with gaboxadol feeding in L2, thermogenetic induction of deeper sleep stages disrupted larval development (Figure 3I and J). These data suggest that sleep cannot be leveraged to enhance cognitive function prematurely because prolonged periods of deep sleep are not energetically sustainable at this stage.

Figure 3. Deeper sleep in L2 is energetically disadvantageous.

(A, B) Sleep duration (A) and arousal threshold (B) of L2 control fed vehicle control (L2) or Gaboxadol (L2 Gab). (C, D) Total body weight of L2 (C) (in groups of 10) or L3 (D) (in groups of 5) fed vehicle control or Gaboxadol (Gab). (E) Long-term aversive memory performance in L2 fed vehicle control (L2) or Gaboxadol (L2 Gab). (F, G) Sleep duration (F) and arousal threshold (G) of L2 expressing R76G11-Gal4>UAS-TrpA1 and genetic controls at 30 °C. (H) Long-term aversive memory performance of L2 expressing R76G11-Gal4>UAS-TrpA1 and genetic controls at 30 °C. (I, J) Total body weight (I) and total body length (J) of L2 expressing R76G11-Gal4>UAS-TrpA1 and genetic controls at 30 °C. (A) n=28 larvae; (B) n=110–220 sleep episodes, 18 larvae per genotype; (C) n=5–7 groups (50–70 larvae); (D) n=8 groups (40 larvae); (E) n=8 PIs (240 larvae) per genotype; (F) n=33–36 larvae; (G) n=234–404 sleep episodes, 30–40 larvae per genotype; (H) n=8 PIs (240 larvae) per genotype; (I) n=5 groups (25 larvae); (J) n=31–32 larvae. Unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-tests (A–E) one-way ANOVAs followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons tests (F–J). Source data in Figure 3—source data 1.

Figure 3—source data 1. Sleep, LTM, and developmental data for L2 manipulations.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Baseline sleep and odor preferences are not disrupted by R76G11-Gal4 manipulations.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A, B) Sleep bout number (A) and bout length (B) of L2 control fed vehicle control (L2) or Gaboxadol (L2 Gab). (C, D) Sleep bout number (C) and bout length (D) of L2 expressing R76G11-Gal4>UAS-TrpA1 and genetic controls at 30 °C. (E–H) Sleep duration (E), bout number (F), bout length (G), and arousal threshold (H) of L2 expressing R76G11-Gal4>UAS-TrpA1 and genetic controls at 22 °C (temperature controls). (I–K) Naïve OCT, AM, and quinine preference in L2 expressing R76G11-Gal4>UAS-TrpA1 and genetic controls at 30 °C. (L) Short-term aversive memory performance of L2 expressing R76G11-Gal4>UAS-TrpA1 and genetic controls at 30 °C. (A, B) n=28 larvae; (C-D) n=33–36 larvae; (E-G) n=27–29 larvae; (H) n=145–316 sleep episodes, 30–40 larvae per genotype; (I-K) n=6 PREFs (180 larvae) per genotype; (L) n=8 PIs (240 larvae) per genotype. Unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-tests (A–B) One-way ANOVAs followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons tests (C–L). Source data in Figure 3—figure supplement 1—source data 1.
Figure 3—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Temperature control and naive preference data for L2 manipulations.

DN1a-Dh44 circuit formation is developmentally plastic

We previously determined that Dh44 arousal neurons anatomically and functionally connect to DN1a clock neurons at the L3 stage (Poe et al., 2023). Therefore, we examined the functional connectivity between clock and arousal loci in the setting of nutritional perturbation by expressing ATP-gated P2X2 receptors Yao et al., 2012 in DN1a neurons and GCaMP6 in Dh44 neurons. As expected, activation of DN1as in L3 raised on control food caused an increase in calcium in Dh44 neurons (Figure 4A and B; Poe et al., 2023). In contrast, in L3 raised on L.S. conditions, activation of DN1as no longer elicited a response in Dh44 neurons (Figure 4C and D). Thus, the nascent connection underlying circadian sleep in Drosophila is developmentally plastic: in an insufficient nutritional environment, this connection is not functional, presumably to facilitate a more constant feeding pattern that fulfills energetic needs of the animal. However, this feeding pattern eschews deep sleep at the expense of LTM.

Figure 4. DN1a-Dh44 circuit formation is developmentally plastic.

Figure 4.

(A, C) GCaMP6 signal in Dh44 neurons with activation of DN1a neurons in L3 controls (A) and L3 raised on L.S. food (C). Red bar indicates ATP application and gray bar indicates AHL application. (B, D) Maximum GCaMP change (ΔF/F) for individual cells in L3 controls (B) and L3 raised on L.S. food (D). (E–G) GCaMP7 signal in Dh44 neurons during bath application of 1 µM CCHa1 synthetic peptide in L3 controls (E), L2 controls (F) and L3 raised on L.S. food (G) brains. Red/blue bar indicates timing of CCHa1 (blue) or buffer (AHL, red) application and gray bar indicates timing of washout AHL application. (A-D) n=12–18 cells, 8–10 brains; (E-G) n=11–15 cells, 5–10 brains. Unpaired two-tailed Student’s t-tests (B and D) Mann-Whitney U test (E–G). Source data in Figure 4—source data 1.

Figure 4—source data 1. Individual cell fluorescence data for P2X2 and CCHa1 imaging.

We previously determined that release of CCHamide-1 (CCHa1) from DN1as to CCHa1mide-1 receptor (CCHa1-R) on Dh44 neurons is necessary for sleep-wake rhythms in L3 (Poe et al., 2023). Our data support a model in which the downstream Dh44 neurons are poised to receive clock (DN1a) input as soon the connection forms between these cellular populations. To test this idea directly, we next asked whether Dh44 neurons in L2, before the DN1a-Dh44 connection has formed, are competent to receive the CCHa1 signal. CCHamide-1 peptide was bath applied onto dissected larval brains expressing UAS-GCaMP7 in Dh44 neurons. As anticipated, we observed an increase in intracellular calcium in early L3 (Figure 4E); surprisingly, CCHa1 application did not alter calcium levels in Dh44 neurons in L2 (Figure 4F), indicating that Dh44 neurons are not capable of receiving CCHa1 input prior to early L3. Moreover, CCHa1 application in L3 reared on L.S. also failed to elicit a response in Dh44 neurons (Figure 4G) suggesting that sub-optimal nutritional milieu influences the development of Dh44 neuronal competency to receive clock-driven cues. Thus, our data indicate that the nutritional environment influences DN1a-Dh44 circuit development.

Dh44 neurons require glucose metabolic genes to regulate sleep-wake rhythms

How do developing larvae detect changes in their nutritional environments to drive the circadian consolidation of sleep and feeding at the L3 stage? In adult Drosophila, Dh44 neurons act as nutrient sensors to regulate food consumption and starvation-induced sleep suppression through the activity of both glucose and amino acid sensing genes (Dus et al., 2015; Yang et al., 2018; Oh et al., 2021; Oh and Suh, 2023). Indeed, Dh44 neurons themselves are activated by changes in bath application of nutritive sugars (Dus et al., 2015). We next asked whether Dh44 neurons in L3 require metabolic genes to regulate sleep-wake rhythms. In L3 raised on regular food, we conducted an RNAi-based candidate screen of different glucose and amino acid sensing genes known to act in adult Dh44 neurons. Sleep duration at CT1 and CT13 was assessed with knockdown of glucose metabolic genes (Hexokinase-C, Glucose transporter 1, and Pyruvate kinase) or amino acid sensing genes (Gcn2 and its downstream target ATF4 or cryptocephal) in Dh44 neurons. We found that knockdown of glucose metabolism genes, Glut1, Hex-C and PyK, in Dh44 neurons resulted in a loss of rhythmic changes in sleep duration and bout number (Figure 5A–C; Figure 5—figure supplement 1A–F) with no effect on L3 feeding in the Hex-C manipulation (Figure 5D). Additionally, knockdown of Hex-C in DN1as did not disrupt rhythmic changes in sleep duration in L3 (Figure 5—figure supplement 1G–I) suggesting a specialized role for glucose metabolism in Dh44 neurons for sleep-wake rhythm maturation. Manipulation of glucose metabolic genes in L2 did not affect sleep duration at CT1 and CT13 (Figure 5E–G; Figure 5—figure supplement 1J–O) providing evidence that nutrient sensing is not required at this stage to regulate sleep. In contrast to their role in adult Dh44 neurons, knockdown of amino acid sensing genes, Gcn2 and crc, in Dh44 neurons did not disrupt rhythmic changes in sleep duration and bout number in L3 (Figure 5H and I; Figure 5—figure supplement 2A–D) suggesting that Dh44 neurons may not act through amino acid sensing pathways to regulate sleep-wake rhythm development. Thus, Dh44 neurons require glucose metabolic genes to drive sleep-wake rhythm development. Our data indicate that the emergence of daily sleep-wake patterns is regulated by developmental changes in energetic capacity, and suggest that Dh44 neurons may be necessary for sensing of larval nutritional environments.

Figure 5. Dh44 neurons require glucose metabolic genes to regulate sleep-wake rhythms.

(A–C) Sleep duration in L3 expressing UAS-Glut1-RNAi (A), UAS-Hex-C-RNAi (B), and UAS-Pyk-RNAi (C) with Dh44-Gal4 and genetic controls at CT1 and CT13. (D) Feeding rate (# of mouth hook contractions per 5 min) of L3 expressing UAS-Hex-C-RNAi with Dh44-Gal4 and genetic controls at CT13. (E–G) Sleep duration in L2 expressing UAS-Glut1-RNAi (E), UAS-Hex-C-RNAi (F), and UAS-Pyk-RNAi (G) with Dh44-Gal4 and genetic controls at CT1 and CT13. (H, I) Sleep duration in L3 expressing UAS-GCN2-RNAi (H) and UAS-crc-RNAi (I) with Dh44-Gal4 and genetic controls at CT1 and CT13. (A-C) n=32–40 larvae; (D) n=20 larvae; (E-I) n=32–40 larvae. Two-way ANOVAs followed by Sidak’s multiple comparison test (A–C) and (E–I) One-way ANOVAs followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons tests (D). Source data in Figure 5—source data 1.

Figure 5—source data 1. Total sleep duration measures for Dh44 metabolism manipulations in L2 and L3.

Figure 5.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1. Glucose metabolic gene manipulations affect L3 sleep.

Figure 5—figure supplement 1.

(A, B) Sleep bout number (A) and bout length (B) in L3 expressing UAS-Glut1-RNAi with Dh44-Gal4 and genetic controls at CT1 and CT13. (C, D) Sleep bout number (C) and bout length (D) in L3 expressing UAS-Hex-C-RNAi with Dh44-Gal4 and genetic controls at CT1 and CT13. (E, F) Sleep bout number (E) and bout length (F) in L3 expressing UAS-PyK-RNAi with Dh44-Gal4 and genetic controls as CT1 and CT13. (G–I) Sleep duration (G), sleep bout number (H), and bout length (I) in L3 expressing UAS-Hex-C-RNAi with cry-Gal4 and genetic controls at CT1 and CT13. (J, K) Sleep bout number (J) and bout length (K) in L2 expressing UAS-Glut1-RNAi with Dh44-Gal4 and genetic controls at CT1 and CT13. (L, M) Sleep bout number (L) and bout length (M) in L2 expressing UAS-Hex-C-RNAi with Dh44-Gal4 and genetic controls at CT1 and CT13. (N, O) Sleep bout number (N) and bout length (O) in L2 expressing UAS-PyK-RNAi with Dh44-Gal4 and genetic controls at CT1 and CT13. (A-O) n=32–40 larvae. Two-way ANOVAs followed by Sidak’s multiple comparison test (A–O). Source data in Figure 5—figure supplement 1—source data 1.
Figure 5—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Complete sleep data for Dh44 glucose metabolism manipulations.
Figure 5—figure supplement 2. Amino acid sensing gene manipulations do not affect L3 sleep.

Figure 5—figure supplement 2.

(A, B) Sleep bout number (A) and bout length (B) in L3 expressing UAS-GCN2-RNAi with Dh44-Gal4 and genetic controls at CT1 and CT13. (C, D) Sleep bout number (C) and bout length (D) in L3 expressing UAS-crc-RNAi with Dh44-Gal4 and genetic controls at CT1 and CT13. (A-D) n=32–40 larvae. Two-way ANOVAs followed by Sidak’s multiple comparison test (A–D). Source data in Figure 5—figure supplement 2—source data 1.
Figure 5—figure supplement 2—source data 1. Raw values for amino acid sensing manipulations in Dh44 neurons.
Figure 5—figure supplement 3. Model Figure.

Figure 5—figure supplement 3.

Clock cells in the larval brain (s-LNv, DN2, and DN1a) communicate to coordinate circadian rhythms. In third instar larvae (L3), a new connection is formed between DN1as and Dh44 neurons, generating daily neural activity rhythms in Dh44 cells that drive sleep-wake patterns, deep sleep, and more enduring memories. In the setting of reduced nutrient availability, the functional connection between the clock (DN1a) and arousal output (Dh44) is not present, facilitating a more constant feeding strategy that benefits the animal under such conditions. However, without clock control of sleep at this stage, deep sleep is lost, as is the ability to exhibit long-term memory.

Discussion

Nutritional environment and energetic status exert profound effects on sleep patterns during development, but mechanisms coupling sleep to these factors remain undefined. We report that the development of sleep-circadian circuits depends on organisms achieving sufficient nutritional status to support the consolidation of deep sleep at night (Figure 5—figure supplement 3). Our data demonstrate that larval Dh44 neurons require glucose metabolic genes but not amino acid sensing genes to modulate sleep-wake rhythms. Larval Dh44 neurons may therefore have distinct functions from their adult counterparts for integrating information about the nutritional environment through the direct sensing of glucose levels to modulate sleep-wake rhythm development. Maintaining energy homeostasis and sensing of the nutritional environment are likely conserved regulators of sleep-wake rhythm development, as young mice exposed to a maternal low-protein diet show disruptions in night-time sleep architecture and energy expenditure later in life (Sutton et al., 2010; Crossland et al., 2017).

Together with our previously published work, our findings support a model in which changes in both overall circuit development and molecular changes in post-synaptic (Dh44) neurons likely drive sleep-wake rhythm circuit development. Our CCHa1 peptide data suggest that Dh44 neurons may undergo changes in CCHa1-R expression or subcellular localization between the L2 and L3 stages: we only observed an increase in Dh44 neural activity in response to the bath application of CCHa1 peptide at the L3 stage (Figure 4E and F). Interestingly, this increase in activity is absent in low nutrient conditions (Figure 4G) suggesting that the larval nutritional environment may also modulate CCHa1-R localization or expression. Indeed, we observed a disruption of sleep rhythms in L3 when glucose metabolic genes are knocked down in Dh44 neurons, demonstrating that post-synaptic processes likely initiate onset of circadian sleep. These findings raise intriguing questions for how changes in an organism’s energetic and nutritional state influence sleep-circadian circuit development. Perhaps larval Dh44 neurons respond to an increase in glucose levels in the environment by promoting CCHa1-R localization to the membrane. In this model, changes in CCHa1-R subcellular localization allow Dh44 neurons to become competent to receive clock-driven cues, while this or other Dh44-derived signals promote circuit connectivity with DN1as to drive consolidation of sleep at the L3 stage. There are no available antibodies or endogenous fluorescent reporters of CCHa1-R, limiting our ability to examine receptor subcellular localization. Additionally, while our study focuses on presumed CCHa1 synaptic signaling between DN1a and Dh44 neurons, we cannot rule out the possibility of CCHa1 volume transmission from DN1as or other sources as contributors to sleep-wake regulation. Regardless, our data open avenues for future work on the molecular and subcellular mechanisms regulating DN1a-Dh44 circuit development.

Our findings demonstrate that larvae exhibit both sleep-wake and feeding daily rhythms at the L3 stage, but not earlier (Poe et al., 2023). This raises the obvious question of whether sleep and feeding are opposite sides of the same coin. While larvae cannot eat when they are sleeping, we have observed distinct effects of certain manipulations on sleep behaviors but not feeding. For example, knockdown of Hex-C in Dh44 neurons disrupts sleep rhythms with no obvious effect on feeding behavior (Figure 5D). It is, of course, possible to affect both sleep and feeding behaviors with the same manipulation (e.g. activation of NPF +neurons) underscoring that they are highly inter-connected behaviors. Future work will leverage the larval system to examine how sleep-wake and feeding circuitry communicate to balance these rhythmic behaviors across developmental periods.

Materials and methods

Fly stocks

The following lines have been maintained as lab stocks or were obtained from Dr. Amita Sehgal: iso31, tim0 (Konopka and Benzer, 1971), Dh44VT-Gal4 (VT039046) (Jenett et al., 2012), cry-Gal4 pdf-Gal80 (Stoleru et al., 2004), UAS-TrpA1 (Hamada et al., 2008), UAS-mCherry RNAi, LexAOP-GCaMP6 UAS-P2X2 (Yao et al., 2012), and UAS-GCaMP7f. Dh44-LexA (80703), npf-Gal4 (25681), R76G11-Gal4 (48333), Hex-C RNAi (57404), Glut1 RNAi (40904), PyK RNAi (35218), GCN2 RNAi (67215), and crc RNAi (80388) were from the Bloomington Drosophila Stock Center (BDSC).

Larval rearing and sleep assays

Larval sleep experiments were performed as described previously (Poe et al., 2023; Szuperak et al., 2018). Briefly, molting 2nd instar or 3rd instar larvae were placed into individual wells of the LarvaLodge containing either 120 µl (for L2) or 95 µl (for L3) of 3% agar and 2% sucrose media covered with a thin layer of yeast paste. The LarvaLodge was covered with a transparent acrylic sheet and placed into a DigiTherm (Tritech Research) incubator at 25 °C for imaging. Experiments were performed in the dark. For thermogenetic experiments, adult flies were maintained at 22 °C. Larvae were then placed into the LarvaLodge (as described above) which was moved into a DigiTherm (Tritech Research) incubator at 30 °C for imaging.

LarvaLodge image acquisition and processing

Images were acquired every 6 s with an Imaging Source DMK 23GP031 camera (2592 X 1944 pixels, The Imaging Source, USA) equipped with a Fujinon lens (HF12.55A-1, 1:1.4/12.5 mm, Fujifilm Corp., Japan) with a Hoya 49mm R72 Infrared Filter as described previously (Poe et al., 2023; Szuperak et al., 2018). We used IC Capture (The Imaging Source) to acquire time-lapse images. All experiments were carried out in the dark using infrared LED strips (Ledlightsworld LTD, 850 nm wavelength) positioned below the LarvaLodge.

Images were analyzed using custom-written MATLAB software (see Churgin et al., 2019 and Szuperak et al., 2018) (Code available in Supplement). Temporally adjacent images were subtracted to generate maps of pixel value intensity change. A binary threshold was set such that individual pixel intensity changes that fell below 40 gray-scale units within each well were set equal to zero (‘no change’) to eliminate noise. For 3rd instars, the threshold was set to 45 to account for larger body size. Pixel changes greater than or equal to threshold value were set equal to one (‘change’). Activity was then calculated by taking the sum of all pixels changed between images. Sleep was defined as an activity value of zero between frames. For 2nd instar sleep experiments done across the day, total sleep was summed over 6 hr beginning 2 hr after the molt to second instar. For sleep experiments performed at certain circadian times, total sleep in the second hour after the molt to second (or third) instar was summed.

Feeding behavior analysis

For feeding rate analysis, newly molted 2nd instar or 3rd instar larvae were placed in individual wells of the LarvaLodge containing 120 µl of 3% agar and 2% sucrose media covered with a thin layer of yeast paste. Larvae were then imaged continuously with a Sony HDR-CX405 HD Handycam camera (B&H Photo, Cat. No: SOHDRCX405) for 5 minutes. The number of mouth hook contractions (feeding rate) was counted manually over the imaging period and raw numbers were recorded. For food intake analysis, newly molted 2nd instar or 3rd instar larvae were starved for 1 hr in petri dishes with water placed on a Kimwipe. To compare groups of larvae of similar body weights, 13 L3 larvae and 26 L2 larvae were grouped together. Larvae were placed in a petri dish containing blue-dyed 3% agar, 2% sucrose, and 2.5% apple juice with blue-dyed yeast paste on top for 4 hr at 25 °C in constant darkness. We found that 4 hr on blue-dyed agar was sufficient to reflect total food consumption in each condition with a shorter period of time (1 hr) causing more variability. After 4 hr, groups of larvae were washed in water, put in microtubes, and frozen at –80 °C for 1 hr. Frozen larvae were then homogenized in 300 µl of distilled water and spun down for 5 min at 13,0000 rpm. The amount of blue dye in the supernatant was then measured using a spectrophotometer (OD629). Food intake represents the OD value of each measurement.

Aversive olfactory conditioning

We used an established two odor reciprocal olfactory conditioning paradigm with 10 mM quinine (quinine hydrochloride, EMSCO/Fisher, Cat. No: 18-613-007) as a negative reinforcement to test short-term or long-term memory performance in L2 and early L3 larvae (Widmann et al., 2016) at CT12-15 (Poe et al., 2023). Experiments were conducted on assay plates (100X15 mm, Genesee Scientific, Cat. No: 32–107) filled with a thin layer of 2.5% agarose containing either pure agarose (EMSCO/Fisher, Cat. No: 16500–500) or agarose plus reinforcer. As olfactory stimuli, we used 10 µl amyl acetate (AM, Sigma-Aldrich, Cat. No: STBF2370V, diluted 1:50 in paraffin oil-Sigma-Aldrich, Cat. No: SZBF140V) and octanol (OCT, Fisher Scientific, Cat. No: SALP564726, undiluted). Odorants were loaded into the caps of 0.6 mL tubes (EMSCO/Fisher, Cat. No: 05-408-123) and covered with parafilm (EMSCO/Fisher, Cat. No: 1337412). For naïve preferences of odorants, a single odorant was placed on one side of an agarose plate with no odorant on the other side. A group of 30 larvae were placed in the middle. After 5 min, individuals were counted on the odorant side, the non-odorant side, or in the middle. The naïve preference was calculated by subtracting the number of larvae on the non-odorant side from the number of larvae on the odorant side and then dividing by the total number of larvae. For naïve preference of quinine, a group of 30 larvae were placed in the middle of a half agarose-half quinine plate. After 5 min, individuals were counted on the quinine side, the agarose side, or in the middle. The naïve preference for quinine was calculated by subtracting the number of larvae on the quinine side from the number of larvae on the agarose side and then dividing by the total number of larvae. Larvae were trained by exposing a group of 30 larvae to AM while crawling on agarose medium plus quinine reinforcer. After 5 min, larvae were transferred to a fresh Petri dish containing agarose alone with OCT as an odorant (AM+/OCT). A second group of 30 larvae received the reciprocal training (AM/OCT+). Three training cycles were used for all experiments. For long-term memory, larvae were transferred after training onto agarose plates with a small piece of Kimwipe moistened with tap water and covered in dry active yeast (LabScientific, Cat. No: FLY804020F). Larvae were then kept in the dark for 1.5 hr before testing memory performance. Training and retention for thermogenetic experiments were conducted at 30 °C. For short-term memory, larvae were immediately transferred after training onto test plates (agarose plus reinforcer) on which AM and OCT were presented on opposite sides of the plate. After 5 min, individuals were counted on the AM side, the OCT side, or in the middle. We then calculated a preference index (PREF) for each training group by subtracting the number of larvae on the conditioned stimulus side from the number of larvae on the unconditioned stimulus side. For one set of experiments, we calculated two PREF values: (1 a) PREFAM+/OCT = (#AM - #OCT)/ # TOTAL; (1b) PREFAM/OCT+ = (#OCT-#AM)/ # TOTAL. We then took the average of each PREF value to calculate an associative performance index (PI) as a measure of associative learning. PI = (PREFAM+/OCT +PREFAM/OCT+)/2.

Arousal threshold

Blue light stimulation was delivered as described in Poe et al., 2023; Szuperak et al., 2018 using 2 high power LEDs (Luminus Phatlight PT-121, 460 nm peak wavelength, Sunnyvale, CA) secured to an aluminum heat sink. The LEDs were driven at a current of 0.1 A (low intensity). We used a low intensity stimulus for 4 s every 2 min for 1 hr beginning the 2nd hr after the molt to second (or third) instar. We then counted the number of larvae that showed an activity change in response to stimulus. The percentage of animals that moved in response to the stimulus was recorded for each experiment. For each genotype, at least four biological replicates were performed. We then plotted the average percentage across all replicates.

P2X2 activation and GCaMP imaging

All live imaging experiments (P2X2 and CCHa1 bath application) were performed as described previously (Poe et al., 2023). Briefly, brains were dissected in artificial hemolymph (AHL) buffer consisting of (in mM): 108 NaCl, 5 KCl, 2 CaCl2, 8.2 MgCl2, 4 NaHCO3, 1 NaH2PO4-H20, 5 Trehalose, 10 Sucrose, 5 HEPES, pH = 7.5. Brains were placed on a small glass coverslip (Carolina Cover Glasses, Circles, 12 mm, Cat. No: 633029) in a perfusion chamber filled with AHL.

For P2X2 imaging, dissections were performed at CT12-15 and AHL buffer was perfused over the brains for 1 min of baseline GCaMP6 imaging, then ATP was delivered to the chamber by switching the perfusion flow from the channel containing AHL to the channel containing 2.5 mM ATP in AHL, pH 7.5. ATP was perfused for 2 min and then AHL was perfused for 2 min. Twelve-bit images were acquired with a 40 X water immersion objective at 256X256-pixel resolution. Z-stacks were acquired every 5 s for 3 min. Image processing and measurement of fluorescence intensity was performed in ImageJ as described previously (Poe et al., 2023). For each cell body, fluorescence traces over time were normalized using this equation: ΔF/F = (Fn-F0)/F0, where Fn = fluorescence intensity recorded at time point n, and F0 is the average fluorescence value during the 1 min baseline recording. Maximum GCaMP change (ΔF/F) for individual cells was calculated using this equation: ΔF/Fmax = (Fmax-F0)/F0, where Fmax = maximum fluorescence intensity value recorded during ATP application, and F0 is the average fluorescence value during the 1 min baseline recording. All analysis was done blind to experimental condition.

For CCHa1 bath application, dissections were performed at CT12-15 and AHL buffer was perfused over the brains for 1 min of baseline GCaMP7f imaging, then CCHa1 peptide was delivered to the chamber by switching the perfusion flow from the channel containing AHL to the channel containing 1 µM synthetic CCHa1 in AHL, pH 7.5. CCHa1 was perfused for 2 min, followed by a 1 min wash-out with AHL. For the AHL negative control, the perfusion flow was switched from one channel containing AHL to another channel containing AHL. Twelve-bit images were acquired with a 40 X water immersion objective at 256X256-pixel resolution. Z-stacks were acquired every 10 s for 4 min. Image processing and measurement of fluorescence intensity was performed in ImageJ. A max intensity Z-projection of each time step and Smooth thresholding was used for analysis. Image analysis was performed in a similar manner as for the P2X2 experiments. All analyses were done blind to experimental condition.

Gaboxadol treatment

Early second or third instar larvae were starved for 1 hr and then fed 75 µl of 25 mg/mL Gaboxadol (hydrochloride) (Thomas Scientific, Cat No: C817P41) in diluted yeast solution for 1 hr prior to loading in LarvaLodge containing 120 µl of 3% agar and 2% sucrose media covered with a thin layer of 25 mg/mL Gaboxadol yeast paste. For LTM experiments, starved early second instars were fed 25 mg/mL Gaboxadol for 1 hr prior to training and maintained on 25 mg/mL Gaboxadol in diluted yeast solution during retention period.

Dietary manipulations

Fly food was prepared using the following recipes (based on Poe et al., 2020):

Ingredients Control food (444 mM glucose) Low sugar (L.S.) (66 mM glucose)
Distilled H2O 234 mL 234 mL
Agar 2 g (10 g/L) 2 g (10 g/L)
Glucose 20 g 3 g
Inactive yeast 20 g 20 g
Acid mix (phosphoric acid +propionic acid) 2 mL 2 mL
Target final solution volume 250 mL 250 mL

Acid Mix was made by preparing Solution A (41.5 ml Phosphoric Acid mixed with 458.5 ml distilled water) and Solution B (418 ml Propionic Acid mixed with 82 ml distilled water) separately and then mixing Solution A and Solution B together.

Adult flies were placed in an embryo collection cage (Genesee Scientific, cat#: 59–100) and eggs were laid on a petri dish containing either control (ctrl) or Low sugar (L.S.) food. Animals developed on this media for three days.

Larval body weight and length measurements

For weight, groups of five early 3rd instar larvae raised on either control- or low sugar (L.S.)-filled petri dishes were washed in tap water and dried using a Kimwipe. The five larvae were then weighed as a group on a scale and the weight in mg was recorded. For the Gaboxadol experiments, groups of 10 early 2nd instar larvae or groups of five early 3rd instar larvae were weighed. For length, images of individual early 3rd instar larvae in the LarvaLodge were measured in ImageJ (Fiji) using the straight line tool. The total body length was determined in pixels for individual larvae on each condition.

Statistical analysis

All statistical analysis was done in GraphPad (Prism). For comparisons between two conditions, two-tailed unpaired t-tests were used. For comparisons between multiple groups, ordinary one-way ANOVAs followed by Tukey’s multiple comparison tests were used. For comparisons between different groups in the same analysis, ordinary one-way ANOVAs followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons tests were used. For comparisons between time and genotype, two-way ANOVAs followed by Sidak’s multiple comparisons tests were used. For comparison of GCaMP signal in CCHa1 experiments, Mann-Whitney test was used. *p<0.05, **p<0.01, ***p<0.001. Representative confocal images are shown from at least 8–10 independent samples examined in each case.

Acknowledgements

We thank members of the Kayser Lab, Raizen Lab, and other members of the Penn Chronobiology and Sleep Institute for helpful discussions and input. This work was supported by NIH DP2NS111996, NIH R01NS120979, and a Burroughs Wellcome Career Award for Medical Scientists to MSK; Hartwell Foundation Fellowship to ARP.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Matthew S Kayser, Email: kayser@pennmedicine.upenn.edu.

Gáspár Jékely, Heidelberg University, Germany.

Albert Cardona, University of Cambridge, United Kingdom.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke NIH DP2NS111996 to Matthew S Kayser.

  • National Institute of Neurological Disorders and Stroke NIH R01NS120979 to Matthew S Kayser.

  • Burroughs Wellcome Fund Career Award for Medical Scientists to Matthew S Kayser.

  • Hartwell Foundation Fellowship to Amy R Poe.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Writing - review and editing.

Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing - review and editing.

Formal analysis, Investigation, Writing - review and editing.

Formal analysis, Investigation.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Methodology, Writing - original draft, Project administration, Writing - review and editing.

Additional files

MDAR checklist
Source code 1. Larval sleep analysis codes.
elife-97256-code1.zip (184.6KB, zip)

Data availability

All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source data files have been provided for all Figures.

References

  1. Adair RH, Bauchner H. Sleep problems in childhood. Current Problems in Pediatrics. 1993;23:147–170. doi: 10.1016/0045-9380(93)90011-z. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Ameen RW, Warshawski A, Fu L, Antle MC. Early life circadian rhythm disruption in mice alters brain and behavior in adulthood. Scientific Reports. 2022;12:7366. doi: 10.1038/s41598-022-11335-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Barber AF, Erion R, Holmes TC, Sehgal A. Circadian and feeding cues integrate to drive rhythms of physiology in Drosophila insulin-producing cells. Genes & Development. 2016;30:2596–2606. doi: 10.1101/gad.288258.116. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Blumberg MS, Seelke AMH, Lowen SB, Karlsson KAE. Dynamics of sleep-wake cyclicity in developing rats. PNAS. 2005;102:14860–14864. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0506340102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Blumberg MS, Gall AJ, Todd WD. The development of sleep-wake rhythms and the search for elemental circuits in the infant brain. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2014;128:250–263. doi: 10.1037/a0035891. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Butterworth FM, Emerson L, Rasch EM. Maturation and degeneration of the fat body in the Drosophila larva and pupa as revealed by morphometric analysis. Tissue & Cell. 1988;20:255–268. doi: 10.1016/0040-8166(88)90047-x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Carmona-Alcocer V, Abel JH, Sun TC, Petzold LR, Doyle FJ, Simms CL, Herzog ED. Ontogeny of circadian rhythms and synchrony in the suprachiasmatic nucleus. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2018;38:1326–1334. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2006-17.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Chung BY, Ro J, Hutter SA, Miller KM, Guduguntla LS, Kondo S, Pletcher SD. Drosophila neuropeptide F signaling independently regulates feeding and sleep-wake behavior. Cell Reports. 2017;19:2441–2450. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2017.05.085. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Churgin MA, Szuperak M, Davis KC, Raizen DM, Fang-Yen C, Kayser MS. Quantitative imaging of sleep behavior in Caenorhabditis elegans and larval Drosophila melanogaster. Nature Protocols. 2019;14:1455–1488. doi: 10.1038/s41596-019-0146-6. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Crossland RF, Balasa A, Ramakrishnan R, Mahadevan SK, Fiorotto ML, Van den Veyver IB. Chronic maternal low-protein diet in mice affects anxiety, night-time energy expenditure and sleep patterns, but not circadian rhythm in male offspring. PLOS ONE. 2017;12:e0170127. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0170127. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Dijk DJ, Stanley N, Lundahl J, Groeger JA, Legters A, Trap Huusom AK, Deacon S. Enhanced slow wave sleep and improved sleep maintenance after gaboxadol administration during seven nights of exposure to a traffic noise model of transient insomnia. Journal of Psychopharmacology. 2012;26:1096–1107. doi: 10.1177/0269881111421971. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Dissel S, Angadi V, Kirszenblat L, Suzuki Y, Donlea J, Klose M, Koch Z, English D, Winsky-Sommerer R, vanSwinderen B, Shaw PJ. Sleep restores behavioral plasticity to Drosophila mutants. Current Biology. 2015;25:1270–1281. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2015.03.027. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Dus M, Lai JS-Y, Gunapala KM, Min S, Tayler TD, Hergarden AC, Geraud E, Joseph CM, Suh GSB. Nutrient sensor in the brain directs the action of the brain-gut axis in Drosophila. Neuron. 2015;87:139–151. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.05.032. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Ednick M, Cohen AP, McPhail GL, Beebe D, Simakajornboon N, Amin RS. A review of the effects of sleep during the first year of life on cognitive, psychomotor, and temperament development. Sleep. 2009;32:1449–1458. doi: 10.1093/sleep/32.11.1449. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Frank MG, Ruby NF, Heller HC, Franken P. Development of circadian sleep regulation in the rat: A longitudinal study under constant conditions. Sleep. 2017;40:zsw077. doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsw077. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Frank MG. The ontogenesis of mammalian sleep: Form and function. Current Sleep Medicine Reports. 2020;6:267–279. doi: 10.1007/s40675-020-00190-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Ghosh S, Körte A, Serafini G, Yadav V, Rodenfels J. Developmental energetics: Energy expenditure, budgets and metabolism during animal embryogenesis. Seminars in Cell & Developmental Biology. 2023;138:83–93. doi: 10.1016/j.semcdb.2022.03.009. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Hamada FN, Rosenzweig M, Kang K, Pulver SR, Ghezzi A, Jegla TJ, Garrity PA. An internal thermal sensor controlling temperature preference in Drosophila. Nature. 2008;454:217–220. doi: 10.1038/nature07001. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Hoffmire CA, Magyar CI, Connolly HV, Fernandez ID, van Wijngaarden E. High prevalence of sleep disorders and associated comorbidities in a community sample of children with Down syndrome. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine. 2014;10:411–419. doi: 10.5664/jcsm.3618. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Jacobs HT, George J, Kemppainen E. Regulation of growth in Drosophila melanogaster: the roles of mitochondrial metabolism. Journal of Biochemistry. 2020;167:267–277. doi: 10.1093/jb/mvaa002. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Jenett A, Rubin GM, Ngo T-TB, Shepherd D, Murphy C, Dionne H, Pfeiffer BD, Cavallaro A, Hall D, Jeter J, Iyer N, Fetter D, Hausenfluck JH, Peng H, Trautman ET, Svirskas RR, Myers EW, Iwinski ZR, Aso Y, DePasquale GM, Enos A, Hulamm P, Lam SCB, Li H-H, Laverty TR, Long F, Qu L, Murphy SD, Rokicki K, Safford T, Shaw K, Simpson JH, Sowell A, Tae S, Yu Y, Zugates CT. A GAL4-driver line resource for Drosophila neurobiology. Cell Reports. 2012;2:991–1001. doi: 10.1016/j.celrep.2012.09.011. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Jones CE, Opel RA, Kaiser ME, Chau AQ, Quintana JR, Nipper MA, Finn DA, Hammock EAD, Lim MM. Early-life sleep disruption increases parvalbumin in primary somatosensory cortex and impairs social bonding in prairie voles. Science Advances. 2019;5:eaav5188. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aav5188. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Konopka RJ, Benzer S. Clock mutants of Drosophila melanogaster. PNAS. 1971;68:2112–2116. doi: 10.1073/pnas.68.9.2112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Konrad C, Seehagen S, Schneider S, Herbert JS. Naps promote flexible memory retrieval in 12-month-old infants. Developmental Psychobiology. 2016;58:866–874. doi: 10.1002/dev.21431. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Kotagal S. Sleep in neurodevelopmental and neurodegenerative disorders. Seminars in Pediatric Neurology. 2015;22:126–129. doi: 10.1016/j.spen.2015.03.003. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Krakowiak P, Goodlin-Jones B, Hertz-Picciotto I, Croen LA, Hansen RL. Sleep problems in children with autism spectrum disorders, developmental delays, and typical development: a population-based study. Journal of Sleep Research. 2008;17:197–206. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00650.x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Levin R, Stern JM. Maternal influences on ontogeny of suckling and feeding rhythms in the rat. Journal of Comparative and Physiological Psychology. 1975;89:711–721. doi: 10.1037/h0077038. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Li X, Zhang C, Gong T, Ni X, Li J, Zhan D, Liu M, Song L, Ding C, Xu J, Zhen B, Wang Y, Qin J. A time-resolved multi-omic atlas of the developing mouse stomach. Nature Communications. 2018;9:4910. doi: 10.1038/s41467-018-07463-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Mendez N, Halabi D, Spichiger C, Salazar ER, Vergara K, Alonso-Vasquez P, Carmona P, Sarmiento JM, Richter HG, Seron-Ferre M, Torres-Farfan C. Gestational chronodisruption impairs circadian physiology in rat male offspring, increasing the risk of chronic disease. Endocrinology. 2016;157:4654–4668. doi: 10.1210/en.2016-1282. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Oh Y, Lai JSY, Min S, Huang HW, Liberles SD, Ryoo HD, Suh GSB. Periphery signals generated by Piezo-mediated stomach stretch and Neuromedin-mediated glucose load regulate the Drosophila brain nutrient sensor. Neuron. 2021;109:1979–1995. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2021.04.028. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Oh Y, Suh GSB. Starvation-induced sleep suppression requires the Drosophila brain nutrient sensor. Journal of Neurogenetics. 2023;37:70–77. doi: 10.1080/01677063.2023.2203489. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Poe AR, Xu Y, Zhang C, Lei J, Li K, Labib D, Han C. Low FoxO expression in Drosophila somatosensory neurons protects dendrite growth under nutrient restriction. eLife. 2020;9:e53351. doi: 10.7554/eLife.53351. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Poe AR, Mace KD, Kayser MS. Getting into rhythm: developmental emergence of circadian clocks and behaviors. The FEBS Journal. 2022;289:6576–6588. doi: 10.1111/febs.16157. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Poe AR, Zhu L, Szuperak M, McClanahan PD, Anafi RC, Scholl B, Thum AS, Cavanaugh DJ, Kayser MS. Developmental emergence of sleep rhythms enables long-term memory in Drosophila. Science Advances. 2023;9:eadh2301. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.adh2301. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Seehagen S, Konrad C, Herbert JS, Schneider S. Timely sleep facilitates declarative memory consolidation in infants. PNAS. 2015;112:1625–1629. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1414000112. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Sehgal A, Price J, Young MW. Ontogeny of a biological clock in Drosophila melanogaster. PNAS. 1992;89:1423–1427. doi: 10.1073/pnas.89.4.1423. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Smarr BL, Grant AD, Perez L, Zucker I, Kriegsfeld LJ. Maternal and early-life circadian disruption have long-lasting negative consequences on offspring development and adult behavior in mice. Scientific Reports. 2017;7:3326. doi: 10.1038/s41598-017-03406-4. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Stoleru D, Peng Y, Agosto J, Rosbash M. Coupled oscillators control morning and evening locomotor behaviour of Drosophila. Nature. 2004;431:862–868. doi: 10.1038/nature02926. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Sutton GM, Centanni AV, Butler AA. Protein Malnutrition during Pregnancy in C57BL/6J mice results in offspring with altered circadian physiology before obesity. Endocrinology. 2010;151:1570–1580. doi: 10.1210/en.2009-1133. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Szuperak M, Churgin MA, Borja AJ, Raizen DM, Fang-Yen C, Kayser MS. A sleep state in Drosophila larvae required for neural stem cell proliferation. eLife. 2018;7:e33220. doi: 10.7554/eLife.33220. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Toro-Ramos T, Paley C, Pi-Sunyer FX, Gallagher D. Body composition during fetal development and infancy through the age of 5 years. European Journal of Clinical Nutrition. 2015;69:1279–1289. doi: 10.1038/ejcn.2015.117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Vallone D, Lahiri K, Dickmeis T, Foulkes NS. Start the clock! Circadian rhythms and development. Developmental Dynamics. 2007;236:142–155. doi: 10.1002/dvdy.20998. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Varcoe TJ, Voultsios A, Gatford KL, Kennaway DJ. The impact of prenatal circadian rhythm disruption on pregnancy outcomes and long-term metabolic health of mice progeny. Chronobiology International. 2016;33:1171–1181. doi: 10.1080/07420528.2016.1207661. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Widmann A, Artinger M, Biesinger L, Boepple K, Peters C, Schlechter J, Selcho M, Thum AS. Genetic dissection of aversive associative olfactory learning and memory in Drosophila Larvae. PLOS Genetics. 2016;12:e1006378. doi: 10.1371/journal.pgen.1006378. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Wu Q, Wen T, Lee G, Park JH, Cai HN, Shen P. Developmental control of foraging and social behavior by the Drosophila neuropeptide Y-like system. Neuron. 2003;39:147–161. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(03)00396-9. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Yang Z, Huang R, Fu X, Wang G, Qi W, Mao D, Shi Z, Shen WL, Wang L. A post-ingestive amino acid sensor promotes food consumption in Drosophila. Cell Research. 2018;28:1013–1025. doi: 10.1038/s41422-018-0084-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Yao Z, Macara AM, Lelito KR, Minosyan TY, Shafer OT. Analysis of functional neuronal connectivity in the Drosophila brain. Journal of Neurophysiology. 2012;108:684–696. doi: 10.1152/jn.00110.2012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

eLife assessment

Gáspár Jékely 1

This study presents valuable findings as it shows that sleep rhythm formation and memory capabilities depend on a balanced and rich diet in fly larvae. The evidence supporting the claims of the authors is convincing with rigorous behavioral assays and state-of-the-art genetic manipulations. The work will be of interest to researchers working on sleep and memory.

Joint public review:

Anonymous

Summary:

This manuscript investigates how energetic demands affect the sleep-wake cycle in Drosophila larvae. L2 stage larvae do not show sleep rhythm and long-term memory (LTM), however, L3 larvae do. The authors manipulate food content to provide insufficient nutrition, which leads to more feeding, no LTM, and no sleep even in older larvae. Similarly, activation of NPF neurons suppresses sleep rhythm. Furthermore, they try to induce a sleep-like state using pharmacology or genetic manipulations in L2 larvae, which can mimic some of the L3 behaviours. A key experimental finding is that activation of DN1a neurons activates the downstream DH44 neurons, as assayed by GCaMP calcium imaging. This occurs only in the third instar and not in the second instar, in keeping with the development of sleep-wake and feeding separation. The authors also show that glucose metabolic genes are required in Dh44 neurons to develop sleep rhythm and that DH44 neurons respond differently in malnutrition or younger larvae.

Strengths:

Previous studies from the same lab have shown that sleep is required for LTM formation in the larvae, and that this requires DN1a and DH44 neurons. The current work builds upon this observation and addresses in more detail when and how this might develop. The authors can show that low quality food exposure and enhanced feeding during larval stage of Drosophila affects the formation of sleep rhythm and long-term memory. This suggests that the development of sleep and LTM are only possible under well fed and balanced nutrition in fly larvae. Non-sleep larvae were fed in low sugar conditions and indeed, the authors also find glucose metabolic genes to be required for a proper sleep rhythm. The paper presents precise genetic manipulations of individual classes of neurons in fly larvae followed by careful behavioural analysis. The authors also combine thermogenetic or peptide bath application experiments with direct calcium imaging of specific neurons.

Weaknesses:

The authors tried to induce sleep in younger L2 larvae with Gaboxadol feeding, however, the behavioral results suggest that they were not able to induce proper sleep behaviour as in normal L3 larvae.

Some of the genetic controls seem to be inconsistent. Given that the experiments were carried out in isogenized background, this is likely due to the high variability of some of the behaviours.

eLife. 2024 Jul 22;13:RP97256. doi: 10.7554/eLife.97256.3.sa2

Author response

Amy R Poe 1, Lucy Zhu 2, Si Hao Tang 3, Ella Valencia 4, Matthew S Kayser 5

The following is the authors’ response to the original reviews.

eLife assessment

This study presents valuable findings as it shows that sleep rhythm formation and memory capabilities depend on a balanced and rich diet in fly larvae. The evidence supporting the claims of the authors is convincing with rigorous behavioral assays and state-of-the-art genetic manipulations. The work will be of interest to researchers working on sleep and memory.

Public Reviews:

Summary:

This manuscript investigates how energetic demands affect the sleep-wake cycle in Drosophila larvae. L2 stage larvae do not show sleep rhythm and long-term memory (LTM), however, L3 larvae do. The authors manipulate food content to provide insufficient nutrition, which leads to more feeding, no LTM, and no sleep even in older larvae. Similarly, activation of NPF neurons suppresses sleep rhythm. Furthermore, they try to induce a sleep-like state using pharmacology or genetic manipulations in L2 larvae, which can mimic some of the L3 behaviours. A key experimental finding is that activation of DN1a neurons activate the downstream DH44 neurons, as assayed by GCaMP calcium imaging. This occurs only in third instar and not in second instar, in keeping with the development of sleep-wake and feeding separation. The authors also show that glucose metabolic genes are required in Dh44 neurons to develop sleep rhythm and that DH44 neurons respond differently in malnutrition or younger larvae.

Strengths:

Previous studies from the same lab have shown the sleep is required for LTM formation in the larvae, and that this requires DN1a and DH44 neurons. The current work builds upon this observation and addresses in more detail when and how this might develop. The authors can show that low quality food exposure and enhanced feeding during larval stage of Drosophila affects the formation of sleep rhythm and long-term memory. This suggests that the development of sleep and LTM are only possible under well fed and balanced nutrition in fly larvae. Non-sleep larvae were fed in low sugar conditions and indeed, the authors also find glucose metabolic genes to be required for a proper sleep rhythm. The paper presents precise genetic manipulations of individual classes of neurons in fly larvae followed by careful behavioural analysis. The authors also combine thermogenetic or peptide bath application experiments with direct calcium imaging of specific neurons.

Weaknesses:

The authors tried to induce sleep in younger L2 larvae, however the behavioral results suggest that they were not able to induce proper sleep behaviour as in normal L3 larvae. Thus, they cannot show that sleep during L2 stage would be sufficient to form LTM.

We agree that the experiments with Gaboxadol feeding in L2 did not perfectly mimic L3 sleep behaviors. However, genetic induction of sleep in L2 was effective in increasing sleep duration and depth similar to that observed in normal L3. As noted below in response to specific reviewer comments, because gaboxadol feeding is standard in the field for adult sleep induction, we prefer to still include this data in the manuscript for transparency. Moreover, the gaboxadol manipulation did cause a significant decrease in arousal threshold compared to control larvae. Together these approaches support the hypothesis that sleeping more/more deeply is not sufficient to promote LTM in L2.

The authors suggest that larval Dh44 neurons may integrate "information about the nutritional environment through the direct sensing of glucose levels to modulate sleep-wake rhythm development". They identify glucose metabolism genes (e.g., Glut1) in the downstream DH44 neurons as being required for the organization of the sleep-wake-feeding rhythm, and that CCHa signaling in DN1a signaling to the DH44 cells via the receptor. However, how this is connected is not well explained. Do the authors think that the nutrient sensing is only occurring in the DH44 neurons and not in DN1a or other neurons? Would not knocking down glucose metabolism in any neuron lead to a functional defect? What is the evidence that Dh44 neurons are specific sensors of nutritional state? For example, do the authors think that e.g. the overexpression of Glut1 in Dh44 neurons, a manipulation that can increase transport of glucose into cells, would rescue the effects of low-sugar food?

We thank the reviewer for these suggestions and have added the experiment proposed. We found that knockdown of Hex-C in DN1a neurons did not disrupt sleep-wake rhythms (Fig. S4G-I) suggesting that Dh44 neurons are specialized in requiring glucose metabolism to drive sleep-wake rhythms. We have also added further clarification in the text regarding the existing evidence that Dh44 neurons act has nutrient sensors.

Some of the genetic controls seem to be inconsistent suggesting some genetic background effects. In Figure 2B, npf-gal4 flies without the UAS show no significant circadian change in sleep duration, whereas UAS-TrpA flies do. The genetic control data in Figure 2D are also inconsistent. Npf-Gal4 seems to have some effect by itself without the UAS. The same is not seen with R76G11-Gal4. Suppl Fig 2: Naïve OCT and AM preference in L3 expressing various combinations of the transgenes show significant differences. npf-Gal4 alone seems to influence preference.

The sleep duration and bout number/length data are highly variable.

All experiments are performed in isogenized background so variability seen in genetic controls likely reflects stochastic nature of behavioral experiments. Indeed, adult sleep data also shows a great deal of variability within the same genetic background (PMID: 29228366). We agree it is an important point, and we attempt to minimize variability as much as possible with backcrossing of flies and tight control of environmental conditions.

Recommendations for the authors:

Reviewer #1 (Recommendations For The Authors):

Low sugar exposure and activation of NPF neurons might not induce the same behavioral changes. LS exposure does not enhance mouth hook movements, but overall food intake. NPF activation seems to enhance mouth hook movements, but the data for food intake is not shown. This information would be necessary to compare the two different manipulations.

We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. However, we elected not to perform food intake experiments with the NPF activation experiments. Since we are not directly comparing the low sugar and NPF manipulations to each other, we think that both experiments together support the conclusion that immature food acquisition strategies (whether food intake or feeding rate) limit LTM performance.

The authors write that the larval feeding assays run for 4 hours, can they explain why that long? Larvae should already have processed food within 4 hours, so that the measurement would not include all eaten food.

We clarified the rationale for doing 4 hour feeding assays in the results section. We did 4 hours on blue dyed food because initial experiments of 1 hour with control L3 at CT1-4 were difficult to interpret. The measurement does not include all of the eaten food in the 4 hours but does reflect more long-term changes in food intake.

Sleep induction with Gaboxadol seems to not really work - sleep duration, bout number and length are not enhanced, and arousal threshold is only slightly lower. Thus, the authors should not use this data as an example for inducing sleep behaviour.

We agree this approach did not have a large effect in larvae. However, because gaboxadol feeding is standard in the field for adult sleep induction, we prefer to still include this data in the manuscript for transparency. Moreover, the Gaboxadol manipulation did cause a mild (but significant) decrease in arousal threshold compared to control larvae. Gaboxadol feeding also caused a significant decrease in total body weight compared to control larvae indicating that even slightly deeper sleep could be detrimental to younger animals.

Activation of R76G11 with TrpA1 seems to work better for inducing sleep like behaviour. However, the authors describe that they permanently activated neurons. To induce a "normal" sleep pattern, the authors might try to only activate these neurons during the normal enhanced sleep time in L3 (CT13?) and not during the whole day. This might also allow larvae to eat during day time and gain more weight.

We apologize that this point was not clearer, but we did do acute activation of R76G11(+) neurons, as proposed by the reviewer. We have clarified the text to make this point.

It would be interesting to see how larvae fed with high sucrose and low protein diet would behave in this assay. Do the authors suggest that sugar is most important for the development of sleep behaviour or that it is a combination of sugar and protein that might be required?

We agree that feeding larvae a high sucrose and low protein diet would be interesting. However, we initially tried a low protein diet and observed significant developmental delays. Therefore, we are concerned that developmental defects on a high sucrose and low protein diet would confound behavioral results. Additionally, the Dh44 manipulations (glucose & GCN2 signaling) suggest that sugar is the most important for the development of sleep behaviors.

Reviewer #3 (Recommendations For The Authors):

The authors could discuss if the interaction between DN1a clock neurons and Dh44 neurons is mediated synaptic or by volume transmission following the extracellular release of the CCHa1 neuropeptide. They write that "the development of Dh44 neuronal competency to receive clock-driven cues" and that "DN1a clock neurons anatomically and functionally connect to Dh44" but a discussion about volume vs. synaptic signalling would be of interest.

We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We revised the discussion to address this point.

line 223 " demonstrating that post-synaptic processes likely". It would be interesting to read a discussion on whether it is known if these are postsynaptic or peptide-mediated volume effects?

We added additional text to the discussion to address these points.

- The authors may want to include a schematic of the circuit and how its position in the general anatomy of the fly larva.

We thank the reviewer for this suggestion. We have added a model figure to Fig. S6.

"Dh44 neurons act through glucose metabolic genes" - consider rewording e.g. require glucose metabolic genes

We revised the text.

- line 45 "Early in development, young animals must obtain enough nutrients to ensure proper growth" - this is too general, many animals do not feed in early life-cycle stages (e.g. lecitotrophic development), consider rewording

We revised the text to be more specific.

- line 90 "however, L3 at CT1 consume more than L3 at CT12 (Figure S1A)" - typo CT13, also consider rewording to match the structure of the sentence before 'however, L3 consumed more at CT1 than at CT13'

We revised the text to fix this error.

- Line 111 "and loss of deep sleep" - how is deep sleep defined and measured in the larvae? It is not clear from the data or the text.

We revised the text to define deep sleep in the results section. We also have a description of how arousal threshold is calculated in the methods.

- In Figure 3B and G the individual data points are not shown

We did not show individual data points for those graphs because we are plotting the average percentage of 4 biological replicates.

Typo:

Figure 1 legend "F, n = n=100-172 "

We revised the text to fix this typo.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    Figure 1—source data 1. Raw values for sleep and feeding in genetic and nutritional manipulations.
    Figure 1—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Measures of all sleep and developmental parameters on L.S. food.
    Figure 2—source data 1. Sleep and LTM values for npf manipulations at 30c.
    Figure 2—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Temperature control data for npf manipulations.
    Figure 3—source data 1. Sleep, LTM, and developmental data for L2 manipulations.
    Figure 3—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Temperature control and naive preference data for L2 manipulations.
    Figure 4—source data 1. Individual cell fluorescence data for P2X2 and CCHa1 imaging.
    Figure 5—source data 1. Total sleep duration measures for Dh44 metabolism manipulations in L2 and L3.
    Figure 5—figure supplement 1—source data 1. Complete sleep data for Dh44 glucose metabolism manipulations.
    Figure 5—figure supplement 2—source data 1. Raw values for amino acid sensing manipulations in Dh44 neurons.
    MDAR checklist
    Source code 1. Larval sleep analysis codes.
    elife-97256-code1.zip (184.6KB, zip)

    Data Availability Statement

    All data generated or analysed during this study are included in the manuscript and supporting files. Source data files have been provided for all Figures.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES