Abstract
Patients with moderate-to-severe atopic dermatitis (AD) experience intense chronic itch and impaired sleep. Reports from parents and teachers suggest that AD patients may also have attention problems. However, attention has not yet been directly assessed in AD patients. We utilized an objective, computer-based continuous performance test (CPT) validated for use in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) diagnosis to formally evaluate attention in adolescent AD subjects. This was a single-visit, cross-sectional, non-interventional study of moderate-to-severe (Investigator’s Global Assessment [IGA] ≥ 3) AD subjects aged 12–17 years without clinician-diagnosed ADHD. Attention was evaluated using two performance-based measures: Conners, CPT-3 and the Stroop Color and Word Test. The primary parameter was CPT-3 detectability (d’) measure. Lesional severity measures included Eczema Area and Severity Index (EASI) and body surface area (BSA) involvement. Subjects completed self-report rating scales assessing sensory responsiveness patterns (Adult/Adolescent Sensory Profile [AASP]), itch (Peak Pruritus Numerical Rating Scale [PP-NRS]), skin pain, quality of life, sleep, anxiety, and depressive symptoms. A total of 44 subjects were included in the study (61.4% female; mean age 15.0 [SD 1.78] years; mean EASI 20.4 [SD 7.8]; mean PP-NRS 7.0 [SD 1.8]). Results indicated substantial disease impact on sleep, quality of life, and comorbid anxiety and depressive symptoms. The mean (SD) Conners, CPT-3 dʹ T-score was 48.7 (SD 10.7), similar to the expected mean from a randomly selected age/gender-matched sample of the general population (50 [SD 10], by definition). Overall, 13.6% of subjects exhibited a dʹ T-score ≥ 60 (clinically significant poor performance), which was not greater than the expected general population value (15.9%). Subject-level data review by two psychologists determined that only 2 subjects demonstrated an overall response pattern that clearly indicated attention deficit. Many subjects had atypical sensory responsiveness profiles: sensory hypersensitivity (38.6%), sensory avoidance (50%), and low registration (hypo-sensitivity, 36.4%). Adolescents with moderate-to-severe AD without existing ADHD diagnosis did not demonstrate greater attention problems on performance-based measures than would be expected in age/gender-matched peers.
Trial registration NCT05203380.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s00403-024-03210-x.
Keywords: Atopic dermatitis; Pruritus; ADHD; Conners, Continuous Performance Test; Attention; Mental health
Plain Language Summary
Atopic dermatitis (often shortened to AD) is a long-term skin disease that causes intense itching. It affects patients’ lives in many ways, including interrupting their sleep. Parents and teachers of young people with AD have sometimes suggested that AD may also cause attention problems. But this has never been tested properly.
We measured the attention of 44 adolescents aged between 12 and 17 years who all had moderate-to-severe AD. We used computerized tests of attention that were developed for young people with ADHD (attention deficit hyperactivity disorder). Also, we made sure that none of the 44 patients had also been diagnosed with ADHD. The severity and extent of the patients’ AD was measured by doctors. We also used some measures that allowed the patients to report how AD affected their lives, including things like itch, skin pain, quality of life, sleep, anxiety, and depression.
The adolescent patients reported that AD had a negative effect on various areas of their lives, including sleep and quality of life, and that it resulted in anxiety and symptoms of depression. However, the results of the attention tests in adolescents with AD were similar to what would usually be expected in adolescents without AD. Only 2 of the 44 patients with AD were found to have clear evidence of attention problems.
The study concluded that adolescents with moderate-to-severe AD did not have any greater attention problems than would usually be expected in adolescents without AD.
Supplementary Information
The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s00403-024-03210-x.
Introduction
Atopic dermatitis (AD) is a chronic, systemic, inflammatory disease characterized by intense itch [1] and sleep loss [2–5]. AD exerts a substantial psychological burden; symptoms of depression and anxiety are commonly reported in patients with moderate-to-severe AD [6–8], and young children (aged 18–48 months) with AD exhibit greater clinginess and fearfulness than matched controls [9]. Since patients in all age groups with moderate-to-severe AD may spend up to 4 months annually experiencing an eczematous flare [10], the psychological impact of AD can be chronic and transcend periods of disease quiescence. Adults with moderate-to-severe AD report worsened mental health compared with the overall US population [11].
Itch is an inherently strong consumer of attention [12], and sleep loss is likewise associated with inattentiveness [13], suggesting that inattentiveness could be part of the overall psychological burden of AD. Several studies have used subjective parent- and/or teacher-rated assessment tools to attempt to address the potential burden of inattentiveness in children with versus without AD (and without diagnosed ADHD). One study demonstrated greater subjective reports of inattentiveness in children with AD [14], while a second study showed that children with AD had a significantly higher prevalence of observer-reported ADHD symptoms [15], despite exclusion of patients with an ADHD diagnosis. Another study concluded that severe AD in pre-school age children was correlated with poor sleep health and attention dysregulation [16]. These findings parallel results observed in a study of children and adolescents with chronic pain, of whom 20% manifested subjectively rated, clinically significant ADHD symptoms, despite fewer than 5% having been diagnosed with ADHD [17].
This study was designed to assess whether adolescents with moderate-to-severe AD demonstrate impaired ability to sustain attention using an objective task. Conners, Continuous Performance Test, Third Edition (CPT-3), was chosen as the primary outcome assessment tool due to its frequent use in clinical evaluation of attention problems and the correlation between outcome parameters of CPT-3 and the specific symptoms of ADHD, including those suggested by the study in AD patients by Kruse and colleagues [14, 18]. Specifically, Conners, CPT-3 yields a set of parameters that describe the subject’s performance, which a neuropsychologist can use to evaluate four distinct domains of attention: inattentiveness, impulsivity, sustained attention, and vigilance. CPT-3 can facilitate ADHD diagnosis, in that ADHD patients exhibit worse performance across most parameters of the task than the general population; patients with other disorders with impaired executive function, such as anxiety disorder and autism spectrum disorder, also demonstrate worse performance on Conners, CPT-3. Conners, CPT-3 has been validated for use in adolescent populations [19].
Subjects
This single-visit, cross-sectional, non-interventional study enrolled adolescents aged ≥ 12 to ≤ 17 years with moderate-to-severe AD, defined by having an Investigator’s Global Assessment (IGA) score of 3 or 4 [20], an Eczema Area and Severity Index (EASI) score of ≥ 12 [21], a Peak Pruritus Numerical Rating Scale (PP-NRS) score of ≥ 4 [22], and Body Surface Area (BSA) of AD involvement of ≥ 10% at enrollment [23].
Exclusion criteria included use of dupilumab within 6 months, systemic antihistamine or nicotine use within one week, or use of the following ADHD medications within 8 weeks or 5 half-lives (whichever was longer) prior to the visit: methylphenidate, dexmethylphenidate, serdexmethylphenidate, amphetamine, dextroamphetamine, lisdexamfetamine, guanfacine, atomoxetine, clonidine, and viloxazine. Also excluded were subjects with a history of clinician-diagnosed ADHD, autism spectrum disorder, epilepsy, major depressive disorder, mania or bipolar disorder, or any Diagnostic and Statistical Manual-V (DSM-V) psychotic disorder such as schizophrenia. Other exclusion criteria were substance abuse, including alcohol or nicotine, in the prior 2 years or requiring institutionalization.
Subject-reported assessments
AD symptoms were quantified by the PP-NRS and Skin Pain (SP)-NRS [24]. Other measures included the Children’s Dermatology Life Quality Index (CDLQI) [25], Patient-Reported Outcomes Measurement Information System (PROMIS) Sleep Disturbance [26], and Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) [27], which were used to assess quality of life affected by AD, sleep disturbance, and symptoms of anxiety and depression, respectively.
Sensory responsiveness patterns were assessed using the Adult/Adolescent Sensory Profile (AASP), a 60-item self-reported questionnaire that measures sensory responsiveness patterns in six sensory modalities, including taste/smell, movement, vision, touch, activity, and auditory processing [28]. Four domains of sensory processing were assessed separately by 15 questions each: sensory sensitivity, low registration (hypo-sensitivity), sensory avoidance, and sensory seeking; for each domain, normed total scores ranged from 15 to 75. For adolescents (aged 11–17 years), a subject was designated “similar to most people” if they had scores of 26–40 for sensory sensitivity and sensory avoidance, 27–40 for low registration, and 42–58 for sensory seeking.
Neuropsychologic assessments
The primary assessment tool was Conners, CPT-3, an objective and computerized measure of attention-related problems [19]. CPT-3 is a 14-minute computerized task, in which the subject is shown strings of letters in 6 blocks and at inter-stimulus intervals (ISIs) of 1, 2, or 4 seconds. The subject is asked to press a specific key as quickly as possible when each letter is displayed, but to not respond when the letter “X” appears on the screen. Response accuracy and response times are measured and converted to standard scores (T-scores) using age and gender norms. Detectability (d’), which measures the subject’s ability to discriminate “non-targets” (i.e., the letter X) from “targets” (all other letters), was used as the primary outcome parameter. Additional norm-referenced data from this task included response style, error analysis (omission errors, commission errors, perseverations), reaction time statistics (hit reaction time [HRT]), HRT standard deviation, variability, HRT block change, and HRT inter-stimulus interval), as well as raw score omissions and commissions by block.
The Stroop Color and Word Test was used to measure selective attention, or the subject’s ability to attend to a salient stimulus while inhibiting an obtrusive irrelevant stimulus [29, 30]. The methods of the Stroop Color and Word Test are detailed in the supplementary methods (Supplementary appendix S1). By definition, average ability to selectively attend to the salient stimulus is represented by a Stroop Interference T-score of 50, with higher values representing better performance (Supplementary appendix S1).
All subject-level data from Conners, CPT-3 and the Stroop tests were interpreted independently by two clinical psychologists (SB and JJ), who categorized subjects into three groups: Group 1: very consistent with impaired attention; Group 2: possible evidence of impaired attention, and Group 3: no evident impairment.
Sample size
By definition, the mean dʹ T-score of a randomly selected sample is expected to be 50, with a standard deviation (SD) of 10; equivalently, 15.9% of a random sample would be expected to have a dʹ T-score of at least 60, a threshold that is considered to represent clinically significant poor performance (here, higher values for T-scores represent worse performance). We hypothesized that a modest but measurable degree of inattentiveness would be prevalent in adolescents with moderate-to-severe AD. To determine an adequate sample size, we therefore assumed that this population would have mean a dʹ T-score of 55 and SD of 10; equivalently, 30.9% of the subjects with AD were expected to have a dʹ T-score of ≥ 60. Under these assumptions, it was determined that 44 subjects would need to be enrolled to test the hypothesis that there are higher rates of attention problems in the AD sample.
Ethical conduct of the study
This study was conducted in accordance with consensus ethical principles derived from international guidelines, including the Declaration of Helsinki, and consistent with the International Conference on Harmonisation Good Clinical Practice guideline and applicable regulatory requirements. All subjects provided informed assent and a parent or caregiver provided informed consent prior to the patient undergoing any study-related procedure. This study was registered at ClinicalTrials.gov (Identifier: NCT05203380).
Results
Patient demographics
A total of 45 subjects were enrolled at 9 US sites between January 2022 and March 2023; however, one individual with a low PP-NRS score was enrolled in error and excluded from further analysis. The evaluable population (N = 44) was 61.4% female and had a mean (SD) age of 15.0 (1.78) years (Table 1). Concomitant medical conditions included seasonal allergy (40.9%), food allergy (34.1%), asthma (31.8%), allergic rhinitis (18.2%), and acne (11.4%) (Table 1). Use of at least one medication was reported by 35 (79.5%) subjects, most frequently topical corticosteroids (20 subjects; 45.5%) and drugs for airway diseases (13; 29.5%). Four subjects used immunosuppressants (4; 9.1%).
Table 1.
Efficacy analysis set | (n = 44) |
---|---|
Age, mean (SD) | 15.0 (1.78) |
Female, n(%) | 27 (61.4) |
Race, n (%) | |
White | 28 (63.6) |
Black/African American | 8 (18.2) |
Asian | 6 (13.6) |
Multiple | 2 (4.5) |
Ethnicity, n(%) | |
Hispanic/Latino | 29 (65.9) |
Not Hispanic/Latino | 15 (34.1) |
Concomitant medical conditionsa | |
Seasonal allergy | 18 (40.9%) |
Food allergy | 15 (34.1%) |
Asthma | 14 (31.8%) |
Allergic rhinitis | 8 (18.2%) |
Acne | 5 (11.4%) |
aConcomitant medical condition reported in ≥ 10% of subjects
A total of 34 (77.3%) subjects had IGA = 3 (moderate) and 10 (22.7%) had IGA = 4 (severe) AD. Subjects had mean (SD) EASI scores of 20.4 (7.8), BSA of 33.8 (17.9), and mean (SD) symptom scores of 7.0 (1.8) for PP-NRS and 5.2 (2.9) for SP-NRS. Together, these scores are consistent with poorly controlled moderate-to-severe AD at time of visit. Subjects had mean scores (SD) of 11.3 (7.4) for CDLQI, 61.1 (8.9) for PROMIS Sleep Disturbance, 10.3 (2.3) for HADS Anxiety, and 13.0 (2.0) for HADS Depression, indicating considerable quality-of-life and sleep impairment, in addition to high levels of anxiety and depressive symptoms, respectively (Table 2)
Table 2.
Disease characteristic | (n = 44) |
---|---|
PROMIS Sleep Disturbance (T-score), mean (SD) | 61.1 (8.9) |
IGA = 3, n (%) | 34 (77.3) |
BSA (percentage range: 0–100%), mean (SD) | 33.8 (17.9) |
EASI (score range: 0–72), mean (SD) | 20.4 (7.8) |
HADS Depression (score range: 0–21), mean (SD) | 13.0 (2.0) |
CDLQI (score range: 0–30), mean (SD) | 11.3 (7.4) |
HADS Anxiety (score range: 0–21), mean (SD) | 10.3 (2.3) |
Peak Pruritus NRS (score range: 0–10), mean (SD) | 7.0 (1.8) |
Skin Pain NRS (score range: 0–10), mean (SD) | 5.3 (2.9) |
Mean (SD) scores for each of the 4 domains of the AASP appeared to be close to normal: sensory sensitivity 38.0 (10.5), sensory avoidance 41.3 (10.6), low registration 37.4 (9.6), and sensory seeking 45.3 (7.9). However, a substantial proportion of the subjects had values that were more or much more than most people for the domains of sensory sensitivity (17 of 44 [38.6%]), sensory avoidance (22 of 44 [50%]), and low registration (16 of 44 [36.4%]). In contrast, only 2 subjects (4.5%) had atypically high sensory seeking values.
Neuropsychologic outcomes
In our sample, the mean (SD) Conners, CPT-3 dʹ T-score was 48.7 (10.7), and 13.6% subjects (CI 3.5–23.8%) had a dʹ T-score ≥ 60, consistent with the proportion expected in the general population. Stroop interference T-scores were not calculated for 7 subjects, per the procedure described in Supplemental Methods. For the remaining 37 subjects, the mean (SD) interference T-score was 55.5 (7.54); the median interference T-score was 55 (IQR 52–61).
Further review of the CPT-3 and Stroop data was conducted by two psychologists, who independently reviewed the results for all parameters of these tests for each participant. Independent ratings had high levels of agreement, suggesting consistency of interpretation across clinicians. Both psychologists agreed that the CPT-3 results for one subject were invalid and excluded this patient from their review. Of the 43 valid CPT-3 profiles, 5 subjects (11.6%) had a dʹ T-score ≥ 60, which remains numerically lower than the expected proportion (15.9%) of individuals of this age group. Regarding the Stroop Interference scores, 2 subjects were rated as having a “below average” performance, while 12 were rated as having either “above average” or “very good” performance (the remaining subjects with valid interference scores had average performance).
Given the Conners, and Stroop subject-level data, the psychologists categorized 2 of 43 subjects (4.7%) into Group 1 (clear indication of attention problems) and 9 of 43 (20.9%) into Group 2 (possible evidence of attention problems). Of the subjects categorized as Group 1, the Stroop Interference T-score of one subject was normal (57), and one subject’s score was invalid. Given the low numbers of subjects with clear evidence of attention problems, the distribution of AD lesional or symptomatic scores across these three groupings could not be compared.
Correlations of neuropsychologic outcomes and sensory processing with AD signs and symptoms
No significant correlations were noted between Conners, CPT-3 d’ T-score and any measure of AD signs and symptoms, sleep disturbance, quality of life, or mood (Table 3). Similarly, no correlations were noted between Stroop Interference T-score and any of the same measures (not shown).
Table 3.
d’ T-score | Sensory sensitivity | Sensory avoidance | Low registration | ||
---|---|---|---|---|---|
EASI | ra | −0.190 | −0.287 | −0.225 | −0.226 |
p-value | 0.219 | 0.059 | 0.143 | 0.141 | |
BSA | ra | −0.194 | −0.265 | −0.246 | −0.234 |
p-value | 0.209 | 0.083 | 0.108 | 0.126 | |
PP-NRS | ra | 0.051 | 0.274 | 0.436 | 0.318 |
p-value | 0.746 | 0.072 | 0.003 | 0.035 | |
SP-NRS | ra | 0.030 | 0.294 | 0.367 | 0.233 |
p-value | 0.848 | 0.052 | 0.014 | 0.128 | |
PROMIS Sleep Disturbance | ra | −0.132 | 0.584 | 0.529 | 0.486 |
p-value | 0.397 | < 0.0001 | 0.0002 | 0.0007 | |
CDLQI | ra | 0.002 | 0.423 | 0.599 | 0.382 |
p-value | 0.991 | 0.004 | < 0.0001 | 0.010 | |
HADS Anxiety subscale | ra | 0.167 | 0.5778 | 0.639 | 0.473 |
p-value | 0.281 | < 0.0001 | < 0.0001 | 0.001 | |
HADS Depression subscale | ra | −0.087 | −0.211 | −0.076 | −0.075 |
p-value | 0.577 | 0.171 | 0.628 | 0.633 | |
IGA | ra | −0.122 | −0.209 | −0.088 | −0.155 |
p-value | 0.433 | 0.175 | 0.573 | 0.318 |
a r, Pearson correlation coefficient. Sensory seeking scores did not significantly correlate with any of the parameters listed in this table
Significant correlations were observed for the sensory sensitivity score and PROMIS Sleep Disturbance, CDLQI, and the HADS Anxiety subscale. Significant correlations were also observed for the sensory avoidance score and Peak Pruritus NRS, Skin Pain NRS, PROMIS Sleep Disturbance, CDLQI, and the HADS Anxiety subscale (Table 3). Interestingly, there was a trend towards an inverse correlation between the sensory sensitivity score and lesional signs (EASI score, BSA, p-values < 0.1; Table 3). Low registration scores correlated with Peak Pruritus-NRS, PROMIS Sleep Disturbance, CDLQI, and HADS Anxiety subscale. (Table 3). Sensory seeking did not correlate with any of the parameters listed in Table 3 (data not shown).
Discussion
In this cross-sectional, non-interventional study, adolescents with moderate-to-severe AD (without clinician-diagnosed ADHD) had no reduced ability to sustain attention on an objective, performance-based continuous task, or in their ability to maintain selective attention. Specifically, the Conners, CPT-3 task has high sensitivity to detect problems in inattentiveness, impulsivity, sustained attention, and vigilance [19]. The Stroop Color and Word Test measures selective attention [29], or the ability to attend to a salient stimulus when confronted with an obtrusive irrelevant stimulus. The combined results from these two neuropsychologic assessment tools demonstrate that the subjects in our sample did not differ from what would be expected from age/gender-matched subjects in the general population across any of these dimensions of attention. This suggests that teens with moderate-to-severe AD are capable of high-quality attention to task and do not have underlying attention deficits, at least for fixed periods of time in a controlled setting.
Our inclusion criteria were meant to ensure enrollment of subjects with moderate-to-severe AD uncontrolled by prescription topical medications (i.e., candidates for escalation of care) [31]. The subjects in our sample clearly suffered from high disease burden, with an average BSA of 33.8%. Their itch scores, mood, and anxiety symptoms, and the quality-of-life impact of AD were similar to those of adolescents with moderate-to-severe AD recruited into a recent trial of a systemic biologic (Table S1) [32].
Subjects were also assessed for their sensory responsiveness, using an instrument (the Adult/Adolescent Sensory Profile [AASP]) that quantifies self-reported “thresholds” of sensory stimuli (either sensory sensitivity or its opposite, low registration) and behavioral responses to stimuli (either sensory avoidance or sensory seeking) across multiple sensory modalities. A substantial proportion of subjects in our sample were rated as having atypically high sensory sensitivity (38.6%), low registration (36.4%), and sensory avoidance (50%), but only 2 (less than 5%) rated high for sensory seeking.
Scores for sensory sensitivity, low registration, and sensory avoidance correlated highly with sleep impairment, quality of life impairment, and anxiety. Interestingly, low registration but not sensory sensitivity correlated significantly with degree of itch.
Many items in the sensitivity and avoidance domains emphasize distractibility and subject behaviors meant to cope with such distractibility. As examples, for sensory sensitivity: “I am distracted if there is a lot of noise around,” and “I am bothered by unsteady or fast-moving visual images in movies or TV,” and for sensory avoidance: “I stay away from noisy settings” and “I only eat familiar foods.” It is notable that the AASP surveys distractive stimuli across sensory modalities (not only tactile stimuli), since the predominant symptom of AD is chronic itch.
We did not objectively measure sensory thresholds or distractibility in our sample; it is possible that respondents overstated the degree to which they actually experience sensory integration difficulties, due to subjective reporting bias. If we take their self-reports at face value, we do not know whether sensory sensitivity would have affected their performance on the CPT task, which was done in conditions of minimal ambient distractions. Sensory distractions can affect performance of sustained attention tasks in various psychiatric clinical populations, such as adolescent patients with ADHD [33, 34] and adults with schizophrenia [35]. However, non-clinical samples of adults and adolescents can perform such tasks with little or no decrement in the presence of sensory distractors [34, 35]. Indeed, by adolescence, selective attention abilities appear to be fully formed, and distractor interference effects in such tasks may be related to issues around sensory perception [36, 37].
It would be of interest in future research to determine whether neurotypical adolescents with moderate-to-severe AD, with demonstrably average attention abilities, are nevertheless more susceptible to distraction interference effects in attention tasks than healthy controls, for at least two reasons. First, such distractor interference effects in adolescents have been shown to be a predictor of distractibility in the classroom [36]. But secondly, there is evidence that sensory stimuli (but not a cognitive attention task) can distract from itch, making the role of sensory processing directly relevant to AD patients. For example, Leibovici and others [38] observed that chronic itch patients had reduced scratching and a lower itch rating when exposed to distracting audiovisual stimuli. In contrast, Stumpf and others [39] performed functional magnetic resonance imaging on subjects while they performed a Stroop task; the Stroop task was intended to distract the subjects from a pruritic stimulus. The imaging studies confirmed that the performance of the Stroop test activated brain regions known to be associated with attention control; however, the Stroop test failed to distract the subjects from the itch.
The exclusion of subjects with clinician-diagnosed ADHD is an important feature of this study, given that numerous epidemiologic studies have shown an association between ADHD and AD. [40–45]. This entry criterion was established to plausibly associate any findings of poor attentiveness with AD itself (presumably related to poor sleep or distractibility due to intense itch), rather than comorbid ADHD.
Recent findings utilizing parent/teacher-rated scales suggested the presence of ADHD-like symptoms in pediatric AD patients without ADHD [14, 15]; combined with the epidemiological association of AD with ADHD, it is perhaps surprising that attention abilities appeared unimpaired in our sample. However, in the report by Feng and colleagues [15], it was hyperactivity that was significantly higher in AD than control patients; inattention was not different between AD and controls. This is also supported by the report of Ma and colleagues [46] wherein it was determined that in patients with AD without neurodevelopmental comorbidities such as ADHD, there was no significant impairment in cognitive function. In contrast, Kruse [14] reported that AD patients had significantly higher symptoms of inattention. However, some of these symptoms may also relate conceptually to distractibility rather than inattentiveness; for example, “is easily distracted by noises or other stimuli,” and “is forgetful in daily activities.”
Strengths of this study include its evaluation in the dermatology practice setting using computerized methodology of a poorly studied potential feature of adolescents and children with moderate-to-severe AD. Furthermore, this study supports the usefulness of the Conners CPT-3, administered in an outpatient healthcare setting, in distinguishing ADHD from sensory processing concerns or distraction due to itch. Distinguishing these potential associations with AD could provide support for physicians trying make decisions about referral for ADHD or focus on skin-based interventions and coping strategies for patients who are experiencing distracting itch symptoms in real-world settings.
This study has several notable limitations. We excluded subjects who took systemic antihistamines (including non-sedating antihistamines) within a week prior to the study visit. Given that approximately 60% of adolescents with moderate-to-severe AD have allergic rhinitis, which is commonly treated with antihistamines, many potential candidates for study were ineligible [32]. The enrolled sample was 61% female, and females tend to perform better on Conners, CPT-3. However, all analysis was performed using T-scores, which are both age- and gender-adjusted.
A further limitation of this study is a lack of data concerning whether the subjects had behavioral signs (i.e., argumentativeness, anger outbursts) that might be associated with conditions involving poor attentiveness, such as were utilized in previous studies [14, 15]. However, results from the subject-rated assessment tools in this study suggest a sample of adolescents who had psychological concerns that may have led to altered behavior patterns, and some subjects (38.6%) reported being unusually sensitive to sensory stimuli across sensory modalities and avoided sensory stimuli when possible (50%). The latter finding is consistent with previously published findings in children with AD using the AASP [47].
In conclusion, we found that inattentiveness, as measured by CPT-3 in adolescents with moderate-to-severe AD, occurs at a prevalence that would be expected in a population without AD. Future research could further explore the sensory issues of children and adolescents with AD, comparing them to age-matched patients with other pruritic dermatological conditions.
Electronic supplementary material
Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.
Acknowledgements
The authors would like to thank Zhixiao Wang, PhD, of Regeneron Pharmaceuticals Inc., for their role as the principal investigator for the Skin Pain Numerical Rating Scale protocol that was used in this manuscript. Funding was provided by Sanofi and Regeneron Pharmaceuticals Inc. Medical writing/editorial assistance was provided by Benjamin Crane, PhD, of Excerpta Medica, and was funded by Sanofi Genzyme and Regeneron Pharmaceuticals Inc.
Author contributions
A.K. wrote the manuscript. A.K., A.S.P., and M.E.G. designed the research. A.S.P. and M. E.G. performed the research. S.B., J.J., and L.S. analyzed the data. All authors interpreted the data, provided critical feedback on the manuscript, approved the final manuscript for submission, and are accountable for the accuracy and integrity of the manuscript.
Funding
This research was sponsored by Sanofi and Regeneron Pharmaceuticals Inc. (ClinicalTrials.gov Identifier: NCT05203380). The study sponsors participated in the study design; the collection, analysis, and interpretation of the data; the writing of the report; and the decision to submit the article for publication. Medical writing/editorial assistance was provided by Benjamin Crane, PhD, of Excerpta Medica, funded by Sanofi and Regeneron Pharmaceuticals Inc., according to the Good Publication Practice guidelines.
Data availability
Data of the present study can be requested from the corresponding author.
Declarations
Ethics approval
This study was conducted in accordance with consensus ethical principles derived from international guidelines, including the Declaration of Helsinki, and consistent with the International Conference on Harmonisation Good Clinical Practice guideline and applicable regulatory requirements. Study registered in Protocol Registration and Results System (ClinicalTrials.gov) registration ID: NCT05203380.
Consent to participate
Written informed consent to participate in the trial and publication was obtained from all subjects’ legal guardians. Patients must also provide separate informed assent to enroll in the study, and sign and date either a separate informed assent form or the informed consent form signed by the parent/legal guardian.
Competing interests
Amy S. Paller is an investigator for AbbVie, Dermavant, Eli Lilly, Incyte, Janssen, Krystal Biotech, LEO Pharma, and UCB; a consultant for Amryt Pharma, Azitra, BioCryst, BMS, Boehringer Ingelheim, Castle Creek Biosciences, Eli Lilly, Janssen, Krystal Biotech, LEO Pharma, Novartis, Regeneron Pharmaceuticals Inc., Sanofi, Seanergy, TWi Biotechnology, and UCB; and part of the data and safety monitoring boards for AbbVie, Abeona Therapeutics, Catawba Research, Galderma, and InMed Pharmaceuticals. Mercedes E. Gonzalez is an investigator for AbbVie, Amgen, Anterogen, Arcutis Biotherapeutics, Dermira, Dermavant, Eli Lilly, Incyte, Krystal Biotech, Regeneron Pharmaceuticals Inc., Sun Pharma, Verrica Pharmaceuticals; a speaker for Cerave, Dermavant, Galderma, Pfizer, Primus Pharmaceuticals, Regeneron Pharmaceuticals Inc., Sanofi, and Verrica Pharmaceuticals; and a consultant for Verrica Pharmaceuticals and Unilever. Sarah Barnum is an employee of CognitionMetrics, LLC, which has provided compensated consultative services to Ovid Therapeutics. Judith Jaeger is the President and owner of CognitionMetrics, LLC. CognitionMetrics, LLC has provided compensated consultative services to Ovid Therapeutics. Liyang Shao and Andrew Korotzer are employees and shareholders of Regeneron Pharmaceuticals Inc. Zafer E. Ozturk is an employee of and may hold stock and/or stock options in Sanofi.
Footnotes
Publisher’s Note
Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.
References
- 1.Weidinger S, Novak N (2016) Atopic dermatitis. Lancet 387:1109–1122. 10.1016/S0140-6736(15)00149-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Lee DG, Gui XY, Mukovozov I, Fleming P, Lynde C (2023) Sleep disturbances in children with atopic dermatitis: a scoping review. J Cutan Med Surg 27:157–164. 10.1177/12034754231159337 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Ramirez FD, Chen S, Langan SM, Prather AA, McCulloch CE, Kidd SA, Cabana MD, Chren MM, Abuabara K (2019) Association of atopic dermatitis with sleep quality in children. JAMA Pediatr 173:e190025. 10.1001/jamapediatrics.2019.0025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Bawany F, Northcott CA, Beck LA, Pigeon WR (2021) Sleep disturbances and atopic dermatitis: relationships, methods for assessment, and therapies. J Allergy Clin Immunol Pract 9:1488–1500. 10.1016/j.jaip.2020.12.007 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Chang YS, Chiang BL (2018) Sleep disorders and atopic dermatitis: a 2-way street? J Allergy Clin Immunol 142:1033–1040. 10.1016/j.jaci.2018.08.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Cheng BT, Silverberg JI (2019) Depression and psychological distress in US adults with atopic dermatitis. Ann Allergy Asthma Immunol 123:179–185. 10.1016/j.anai.2019.06.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Silverberg JI, Gelfand JM, Margolis DJ, Boguniewicz M, Fonacier L, Grayson MH, Ong PY, Chiesa Fuxench ZC, Simpson EL (2019) Symptoms and diagnosis of anxiety and depression in atopic dermatitis in U.S. adults. Br J Dermatol 181:554–565. 10.1111/bjd.17683 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Thyssen JP, Hamann CR, Linneberg A, Dantoft TM, Skov L, Gislason GH, Wu JJ, Egeberg A (2018) Atopic dermatitis is associated with anxiety, depression, and suicidal ideation, but not with psychiatric hospitalization or suicide. Allergy 73:214–220. 10.1111/all.13231 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Daud LR, Garralda ME, David TJ (1993) Psychosocial adjustment in preschool children with atopic eczema. Arch Dis Child 69:670–676. 10.1136/adc.69.6.670 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Zuberbier T, Orlow SJ, Paller AS, Taïeb A, Allen R, Hernanz-Hermosa JM, Ocampo-Candiani J, Cox M, Langeraar J, Simon JC (2006) Patient perspectives on the management of atopic dermatitis. J Allergy Clin Immunol 118:226–232. 10.1016/j.jaci.2006.02.031 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Paller A, Jaworski JC, Simpson EL, Boguniewicz M, Russell JJ, Block JK, Tofte S, Dunn JD, Feldman SR, Clark AR, Schwartz G, Eichenfield LF (2018) Major comorbidities of atopic dermatitis: beyond allergic disorders. Am J Clin Dermatol 19:821–838. 10.1007/s40257-018-0383-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Evers AWM, Peerdeman KJ, van Laarhoven AIM (2019) What is new in the psychology of chronic itch? Exp Dermatol 28:1442–1447. 10.1111/exd.13992 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Gruber R, Wiebe S, Montecalvo L, Brunetti B, Amsel R, Carrier J (2011) Impact of sleep restriction on neurobehavioral functioning of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. Sleep 34:315–323. 10.1093/sleep/34.3.315 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Kruse L, Cices A, Fishbein AB, Paller AS (2019) Neurocognitive function in moderate-severe pediatric atopic dermatitis: a case-control study. Pediatr Dermatol 36:110–114. 10.1111/pde.13710 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Feng LJ, Chen AW, Luo XY, Wang H (2020) Increased attention deficit/hyperactivity and oppositional defiance symptoms of 6–12 years old Chinese children with atopic dermatitis. Med (Baltim) 99:e20801. 10.1097/MD.0000000000020801 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Zhou NY, Nili A, Blackwell CK, Ogbuefi N, Cummings P, Lai JS, Griffith JW, Paller AS, Wakschlag LS, Fishbein AB (2022) Parent report of sleep health and attention regulation in a cross-sectional study of infants and preschool-aged children with atopic dermatitis. Pediatr Dermatol 39:61–68. 10.1111/pde.14889 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Lipsker CW, Bölte S, Hirvikoski T, Lekander M, Holmström L, Wicksell RK (2018) Prevalence of autism traits and attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms in a clinical sample of children and adolescents with chronic pain. J Pain Res 11:2827–2836. 10.2147/JPR.S177534 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Epstein JN, Erkanli A, Conners CK, Klaric J, Costello JE, Angold A (2003) Relations between continuous performance test performance measures and ADHD behaviors. J Abnorm Child Psychol 31:543–554. 10.1023/a:1025405216339 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Conners KC, Sitarenios G, Ayearst LE (2018) Conners’ continuous performance test third edition. In: Kreutzer JS, DeLuca J, Caplan B (ed) Encyclopedia of clinical neuropsychology. Springer, Cham. 10.1007/978-3-319-57111-9_1535 [Google Scholar]
- 20.Simpson E, Bissonnette R, Eichenfield LF, Guttman-Yassky E, King B, Silverberg JI, Beck LA, Bieber T, Reich K, Kabashima K, Seyger M, Siegfried E, Stingl G, Feldman SR, Menter A, van de Kerkhof P, Yosipovitch G, Paul C, Martel P, Dubost-Brama A, Armstrong J, Chavda R, Frey S, Joubert Y, Milutinovic M, Parneix A, Teixeira HD, Lin CY, Sun L, Klekotka P, Nickoloff B, Dutronc Y, Mallbris L, Janes JM, DeLozier AM, Nunes FP, Paller AS (2020) The validated investigator global assessment for atopic dermatitis (vIGA-AD): the development and reliability testing of a novel clinical outcome measurement instrument for the severity of atopic dermatitis. J Am Acad Dermatol 83:839–846. 10.1016/j.jaad.2020.04.104 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Hanifin JM, Baghoomian W, Grinich E, Leshem YA, Jacobson M, Simpson EL (2022) The Eczema Area and Severity Index-A practical guide. Dermatitis 33:187–192. 10.1097/DER.0000000000000895 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Yosipovitch G, Reaney M, Mastey V, Eckert L, Abbé A, Nelson L, Clark M, Williams N, Chen Z, Ardeleanu M, Akinlade B, Graham NMH, Pirozzi G, Staudinger H, Plaum S, Radin A, Gadkari A (2019) Peak pruritus numerical rating scale: psychometric validation and responder definition for assessing itch in moderate-to-severe atopic dermatitis. Br J Dermatol 181:761–769. 10.1111/bjd.17744 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Thyssen JP, de Bruin-Weller M, Costanzo A, Grond S, Schuster C, Liu C, Rueda MJ, Chen YF, Pinter A, Bieber T (2023) Baseline body surface area and itch severity define response to baricitinib in patients with moderate-to-severe atopic dermatitis at Week 16. Adv Ther 40:3574–3587. 10.1007/s12325-023-02528-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Paller AS, Siegfried EC, Marron SE, Clark M, Harris N, Kosa K, Qin S, Whalley D, Chao J, Bansal A, Chuang CC, Wang Z (2024) Skin pain and sleep quality numeric rating scales for children aged 6 months to 5 years with atopic dermatitis. J Eur Acad Dermatol Venereol. 10.1111/jdv.20199 [DOI] [PubMed]
- 25.Lawson V, Lewis-Jones MS, Reid P (1995) Summaries of papers. Br J Dermatol 133(suppl 45):19 [Google Scholar]
- 26.Hays RD, Bjorner JB, Revicki DA, Spritzer KL, Cella D (2009) Development of physical and mental health summary scores from the patient-reported outcomes measurement information system (PROMIS) global items. Qual Life Res 18:873–880. 10.1007/s11136-009-9496-9 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Michopoulos I, Douzenis A, Kalkavoura C, Christodoulou C, Michalopoulou P, Kalemi G, Fineti K, Patapis P, Protopapas K, Lykouras L (2008) Hospital anxiety and depression scale (HADS): validation in a greek general hospital sample. Ann Gen Psychiatry 7:4. 10.1186/1744-859X-7-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Brown C, Dunn W (2002) Adolescent/adult sensory profile. Psychological Corporation, San Antonio, TX [Google Scholar]
- 29.Stroop JR (1935) Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. J Exp Psychol 18:643–662. 10.1037/h0054651 [Google Scholar]
- 30.Scarpina F, Tagini S (2017) The Stroop Color and Word Test. Front Psychol 8:557. 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00557 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.American Academy of Dermatology Association (2024) Atopic dermatitis clinical guideline. https://www.aad.org/member/clinical-quality/guidelines/atopic-dermatitis. Accessed 16 May 2024
- 32.Simpson EL, Paller AS, Siegfried EC, Boguniewicz M, Sher L, Gooderham MJ, Beck LA, Guttman-Yassky E, Pariser D, Blauvelt A, Weisman J, Lockshin B, Hultsch T, Zhang Q, Kamal MA, Davis JD, Akinlade B, Staudinger H, Hamilton JD, Graham NMH, Pirozzi G, Gadkari A, Eckert L, Stahl N, Yancopoulos GD, Ruddy M, Bansal A (2020) Efficacy and safety of dupilumab in adolescents with uncontrolled moderate to severe atopic dermatitis: a phase 3 randomized clinical trial. JAMA Dermatol 156:44–56. 10.1001/jamadermatol.2019.3336 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Huang-Pollock CL, Karalunas SL, Tam H, Moore AN (2012) Evaluating vigilance deficits in ADHD: a meta-analysis of CPT performance. J Abnorm Psychol 121:360–371. doi: 10.1037/a0027205 Erratum in: (2012) J Abnorm Psychol 121:423 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
- 34.Berger I, Cassuto H (2014) The effect of environmental distractors incorporation into a CPT on sustained attention and ADHD diagnosis among adolescents. J Neurosci Methods 222:62–68. 10.1016/j.jneumeth.2013.10.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Demeter E, Guthrie SK, Taylor SF, Sarter M, Lustig C (2013) Increased distractor vulnerability but preserved vigilance in patients with schizophrenia: evidence from a translational sustained attention Task. Schizophr Res 144:136–141. 10.1016/j.schres.2013.01.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Hobbiss MH, Lavie N (2024) Sustained selective attention in adolescence: cognitive development and predictors of distractibility at school. J Exp Child Psychol 238:105784. 10.1016/j.jecp.2023.105784 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Lavie N, Hirst A, de Fockert JW, Viding E (2004) Load theory of selective attention and cognitive control. J Exp Psychol Gen 133:339–354. 10.1037/0096-3445.133.3.339 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Leibovici V, Magora F, Cohen S, Ingber A (2009) Effects of virtual reality immersion and audiovisual distraction techniques for patients with pruritus. Pain Res Manag 14:283–286. 10.1155/2009/178751 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Stumpf A, Pfleiderer B, Schneider G, Heuft G, Schmelz M, Phan NQ, Ständer S, Burgmer M (2017) Distraction from itch shows brainstem activation without reduction in experimental itch sensation. Acta Derm Venereol 97:1074–1080. 10.2340/00015555-2732 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Chen MH, Su TP, Chen YS, Hsu JW, Huang KL, Chang WH, Chen TJ, Pan TL, Bai YM (2014) Is atopy in early childhood a risk factor for ADHD and ASD? A longitudinal study. J Psychosom Res 77:316–321. 10.1016/j.jpsychores.2014.06.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Genuneit J, Braig S, Brandt S, Wabitsch M, Florath I, Brenner H, Rothenbacher D (2014) Infant atopic eczema and subsequent attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder–a prospective birth cohort study. Pediatr Allergy Immunol 25:51–56. 10.1111/pai.12152 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Lin YT, Chen YC, Gau SS, Yeh TH, Fan HY, Hwang YY, Lee YL (2016) Associations between allergic diseases and attention deficit hyperactivity/oppositional defiant disorders in children. Pediatr Res 80:480–485. 10.1038/pr.2016.111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Riis JL, Vestergaard C, Deleuran MS, Olsen M (2016) Childhood atopic dermatitis and risk of attention deficit/hyperactivity disorder: a cohort study. J Allergy Clin Immunol 138:608–610. 10.1016/j.jaci.2016.01.027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Schmitt J, Romanos M, Schmitt NM, Meurer M, Kirch W (2009) Atopic eczema and attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in a population-based sample of children and adolescents. JAMA 301:724–726. 10.1001/jama.2009.136 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Strom MA, Fishbein AB, Paller AS, Silverberg JI (2016) Association between atopic dermatitis and attention deficit hyperactivity disorder in U.S. children and adults. Br J Dermatol 175:920–929. 10.1111/bjd.14697 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Ma EZ, Chang HR, Radtke S, Wan J (2024) Symptoms of cognitive impairment among children with atopic dermatitis. JAMA Dermatol 160:447–452. 10.1001/jamadermatol.2024.0015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Shani-Adir A, Rozenman D, Kessel A, Engel-Yeger B (2009) The relationship between sensory hypersensitivity and sleep quality of children with atopic dermatitis. Pediatr Dermatol 26:143–149. 10.1111/j.1525-1470.2009.00904.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Data of the present study can be requested from the corresponding author.