Skip to main content
Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences logoLink to Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences
. 2024 Jul 31;291(2027):20240898. doi: 10.1098/rspb.2024.0898

Ant social network structure is highly conserved across species

Tomas Kay 1,2,, Alba Motes-Rodrigo 2, Arthur Royston 2, Thomas O Richardson 3, Nathalie Stroeymeyt 3, Laurent Keller 2,
PMCID: PMC11288679  PMID: 39079671

Abstract

The ecological success of social insects makes their colony organization fascinating to scientists studying collective systems. In recent years, the combination of automated behavioural tracking and social network analysis has deepened our understanding of many aspects of colony organization. However, because studies have typically worked with single species, we know little about interspecific variation in network structure. Here, we conduct a comparative network analysis across five ant species from five subfamilies, separated by more than 100 Myr of evolution. We find that social network structure is highly conserved across subfamilies. All species studied form modular networks, with two social communities, a similar distribution of individuals between the two communities, and equivalent mapping of task performance onto the communities. Against this backdrop of organizational similarity, queens of the different species occupied qualitatively distinct network positions. The deep conservation of the two community structure implies that the most fundamental behavioural division of labour in social insects is between workers that stay in the nest to rear brood, and those that leave the nest to forage. This division has parallels across the animal kingdom in systems of biparental care and probably represents the most readily evolvable form of behavioural division of labour.

Keywords: ants, social insects, collective behaviour, social network analysis, social organization, division of labour

1. Introduction

Social insects are among the most ecologically successful organisms on the planet. There is an estimated 12 Mt of dry carbon ant biomass on Earth—more than that of all wild mammals and birds combined [1]. Their ecological success is thought to result from their sophisticated social organization, which increases per capita productivity [24] and confers a range of other benefits including mitigating predation risk and increasing territory defence capability [5,6]. The study of social insect colony organization has recently been revolutionized by the advent of high-throughput behavioural data [7]. The extraction of the coordinates of hundreds of colony members multiple times per second for extended periods of time has allowed the automatic inference of social interactions and task-related behaviours [712]. These data have greatly advanced our understanding of colony social organization. For example, we now know that increased group size and genetic heterogeneity can confer fitness benefits by enhancing division of labour [2,13] (though do not always seem to [14]), how fundamental aspects of insect biology map onto colony social networks [15,16] and how social network structure is influenced by both pathogens [17,18] and symbionts [19]. We also know more about the dynamics of individual-level behavioural change [9,20,21], foraging and food dissemination [22,23], cooperative transport [24] and other aspects of self-organization [12,2527], all of which are important topics for scientists investigating biological and artificial collective systems, and optimization algorithms [2830].

Studies investigating insect social organization have generally—though not exclusively [12]—used a single species to assess how one or few biological variables change in response to a given factor. Consequently, while social network structure (and its implication for colony function) is understood for certain species in detail, we know little about its natural variation among species. Ant social systems are tremendously diverse: there is variation in the number and type of reproductive individuals [31], the number and types of worker castes [32], mating system [33], nest structure [34], foraging strategy [35], system of food re-distribution [36], etc. It is possible that certain aspects of social network structure vary as a consequence of some of these traits, that certain aspects of network structure are directly selected according to the life history of the species and that other aspects of network structure are fundamental and invariant across taxa. Here, we take a first step towards addressing this knowledge gap by characterizing and comparing the social networks of five ant species (Camponotus fellah, Iridomyrmex purpureus, Diacamma rugosum, Pogonomyrmex rugosus and Rhytidoponera metallica; figure 1a ) from five different subfamilies (Formicinae, Dolichoderinae, Ponerinae, Myrmicinae and Ectatomminae, respectively). While this sampling is too sparse to demonstrate how social network structure varies with any particular life-history trait, it is sufficient to establish which aspects of network structure vary across species, and which may be conserved. Moreover, we selected these particular study species because they naturally differ in various important life-history traits including the presence and number of queens, number of matings, mature worker population size, colony structure, colony founding and foraging behaviour. Observed differences provide a first indication as to the co-variation of life-history features and social network structure.

Figure 1.

Experimental overview.

Experimental overview. (a) Single workers of each of the study species. Clockwise from top-left: R. metallica, P rugosus, D. rugosum, C. fellah and I. purpureus. Images not to scale. Photo credit: Bart Zijlstra. (b) The double-box tracking set-up comprises two boxes positioned within two tracking systems that can be independently climatically controlled to simulate a nest box and a foraging box. (c) The automated detection of tags inside the tracking system. Red dots are superimposed onto the tags that have been recognized. (d) An example C. fellah social network. Edge colour intensity and width correspond to pairwise interaction frequency (darker and thicker, higher frequency). Circular nodes represent workers (coloured according to the proportion of time they spent in the foraging arena: darker, more time foraging), while the magenta square represents the queen. Edges are weighted and node positions are defined by a force-directed layout meaning that ants that interact more frequently are positioned closer together. Inset is an illustration of how interactions were detected through the intersection of ‘head capsules’.

Camponotus fellah has a relatively standard life history for an ant: singly mated queens found colonies independently following mating flights [37,38]. Queens and workers are dimorphic and mature colonies naturally comprise approx. 10 000 workers [37]. Pogonomyrmex rugosus is broadly similar but differs importantly in that queens mate multiply, increasing genetic diversity among workers [39]. In I. purpureus, queens mate with one or few males, there can be many queens per colony, and colonies can spread across multiple nest sites and reach worker population sizes of up to 300 000 [40,41]. In both R. metallica and D. rugosum, workers can mate and become functional reproductives known as gamergates. In D. rugosum, the queen caste has been completely lost. Instead, each colony includes a single gamergate which physically sterilizes her daughter workers by removing thoracic appendages from them as they emerge as adults [42,43]. In R. metallica, the queen caste persists but is not always present within functional colonies [44,45]. Refer to table 1 for an overview of the biology of these species.

Table 1.

Summary of the life history and ecology of the study species. (DD, data deficient.)

trait Diacamma rugosum Camponotus fellah Rhytidoponera metallica Pogonomyrmex rugosus Iridomyrmex purpureus
reproductives 1 gamergate [42,43] 1 queen [37] 0—many queens + gamergates [44,45] 1 queen [37,46] 1—many queens [47,48]
matings 1 [42,43] 139 1—few [44] 3–1240 1—few [48,49]
workers 100 [50] 10 K [37] 30–1 K [51] 600–15 K [52,53] 11–300 K
colony structure monodomous [43] DD DD monodomous [37] polydomous [40,41]
founding probably budding [43] independent [54] independent + budding [44] independent [37,55] independent [48]
foraging solitary [50] DD solitary solitary + group [56] DD

Here, we focus on two fundamental social network properties. The first is modularity, which measures whether and how the network segregates into clusters of individuals—known as modules or communities—that interact frequently among themselves and rarely with others. The second is (weighted) node strength, the total number of interactions that a given individual has during the experiment. These properties are critical to understanding the dynamic functioning of any network [57,58], and have been previously shown to relate to division of labour and colony performance [7,17,20]. We examine how these network features relate to reproductive and behavioural division of labour and explore some of the behavioural mechanisms governing these relationships. We find that the network structures are broadly similar across species. All species formed social networks that (i) exhibit community structure; and (ii) are best characterized as comprising two social communities—a ‘nurse community’ and a ‘forager community’. The extent of community structure correlates with the extent to which the species exhibits division of labour. We find that workers tend to be strongly affiliated to one or the other social community, and that the foraging community is larger than the nurse community in all species. Across species, the relationship between foraging behaviour and social network position is similarly nonlinear, and individuals that forage more tend to interact less. The latter finding seemingly stems from foragers roaming over larger and less densely populated areas within the nest than nurses. Finally, the queen’s position in the social network appears to be highly variable across species. Queens range from being no different to the average member of the nurse community to being stand-out ‘hub nodes’ at the centre of the nurse community to being peripheral to the nurse community and highly specialized in their interaction profile (i.e. interacting frequently with a small subset of nurses). This variation appears not to be driven by differences in queen space-use patterns but rather queen–worker encounter kinetics in terms of (i) the number of workers to encounter the queen per unit of time; (ii) the frequency with which those same workers encounter the queen; and (iii) the rate of turn-over in the identity of the workers that encounter the queen.

2. Results and discussion

We tracked five colonies of each species in double-box set-ups (i.e. with a constantly dark nest box and a foraging box subject to day–night condition cycles; figure 1b,c ). Experimental subcolonies comprised the queen (all colonies in the laboratory were maintained with one queen), 100 randomly sub-sampled workers and a proportion of the total brood that approximately matched the proportion of total workers that had been sampled (i.e. if 100 workers was 50% of the work force then 50% of the brood was sampled). Data from across the 5 day period was pooled for static network construction and analysis. This design constrains the generality of our findings, but allows for effective comparison of the different behavioural tendencies of the five species in a common garden environment. Additionally, since the laboratory environment differs from the natural environment of each species in different ways, some of the observed species-level differences may reflect different responses to being in the laboratory.

(a). Ant social networks comprise two communities

To evaluate how a proposed number of social communities fits a given network we calculated soft modularity scores, maximizing and quantifying the ratio of within : between group connections [12,59]. The social networks (e.g. figure 1d5; electronic supplementary material, figures S1–S5) of all species were, on average, significantly more modular than expected by chance (permutation test comparing each network against randomly rewired versions of the same network: all p-values < 0.001; electronic supplementary material, figure S6). The same was independently true for 22 out of 25 individual colonies, the exceptions being one C. fellah colony and two D. rugosum colonies. Furthermore, in all species, the networks were most parsimoniously partitioned into two communities: the cross-colony average soft modularity score peaked for all five species when community number was set to 2, and decreased monotonically as community number was increased to 5 (see figure 2a ; electronic supplementary material, figure S7 for colony-level plots). The differences were statistically significant in paired t-tests for 2 versus 3 (t = 3.42; p = 0.027), 4 (t = 3.88; p = 0.018) and 5 (t = 5; p = 0.008) communities for C. fellah. The same was true for D. rugosum, while in P. rugosus and R. metallica the differences were significant only between 2 versus 4 and 5 communities, and none of the differences were significant for I. purpureus. While the presence of the two communities seems qualitatively consistent, there was considerable quantitative variation in how clearly segregated these communities were, with the most modular network (C. fellah colony 3 = 0.21) being five-fold more modular than the least modular (I. purpureus colony 3 = 0.04).

Figure 5.

Node strength, social maturity and spatial dispersion.

Node strength, social maturity and spatial dispersion. Top row: in all species, there was a quantitatively similar negative relationship between social maturity and node strength (i.e. interaction frequency) when node strength was normalized within colonies to have a mean of 0 and a standard deviation of 1. Bottom row: in all species, there was a positive correlation between social maturity and proportion of the nest box occupied by the 90% home range. Colour indicates colony and lines represent linear regressions (top row) and generalized additive models (bottom row).

Figure 2.

Community structure and division of labour.

Community structure and division of labour. (a) On average, soft modularity (a measure of how well a proposed number of communities fits a given network) decreased with community number in all five species. Points represent individual colony values and lines represent species averages. (b) The extent of division of labour (D.O.L.), measured per colony as the standard deviation in proportion of time spent outside of the nest. An asterisk (*) indicates p < 0.05 in two-sample t-tests. (c) Across all colonies, network modularity correlates with division of labor (Pearson’s r = 0.553; p = 0.004).

We next wondered whether colonies that exhibited more modular social networks also exhibited more pronounced division of labour. Since we measured only a single task (foraging), we used the colony-level standard deviation in the proportion of time spent foraging to quantify the division of labour at the colony level. A colony in which all individuals had foraged at exactly the same rate would have a score of 0, and the score would increase as foraging became more biased towards a subset of individuals. Variation in network modularity was associated with the extent of division of labour (figure 2b ): colonies with more modular networks exhibit more pronounced division of labour (Pearson’s r = 0.553; p = 0.004; figure 2c ). The species-level differences in the extent of division of labour were only statistically significant between C. fellah and I. purpureus, which exhibited the most and least division of labour respectively (Welch two-sample t‐test: t = 4.58, p = 0.005).

Previous work with C. fellah established that the two social communities correspond primarily to a division between nurses and foragers [7,20]. To test whether this was true for all species in this study, we quantified individual network positions using ‘social maturity’—a measure of community membership that ranges continuously from 0 to 1, with 0 representing an individual deeply embedded in the nurse social community, 0.5 representing an individual equally affiliated to both communities and 1 representing an individual deeply embedded in the forager community [20]. Since individuals shift from nursing to foraging as they age (‘temporal polyethism’), and move through the colony social network accordingly, their social maturity scores should increase over their lives. While we do not have data on nursing behaviour, the two communities consistently seem to separate foragers from nurses as all species exhibited a clear positive correlation between social maturity and the proportion of time spent in the foraging arena (species-level generalized linear mixed models (GLMMs) testing the relationship between foraging and social maturity, with colony as a random effect for C. fellah: t = 10.4, p < 0.001; D. rugosum: t = 16.5, p < 0.001; I. purpureus: t = 12.7, p < 0.001; P. rugosus: t = 22.0, p < 0.001 and R. metallica: t = 23.2, p < 0.001; figure 3). Across species, individuals that spent a considerable time in the foraging arena were almost always restricted to one of the two communities (i.e. the foraging community). The presence of two social communities in all five species implies that the segregation of the social network into nurse and forager communities is probably a fundamental characteristic of ant social networks.

Figure 3.

Community membership and foraging behaviour.

Community membership and foraging behaviour. In all species, there was a clear and nonlinear relationship between social maturity and foraging behaviour, and there appears to be a strong colony effect on the maturity at which workers start foraging. For example, foraging seems to only start increasing with maturity after a maturity of 0.8 in all five C. fellah colonies, while in two D. rugosum colonies foraging increases with maturity even at the lowest end of the maturity range. Colour indicates colony and lines represent locally estimated scatterplot smoothing regressions.

(b). Temporal polyethism and the distribution of individuals across communities

The distribution of workers between the two social communities reflects the dynamics of temporal polyethism. Long-term tracking of C. fellah showed that workers spend 80% of their lives strongly affiliated with either the nurse or forager community and 20% of their lives in transition between the two [20]. This was reflected in the fact that 20% of workers had intermediate maturity values at a given snapshot in time, as would be expected under stable demography.

We found that for all five species, there were more individuals with extremal than intermediate maturity values (one-sample proportions test with continuity correction for the numbers of individuals with maturity scores between 0.25 and 0.75 versus the numbers of individuals with maturity scores outside of that range: p < 0.01 for all species; figure 4).

Figure 4.

Distribution of workers across the two communities.

Distribution of workers across the two communities. In all species workers tended to be deeply embedded in one or the other social community. Colour indicates colony.

This implies that, based on analogy with previously collected C. fellah data [20], workers of all species tend to spend a greater proportion of their lives strongly associated with one or the other community, and a smaller proportion of their lives in transition. While there were always less individuals with intermediate than with extremal maturity scores, there was considerable variation in the percentage of individuals with intermediate social maturities. The values were 20% for C. fellah, 27% for D. rugosum, 37% for I. purpureus, 19% for P. rugosus and 23% for R. metallica, with species identity having a statistically significant effect (one-way ANOVA, F = 2.97; p = 0.045). Higher proportions of intermediate individuals should reflect slower transitions from nurse to forager relative to worker lifespan. There was additionally a similar imbalance of workers between the two communities across species. In C. fellah, there were 44% more individuals with maturity scores of greater than 0.9 than with maturity scores of less than 0.1. The equivalent figure was: 53% for D. rugosum, 107% for I. purpureus, 65% for P. rugosus and 83% for R. metallica, although the variation within-species was large such that the variation between species was not statistically significant (one-way ANOVA, F = 0.394; p = 0.811). The balance of the workers between these two communities should reflect the average age at which workers transition, with younger transitioning species having comparatively more high-maturity workers.

(c). Nurses interact more than foragers

It has previously been established that in C. fellah, nurses interact more frequently with other workers than do foragers (i.e. that strength decreases as social maturity increases), and we replicated this finding here (GLMM with strength as response variable, social maturity as explanatory variable and colony as random effect: t = −17.2; p < 0.0015). The same pattern held for the other four species (D. rugosum: t = −20.2, p < 0.001; I. purpureus: t = −11.1, p < 0.001; P. rugosus: t = −15.2, p < 0.001; R. metallica: t = −16.6, p < 0.001; figure 5). If this pattern resulted from foragers spending more time outside of the nest (where the density of individuals is lower), we would expect a positive association between total foraging output and the strength of association between community membership and node strength. However, while the average percentage of time that foragers (those that forage at least once) spent outside of the nest ranged from 1% in one D. rugosum colony up to 60% in one C. fellah colony, there was no relationship between these averages and the strength of correlation between social maturity and node strength (Pearson’s r = 0.011, p = 0.593; electronic supplementary material, figure S8a,b). This implies that foragers spending time in the foraging arena is not what makes their strength lower than nurses. The difference in strength between foragers and nurses instead seems to result from different space-use patterns within the nest, where foragers roam over larger areas, with lower ant density and encounter other ants less frequently (GLMM with social maturity as response variable, within-nest home range size as explanatory variable and species identity as random effect: β = 1.70; t = 30.2; p < 0.01). Interestingly, social interaction frequency seems to decrease with age in various other animals including red deer [60] and humans [61].

More generally, the overall distribution of node strength is an important network property in determining the efficiency of information or resource spread and robustness to perturbation. Early work on biological networks suggested that they were generally ‘scale-free’ [62]. In scale-free networks, the distribution of node strength follows a power law, with highly connected ‘hub’ nodes, while in random networks node strength is normally distributed [63]. Hub nodes both increase the speed of transmission through the network and make it more failure-prone. The emerging consensus is now that scale-free networks are rather rare in nature and that node strength is typically normally distributed [64]. We find that the distribution of node strength is highly consistent across species, and follows an approximately log-normal distribution (i.e. somewhat intermediate between a normal and a power law distribution; figure 6).

Figure 6.

Degree distribution.

Degree distribution. The distributions of node strengths were consistent across species and were approximately log-normal.

(d). Queens occupy unique social network positions

The dynamics of queen–worker interactions are subject to a number of potentially antagonistic evolutionary pressures. For example, there is evidence from various species that the queen, as the most valuable member of the colony, is kept socially isolated from certain other colony members to reduce the likelihood of her becoming infected with a pathogen [13,17]. Contrastingly, in various species, queens deliberately interact with many individuals to maintain or communicate their status—either through dominance interactions or the transmission of queen pheromone [65,66].

We quantified the queen’s position in the social network with two parameters: node strength and entropy (i.e. how evenly distributed their interactions were across colony members, with low values indicating that queens interact frequently with a small subset of workers and high values indicating that queens interact at a similar frequency with all workers). The difference in social position between queens and workers varied considerably among species (figure 7). In C. fellah, I. purpureus and D. rugosum, queens/gamergates had lower entropies than workers but worker-typical strengths (i.e. they interacted as frequently as workers but were far more specific in their interaction partners; Welch two-sample t-tests comparing C. fellah queen versus worker normalized entropies: t = 4.49, p = 0.01; strengths: t = −0.675, p = 0.536; comparing I. purpureus queen versus worker normalized entropies: t = 3.36, p = 0.028; strengths: t = −0.608, p = 0.576, and comparing D. rugosum gamergate versus worker normalized entropies: t = 3.73, p = 0.02; strengths: t = −1.24, p = 0.28). Contrastingly, P. rugosus queens had entropies that were not significantly different from workers but had higher strengths (i.e. their interaction profile was not more specific than those of workers, but they interacted more frequently overall; Welch two-sample t-tests comparing P. rugosus queen versus worker normalized entropies: t = 2.12, p = 0.1; strengths: t = −5.41, p < 0.01). Finally, R. metallica queens were not statistically different from workers in either strength or entropy (Welch two-sample t-tests comparing queen versus worker normalized entropies for R. metallica: t = 1.51, p = 0.204; strengths: t = −0.365, p = 0.733). The similarity between the queen and worker network positions in R. metallica is consistent with this species’ reduced reproductive division of labour. Rhytidoponera metallica colonies typically contain multiple gamergates, with the queens being non-essential, sometimes absent, and possibly in the process of being evolutionarily lost [45].

Figure 7.

Queen role in the social network differs across species.

Queen role in the social network differs across species (a) Node entropy plotted as a function of node strength. Grey circles, workers; magenta circles, queens. Boxplots of queen and worker strengths and entropies are inset, and asterisks correspond to statistical significance in Welch two-sample t-tests. (b) An example C. fellah network (refer also to figure 1e ), in which the queen has low entropy but average strength, and (c) an example P. rugosus network in which the queen has high strength but average entropy. Edge colour intensity and width correspond to pairwise interaction frequency (darker, more frequent). Circular nodes represent workers (coloured according to the proportion of time they spent in the foraging arena: darker, more time), while the square magenta node represents the queen.

The observed variation in queen strength and entropy could result from variation in queen space-use: queens with higher entropy (those that interact more evenly) may move around the nest box more to encounter more and different workers. To test this, we first discretized the arenas into tessellated hexagons of two ant body lengths, and then calculated the proportion of the nest box occupied by the queen’s 90% home range. Calibrating hexagon size with ant body length meant that the total number of hexagons per arena varied by colony/species, however this variation did not correlate with our home range size estimates (Pearson’s r = −0.193; p = 0.354). Despite considerable variation in queen home range size (12 ± 11% mean ± s.d.), differences in queen movement do not correlate with queen entropy or strength (GLMM with entropy as the response variable, home range size as the explanatory variable and species identity as a random effect: t = 1.25; p = 0.227; GLMM with social strength as the response variable, home range size as the explanatory variable and species identity as a random effect: t = −0.754; p = 0.46; electronic supplementary material, figure S9a,b).

The observed variation in queen strength and entropy may alternatively result from the kinetics of queen-worker encounters independently of queen movement. For example, queen strength and entropy would be increased by more queen–worker interactions occurring at a given instant (either because worker attraction to queens is higher or because queens position themselves in areas of higher worker density). Entropy could be increased without increasing strength if the number of queen–worker interactions at a given instant did not vary, but there was a higher turn-over in the identity of workers involved in queen–worker interactions. Also finally, strength could be increased and entropy decreased by the same subset of workers engaging more frequently in interactions with the queen. We quantified (i) the average number of workers a queen contacts per hour; (ii) the average number of times she interacts with each of these workers; and (iii) the conservation in the identity of the workers with which she interacts from one hour to the next. These analyses revealed that queen strength was mostly determined by the frequency of contact between the queen and the workers. Contrastingly, queen entropy was determined by the number of workers contacted per hour and the conservation in the identity of the contacted workers from one hour to the next. Specifically, there was a significant association between the average proportion of the workforce that the queen contacts per hour and queen entropy (GLMM with species identity as a random effect: t = 2.48; p = 0.022; electronic supplementary material, figure S9d) and a statistically insignificant trend for queens with higher overall strength to interact with a larger proportion of the workforce in a given hour (GLMM with species identity as a random effect: t = 1.85; p = 0.077; electronic supplementary material, figure S9c). There was a strong positive correlation between the average number of interactions per worker per hour (excluding 0 values) and queen strength (GLMM with species identity as a random effect: t = 3.32, p = 0.003; electronic supplementary material, figure S9g) but the relationship with entropy was not statistically significant (t = −0.233, p = 0.818; electronic supplementary material, figure S9h). Finally, there was a significant association between conservation in identity of the workers with which a queen interacts from one hour to the next and queen entropy (GLMM with species identity as a random effect: t = −2.70; p = 0.013; electronic supplementary material, figure S9f). Together these results indicate that variation in queen network position is probably driven by a combination of the number of different workers that come into contact with the queen per hour, how frequently these workers contact the queen and how much turn-over there is in which workers come into contact with the queen. It would be interesting to study how the social network positions occupied by queens vary with the queen number in the species that naturally have multiple queens—variation that we miss here by constraining queen number to one for all colonies.

3. Conclusion

Our comparative analysis revealed striking qualitative similarity in social organization across five ant species, from five different subfamilies, which last shared a common ancestor over 100 Mya [6769]. All species formed modular networks that were most parsimoniously divided into two communities—one comprising workers that stay within the nest to raise the young, and the other comprising workers that sometimes leave the nest to collect food. At any given moment, most workers were deeply embedded in one or the other community and few workers were similarly associated with both communities. The absence of qualitative differences in this social organization between species suggests that it has been maintained by selection. Moreover, the division between carers and foragers has independently arisen many times across the animal kingdom in systems of biparental and cooperative care from mongooses to humans [70,71], and probably represents the most readily evolvable form of division of labour.

Our results also highlight striking interspecific variation in the social network position of the queen. Queens seem to occupy one of the three social positions: (i) stand-out hub nodes at the centre of the nurse social community; (ii) peripheral to the nurse social community and highly specialized in their interaction profile; and (iii) no different to the average worker. These differences could relate to various aspects of queen biology: C. fellah and P. rugosus are monogynous species with large queen–worker dimorphism and their queens are most different from workers in social network position. In D. rugosum, where there is no queen–worker dimorphism and R. metallica, where there is low queen–worker dimorphism and queens are relatively unimportant, queens/gamergates occupy the most worker-like social network positions. It would be particularly interesting to see whether the observed difference between the social positions of C. fellah and P. rugosus queens are typical between the queens of Formicine and Myrmicine species, and how variation in queen–worker polymorphism and queen number relates to the social network positions of queens within genera where these traits are most plastic. More generally, broader taxonomic sampling would also allow us to test (i) whether the apparently conserved features identified here are conserved when more species (with more diverse life histories) are considered; and (ii) which environmental/biological variables drive the observed species-level variation in network structure.

4. Material and methods

(a). Ant colonies

Camponotus fellah colonies were established from queens collected after mating flights in Tel Aviv, Israel in 2007 and 2011. Pogonomyrmex rugosus colonies were established from queens collected after mating flights in Arizona, USA in 2008 and 2013. Iridomyrmex purpureus and R. metallica colonies were established from queens collected after mating flights in Victoria, Australia in 2021. Diacamma rugosum colonies were field collected in Chiang Mai, Thailand in 2018. The colonies of the different species were reared in the laboratory under different temperature and humidity regimes according to their ecological preferences: C. fellah = 29°C, 60% humidity; P. rugosus = 30°C, 60% humidity; D. rugosum, I. purpureus and R. metallica = 26°C, 60% humidity. All colonies were subject to 12 L : 12 D cycles, provided water ad libitum and fed weekly with flies and honey solution (at approx. 5% concentration). Camponotus fellah and P. rugosus were additionally fed ad libitum with artificial ant food and a seed mix, respectively [72]. All colonies contained either a single functional queen or gamergate and had been kept in the laboratory for at least six months.

To control for colony size, 100 workers were randomly selected for tracking from each colony along with the queen/gamergate and approximately the same proportion of brood as the 100 workers constituted of the total worker population (our laboratory colonies of all species generally contained 200–500 workers).

(b). Automated behavioural tracking

The colonies were tracked in double-chamber set-ups, with a nest box (169 mm × 223 mm for C. fellah, D. rugosum, I. purpureus and P. rugosus and 131 mm × 179 mm for R. metallica) kept in constant darkness and connected to a foraging arena (subject to 12 L : 12 D cycles) of the same dimensions through a plastic tube (internal diameter 19 mm for C. fellah, D. rugosum, I. purpureus and P. rugosus and 10 mm for R. metallica). A single test tube filled with water and bunged with cotton was placed into each box to provide a constant water source. Unique matrix barcodes (1.4 mm2 for C. fellah, D. rugosum, I. purpureus and P. rugosus and 1.0 mm2 for R. metallica) from the ARTag library [73] were glued to the thorax of each ant using SAUER skin adhesive (12% resin). This glue is designed to attach urinary sheaths to skin and the combination of natural latex and resin makes it non-toxic, water-resistant and keeps tags attached for longer than superglue, as it does not set brittle.

Colonies were continuously tracked for five full days (from 00.01 to 23.59) at 5 frames s−1. Three of the replicates (one I. purpureus and two R. metallica) terminated early owing to technical failures, but the reduction in data did not result in these colonies having outlier values for the measured metrics. The tracking system saves video files and the position and orientation of each tag in each frame. Full technical specifications and source code for the tracking systems [19,74] are available at: https://github.com/formicidae-tracker.

The tracking data were post-processed in FortStudio, where the head–tail axis (front edge of clypeus to tip of abdomen) of each ant was annotated to establish the deviation of the inherent tag orientation from the head–tail axis, and to estimate body length (electronic supplementary material, figure S10a). Head and body capsules were annotated around each tag so that pairwise interactions could be inferred from the overlap of head capsules (electronic supplementary material, figure S10b,c). The proportion of time each individual spent in each box and the number of times each individual interacted with each other individual were then calculated using the python package FortMyrmidon. Individuals that were detected less than 2 × s.d. below the colony mean detection count were excluded from all analyses.

(c). Statistical methods

All statistical analyses were conducted using R v. 4.3.2. The t-tests, Pearson’s correlations, one-sample proportion tests and ANOVAs were calculated in base R using functions t.test(), cor.test(), aov() and prop.test(), respectively. GLMM’s and generalized additive models were calculated using packages lme4 and mgcv, respectively. Analysis code is available at: [75].

(d). Soft community detection

Soft modularity, a measure of how well a proposed number of communities fits a given network, was calculated for n communities = 2, 3, 4 and 5 for every network using a freely available implementation of the FacetNet algorithm (https://c4science.ch/source/facet_unil) [12,59,76]. For all species, soft modularity peaked when n communities was set to 2, and so using this community number, the soft modularity was calculated for each of the 100 randomly rewired networks for all 25 colonies. The values observed for the original social contact networks were then compared to the distributions obtained from randomly rewired networks to test whether the observed networks were more modular than expected by chance (i.e. whether there was significant discontinuity in social interactions).

FacetNet additionally outputs community membership scores for each individual to each community, and we use membership to the foraging community (identified by the absence of the queen [20]) to define ‘social maturity’. This metric ranges from 0 to 1, with 0 indicating that an individual is maximally affiliated with the nurse community and 1 indicating that an individual is maximally affiliated with the forager community.

(e). Summary metric calculation

  • Node strength was calculated as the summed weight of all node edges using the function strength in R package ‘iGraph’ [77];

  • node entropy was calculated using the function Entropy in R package ‘DescTools’ [78] over the vector of the number of interactions that a given node has with all other nodes, including 0 values where the two individuals did not interact;

  • 90% home range sizes were calculated for every individual by ranking hexagons from the most visited to the least visited and then, starting with the most visited, sequentially adding hexagons to the home range until it covered at least 90% of total detection events for the individual; and

  • the conservation in the identity of workers with which a queen interacts from one hour to the next was calculated as the number of workers that the queen interacted with in hour t that the queen had also interacted with in hour t − 1 divided by the total number of workers with which the queen interacted in hour t − 1.

Acknowledgements

We thank Christine La Mendola for help setting up the experiments and Daniel Kronauer for feedback on an earlier version of this manuscript.

Contributor Information

Tomas Kay, Email: tkay@rockefeller.edu.

Alba Motes-Rodrigo, Email: Alba.MotesRodrigo@unil.ch.

Arthur Royston, Email: arthuroyston@gmail.com.

Thomas O. Richardson, Email: thomas.o.richardson@bristol.ac.uk.

Nathalie Stroeymeyt, Email: Nathalie.Stroeymeyt@bristol.ac.uk; nathalie.Stroeymeyt@gmail.com.

Laurent Keller, Email: laurentkeller01@gmail.com.

Ethics

This work did not require ethical approval from a human subject or animal welfare committee.

Data accessibility

Data and code are available on Dryad [79].

Supplementary material is available online [80].

Declaration of AI use

We have not used AI-assisted technologies in creating this article.

Authors’ contributions

T.K.: conceptualization, data curation, formal analysis, investigation, project administration, visualization, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; A.M.-R.: data curation, investigation, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; A.R.: data curation, investigation, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; T.O.R.: formal analysis, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; N.S.: formal analysis, writing—original draft, writing—review and editing; L.K.: supervision, writing—original draft, writing— review and editing.

All authors gave final approval for publication and agreed to be held accountable for the work performed therein.

Conflict of interest declaration

We declare we have no competing interests.

Funding

L.K. acknowledges funding by the European Research Council (ERC advanced grant, ‘resiliANT’, no. 741491).

References

  • 1. Schultheiss P, Nooten SS, Wang R, Wong MKL, Brassard F, Guénard B. 2022. The abundance, biomass, and distribution of ants on earth. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 119 , e2201550119. ( 10.1073/pnas.2201550119) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2. Ulrich Y, Saragosti J, Tokita CK, Tarnita CE, Kronauer DJ. 2018. Fitness benefits and emergent division of labour at the onset of group living. Nature 560 , 635–638. ( 10.1038/s41586-018-0422-6) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3. Berdahl A, Torney CJ, Ioannou CC, Faria JJ, Couzin ID. 2013. Emergent sensing of complex environments by mobile animal groups. Science 339 , 574–576. ( 10.1126/science.1225883) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4. Waters JS, Holbrook CT, Fewell JH, Harrison JF. 2010. Allometric scaling of metabolism, growth, and activity in whole colonies of the seed-harvester ant Pogonomyrmex californicus. Am. Nat. 176 , 501–510. ( 10.1086/656266) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5. Strassmann JE, Queller DC. 2019. Ecological determinants of social evolution. In The genetics of social evolution (eds Breed M, Page, Jr. R), pp. 81–101. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 6. Dornhaus A, Powell S, Bengston S. 2012. Group size and its effects on collective organization. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 57 , 123–141. ( 10.1146/annurev-ento-120710-100604) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7. Mersch DP, Crespi A, Keller L. 2013. Tracking individuals shows spatial fidelity is a key regulator of ant social organization. Science 340 , 1090–1093. ( 10.1126/science.1234316) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8. Gal A, Saragosti J, Kronauer DJ. 2020. anTrax, a software package for high-throughput video tracking of color-tagged insects. Elife 9 , e58145. ( 10.7554/eLife.58145) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9. Smith ML, Davidson JD, Wild B, Dormagen DM, Landgraf T, Couzin ID. 2022. Behavioral variation across the days and lives of honey bees. iScience 25 , 104842. ( 10.1016/j.isci.2022.104842) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10. Smith MAY, Easton-Calabria A, Zhang T, Zmyslony S, Thuma J, Cronin K, Pasadyn CL, de Bivort BL, Crall JD. 2022. Long-term tracking and quantification of individual behavior in bumble bee colonies. Artif. Life Robot. 27 , 401–406. ( 10.1007/s10015-022-00762-x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 11. Choi SH, et al. 2021. Universal scaling laws of interaction time distribution in honeybee and human social networks. APS March Meet. Abstr. 2021 , E14–E005. [Google Scholar]
  • 12. Richardson TO, Stroeymeyt N, Crespi A, Keller L. 2022. Two simple movement mechanisms for spatial division of labour in social insects. Nat. Commun. 13 , 6985. ( 10.1038/s41467-022-34706-7) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13. Ulrich Y, Kawakatsu M, Tokita CK, Saragosti J, Chandra V, Tarnita CE, Kronauer DJC. 2021. Response thresholds alone cannot explain empirical patterns of division of labour in social insects. PLoS Biol. 19 , e3001269. ( 10.1371/journal.pbio.3001269) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14. Rosset H, Keller L, Chapuisat M. 2005. Experimental manipulation of colony genetic diversity had no effect on short-term task efficiency in the Argentine ant Linepithema humile. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 58 , 87–98. ( 10.1007/s00265-004-0890-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 15. Wild B, Dormagen DM, Zachariae A, Smith ML, Traynor KS, Brockmann D, Couzin ID, Landgraf T. 2021. Social networks predict the life and death of honey bees. Nat. Commun. 12 , 1–12. ( 10.1038/s41467-021-21212-5) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16. Kay T, et al. 2023. Social network position is a major predictor of ant behavior, microbiota composition, and brain gene expression. PLoS Biol. 21 , e3002203. ( 10.1371/journal.pbio.3002203) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17. Stroeymeyt N, et al. 2018. Social network plasticity decreases disease transmission in a eusocial insect. Science 362 , 941–945. ( 10.1126/science.aat4793) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18. Alciatore G, Ugelvig LV, Frank E, Bidaux J, Gal A, Schmitt T, Kronauer DJC, Ulrich Y. 2021. Immune challenges increase network centrality in a queenless ant. Proc. R. Soc. B 288 , 20211456. ( 10.1098/rspb.2021.1456) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19. Liberti J, Kay T, Quinn A, Kesner L, Frank ET, Cabirol A, Richardson TO, Engel P, Keller L. 2022. The gut microbiota affects the social network of honeybees. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 6 , 1471–1479. ( 10.1038/s41559-022-01840-w) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20. Richardson TO, Kay T, Braunschweig R, Journeau OA, Rüegg M, McGregor S, Los Rios PD, Keller L. 2021. Ant behavioral maturation is mediated by a stochastic transition between two fundamental states. Curr. Biol. 31 , 2253–2260.( 10.1016/j.cub.2020.05.038) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21. Traniello IM, et al. 2022. Context-dependent influence of threat on honey bee social network dynamics and brain gene expression. J. Exp. Biol. 225 , jeb243738. ( 10.1242/jeb.243738) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22. Greenwald E, Segre E, Feinerman O. 2015. Ant trophallactic networks: simultaneous measurement of interaction patterns and food dissemination. Sci. Rep. 5 , 1–11. ( 10.1038/srep12496) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23. Baltiansky L, Frankel G, Feinerman O. 2023. Emergent regulation of ant foraging frequency through a computationally inexpensive forager movement rule. Elife 12 , e77659. ( 10.7554/eLife.77659) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24. Feinerman O, Pinkoviezky I, Gelblum A, Fonio E, Gov NS. 2018. The physics of cooperative transport in groups of ants. Nat. Phys. 14 , 683–693. ( 10.1038/s41567-018-0107-y) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 25. Gal A, Kronauer DJ. 2022. The emergence of a collective sensory response threshold in ant colonies. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 119 , e2123076119. ( 10.1073/pnas.2123076119) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26. Gernat T, Jagla T, Jones BM, Middendorf M, Robinson GE. 2023. Automated monitoring of honey bees with barcodes and artificial intelligence reveals two distinct social networks from a single affiliative behavior. Sci. Rep. 13 , 1541. ( 10.1038/s41598-022-26825-4) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27. Guo X, Lin MR, Azizi A, Saldyt LP, Kang Y, Pavlic TP, Fewell JH. 2022. Decoding alarm signal propagation of seed-harvester ants using automated movement tracking and supervised machine learning. Proc. R. Soc. B 289 , 20212176. ( 10.1098/rspb.2021.2176) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28. Shtovba SD. 2005. Ant algorithms: theory and applications. Program. Comput. Soft. 31 , 167–178. ( 10.1007/s11086-005-0029-1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29. Wilson S, Pavlic TP, Kumar GP, Buffin A, Pratt SC, Berman S. 2014. Design of ant-inspired stochastic control policies for collective transport by robotic swarms. Swarm Intell. 8 , 303–327. ( 10.1007/s11721-014-0100-8) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 30. Kube CR, Bonabeau E. 2000. Cooperative transport by ants and robots. Rob. Auton. Syst. 30 , 85–101. ( 10.1016/S0921-8890(99)00066-4) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 31. Keller L. 1993. Queen number and sociality in insects. Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 32. Molet M. 2021. Worker polymorphism: ants. In Encyclopedia of social insects (ed. Starr C), pp. 1047–1049. New York, NY: Springer. ( 10.1007/978-3-030-28102-1_138) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 33. Sundström L. 1994. Sex ratio bias, relatedness asymmetry and queen mating frequency in ants. Nature 367 , 266–268. ( 10.1038/367266a0) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 34. Tschinkel WR. 2015. The architecture of subterranean ant nests: beauty and mystery underfoot. J. Bioecon. 17 , 271–291. ( 10.1007/s10818-015-9203-6) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 35. Traniello JF. 1989. Foraging strategies of ants. Annu. Rev. Entomol. 34 , 191–210. ( 10.1146/annurev.ento.34.1.191) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 36. Meurville MP, LeBoeuf AC. 2021. Trophallaxis: the functions and evolution of social fluid exchange in ant colonies (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Myrmecol. News 31 , 1–30. ( 10.25849/myrmecol.news_031:001) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 37. Bertelsmeier C, Luque GM, Confais A, Courchamp F. 2013. Antprofiler-a database of ecological characteristics of ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Myrmecol. News 18 , 73–76. ( 10.25849/myrmecol.news_018:073) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 38. Mersch DP, La Mendola C, Keller L. 2017. Camponotus fellah queens are singly mated. Insectes Soc. 64 , 269–276. ( 10.1007/s00040-017-0543-1) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 39. Gadau J, Strehl C ‐P., Oettler J, Hölldobler B. 2003. Determinants of intracolonial relatedness in Pogonomyrmex rugosus (Hymenoptera; Formicidae): mating frequency and brood raids. Mol. Ecol. 12 , 1931–1938. ( 10.1046/j.1365-294X.2003.01853.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40. van Wilgenburg E, Ryan D, Morrison P, Marriott PJ, Elgar MA. 2006. Nest-and colony-mate recognition in polydomous colonies of meat ants (Iridomyrmex purpureus). Naturwissenschaften 93 , 309–314. ( 10.1007/s00114-006-0109-y) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41. van Wilgenburg E, Elgar MA. 2007. Colony structure and spatial distribution of food resources in the polydomous meat ant Iridomyrmex purpureus. Insectes Soc. 54 , 5–10. ( 10.1007/s00040-007-0903-3) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 42. Kikuta N, Tsuji K. 1999. Queen and worker policing in the monogynous and monandrous ant, Diacamma sp. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 46 , 180–189. ( 10.1007/s002650050608) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 43. Annagiri S. 2021. Diacamma. In Encyclopedia of social insects (ed. Starr C), pp. 325–330. New York, NY: Springer. ( 10.1007/978-3-030-28102-1_35) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 44. Haskins CP, Whelden RM. 1965. 'queenlessness,’ worker sibship, and colony versus population structure in the formicid genus Rhytidoponera. Psyche 72 , 87–112. ( 10.1155/1965/40465) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 45. Chapuisat M, Crozier R. 2001. Low relatedness among cooperatively breeding workers of the greenhead ant Rhytidoponera metallica. J. Evol. Biol. 14 , 564–573. ( 10.1046/j.1420-9101.2001.00310.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 46. MacKay WP. 1981. A comparison of the nest phenologies of three species of pogonomyrmex harvester ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae). Psyche 88 , 25–74. ( 10.1155/1981/78635) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47. Carew M, Tay W, Crozier R. 1997. Polygyny via unrelated queens indicated by mitochondrial DNA variation in the Australian meat ant Iridomyrmex purpureus. Insectes Soc. 44 , 7–14. ( 10.1007/s000400050018) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48. van Wilgenburg E, Mulder RA, Elgar MA. 2006. Intracolony relatedness and polydomy in the Australian meat ant, lridomyrmex purpureus. Aust. J. Zool. 54 , 117. ( 10.1071/ZO05075) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49. Halliday RB. 1981. Heterozygosity and genetic distance in sibling species of meat ants (Iridomyrmex purpureus group). Evolution 35 , 234–242. ( 10.1111/j.1558-5646.1981.tb04883.x) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50. Fukumoto Y, Abe T. 1983. Social organization of colony movement in the tropical ponerine ant, Diacamma rugosum (Le Guillou). J. Ethol. 1 , 101–108. ( 10.1007/BF02347836) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51. Thomas ML. 2002. Causes and consequences of colony size in the ponerine ant Rhytidoponera metallica. PhD thesis, Department of Zoology, University of Melbourne, Melbourne, Australia. [Google Scholar]
  • 52. Capinera JL. 2008. Encyclopedia of entomology. New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  • 53. Hölldobler B, Wilson EO. 1990. The ants. Cambridge, Massachusetts, United States: Harvard University Press. [Google Scholar]
  • 54. Boulay R, Katzav-Gozansky T, Vander Meer RK, Hefetz A. 2003. Colony insularity through queen control on worker social motivation in ants. Proc. R. Soc. B 270 , 971–977. ( 10.1098/rspb.2002.2325) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55. Hölldobler B. 1976. The behavioral ecology of mating in harvester ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae: Pogonomyrmex). Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 1 , 405–423. ( 10.1007/BF00299401) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 56. Whitford WG. 1976. Foraging behavior of chihuahuan desert harvester ants. Am. Midl. Nat. 95 , 455–458. ( 10.2307/2424409) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57. Newman ME. 2006. Modularity and community structure in networks. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103 , 8577–8582. ( 10.1073/pnas.0601602103) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58. Li X, Sun Q. 2021. Identifying and ranking influential nodes in complex networks based on dynamic node strength. Algorithms 14 , 82. ( 10.3390/a14030082) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 59. Lin YR, Chi Y, Zhu S, Sundaram H, Tseng BL. Facetnet: a framework for analyzing communities and their evolutions in dynamic networks. In Proc. of the 17th Int. Conf. on World Wide Web, pp. 685–694. New York, NY, United States: Association for Computing Machinery. ( 10.1145/1367497.1367590) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 60. Albery GF, Clutton-Brock TH, Morris A, Morris S, Pemberton JM, Nussey DH, Firth JA. 2022. Ageing red deer alter their spatial behaviour and become less social. Nat. Ecol. Evol. 6 , 1231–1238. ( 10.1038/s41559-022-01817-9) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61. Wrzus C, Hänel M, Wagner J, Neyer FJ. 2013. Social network changes and life events across the life span: a meta-analysis. Psychol. Bull. 139 , 53–80. ( 10.1037/a0028601) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62. Barabási AL. 2009. Scale-free networks: a decade and beyond. Science 325 , 412–413. ( 10.1126/science.1173299) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63. Barabási AL, Bonabeau E. 2003. Scale-free networks. Sci. Am. 288 , 60–69. ( 10.1038/scientificamerican0503-60) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64. Broido AD, Clauset A. 2019. Scale-free networks are rare. Nat. Commun. 10 , 1–10. ( 10.1038/s41467-019-08746-5) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65. Monnin T, Ratnieks FL. 2001. Policing in queenless ponerine ants. Behav. Ecol. Sociobiol. 50 , 97–108. ( 10.1007/s002650100351) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 66. Van Oystaeyen A, et al. 2014. Conserved class of queen pheromones stops social insect workers from reproducing. Science 343 , 287–290. ( 10.1126/science.1244899) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67. Borowiec ML, Rabeling C, Brady SG, Fisher BL, Schultz TR, Ward PS. 2019. Compositional heterogeneity and outgroup choice influence the internal phylogeny of the ants. Mol. Phylogenet. Evol. 134 , 111–121. ( 10.1016/j.ympev.2019.01.024) [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68. Economo EP, Narula N, Friedman NR, Weiser MD, Guénard B. 2018. Macroecology and macroevolution of the latitudinal diversity gradient in ants. Nat. Commun. 9 , 1778. ( 10.1038/s41467-018-04218-4) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69. Borowiec ML, Moreau CS, Rabeling C. 2021. Ants: phylogeny and classification. In Encyclopedia of social insects (ed. Starr C), pp. 52–69. New York, NY: Springer. [Google Scholar]
  • 70. Rood J. 1978. Dwarf mongoose helpers at the den 1. Z. Tierpsychol. 48 , 277–287. ( 10.1111/j.1439-0310.1978.tb00260.x) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 71. Clutton-Brock TH, Brotherton PN, O’Riain MJ, Griffin AS, Gaynor D, Sharpe L, Kansky R, Manser MB, McIlrath GM. 2000. Individual contributions to babysitting in a cooperative mongoose, Suricata suricatta. Proc. R. Soc. B 267 , 301–305. ( 10.1098/rspb.2000.1000) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72. Bhatkar A, Whitcomb WH. 1970. Artificial diet for rearing various species of ants. Flo. Entomol. 53 , 229. ( 10.2307/3493193) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 73. Bruno F, Angilica A, Cosco F, Luchi ML, Muzzupappa M. 2011. Mixed prototyping environment with different video tracking techniques. In IMProVe 2011 Int. Conf. on Innovative Methods in Product Design, pp. 105–113. Padova, Italy: Libreria Internazionale Cortina Padova. [Google Scholar]
  • 74. Rüegg M, Motes‐Rodrigo A, Tuleu A, Stroeymeyt N, Richardson TO, Sakar MS, Keller L. 2024. Precise tactile stimulation of worker ants by a robotic manipulator reveals that individual responses are density-and context-dependent. Methods Ecol. Evol. 15 , 117–129. ( 10.1111/2041-210X.14244) [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 75. Kay T. 2024. Data from: Ant social network structure is highly conserved. Dryad Digital Repository. ( 10.5061/dryad.dfn2z358r) [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 76. Ruegg M. 2019. facetUNIL. See https://c4science.ch/source/facet_unil.
  • 77. Csardi MG, Nepusz T. 2006. The igraph software. Complex Syst. 1695 , 1–9. [Google Scholar]
  • 78. Signorell A, et al. 2023. DescTools: tools for descriptive statistics. See https://cran.r-project.org/package=DescTools.
  • 79. Kay T, Motes-Rodrigo A, Royston A, Richardson TO, Stroeymeyt N, Keller L. 2024. Data from: Ant social network structure is highly conserved across species. Dryad Digital Repository. ( 10.5061/dryad.dfn2z358r) [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 80. Kay T, Motes Rodrigo A, Arthur R, Richardson TO, Stroeymeyt N, Keller L. 2024. Data from: Ant social network structure is highly conserved across species. Figshare. ( 10.6084/m9.figshare.c.7361875) [DOI] [PubMed]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Data Availability Statement

Data and code are available on Dryad [79].

Supplementary material is available online [80].


Articles from Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences are provided here courtesy of The Royal Society

RESOURCES