Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2024 May 1;183(8):3199–3210. doi: 10.1007/s00431-024-05578-4

Effect of fractional exhaled nitric oxide (FENO)-based asthma management during pregnancy versus usual care on infant development, temperament, sensory function and autism signs

Olivia M Whalen 1,2,3, Linda E Campbell 1,2,3, Alison E Lane 4, Frini Karayanidis 1,2,3, Carly A Mallise 3,5,6, Alix J Woolard 7, Elizabeth G Holliday 3,6, Joerg Mattes 3,6,8,9, Adam Collison 3,6,9, Peter G Gibson 10,3,6,9, Vanessa E Murphy 3,6,9,
PMCID: PMC11263417  PMID: 38691129

Abstract

Asthma during pregnancy is associated with a range of adverse perinatal outcomes. It is also linked to increased rates of neurodevelopmental conditions in the offspring. We aimed to assess whether fractional exhaled nitric oxide (FENO)-based asthma management during pregnancy improves child developmental and behavioural outcomes compared to usual care. The Breathing for Life Trial was a randomised controlled trial that compared FENO-based asthma management during pregnancy to usual care. Participants were invited to the developmental follow-up, the Breathing for Life Trial – Infant Development study, which followed up infants at 6 weeks, 6 months and 12 months. The primary outcomes were measured in infants at 12 months using the Bayley-III: Cognitive, Language and Motor composite scores. Secondary outcomes included Bayley-III social-emotional and adaptive behaviour scores, autism likelihood and sensory and temperament outcomes. The exposure of interest was the randomised intervention group. Two hundred and twenty-two infants and their 217 participating mothers were recruited to the follow-up; 107 mothers were in the intervention group and 113 were in the control group. There was no evidence of an intervention effect for the primary outcomes: Bayley-III cognitive (mean = 108.9 control, 108.5 intervention, p = 0.93), language (mean = 95.9 control, 95.6 intervention, p = 0.87) and motor composite scores (mean = 97.2 control, 97.9 intervention, p = 0.25). Mean scores for secondary outcomes were also similar among infants born to control and FENO group mothers, with few results reaching p < 0.05.

Conclusion: In this sample, FENO-guided asthma treatment during pregnancy did not improve infant developmental outcomes in the first year of life.

Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: ACTRN12613000202763.

What is Known:

• Maternal asthma during pregnancy has been associated with increased rates of neurodevelopmental conditions in offspring, including intellectual disability and autism.

What is New:

• This is the first study to examine how managing asthma during pregnancy via a FENO-guided algorithm or usual care affects infant developmental and behavioural outcomes. While the results of the study showed no impact of the intervention, and therefore do not support the integration of FENO-based management of asthma in antenatal settings for optimal infant development, they do send a positive message about the implications of active asthma management during pregnancy on infant developmental outcomes.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s00431-024-05578-4.

Keywords: Asthma, Pregnancy, Postpartum, FENO, Child development, Temperament, Sensory processing, Autism

Introduction

Asthma is one of the most common chronic illnesses to affect pregnant women [1, 2], affecting about 12–13% of pregnant women in Australia [2, 3]. Maternal asthma is linked to various perinatal complications [48] and adverse perinatal outcomes [911]. However, active asthma management during pregnancy has been shown to reduce some of these risks [9, 10, 12, 13], suggesting that managing asthma during pregnancy could potentially modify its impact on pregnancy outcomes.

Maternal asthma during pregnancy has also been associated with increased rates of neurodevelopmental conditions in offspring. Some studies suggest a link between maternal asthma and an increased likelihood of autism [14] and intellectual disability [15, 16] in children. Autistic children whose mothers had immune conditions, including asthma, have been found to experience more behavioural and emotional challenges [17]. However, not all studies have found such associations [e.g. 1821], with some reporting no negative developmental outcomes when maternal asthma is well-managed during pregnancy [22].

No studies have explored the specific effects of different asthma medications or asthma management strategies on cognitive and behavioural outcomes in children. FENO (fractional exhaled nitric oxide)-based asthma management is a suitable method of assessing airway inflammation in asthmatic pregnant women [23] and has been shown to significantly reduce exacerbations in asthmatic pregnant women [24] and improve the respiratory health of offspring [25, 26]. This highlights the need for more comprehensive research in this area, to identify children who may be predisposed to poorer outcomes in later childhood. Early, targeted support can be provided when it can have the most benefit to children and their families.

Aim and hypotheses

In this study, we examined child developmental outcomes in infants born to mothers whose asthma was managed during pregnancy by a FENO-guided algorithm (intervention group) versus those receiving standard asthma care (control group), as a substudy of the Breathing for Life Trial [BLT; 27, 28]. A range of measures was used to assess development, sensory function, temperament and likelihood for autism at three timepoints in the first year of the infant’s life: 6 weeks, 6 months and 12 months of age. We hypothesised that the infants of pregnant women in the intervention group would experience better developmental outcomes in their first year, compared to infants of pregnant women in the control group.

Methods

Participants

Two hundred and twenty infants and their 217 participating mothers were recruited as part of the Breathing for Life Trial – Infant Development (BLT-ID) study. The BLT-ID study was a longitudinal follow-up of infants following their mother’s participation in The Breathing for Life Trial [27, 28], a randomised controlled trial (RCT) that assessed the effect of FENO-guided asthma management during pregnancy at the John Hunter Hospital site in Newcastle, Australia. Postpartum, BLT participants were given the option of participating in the BLT-ID study during their 6-week, 6-month or 12-month BLT infant follow-up appointment. Participants could complete one, two or three sessions of the BLT-ID protocol (Fig. 1).

Fig. 1.

Fig. 1

Consolidated Standards of Reporting Trials diagram. FENO: fractional exhaled nitric oxide. Note. Not all participants from the parent Breathing for Life Trial were invited to participate in the follow-up Breathing for Life – Infant Development substudy. Only those who attended their 6 week (or 6-month or 12-month appointment) postnatal follow-up appointment for the BLT from June 2015 through to September 2018 were invited to participate in BLT-ID. *includes n = 11 participants who provided data for the 6-week and 12-month timepoints only (skipped 6-month timepoint)

All participants gave written informed consent for participation. This research was conducted in accordance with the National Health and Medical Research Council (NHMRC)’s National Statement on Ethical Conduct in Human Research [29]. Ethics approval was obtained through the Hunter New England Local Health District Research ethics committee (Reference number: 15/05/20/4.05; HREC/15/HNE/164), and The University of Newcastle Human Ethics Committee (Reference number: H-2015-0307), while site-specific approval was also sought (Reference number: SSA/15/HNE/196).

Inclusion and exclusion criteria

For the parent trial BLT, participants were recruited from antenatal appointments at a gestational age of 12–23 weeks (supported by a dated ultrasound or clinical assessment). Women were invited to participate if they were aged over 18 years, had an asthma diagnosis from a physician, had asthma symptoms and received treatment for asthma in the last 12-months and were able to perform spirometry and FENO assessments. Exclusion criteria included concomitant chronic illness that may affect participation (such as heart disease), other lung diseases and drug or alcohol dependence. Further information on primary outcomes and measures of the BLT RCT have been published elsewhere [27].

Procedure

After confirming eligibility, BLT participants were randomly allocated to either a control (asthma managed as usual) or intervention (fractional exhaled nitric oxide; FENO-based management) group. Neither participants nor researchers were blinded to treatment group due to the differences between the intervention (regular visits during pregnancy) and usual care (one visit only). Women who were allocated to the usual care group of the trial received an asthma assessment and education on how to self-manage their asthma and did not receive any trial-specified treatment adjustment. Any changes to their treatment were managed by their general practitioner only and not the researchers. Mothers randomly allocated to receive the FENO-based management intervention had their asthma assessed every 3–6 weeks during pregnancy and their ICS dose adjusted every 2nd visit using the FENO algorithm, based on their exhaled nitric oxide measure and symptoms. Regardless of intervention status, women continued their usual antenatal appointments. There was no placebo group.

The parent trial showed no significant difference between the usual care control group and the FENO-based management intervention group on perinatal outcomes. However, in the intervention group compared to control, there was a non-significant reduction in exacerbations requiring medical intervention of 20% (OR 0.80, 95% CI 0.58, 1.10).

Measures

Baseline pregnancy and birth measures

Baseline prenatal data, including maternal date of birth, estimated date of birth, postcode and smoking status were collected between 12- and 23-week gestation (Table 1). Socioeconomic status was determined via the Socioeconomic Index For Areas (SEIFA) approach, where residential postcode was used as an index of relative socioeconomic status. Participants were assigned a quintile, with lower quintiles (first, second) reflecting more socioeconomic disadvantage. Infant sex and date of birth, gestational age at birth, preterm status and birthweight information was collected from medical records.

Table 1.

Measures used at pregnancy, birth and postpartum timepoints

Pregnancy Birth Postpartum
Measure Baselinea 6-weeks 6-months 12-months
Demographic data
  – Maternal DOB
  – EDB
  – Postcode (SEIFA)
  – Smoking status
Demographic data
  – Infant DOB
  – Infant sex
  – Gestational age at birth
  – Birthweight
Demographic data
  – Maternal country of birth
  – Primiparity
  – Relationship statusb
  – Feeding statusb
  – Highest level of education (maternal & partner)b
  – Current occupation status (maternal & partner)b
  – Annual incomeb
  – Mental health conditionb  
Sensory processing (Sensory Profile 2) (ISP-2) (ISP-2) (TSP-2)
Temperament (Carey Temperament Scales) (EITQ) (RITQ) (TTS)
Autism signs (First Year Inventory 2.0)
Development (Bayley Scales of Infant and Toddler Development, 3rd Edition) (Bayley-III Screening Test) (Full Bayley-III Test)
Social and emotional development (Bayley-III SEABQ)

aBaseline refers to 12–23 weeks’ gestation

bDemographic information which may have changed (relationship status, current breastfeeding status, level of education, current occupation status, annual income and presence of a mental health condition) was collected again at 6 and 12-months postpartum

Postpartum measures

Demographic characteristics

At 6 weeks postpartum, further demographic data was collected via self-report including maternal country of birth, parity, relationships status, current breastfeeding (breastfed, formula fed or both), level of education and current occupation status, partner’s level of education and current occupation status, household gross annual income and presence of a mental health condition. Demographic information that could change over time (e.g. relationship status) was collected again at 6 and 12-months postpartum.

Clinical characteristics

Infants that were born before 37-weeks gestation were defined as being born prematurely. Infants of parents that answered “yes” to infants being either breastfed or both breastfed and formula fed were defined as being breastfed. Mothers that answered “yes” to having a mental health condition or reported taking medication for the purpose of treating a mental health condition were defined as having a mental health condition. Psychometric properties and further details for the following development measures are provided in the supplement.

Sensory processing

Sensory processing was assessed via the parent-report Sensory Profile 2 (SP2; [30]). Age-appropriate versions were employed: Infant Sensory Profile 2 (ISP2) at 6 weeks and 6 months and Toddler Sensory Profile (TSP2) at 12 months. Scores are produced for eight processing domains: general, auditory, visual, touch, movement, oral, behavioural (TSP2 only) and total score (ISP2 only). Higher scores reflect more sensory behaviours observed. Additionally, the TSP2 provides scores for four sensory quadrants: seeking/seeker (sensory stimulation seeking), avoiding/avoider (sensory stimulation avoidance), sensitivity/sensor (sensory event awareness) and registration/bystander (sensory event detection).

Temperament

Temperament was assessed using age-appropriate parent-report questionnaires from the Carey Temperament Scales (CTS); Early Infancy Temperament Questionnaire (EITQ; [31]) at 6 weeks; Revised Infant Temperament Questionnaire (RITQ; [32]) at 6 months; Toddler Temperament Scale (TTS; [33]) at 12 months. These measures yield mean scores for nine temperament domains: activity (motor activity; higher scores indicate more activity); rhythmicity (bodily function predictability; higher scores imply less predictability); approach (acceptance/withdrawal of novel situations, objects or people; higher scores indicate more withdrawal); adaptability (compliance with change; higher scores indicate slower adaptation); intensity (energy of responses; higher scores indicate more intense responses); mood (positive and negative emotions; higher scores indicate more negative mood); distractibility (attention diversion by stimuli; higher scores suggest less distractibility for infants under 12 months and more for those aged 12–24 months); persistence (the time activities are pursued and continued when distractions are present; higher scores indicate less persistence) and threshold (stimulus intensity needed to evoke a response; higher scores signify greater sensory sensitivity); [34]. Higher scores indicate more challenging temperament behaviours, while lower scores reflect a more manageable temperament.

Autism likelihood

An infant’s likelihood for autism was measured with the parent-report First Year Inventory 2.0 (FYI; [35, 36]). The FYI is a validated, general population-screening tool to identify 12-month-old infants who show characteristics of autism or related developmental conditions. Scores are calculated for the social communication and sensory regulation domains, and an overall total score. Scores ≥ 22.5 for social communication, ≥ 14.75 for sensory regulation and/or ≥ 19.2 for the total score suggest infants have an elevated likelihood for autism.

Cognitive, language, motor, socioemotional and adaptive behaviour development

At 6 months, infants completed The Bayley-III Scales of Infant and Toddler Development [37] screening test. The Bayley Screening test is designed to quickly identify infants at risk of delays in cognitive, language and motor development. Results are presented as raw scores for each domain and categorised as follows: “At risk” indicates potential delay, “Emerging” suggests age-appropriate skills emerging, and “Competent” signifies age-appropriate competence.

At 12 months, infants underwent the full Bayley-III developmental assessment, which includes cognitive, language (receptive and expressive) and motor (fine and gross) subscales. Performance is compared to a standardisation group, with raw scores adjusted for gestational age. Receptive and expressive language scores form a language composite, while fine and gross motor scores create an overall motor composite, both with a mean of 100 (±15). Higher scores are reflective of better developmental ability.

The parent-report Bayley-III Socioemotional and Adaptive Behaviour Questionnaire (Bayley SEABQ) were used at 6 and 12 months to gauge infants’ social and emotional development and daily living skills at home and elsewhere (adaptive behaviour). Scores cover seven skill areas for children aged under one year and 10 skill areas for children aged 1–4 years, spanning communication, community use, functional pre-academics, home living, health and safety, leisure, self-care, self-direction, social and motor skills. Like the Bayley-III assessment, performance aligns with a standardisation group. Normative composite scores have a mean of 100 (±15); higher scores are reflective of better developmental ability. These contribute to a general adaptive composite (GAC) score and scores for conceptual, social and practical domains of adaptive behaviour.

Analysis

Primary outcomes

Three continuous primary outcomes were pre-specified and were all measured in infants at 12 months using the Bayley-III: cognitive composite score; language composite score and motor composite score.

Secondary outcomes

The following measures were obtained from the Bayley-III SEABQ at 6 months and 12 months: Social-Emotional Composite score; General Adaptive Composite (GAC) score; Adaptive Behaviour: Conceptual score; Adaptive Behaviour: Social score; Adaptive Behaviour: Practical score.

From the Bayley-III Screening test, 10 secondary outcomes were specified. The five continuous outcomes were Cognitive score; Receptive language score; Expressive language score; Fine motor score; and Gross motor score. The five categorical outcomes were: Cognitive risk (at risk/emerging/competent); Receptive language risk (at risk/emerging/competent); Expressive language risk (at risk/emerging/competent); Fine motor risk (at risk/emerging/competent) and Gross motor risk (at risk/emerging/competent).

At each timepoint, a measure of each of the following nine temperament domains was obtained (9 × 3 = 27 outcomes): Activity; Rhythmicity; Approach; Adaptability; Intensity; Mood; Persistence; Distractibility; and Threshold.

Sensory processing outcomes at 6 weeks and 6 months were measured across the following seven domains (7 × 2 = 14 outcomes): General; Auditory; Visual; Touch; Movement; Oral; and Total Processing (derived from the seven domain scores). At 12-months, scores were measured across the following seven domains: General; Auditory; Visual; Touch; Movement; Oral; Behavioural and four quadrants: Sensation Seeking; Sensation Avoidance; Sensation Sensitivity and Low Registration. The four quadrant scores are derived from the domain scores.

Three continuous measures were assessed for likelihood for autism: FYI social communication domain score; FYI sensory regulatory score and FYI total score. Three binary measures were also assessed, reflecting whether infants had a high likelihood of autism based on the FYI continuous scores: autism likelihood high (social communication score ≥ 22.5); autism likelihood high (sensory regulation score ≥ 14.75); autism likelihood high (FYI total score ≥ 19.2).

Exposure and explanatory variables

The exposure of interest was participation in the intervention. Potentially confounding variables included birthweight, gestational age, maternal smoking status, maternal socioeconomic status (expressed as SEIFA quintile by postcode), infant sex, preterm birth, breastfeeding and the presence of any mental health condition in the mother (Fig. S2: Directed Acyclic Graph. Supplemental data). Potential confounding of intervention effect estimates was of interest due to self-selection by mothers into this substudy, and the potential for selection bias to influence covariate balance between groups.

Statistical analysis

Data were analysed on an intention to treat basis, by classifying infants into the intervention group their mother was randomised to. Baseline participant characteristics were summarised by intervention group using mean with standard deviation (SD) for continuous variables and frequency with percent for categorical variables.

Intervention effects were estimated using two models for each outcome. The first model adjusted only for the randomisation stratification variable of maternal smoking; the second additionally adjusted for potentially confounding variables of any breastfeeding at six weeks (yes/no), SEIFA quintile, birthweight (continuous) and preterm birth (yes/no). Since all sensitivity analyses produced similar effect estimates to the minimally adjusted model, results from the minimally adjusted model are reported here.

For continuous outcomes, the intervention effect was estimated using linear regression. Results were expressed as the least square mean difference between groups, presented as beta coefficients with 95% confidence interval (CI) and p-values.

Categorical Bayley-III (developmental) outcomes were converted to binary variables for analysis, combining “At risk” with “Emerging”, and comparing this combined group with “Competent”, based on low frequencies in the “At risk” category. Intervention effects for these binary outcomes were estimated using logistic regression, with results presented as odds ratios (OR) with 95% CI and p-values.

Given the exploratory nature of the study, a significance threshold of 0.05 was used for all outcomes. Given potential type I error inflation by multiple testing, two Bayesian quantities were also estimated and reported for each outcome: (i) Bayes factor (BF01), representing the Bayes factor in favour of the null hypothesis (H0) of no intervention effect; and (ii) the posterior probability of H0. Bayes factors were estimated using the BIC approximation and interpretation described by Wagenmakers [38], with BF01 values of 1–3, 3–20 and 20–150 considered to provide weak, positive and strong evidence in favour of H0, respectively. The posterior probability of H0 was estimated as BF011+BF01 [38] and ranges from 0–1, with higher values providing greater evidence for the null hypothesis.

Given low rates of missing data, all analyses were conducted as complete case analyses, assuming data were missing completely at random (MCAR). Data management and statistical analyses were performed using Stata V16, SAS V9.4 and JASP 0.16.3.0.

Results

Sample characteristics

Two hundred twenty-two infants (112 male; N = 145 6 weeks, N = 116 6-month and N = 122 12 months) and their 217 mothers participated in the present study. Of the 217 mothers, 213 contributed data for a single infant and 3 mothers each contributed two infants from twin births. Of the 220 infants, 107 were born to mothers randomised to the FENO algorithm and 113 were born to mothers randomised to the control group. Distributions for demographic and explanatory variables were similar across the two groups at each timepoint (Table 2). Sample characteristics by intervention group for key explanatory variables is presented in Supplementary data (6-month Bayley outcomes: Table 1; 12-month Bayley outcomes: Table 2).

Table 2.

Demographic data by intervention group

Outcome Control
M (SD)
FENO
M (SD)
Total
M (SD)
P Value
Birthweight (g) 3397.9 (538.8) 3311.7 (578.5) 3353.6 (559.9) 0.26
Gestational age (w) 38.9 (1.7) 38.8 (1.7) 38.9 (1.7) 0.62
Control
N (%)
FENO
N (%)
Total
N (%)
SEIFA quintile
  1 10 (9) 13 (12) 23 (11) 0.30
  2 20 (19) 16 (15) 36 (17)
  3 43 (41) 57 (52) 100 (46)
  4 32 (30) 24 (22) 56 (26)
  5 1 (1) 0 (0) 1 (0)
Infant sex
  Male 56 (52) 56 (50) 112 (51) 0.68
  Female 51 (48) 57 (50) 108 (49)
Preterm
  No 97 (91) 100 (88) 197 (90) 0.60
  Yes 10 (9) 13 (12) 23 (10)
Breastfed at 6-weeks
  No 20 (26) 20 (26) 40 (26) 0.92
  Yes 58 (74) 56 (74) 114 (74)
Breastfed at 6-months
  No 21 (45) 16 (34) 37 (39) 0.29
  Yes 26 (55) 31 (66) 57 (61)
Breastfed at 12-months
  No 14 (50) 26 (49) 40 (49) 0.94
  Yes 14 (50) 27 (51) 41 (51)

Intervention effect estimates for primary outcomes

For all three primary outcomes, mean scores were similar among infants born to Control or FENO group mothers (control vs FENO: cognitive 108.9 vs 108.5, coefficient (95% CI): − 0.5 (− 4.9, 3.9), p = 0.83; language 95.9 vs 95.6, coefficient (95% CI) =  − 0.2 (− 4.1–3.6), p = 0.90; motor 97.2 vs 97.9, coefficient (95% CI) = 0.7 (− 3.8–5.1), p = 0.77; Table 3). All Bayes factors were between 3 and 20 (range 10.55–10.91), providing “positive” evidence for the null hypothesis of no intervention effect for all outcomes. The posterior probability of H0 was above 0.9 for all outcomes, providing further support for all null hypotheses. Results were similar in sensitivity analyses adjusting for additional potential confounding variables (Table 3 – Supplementary data).

Table 3.

Effect estimates for Bayley-III at 12-months (primary outcomes)

Outcome Control mean (SD) FENO mean (SD) Coefficient (95% CI)FENO - Control P Value Bayes Factor Posterior probability of H0
Cognitive Composite 108.9 (11.1) 108.5 (12.8) -0.5 ( -4.9, 3.9) 0.83 10.81 0.92
Language Composite 95.9 (11.7) 95.6 (10.0) -0.2 ( -4.1, 3.6) 0.90 10.91 0.92
Motor Composite 97.2 (12.8) 97.9 (12.1) 0.7 ( -3.8, 5.1) 0.77 10.55 0.91

Secondary outcomes

Bayley-III outcomes at 12 months

For all five secondary outcomes measured at 12 months, mean scores were similar among infants born to Control or FENO group mothers (control vs FENO: Social-Emotional Composite 99.1 vs 104.7, coefficient (95% CI) = 5.7 (− 2.8–14.2), p = 0.19; General Adaptive Composite 105.9 vs 104.6, coefficient (95% CI) =  − 1.0 (− 7.1–5.0), p = 0.74; Adaptive Behaviour: Conceptual 107.1 vs 105.7, coefficient (95% CI) =  − 1.2 (− 7.2–4.7), p = 0.68; Adaptive Behaviour: Social 110.4 vs 111.8, coefficient (95% CI) = 1.6 (− 4.3–7.5), p = 0.60; Adaptive Behaviour: Practical 99.0 vs 96.5, coefficient (95% CI) =  − 2.6 (− 8.4–3.2), p = 0.38; Table 4). All Bayes Factors were between 3 and 20 (range 3.75–7.48), providing “positive” evidence for the null hypothesis of no intervention effect for all outcomes. The posterior probability of H0 was above 0.79 for all outcomes, providing further support for all null hypotheses. Results were similar in sensitivity analyses (Table 4 – Supplementary data).

Table 4.

Effect estimates for Bayley-III at 12-months (secondary outcomes)

Outcome Control mean (SD) FENO mean (SD) Coefficient (95% CI)FENO - Control P Value Bayes Factor Posterior probability of H0
Social-Emotional Composite 99.1 (19.9) 104.7 (17.3) 5.7 (-2.8, 14.2) 0.19 3.75 0.79
General Adaptive Composite (GAC) 105.9 (13.9) 104.6 (11.0) -1.0 (-7.1, 5.0) 0.74 7.44 0.88
Adaptive Behaviour: Conceptual 107.1 (13.6) 105.7 (11.8) -1.2 (-7.2, 4.7) 0.68 7.48 0.88
Adaptive Behaviour: Social 110.4 (15.7) 111.8 (10.0) 1.6 (-4.3, 7.5) 0.60 7.31 0.88
Adaptive Behaviour: Practical 99.0 (12.7) 96.5 (10.9) -2.6 (-8.4, 3.2) 0.38 5.52 0.85

Bayley-III outcomes at 6 months

Most continuous outcomes showed negligible differences between groups (Table 5). Infants of FENO mothers scored higher on the General Adaptive Composite (GAC) than infants of Control mothers (113.2 vs 108.8, p = 0.05). Bayes factors were between 1 and 3 for three outcomes whose p-values were in the range 0.05–0.1 (General Adaptive Composite, Adaptive Behaviour: Conceptual and Adaptive Behaviour: Social), providing “weak” evidence for the null hypothesis of no intervention effect. Bayes Factors were between 3 and 20 for all other outcomes, providing “positive” evidence for the null hypothesis of no intervention effect. The posterior probability of H0 ranged from 0.55 to 0.91 across outcomes, providing further support for all null hypotheses. Results were similar in sensitivity analyses (Table 5 – Supplementary data) and for analyses of the categorical outcomes (Tables 6 & 7 – Supplementary data).

Table 5.

Effect estimates for Bayley-III at 6-months (continuous)

Outcome Control mean (SD) FENO mean (SD) Coefficient (95% CI)FENO - Control P Value Bayes Factor Posterior probability of H0
Cognitive 10.7 (2.5) 10.9 (2.4) 0.2 (-0.7, 1.1) 0.63 9.58 0.91
Receptive language 7.8 (1.8) 7.9 (1.7) 0.0 (-0.6, 0.7) 0.90 10.64 0.91
Expressive language 6.8 (2.3) 6.5 (1.7) -0.2 (-1.0, 0.5) 0.53 8.81 0.90
Fine motor 9.0 (1.5) 8.8 (1.7) -0.3 (-0.8, 0.3) 0.39 7.41 0.88
Gross motor 9.0 (1.9) 8.9 (2.2) -0.1 (-0.8, 0.7) 0.85 10.49 0.91
Social-Emotional Composite 105.0 (12.7) 104.8 (17.9) 0.0 (-6.6, 6.6) 0.99 9.27 0.90
General Adaptive Composite (GAC) 108.8 (13.7) 113.2 (9.6) 5.3 (0.1, 10.6) 0.05 1.25 0.55
Adaptive Behaviour: Conceptual 106.3 (14.3) 110.2 (11.9) 4.9 (-0.8, 10.7) 0.09 2.19 0.69
Adaptive Behaviour: Social 111.7 (10.9) 114.8 (8.2) 4.0 (-0.2, 8.2) 0.06 1.62 0.62
Adaptive Behaviour: Practical 105.4 (12.4) 107.3 (9.7) 2.2 (-2.7, 7.2) 0.38 6.01 0.86

Effect estimates for temperament and sensory outcomes and autism likelihood

All temperament outcomes across all 3 timepoints showed negligible differences between groups (Table 8 – Supplementary data). Results were similar in sensitivity analyses adjusting for additional potential confounding variables (Table 9 – Supplementary data), except for the Mood domain at 6 months, with a higher mean Mood score (indicating more negative mood) in infants of mothers randomised to FENO (2.8 vs 2.6, p = 0.03).

Results were similar for sensory outcomes across all three timepoints, with only the domain of oral processing at 6 months reaching significance with higher mean oral processing score (indicating more sensory behaviours observed) in infants of mothers randomised to the Control group (6.0 vs 5.6, p = 0.04; Table 10 – Supplementary data). Results were similar in sensitivity analyses adjusting for additional potential confounding variables (Table 11 - Supplementary data).

Continuous (Table 12 – Supplementary data) and binary (Table 13 – Supplementary data) outcomes of autism likelihood across all domains showed no differences between groups.

Discussion

This study aimed to compare the developmental, temperament, sensory functioning and autism likelihood in infants from mothers who had their asthma managed by a FENO-guided treatment algorithm to those who had their asthma managed with usual best care during pregnancy. We found no evidence of differences in development, temperament, sensory functioning and autism likelihood between infants of mothers in the two groups. This is not surprising given that there was also no evidence of any difference in a composite adverse perinatal outcome at birth between infants of mothers in the FENO vs the control group [27].

The Breathing for Life Trial [27, 28] is the first randomised controlled trial (RCT) to test the effect of an intervention for asthma management on perinatal outcomes. The present study, the Breathing for Life – Infant Development (BLT-ID) study is the first to compare behavioural development in infants of the above mothers. The conclusions of the primary trial were that FENO-guided asthma intervention during pregnancy did not improve perinatal outcomes. Specifically, FENO -based asthma management used here measured and targeted immune activation by using an exhaled biomarker of Interleukin-13 mediated airway inflammation (exhaled nitric oxide) to assess the degree of T2 immune activation. Adjustments were made to the participant’s inhaled corticosteroid therapy to reduce FENO levels. Our findings that infants of mothers with asthma managed by a FENO algorithm do not differ significantly in temperament, sensory functioning, autism likelihood and developmental outcomes in the first year of life compared to infants of mothers with usual care. However, in the parent trial there was a non-significant, 20% reduction in exacerbations requiring medical intervention in the FENO group (OR 0.80, 95% CI 0.58, 1.10). It is possible that if this reduction was greater, it may have resulted in a difference in child neurodevelopmental outcomes. It is also important to consider that the embryonic period (~ gestational weeks 3–8) is a critical period for central nervous system development and neurogenesis (formation of neurons [39]). The effect of timing of environmental exposures on neurogenesis is less clear. It is therefore possible, that given the intervention was delivered in the second half of pregnancy, that it was not delivered during a critical window of neurodevelopment in pregnancy.

These findings are consistent with those of other prospective studies, which provide evidence that well-managed asthma during pregnancy is not associated with atypical developmental outcomes in children in the first few years of life [22] and are consistent with the conclusions of a recent systematic review on the subject [40]. A direct comparison between this study and Schatz [22] is not possible as they used different versions of the Bayley Scales (Bayley-I vs Bayley-III) which produce different outcome measures (mental developmental index, MDI & psychomotor developmental index, PDI vs cognitive, language and motor composite scores). Our results are consistent with a recent cross-sectional study using these data [41], that found no significant differences in sensory and temperament outcomes between children of mothers with and without asthma. Maternal asthma severity and control during pregnancy also did not affect these outcomes. However, this study compared children of mothers with and without asthma and was not an intervention study.

Importantly, we cannot comment on whether infants of mothers with untreated asthma are at an increased likelihood of autism, intellectual disability or other behavioural differences as that was not within the scope of the current study. Although there was a trend for less autism likelihood in the FENO group, the difference between the groups was not significant, likely due to the small numbers. It is important to note that this measure does not have perfect predictive power for later autism diagnoses, and it is possible that developmental effects do not manifest until the child is older. Studies which have assessed the longitudinal effects of asthma on child outcomes [22, 42] have not followed up infants beyond age 2 years, which leaves an important avenue for future research. The authors are currently following up this cohort on a range of developmental measures at ages 3–6. While they found no developmental differences between the intervention groups, Schatz [22] assessed 379 intervention and 376 control infants. As we assessed 107 intervention and 113 control infants, it is also possible that our sample was underpowered.

It is also worth noting that there was no placebo; instead, women were randomised into either the intervention or ‘treatment as usual’ control group. Women randomised to the control group may not have experienced typical “treatment-as-usual” asthma, given that they received self-management education and at least one prenatal asthma assessment. Meta-analyses have revealed that studies which report active management of asthma during pregnancy find no risk of adverse perinatal outcomes, such as preterm birth and neonatal hospitalisation, whereas statistically significant increased risks are found in studies which do not report active asthma management [9, 10]. This was found to be true for child developmental outcomes as well [22]. Further, asthma education during pregnancy has been shown to reduce perceptions of the teratogenic effects of medications used to treat asthma, and results in visible improvements in a woman’s inhaler technique and reduction in symptoms [43, 44]. Therefore, the control group having received an asthma assessment and self-management education during pregnancy may have changed their “as-usual” behaviour as compared to pregnant women with asthma not enrolled in the RCT.

The null findings between intervention and control groups may also be the result of trial participation. While the strengths of this study were the inclusion of participants who differed on socioeconomic status, age, ethnic background, asthma severity and level of education, as well as a proportion of women who smoked and were above a healthy weight, we have reported previously on a clear ascertainment bias in this study [45]. Given that we were able to access prenatal mental health data from hospital records, we compared the prenatal mental health of participants and non-participants of the postnatal follow-up and found that a significantly greater percentage of non-participants (31%) scored in the medium and high-risk categories of the depression measure than participants. This bias is likely to have persisted into the sample used in the present study, as we likely sampled infants of mothers with high adaptive functioning skills and help-seeking behaviours that may not be representative of the general population. It is possible that infants lost to follow-up may have mothers with more psychological distress or poorer asthma control and more severe asthma, who were not able or willing to participate in the study requirements. Alternatively, these infants could have had higher support needs or more behaviours of concern, which resulted in their mothers declining participation or withdrawing from the study. Alternatively, more participants were followed up at 12 months in the intervention group than the control, suggesting more engagement with the study. It is also possible that there is a competing effect, whereby the more intensive monitoring in this group may have contributed to higher psychological stress, potentially counteracting any beneficial effect of the intervention on developmental outcomes.

Conclusion

While this study does not support the integration of FENO-based management of asthma in antenatal settings for optimal infant development, it does send a positive message about the implications of asthma management during pregnancy on infant developmental outcomes, temperament, sensory processing and autism signs in the first year of life. While the findings of our study indicate that these aspects of development do not appear to be impacted, questions remain as to the long-term outcomes of these infants, and whether interventions should be implemented during the first postnatal year, where deficits are not yet apparent, to hopefully improve future outcomes.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Acknowledgements

The authors wish to acknowledge the contributions of Kelly Steel for participant recruitment and Annelies Robijn who assisted with pregnancy data. We also wish to acknowledge the many staff, students and volunteers from the University of Newcastle for assistance with data collection, data entry and study administration: Kathryn Jesson, Patricia Belinelo, Kelsey Philpott-Robinson, Declan Murphy, Sarah Morris, Jordan Tait, Brooke Sainsbury, Hiu Chung Esther Chan, Briana Shailer, Meagan Van Aswegen, Nicola Jarman, Gabrielle Easey, Helen Armstrong, Joshua Smith, Madeline Cordingley, Tanayah Tooze, Madison Turner-Presker, Tanya Crawford, Belinda Allen, Olivia Spohr and Taylah Armstrong. We would also like to warmly thank the mothers and babies who participated in this study.

Abbreviations

FENO

Fractional exhaled nitric oxide

BLT

Breathing for Life Trial

BLT-ID

Breathing for Life Infant Development

RCT

Randomised Controlled Trial

GP

General Practitioner

SEIFA

Socioeconomic Index For Areas

ISP

Infant Sensory Profile

TSP

Toddler Sensory Profile

CTS

Carey Temperament Scales

EITQ

Early Infancy Temperament Questionnaire

RITQ

Revised Infant Temperament Questionnaire

TTS

Toddler Temperament Scale

FYI

First Year Inventory

SEABQ

Bayley-III Socioemotional and Adaptive Behaviour Questionnaire

GAC

General Adaptive Composite

M

Mean

SD

Standard deviation

CI

Confidence interval

OR

Odds Ratio

BF

Bayes factor

MCAR

Data Missing Completely at Random

MDI

Mental Developmental Index

PDI

Psychomotor Developmental Index

NHMRC

National Health and Medical Research Council

Author contributions

All authors contributed to the study conception and design. Material preparation, project management and data collection for the BLT-ID study were performed by Olivia Whalen, Linda Campbell, Alison Lane, Frini Karayanidis, Carly Mallise, Alix Woolard and Vanessa Murphy. Analyses were performed by Olivia Whalen and Elizabeth Holliday. The first draft of the manuscript was written by Olivia Whalen and all authors commented on previous versions of the manuscript. All authors read and approved the final manuscript.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by CAUL and its Member Institutions This work was supported by the University of Newcastle and Hunter Medical Research Institute. This research was conducted with financial assistance from the Priority Research Centre GrowUpWell® at the University of Newcastle. The funding bodies had no role in the design of the study, data collection, data analysis, interpretation of data nor in writing the manuscript.

Data availability

Data are available on request.

Declarations

Conflict of interest

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Clark JM, Hulme E, Devendrakumar V, Turner MA, Baker PN, Sibley CP, D’Souza SW (2007) Effect of maternal asthma on birthweight and neonatal outcome in a British inner-city population. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol 21:154–162 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Sawicki E, Stewart K, Wong S, Leung L, Paul E, George J (2011) Medication use for chronic health conditions by pregnant women attending an Australian maternity hospital. Aust N Z J Obstet Gynaecol 51:333–338 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Clifton VL, Engel P, Smith R, Gibson P, Brinsmead M, Giles WB (2009) Maternal and neonatal outcomes of pregnancies complicated by asthma in an Australian population. Aust N Z J Obstet Gynaecol 49:619–626 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Perlow JH, Montgomery D, Morgan MA, Towers CV, Pronto M (1992) Severity of asthma and perinatal outcome. Am J Obstet Gynecol 167:963–967 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Wen SW, Demissie K, Liu S (2001) Adverse outcomes in pregnancies of asthmatic women: results from a Canadian population. Ann Epidemiol 11:7–12 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Dombrowski MP, Schatz M, Wise R, Momirova V, Landon M, Mabie W, Newman RB, McNellis D, Hauth JC, Lindheimer M (2004) Asthma during pregnancy. Obstet Gynecol 103:5–12 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Norjavaara E, de Verdier MG (2003) Normal pregnancy outcomes in a population-based study including 2968 pregnant women exposed to budesonide. J Allergy Clin Immunol 111:736–742 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Sobande A, Archibong E, Akinola S (2002) Pregnancy outcome in asthmatic patients from high altitudes. Int J Gynecol Obstet 77:117–121 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Murphy V, Namazy J, Powell H, Schatz M, Chambers C, Attia J, Gibson P (2011) A meta-analysis of adverse perinatal outcomes in women with asthma. BJOG: Int J Obst Gynaecol 118:1314–1323 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Murphy V, Wang G, Namazy J, Powell H, Gibson P, Chambers C, Schatz M (2013) The risk of congenital malformations, perinatal mortality and neonatal hospitalisation among pregnant women with asthma: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BJOG Int J Obst Gynaecol 120:812–822 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Lim RH, Kobzik L, Dahl M (2010) Risk for asthma in offspring of asthmatic mothers versus fathers: a meta-analysis. PLoS ONE 5:e10134 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Namazy JA, Murphy VE, Powell H, Gibson PG, Chambers C, Schatz M (2013) Effects of asthma severity, exacerbations and oral corticosteroids on perinatal outcomes. Eur Respir J 41:1082–1090 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Wang G, Murphy VE, Namazy J, Powell H, Schatz M, Chambers C, Attia J, Gibson PG (2014) The risk of maternal and placental complications in pregnant women with asthma: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Matern Fetal Neonatal Med 27:934–942 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Croen LA, Grether JK, Yoshida CK, Odouli R, Van de Water J (2005) Maternal autoimmune diseases, asthma and allergies, and childhood autism spectrum disorders: a case-control study. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 159:151–157 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Langridge AT, Glasson EJ, Nassar N, Jacoby P, Pennell C, Hagan R, Bourke J, Leonard H, Stanley FJ (2013) Maternal conditions and perinatal characteristics associated with autism spectrum disorder and intellectual disability. PLoS ONE 8:e50963 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Leonard H, De Klerk N, Bourke J, Bower C (2006) Maternal health in pregnancy and intellectual disability in the offspring: a population-based study. Ann Epidemiol 16:448–454 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Patel S, Dale RC, Rose D, Heath B, Nordahl CW, Rogers S, Guastella AJ, Ashwood P (2020) Maternal immune conditions are increased in males with autism spectrum disorders and are associated with behavioural and emotional but not cognitive co-morbidity. Transl Psychiatry 10:286 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Flannery KA, Liederman J (1994) A test of the immunoreactive theory for the origin of neurodevelopmental disorders in the offspring of women with immune disorder. Cortex 30:635–646 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Lyall K, Schmidt RJ, Hertz-Picciotto I (2014) Maternal lifestyle and environmental risk factors for autism spectrum disorders. Int J Epidemiol 43:443–464 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Micali N, Chakrabarti S, Fombonne E (2004) The broad autism phenotype: findings from an epidemiological survey. Autism 8:21–37 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Mouridsen SE, Rich B, Isager T, Nedergaard NJ (2007) Autoimmune diseases in parents of children with infantile autism: a case—control study. Dev Med Child Neurol 49:429–432 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Schatz M, Harden K, Kagnoff M, Zeiger RS, Chilingar L (2001) Developmental follow-up in 15-month-old infants of asthmatic vs. control mothers. Pediatr Allergy Immunol 12:149–153 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Tamási L, Bohács A, Bikov A, Andorka C, Rigó J Jr, Losonczy G, Horváth I (2009) Exhaled nitric oxide in pregnant healthy and asthmatic women. J Asthma 46:786–791 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Powell H, Murphy VE, Taylor DR, Hensley MJ, McCaffery K, Giles W, Clifton VL, Gibson PG (2011) Management of asthma in pregnancy guided by measurement of fraction of exhaled nitric oxide: a double-blind, randomised controlled trial. Lancet 378:983–990 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Mattes J, Murphy VE, Powell H, Gibson PG (2013) Prenatal origins of bronchiolitis: protective effect of optimised asthma management during pregnancy. Thorax 69:383–384 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Morten M, Collison A, Murphy V, Barker D, Meredith J, Powell H, Robinson P, Sly P, Gibson P, Mattes J (2017) Asthma Control During Pregnancy, 17Q21 Variants and Childhood-Onset Asthma. Respirol 22:100–100 [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Murphy VE, Jensen ME, Holliday EG, Giles WB, Barrett HL, Callaway LK, Bisits A, Peek MJ, Seeho SK, Abbott A (2022) Effect of asthma management with exhaled nitric oxide versus usual care on perinatal outcomes. Eur Respir J 60 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 28.Murphy VE, Jensen ME, Mattes J, Hensley MJ, Giles WB, Peek MJ, Bisits A, Callaway LK, McCaffery K, Barrett HL (2016) The Breathing for Life Trial: a randomised controlled trial of fractional exhaled nitric oxide (FENO)-based management of asthma during pregnancy and its impact on perinatal outcomes and infant and childhood respiratory health. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth 16:1–10 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.NHMRC (2007) National statement on ethical conduct in human research. Canberra, NHMRC [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 30.Dunn W (2014) Sensory profile 2. Psych Corporation Bloomington, MN, USA [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Medoff-Cooper B, Carey WB, McDevitt SC (1993) The early infancy temperament questionnaire. J Dev Behav Pediatr 14:230–235 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Carey WB, McDevitt SC (1978) Revision of the infant temperament questionnaire. Pediatrics 61:735–739 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Fullard W, McDevitt SC, Carey WB (1984) Assessing temperament in one-to three-year-old children. J Pediatr Psychol 9:205–217 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Thomas A, Chess S, Birch HG, Hertzig ME, Korn S (1963) Behavioral individuality in early childhood. New York University Press [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Reznick JS, Baranek GT, Reavis S, Watson LR, Crais ER (2007) A parent-report instrument for identifying one-year-olds at risk for an eventual diagnosis of autism: the first year inventory. J Autism Dev Disord 37:1691–1710 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Turner-Brown LM, Baranek GT, Reznick JS, Watson LR, Crais ER (2013) The First Year Inventory: A longitudinal follow-up of 12-month-old to 3-year-old children. Autism 17:527–540 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Bayley N (2006) Bayley scales of infant and toddler development. Harcourt Assessment, San Antonio, TX [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Wagenmakers E-J (2007) A practical solution to the pervasive problems of p values. Psychon Bull Rev 14:779–804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Nelson CA (2011) Neural development and lifelong plasticity. In: Keating DP (ed) Nature and nurture in early child development:45–69. Cambridge University Press [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Whalen OM, Karayanidis F, Murphy VE, Lane AE, Mallise CA, Campbell LE (2019) The effects of maternal asthma during pregnancy on child cognitive and behavioral development: A systematic review. J Asthma 56:130–141 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Mallise CA, Murphy VE, Campbell LE, Woolard AJ, Whalen OM, Milton G, Mattes J, Collison A, Gibson PG, Karayanidis F (2021) Early Sensory and Temperament Features in Infants Born to Mothers With Asthma: A Cross-Sectional Study. Front Psychol 12:713804 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Gordon M, Niswander KR, Berendes H, Kantor AG (1970) Fetal morbidity following potentially anoxigenic obstetric conditions: VII. Bronchial asthma. Am J Obstet Gynecol 106:421–429 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Robijn AL, Jensen ME, Gibson PG, Powell H, Giles WB, Clifton VL, Mattes J, Peek MJ, Barrett HL, Seeho SK (2019) Trends in asthma self-management skills and inhaled corticosteroid use during pregnancy and postpartum from 2004 to 2017. J Asthma 56:594–602 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Murphy VE, Gibson P, Talbot PI, Clifton VL (2005) Severe asthma exacerbations during pregnancy. Obstet Gynecol 106:1046–1054 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Whalen OM, Campbell LE, Murphy VE, Lane AE, Gibson PG, Mattes J, Collison A, Mallise CA, Woolard A, Karayanidis F (2020) Observational study of mental health in asthmatic women during the prenatal and postnatal periods. J Asthma 57:829–841 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Data Availability Statement

Data are available on request.


Articles from European Journal of Pediatrics are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES