Skip to main content
eLife logoLink to eLife
. 2024 Aug 12;12:RP88733. doi: 10.7554/eLife.88733

Periaqueductal gray activates antipredatory neural responses in the amygdala of foraging rats

Eun Joo Kim 1, Mi-Seon Kong 2, Sanggeon Park 3,4, Jeiwon Cho 3,4,, Jeansok John Kim 1,5,
Editors: Kate M Wassum6, Kate M Wassum7
PMCID: PMC11318971  PMID: 39133827

Abstract

Pavlovian fear conditioning research suggests that the interaction between the dorsal periaqueductal gray (dPAG) and basolateral amygdala (BLA) acts as a prediction error mechanism in the formation of associative fear memories. However, their roles in responding to naturalistic predatory threats, characterized by less explicit cues and the absence of reiterative trial-and-error learning events, remain unexplored. In this study, we conducted single-unit recordings in rats during an ‘approach food-avoid predator’ task, focusing on the responsiveness of dPAG and BLA neurons to a rapidly approaching robot predator. Optogenetic stimulation of the dPAG triggered fleeing behaviors and increased BLA activity in naive rats. Notably, BLA neurons activated by dPAG stimulation displayed immediate responses to the robot, demonstrating heightened synchronous activity compared to BLA neurons that did not respond to dPAG stimulation. Additionally, the use of anterograde and retrograde tracer injections into the dPAG and BLA, respectively, coupled with c-Fos activation in response to predatory threats, indicates that the midline thalamus may play an intermediary role in innate antipredatory-defensive functioning.

Research organism: Rat

Introduction

The interaction between the dorsal periaqueductal gray (dPAG) and the basolateral amygdala (BLA) plays a pivotal role in defensive mechanisms across both animals and humans. Research on Pavlovian fear conditioning (FC), which involves pairing a conditional stimulus (CS; e.g., tones, lights, contexts) with an unconditional stimulus (US; e.g., electric shock) to produce conditional fear responses (CRs), suggests that the dPAG is part of the ascending US pain transmission pathway to the BLA, the presumed site for CS-US association formation (De Oca et al., 1998; Gross and Canteras, 2012; Herry and Johansen, 2014; Kim et al., 1993; Ressler and Maren, 2019; Walker and Davis, 1997). This pathway is thought to be mediated through the lateral and dorsal-midline thalamus regions, including the posterior intralaminar nucleus and paraventricular nucleus of the thalamus (PVT) (Krout and Loewy, 2000; McNally et al., 2011; Yeh et al., 2021; but see Brunzell and Kim, 2001). Supporting this model, rodent studies have shown that electrical stimulation of the dPAG, which triggers robust activity bursts (jumping, running, escaping) similar to footshock-induced unconditional responses (URs) (Jenck et al., 1995; Olds and Olds, 1963), can effectively act as a surrogate US for both auditory and contextual FC in rats (Di Scala et al., 1987; Kim et al., 2013). In parallel, Johansen et al., 2010 found that pharmacological inhibition of the PAG, encompassing both dorsal (dPAG) and ventral (vPAG) regions, diminishes the behavioral and neural responses in the amygdala elicited by periorbital shock US, thereby impairing the acquisition of auditory FC. Furthermore, while dPAG neurons initially respond to the shock US, the development of fear CRs results in a reduction of US-evoked neural responses in the PAG due to increased amygdala-PAG pathway-mediated analgesia that dampens footshock nociception (Johansen et al., 2010). Fundamentally, the negative feedback circuit between the amygdala and the dPAG serves as a biological implementation of the Rescorla–Wagner model (Rescorla and Wagner, 1972), a foundational theory of associative learning that emphasizes the importance of prediction errors in reinforcement (i.e., US), as applied to FC (Fanselow, 1998). However, the relevance of PAG–amygdala interactions observed in controlled conditioning chambers remains to be assessed in more realistic threat scenarios.

In human cases of intractable pain surgery, stimulation of the dPAG has been reported to induce intense fear and panic sensations (Carrive and Morgan, 2012; Magierek et al., 2003). For instance, one patient described the sensation of PAG stimulation as “Something horrible is coming, somebody is now chasing me, I am trying to escape from him” (Amano et al., 1982). Similarly, our previous work demonstrated that electrical stimulation of either the dPAG or BLA prompted naïve rats foraging for food in a naturalistic environment to flee to a safe nest in the absence of external threats (Kim et al., 2013). Notably, the fleeing behavior induced by dPAG stimulation was contingent on an intact amygdala, whereas BLA stimulation-induced fleeing did not require an intact PAG. This pattern suggests that the fear/panic sensations reported in humans may originate from an innate fear information flow from the dPAG to the BLA.

To further explore this, we employed single-unit recordings and optogenetics in an ‘approach food-avoid predator’ paradigm (Choi and Kim, 2010; Wang et al., 2015), focusing on the responsiveness of dPAG and BLA neurons to an approaching robotic predator. Our findings reveal that optogenetic activation of the dPAG not only precipitated escape behaviors but also significantly increased BLA neuron activity. BLA neurons activated through dPAG optogenetic stimulation exhibited immediate and heightened synchronous activity in response to the predator threat, unlike BLA neurons unresponsive to dPAG stimulation. Considering the lack of direct monosynaptic projections between dPAG and BLA neurons (Vianna and Brandão, 2003; McNally et al., 2011; Cameron et al., 1995), we utilized anterograde and retrograde tracers in the dPAG and BLA, respectively. This was complemented by c-Fos expression analysis following exposure to predatory threats. Our anatomical findings suggest that the PVT may be part of a network that conveys predatory threat information from the dPAG to the BLA.

Results

dPAG neurons respond to extrinsic predatory threats during a risky foraging task

Five male Long–Evans rats, maintained at 85% of their normal weight through food restriction, were implanted with microdrive arrays positioned above the dPAG. The dPAG includes dorsomedial (dmPAG), dorsolateral (dlPAG), and lateral (lPAG) regions, as designated by extensive track-tracing, neurochemical, and immunohistochemical studies (e.g., Bandler et al., 1991; Bandler and Keay, 1996; Carrive, 1993). During baseline sessions, rats were required to procure a food pellet in an open arena (202 cm length × 58 cm width × 6 cm height; Figure 1A) and return to their nest to consume it. The electrodes were gradually lowered (<120 µm/day) into the dPAG during these sessions (Figure 1B). Once well-isolated neural activities were detected, rats underwent predator testing with successive pre-robot, robot, and post-robot sessions (5–15 pellet attempts/session). During the robot session, the animals’ outbound foraging time (latency to reach the pellet or the predator-triggering zone) increased, while the pellet success rate decreased (Figure 1C and D). We collected a total of 94 dPAG units (Figure 1—figure supplement 1A and B) during predator testing, with 23.4% (n = 22) showing increased firing rates (z > 3) specifically to the approaching robot with short-latency responses (<500 ms) during the robot session, but not during the pre- and post-robot sessions (robot cells; Figure 1E and F, Figure 1—figure supplement 1C and D). We focused our analysis on excited robot cells, excluding other types of units that showed food-specific and mixed robot encounter + pellet procurement responses (pellet and BOTH cells; Figure 1F, Figure 1—figure supplement 1A and B), to investigate predator-related dPAG activity. One cell that exhibited decreased firing rates (z < –3) specifically to the robot was also excluded (Figure 1—figure supplement 1E). The majority of the robot cells (90.9%) did not show significant correlations between movement speed and neuronal firing rate (Figure 1—figure supplement 1F). These results suggest that dPAG neurons participate in detecting an advancing robot and eliciting antipredatory-defensive behaviors, such as fleeing to a safe nest.

Figure 1. Dorsal periaqueductal gray (dPAG) single-unit recordings during risky foraging.

(A) Rats underwent pre-robot, robot, and post-robot sessions, successfully securing pellets in pre- and post-robot trials, and failing during robot trials due to robot interference. (B) Tetrode implantation in dPAG with a photomicrograph of the tip (arrowhead). (C) Outbound foraging time increased significantly in the robot session (Χ2 = 64.00, p<0.0001, Friedman test; ps<0.05 for all comparisons, Dunn’s test, n = 42 recording days from 5 rats). ***p<0.001 compared to pre-robot and post-robot sessions. #p<0.05 compared to the pre-robot session. (D) The pellet success rate significantly decreased during robot session (Χ2 = 84.00, p<0.0001, Friedman test; ps<0.0001 for all comparisons, Dunn’s test, n = 42 recording days from 5 rats). ***p<0.001 compared to pre-robot and post-robot sessions. (E) Cell-type proportions revealed that 23.4% cells responded to robot activation (robot cells). (F) Representative dPAG robot cell raster/event histograms aligned with robot activations. (G) Population activity of robot cells around the time of robot activation (t = 0) with 0.1 s and 1 s bins. Firing rates of the robot cells were higher during robot session (0–3 s blocks; Friedman test, all Χ2s > 6.952, all ps<0.05; Dunn’s test, all ps<0.05, n = 22 units). Shaded areas indicate SEM. **p<0.01 compared to pre-robot session. #, ##, and ### denote p<0.05, p<0.01, and p<0.001, respectively, compared to post-robot session. Panel (E) created with BioRender.com, and published using a CC BY-NC-ND license with permission.

© 2024, BioRender Inc

Figure 1E was created using BioRender, and is published under a CC BY-NC-ND 4.0. Further reproductions must adhere to the terms of this license.

Figure 1.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1. Dorsal periaqueductal gray (dPAG) unit cell types.

Figure 1—figure supplement 1.

(A) Representative multiple single units recorded in the dPAG. (B) A subset (35.1%) of dPAG neurons showed increased firing rates in response to the robot, food pellet, or both, with 66.7% responding exclusively to the robot. Units were categorized based on z-score responses: ‘robot cells’ for z > 3 during robot phase and z < 3 during pre-robot phase; ‘pellet cells’ for z > 3 during pre-robot phase and z < 3 during robot phase; and ‘BOTH cells’ for z > 3 in both phases. Raster plots display activity of robot, pellet, and BOTH cells across pre-robot, robot, and post-robot sessions. (C, D) Average (C) and maximum (D) firing rates of dPAG robot cells during pre-robot, robot, and post-robot sessions (average firing rate: Χ2 = 7.636, p<0.05, Friedman test, p<0.05, Pre-robot vs. Robot, Dunn’s test, n = 22 units; maximum firing rate: Χ2 = 12.29, p<0.01, Friedman test; p<0.05, Pre-robot vs. Robot, Dunn’s test, n = 22 units). (E) One neuron showed decreased firing rates (z –3) in response to the robot during the robot phase. (F) Top row: mean firing rate (± SEM; shaded areas) of robot cells and movement speed (± SEM; shaded areas) of animals across sessions (n = 22 units). Bottom row: correlation coefficients between firing rate and movement speed for the robot cell group during each session. * and ** denote p<0.05 and p<0.01, respectively.

dPAG neurons can intrinsically evoke fear in the absence of external threats

We next investigated if optogenetic activation of dPAG neurons could elicit antipredatory behaviors without an external predator. To do so, we first unilaterally injected rats with ChR2 and implanted an optrode into their dPAG (Figure 2A, Figure 2—figure supplement 1A). In two anesthetized rats, 20 Hz blue light stimulations (473 nm, 10 ms pulse width, 2 s duration) elicited excited responses in 48% of the recorded units (12 out of 25 cells; Figure 2B, Figure 2—figure supplement 1B), while only one unit showed inhibited responses to the stimulation (Figure 2—figure supplement 1C), confirming the effectiveness of dPAG opto-stimulation. A separate group of rats with unilateral ChR2 (n = 9) or EYFP (n = 4) injections and optic fiber implantation (Figure 2C) underwent 4 days of baseline training followed by testing sessions. During testing, rats were allowed to procure a pellet without light stimulation (Off) and with light stimulation (On) (Figure 2D). The stimulation parameters employed were identical to those used in the anesthetized rats. During light-on trials (Figure 2E), the ChR2-expressing animals were not able to procure the pellet and consistently fled into the nest, whether the pellet was placed at a 76.2 cm long distance from the nest (OnL) or at a 25.4 cm short distance from the nest (OnS) (Figure 2F and G). However, EYFP control rats did not show any defensive behaviors. Latency to procure pellets increased as a function of the stimulation intensity, frequency, and duration (Figure 2H–J). These results demonstrate that optical stimulation of CaMKII-expressing dPAG neurons effectively caused naïve rats approaching a food pellet to flee to the nest without an external threat.

Figure 2. Dorsal periaqueductal gray (dPAG) optical stimulation evokes fear.

(A) Virus injection, optrode implantation in dPAG, and light stimulation during single-unit recordings in anesthetized rats. (B) Raster plots and peri-event time histograms for 20 Hz light stimulations (10 ms width, left; 2 s duration, center). 48% of 25 units had increased firing during 2 s light stimulation (right). (C) Virus injection, expression, and optic fiber placement in dPAG. (D) Stimulation testing: baseline trials at 75 cm distance (long) without light; stimulation trials with 2 s light as the rat approached (~25 cm) long pellet; light applied as the rat approached 25 cm (short) pellet if long pellet unsuccessful. (E) Representative trajectories for EYFP- and ChR2-expressing rats during stimulation testing. (F) Rat behaviors during light stimulation trials. (G) ChR2 (n = 9) rats showed increased latency to procure pellet upon opto-stimulation (OnL) compared to EYFP (n = 4) rats (OffL, Z = 1.013, p=0.311; OnL, U = 0.0, p<0.001; Mann–Whitney U test). (H–J) ChR2 group (n = 9) exhibited increased latency to procure pellet compared to EYFP group (n = 4) based on stimulation intensity (H; Us <for all intensities < 3.5, ps for all intensities <0.025; Mann–Whitney U test), frequency (I; Us for 10 Hz and 20 Hz <2.5, ps for 10 and 20 Hz <0.014; Mann–Whitney U test), and duration (J; Us for all durations <4.5, ps for all durations <0.032; Mann–Whitney U test). *, **, and *** denote p<0.05, p<0.01, and p<0.001, respectively. Panel D created with BioRender.com, and published using a CC BY-NC-ND license with permission.

© 2024, BioRender Inc

Figure 2D was created using BioRender, and is published under a CC BY-NC-ND 4.0. Further reproductions must adhere to the terms of this license.

Figure 2.

Figure 2—figure supplement 1. Optrode recording in the dorsal periaqueductal gray (dPAG).

Figure 2—figure supplement 1.

(A) Virus injection and optrode implantation targeted at the dPAG (left). Photomicrographs display virus expression, optic fiber/electrode tips, and a representative dPAG neuron (right). Light stimulation was applied during single-unit recordings in anesthetized rats. (B) Response latencies of stimulation-responsive dPAG cells (n = 12 cells). (C) A single dPAG cell exhibited inhibited responses to optical stimulation of the dPAG.

BLA neurons that respond intrinsically to dPAG optical stimulation also respond extrinsically to a robot predator

Our previous data indicated that PAG may transmit innate fear signals to the amygdala (Kim et al., 2013). To investigate amygdala neuron responses to dPAG activity changes, we recorded BLA neurons while optically stimulating the dPAG in foraging rats using the same parameters employed in the anesthetized rats during optrode recording (473 nm, 20 Hz, 10 ms pulse width, 2 s duration). Six rats were injected with ChR2 and implanted with an optic fiber in the dPAG and tetrode arrays in the BLA (Figure 3A). After the electrodes were lowered to the target structures between the baseline sessions, animals underwent testing comprising pre-stim, stim, and post-stim sessions (Figure 3B). The dPAG stimulation increased outbound foraging time (Figure 3C) and decreased success rate (Figure 3D), mimicking predator-induced cautious behavior (Figure 1C and D). While rats were escaping the foraging area without the pellet (stim session), we collected data from 320 BLA neurons. Subsets of neurons in the BLA (n = 34) exhibited firing increases in response to dPAG stimulation (Figure 3E, Figure 3—figure supplement 1A–C).

Figure 3. Dorsal periaqueductal gray (dPAG) optical stimulation and amygdala recordings.

(A) Virus injection in dPAG and tetrode array implantation targeting basolateral amygdala (BLA). Light stimulation during single-unit recordings in freely moving rats. (B) Stimulation testing sessions: in pre- and post-stim trials, rats freely procured pellets; in stim trials, optical stimulation prevented procurement of pellets. (C, D) During dPAG stimulation, animals showed increased outbound foraging time (C; Χ2 = 117.8, p<0.0001, Friedman test; ps<0.0001, Dunn’s test, n = 78 recording days from 7 rats) and decreased success rate (D; Χ2 = 154.0, p<0.0001, Friedman test; ps<0.0001, Dunn’s test, n = 78 recording days from 7 rats). ****p<0.0001 compared to pre-robot and post-robot sessions. ####p<0.0001 compared pre-robot session. (E) Subset of BLA units (10.0%) responsive to optical stimulation (Stim cells; left), and a representative (center) and all stimulation-responsive (Stim cells; right; n = 320 cells) raster plots with peri-event time histograms (PETHs). (F) Subset of animals (n = 3 rats) underwent additional robot trials following the post-stim session. (G) Increased outbound foraging time during robot session compared to post-stimulation session (t(15) = 6.655, p<0.0001; paired t-test; 16 recording days from 3 rats). ****p<0.0001. (H) Twenty-two BLA units were dPAG stimulation-responsive. (I) Representative raster plots of dPAG stimulation-responsive and -nonresponsive units. (J) Proportions of robot vs. non-robot cells differed between stimulation-responsive and -nonresponsive units (Χ2 = 11.134, p<0.001; chi-square test). ***p<0.001. (K) PETHs of stim (n = 23 cells) and non-stim cells (n = 62 cells) during stimulation and robot sessions. (L) Relationship between maximal firing rates during first 500 ms subsequent to robot activation and maximal firing rates during first 500 ms after stimulation onset (r(85) = 0.405, p<0.001; Pearson correlation). (M) Population cross-correlograms (CCs) with significant synchrony during robot session (n = 26 CCs) were higher than other sessions. Dotted vertical lines indicate 0–100 ms window for testing significance. Gray, blue, and dark yellow ***p<0.001 compared to pre-stimulation, stimulation, and post-stimulation sessions, respectively. (N) Among synchronized BLA cell pairs, those including dPAG stimulation-responsive cell(s) (stim pairs; 61.5%; n = 16 pairss) showed increased correlated firings (area under the curve [AUC] during 0–100 ms window) during the robot session compared to other sessions. In contrast, synchronized BLA cell pairs that consisted of stimulation nonresponsive cells only (n = 10 pairs) showed no AUC differences across sessions. Gray *, blue **, and dark yellow **p<0.05 compared to pre-stimulation, p<0.01 compared to stimulation, and p<0.001 compared to post-stimulation sessions, respectively. (O) Comparing CCs during testing windows (0–50 ms and 50–100 ms) between stim (n = 16) and non-stim pairs (n = 10), stim pairs exhibited higher correlated firing than non-stim pairs during the 0–50 ms block (t(21.99) = 2.342, p=0.0286; t-test), while displaying decreased correlated firings in the second block (50–100 ms; U = 42, p=0.045; Mann–Whitney U test). *p<0.05 compared to the non-stim pairs. Panels B and F created with BioRender.com, and published using a CC BY-NC-ND license with permission.

© 2024, BioRender Inc

Figure 3B was created using BioRender, and is published under a CC BY-NC-ND 4.0. Further reproductions must adhere to the terms of this license.

© 2024, BioRender Inc

Figure 3F was created using BioRender, and is published under a CC BY-NC-ND 4.0. Further reproductions must adhere to the terms of this license.

Figure 3.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1. Characteristics of basolateral amygdala (BLA) cells.

Figure 3—figure supplement 1.

(A) Representative waveforms of BLA cells. (B) A representative raster plot and peri-event time histograms for 20 Hz light stimulations (10 ms pulse width, 2 s duration). (C) Response latencies of BLA cells to dorsal periaqueductal gray (dPAG) stimulation. (D) Out of 85 units from the BLA, 25.9% immediately responded to the robot. (E) A pre-event time histogram aligning BLA activity (n = 23) with pellet procurement (pre-stim and post-stim sessions), dPAG stimulation (stim session), or robot activation (robot session). (F) Firing rates of robot cells and movement speeds during pellet procurement (post-robot session) and robot activation (robot sessions). (G) All but one BLA cell showed no significant correlation between firing rate and movement speed. (H) Representative raster plots and peri-event time histograms for a dPAG stimulation-responsive BLA cell reacting to the robot predator (top; also see Figure 3I, left column) and a dPAG stimulation-nonresponsive BLA cell (bottom; also see Figure 3I, right column). (I, J) dPAG stimulation-responsive units (n = 23) showed higher levels of robot-evoked firings than stimulation-nonresponsive units (n = 62) during both short (I; 0–0.5 s post-robot activation; U = 339.0, p=0.0001; Mann–Whitney U test) and extended (J; 0–5 s post-robot activation; U = 384.5, p=0.0009; Mann–Whitney U test) periods. *** denotes p<0.001 compared to the non-stim pairs.
Figure 3—figure supplement 2. Spike synchrony in dorsal periaqueductal gray (dPAG)-stimulated basolateral amygdala (BLA) neurons under predatory threats.

Figure 3—figure supplement 2.

(A) Of 185 cross-correlograms (CCs) from simultaneously recorded BLA neuron pairs, 14.1% exhibited significant peaks (z > 3) during robot sessions. (B, C) Mean CC area under the curve (AUC) (B) and CC peak values (C) increased during robot sessions compared to other sessions (Zs > 29.36, p<0.0001, Friedman test; ps<0.01 for pre-stimulation vs. robot, stimulation vs. robot, and post-stimulation vs. robot sessions comparisons, Dunn’s test; n = 26 pairs). **, ***, and **** denote p<0.01, p<0.001, and p<0.0001 compared to robot session, respectively. (D) Among BLA cell pairs with significant synchrony during robot sessions, 61.5% included dPAG-stimulated BLA cells (stim pairs) and 38.5% did not (non-stim pairs). (E) Stim pairs (top, a representative pair) showed enhanced spike synchrony during robot sessions, with higher mean CC AUC (bottom left) and peak (bottom right) values compared to other sessions (Zs > 17.42, p<0.001, Friedman test; ps<0.037 for pre-stimulation vs. robot, stimulation vs. robot, and post-stimulation vs. robot sessions comparisons, Dunn’s test; n = 16 pairs). *, **, and *** denote p<0.05, p<0.01, and p<0.001 compared to the robot session, respectively. (F) Non-stim pairs (top, a representative pair) also displayed significant synchrony during robot sessions, with no difference in CC AUC (bottom left) between robot and pellet-only sessions, but higher peak area during robot vs. stimulation session (Z = 13.84, p<0.01, Friedman test; p=0.0017 for the stimulation vs. robot sessions comparison, Dunn’s test; n = 10 pairs). CC peak (bottom right) during robot session was higher than during stimulation and post-stimulation sessions (Z = 15.92, p<0.01, Friedman test; ps<0.0335 for stimulation vs. robot and post-stimulation vs. robot sessions comparisons, Dunn’s test), but not the pre-stimulation session. *, **, and *** denote p<0.05, p<0.01, and p<0.001 compared to the robot session, respectively. (G) Stim pairs (n = 16) had higher correlated firing than non-stim pairs (n = 10) during the 20–40 ms post-fire period (U = 32, p=0.011; Mann–Whitney U test) but lower during the 60–100 ms window (Us < 37, ps<0.023; Mann–Whitney U test). *denotes p<0.05 compared to the non-stim pairs. (H) CC peaks of stim pairs during the robot session tended to be higher than those of non-stim pairs (U = 43.0, p=0.051; Mann–Whitney U test).

To further investigate how stimulation-responsive neurons respond to an actual predator, three out of the six rats were tested with an approaching robot following the post-stim session (Figure 3F). The predatory robot increased outbound foraging time compared to the post-stim session (Figure 3G). Among the 85 units recorded in the BLA, robot-specific cells increased firing in response to the advancing robot (robot cells; BLA, 25.9%; Figure 3—figure supplement 1D–F). Correlation analysis revealed that 95.4% of BLA robot cells did not show significant correlations between firing rates and movement speed (Figure 3—figure supplement 1G). Additionally, 23/85 BLA units were responsive to optical stimulation during the stim session but not pre-/post-stim sessions (Figure 3H and I, Figure 3—figure supplement 1H). The proportions of the robot and non-robot cells differed between stimulation-responsive and nonresponsive cells, with a lower proportion of robot cells in the stimulation-nonresponsive cells (Figure 3J). Stimulation-responsive cells exhibited higher firing rates to the robot predator than stimulation-nonresponsive cells (Figure 3K, Figure 3—figure supplement 1I and J). The higher the maximal firing rate was during the stimulation, the greater the cells fired to the actual robot predator (Figure 3L).

We computed cross-correlations between simultaneously recorded BLA cell pairs to test how subpopulations of BLA neurons co-activate to the predatory threat differently. From 66 BLA recorded neurons, 185 BLA pairs were computed for cross-correlograms (CCs) during post-robot surge (2 s periods subsequent to robot activation; robot session), post-pellet, and post-stim (2 s periods subsequent to pellet procurement; pre- and post-stim sessions) epochs. Twenty-six CCs showed significant peaks (z scores > 3) around the paired spikes (between 0 ms and 100 ms) during the post-surge epoch (Figure 3—figure supplement 2A). Cell pairs with spike synchrony during the post-robot surge epoch did not show correlated firing during the post-pellet or post-stim epochs (Figure 3M, Figure 3—figure supplement 2B and C). This effect was prominent when pairs contained at least one of the stimulation-responsive cells (stim pairs; Figure 3N, Figure 3—figure supplement 2D and E), but not when pairs include only stimulation-nonresponsive cells (non-stim pairs; Figure 3N, Figure 3—figure supplement 2D and F). Stim pairs exhibited greater synchronous firing compared to non-stim pairs during the 0–50 ms window but had lower correlated firing during the 50–100 ms window, indicating that dPAG-stimulation responsive cells fire together more closely than dPAG-stimulation non-responsive cells (Figure 3O, Figure 3—figure supplement 2G). Additionally, the stim pairs tended to have higher peaks of the CCs than the non-stim pairs (Figure 3—figure supplement 2H). Taken together, these results suggest that dPAG stimulation produces activity changes in subpopulations of BLA neurons, primarily in predator detection cells (robot cells), supporting the idea that PAG conveys innate fear signals to the amygdala.

The midline thalamus interconnects the dPAG and BLA

Based on previous anatomical studies (Cameron et al., 1995; Krout and Loewy, 2000) and response latency data in the present study (Figure 3—figure supplement 1B and C), it is likely that projections from the dPAG to the BLA are indirect. To identify potential mediators that relay predator signals from the dPAG to the BLA, we injected the anterograde tracer AAV-CaMKII-EYFP into the dPAG and investigated robot-induced c-Fos activities in AAV-expressed terminal areas of the dPAG. Cholera toxin subunit B (CTB) was also injected into the BLA in a subset of AAV-injected animals to examine potential anatomical evidence of relays between the dPAG and BLA (Figure 4A and B). After recovery, rats were trained to procure a pellet from the foraging arena. Animals were randomly assigned to foraging-only and robot-experienced groups. The foraging-only group quickly procured the pellet, while the robot-experienced group failed during the testing session (Figure 4C). Ninety minutes after testing, animals were perfused to analyze c-Fos activity and the tracers. Consistent with prior research (Cameron et al., 1995; Krout and Loewy, 2000), we also observed strong terminal expression in midline thalamic areas emerging from dPAG cell body AAV infection (Figure 4D). Among these, the PVT showed increased levels of c-Fos-positive cells in rats encountering the robot predator compared to controls (Figure 4E and F). In animals that were also injected with CTB in the BLA, the robot encounter increased the percentage of CTB-labeled PVT neurons expressing c-Fos, compared to the foraging-only control, while the density of CTB remained comparable between the groups (Figure 4G and H, Figure 4—figure supplement 1). Collectively, these results suggest predator-induced dPAG activity may convey information to the BLA, potentially through PVT activity.

Figure 4. Paraventricular nucleus of the thalamus’ (PVT) hypothesized role in dorsal periaqueductal gray (dPAG) to amygdala signaling and antipredatory behavior model.

(A) Cholera toxin subunit B (CTB) retrograde tracer and AAV-CaMKII-EYFP used to trace dPAG signals to the basolateral amygdala (BLA). (B) Representative images of AAV and CTB expressions in dPAG and BLA, respectively. (C) Robot encounters hindered pellet procurement compared to foraging-only rats (Base, U = 26, p=0.6926; Test, U = 0, p=0.0001; Mann–Whitney U test). ***p<0.001 compared to the foraging-only group. (D) Terminal expressions of AAV injected into the dPAG cell bodies were predominantly observed in the midline nuclei of the thalamus. (E) PVT showed higher c-Fos-positive cells in robot-experienced rats (n = 10) compared to foraging-only control rats (n = 6) (U = 4.0, p=0.0027; Mann–Whitney U test), while other midline thalamic areas showed no differences (t(14)s < 1.611, ps>0.129; t-test). **p<0.01 compared to the foraging-only group. Values were normalized to the mean of the corresponding control group. (F) Representative photomicrographs of PVT c-Fos staining from foraging-only (upper) and robot-experienced (bottom) rats. (G) Representative microphotographs of AAV, CTB, c-Fos, and triple staining in PVT comparing foraging-only and robot-experienced animals. (H) Robot exposure increased the percentage of CTB-labeled PVT neurons expressing c-Fos (t(9) = 3.171, p=0.0113), while CTB density levels were comparable between the two groups (robot-experienced, n = 5; foraging-only, n = 6; t(9) = 0.9039, p=0.3896; t-test). Values were normalized to the mean of the corresponding control group. (I) Proposed model: predator surge detection via visual pathways, e.g., superior colliculus (Furigo et al., 2010; Rhoades et al., 1989), leads to dPAG activation, signaling through PVT to excite BLA. The BLA then projects to regions controlling escape responses, such as dorsal/posterior striatum (Li et al., 2021; Menegas et al., 2018) and ventromedial hypothalamus (Silva et al., 2013; Kunwar et al., 2015; Wang et al., 2015).

Figure 4.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1. Double labeling of cholera toxin subunit B and c-FOS in paraventricular nucleus of the thalamus (PVT) cells.

Figure 4—figure supplement 1.

Robot exposure increased the percentage of CTB-labeled PVT neurons expressing c-Fos (t(9) = 2.278, p=0.0487; robot-experienced, t-test; n = 5; foraging-only, n = 6). CTB density levels remained similar between groups (t(9) = 0.7121, p=0.4945; t-test). * denotes p<0.05.

Discussion

Prehistoric rock carvings and cave drawings of large carnivores from the Ice Age suggest that the fear of predation played a crucial role in the lives of early humans in their habitats (Mithen, 1999). As such, the brain’s fear system, which evolved to counteract unpredictable, uncontrolled threats originating from biological agents, likely influences our everyday behavior when confronted with perceived risks. In line with this view, studies using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) and an ‘active escape paradigm’—in which virtually represented subjects are chased by a virtual predator and receive an electric shock when ‘caught’—have shown that as the threat transitions from a distant position to an impending encounter with the subject, neural activity shifts from the ventromedial PFC and amygdala toward the PAG (Mobbs et al., 2009; Mobbs et al., 2007).

The present study investigated the neural mechanisms underlying antipredatory-defensive behaviors in rats engaged in a naturalistic goal-directed behavior of foraging for food in an open arena, using a combination of electrophysiology, optogenetics, and tracing techniques. We found that neurons in the dPAG are involved in detecting predatory threats and eliciting antipredatory-defensive behaviors in rats. Specifically, dPAG neurons displayed increased spiking in response to an approaching robot predator as rats reflexively fled from the open foraging arena into a safe nest. However, dPAG neuronal activity did not increase when the robot was stationary, which contrasts with a previous study that reported enhanced dPAG neuronal activity in mice exposed to an anesthetized (motionless) rat separated by a wire mesh in a chamber (Deng et al., 2016) or a live rat tethered (restrained) with a harness (Reis et al., 2021). It has been suggested that looming stimuli may serve as simple, evolutionarily reliable signals of danger because genes are not capable of providing the brain with detailed information about all potential predatory threats, and all predators must approach their prey for consumption (Kim and Jung, 2018). Our study also demonstrated that optogenetic stimulation of CaMKII-expressing dPAG neurons caused naïve rats approaching a food pellet to instantly flee to the nest without an external threat (see also Tsang et al., 2023). This confirmed escape behavior in foraging rats with dPAG electrical stimulation (Kim et al., 2013) was evoked by intrinsic dPAG neurons, not the fibers of passage or current spread to other brain regions. In contrast to our observation of escape-to-the-nest behavior, optogenetic activation of dPAG in mice elicited various defensive behaviors, such as running, freezing, and conditioned avoidance, when tested in a chamber (Deng et al., 2016). This is consistent with the notion that the behavioral outcome from brain stimulation is influenced by environmental settings (Kim et al., 2013).

As mentioned in the introduction, the dPAG is recognized as part of the ascending nociceptive pathway to the BLA (De Oca et al., 1998; Gross and Canteras, 2012; Herry and Johansen, 2014; Kim et al., 1993; Ressler and Maren, 2019; Walker and Davis, 1997). The dPAG is also implicated in non-opioid analgesia (Bagley and Ingram, 2020; Cannon et al., 1982; Fields, 2000). However, it is essential to emphasize that, despite its roles in pain modulation, the primary behavior observed in dPAG-stimulated, naive rats foraging for food in an open arena was goal-directed escape to the safe nest, underscoring the dPAG’s critical function in survival behaviors.

Our study also revealed that BLA neurons respond both to direct dPAG optical stimulation and to an external robot predator. Specifically, BLA cells responsive to dPAG stimulation showed increased firing rates and were more likely to be co-activated by predatory threats compared to dPAG stimulation-non-responsive BLA cells. Additionally, the firing rates of BLA cells in response to dPAG stimulation highly correlated with their responses to the actual predatory agent, indicating that BLA neurons involved in processing dPAG signals may play a crucial role in enhancing the coherence of the BLA network, thereby effectively managing predatory threat information. These findings, combined with previous research demonstrating that the amygdala is necessary for both intrinsic dPAG stimulation-induced (Kim et al., 2013) and extrinsic robot-evoked (Choi and Kim, 2010) escape behavior, suggest the dPAG-amygdala pathway’s involvement in processing innate fear signals and generating antipredatory-defensive behavior. However, it is crucial to consider the recent discovery that optogenetic stimulation of CA3 neurons (3000 pulses) leads to gain-of-function changes in CA3-CA3 recurrent (monosynaptic) excitatory synapses (Oishi et al., 2019). Although there is no direct connection between dPAG neurons and the BLA (Vianna and Brandão, 2003; McNally et al., 2011; Cameron et al., 1995), and no studies have yet demonstrated gain-of-function changes in polysynaptic pathways to our knowledge, the potential for our dPAG photostimulation (320 pulses) to induce similar changes in amygdalar neurons, thereby enhancing their sensitivity to predatory threats, cannot be dismissed.

We confirmed strong terminal expressions in the midline thalamic areas (Cameron et al., 1995; Krout and Loewy, 2000) following AAV injection into the dPAG using immunohistochemistry. Notably, the PVT, in contrast to other midline thalamic subregions, exhibited increased c-Fos reactivity in response to the robot predator. This observation aligns with recent findings that dPAG projections specifically target the PVT rather than the centromedial intralaminar thalamic nucleus (Yeh et al., 2021). Additionally, subpopulations of the c-Fos-reactive PVT neurons were found to express CTB, retrogradely labeled by injection into the BLA. These results, in conjunction with previous research on the roles of the dPAG, PVT, and BLA in producing flight behaviors in naïve rats (Choi and Kim, 2010; Daviu et al., 2020; Deng et al., 2016; Kim et al., 2013; Kim et al., 2018; Kong et al., 2021; Ma et al., 2021; Reis et al., 2021), the anterior PVT’s involvement in cat odor-induced avoidance behavior (Engelke et al., 2021), and the PVT’s regulation of behaviors motivated by both appetitive and aversive stimuli (Choi et al., 2019; Choi and McNally, 2017), suggest the involvement of the dPAG→PVT→BLA pathways in antipredatory-defensive mechanisms, particularly as rats leave the safety of the nest to forage in an open arena (Figure 4I; Reis et al., 2023). Given the recent significance attributed to the superior colliculus in detecting innate visual threats (Lischinsky and Lin, 2019; Wei et al., 2015; Zhou et al., 2019) and the cuneiform nucleus in the directed flight behavior of mice (Bindi et al., 2023; Tsang et al., 2023), further exploration into the communication between these structures and the dPAG-BLA circuitry is warranted.

While our findings demonstrate that opto-stimulation of the dPAG is sufficient to trigger both fleeing behavior and increased BLA activity, we have not established that the dPAG-PVT circuit is necessary for the BLA’s response to predatory threats. To establish causality and interregional relationships, future studies should employ methods such as pathway-specific optogenetic inhibition (using retrograde Cre-recombinase virus with inhibitory opsins; Lavoie and Liu, 2020; Li et al., 2016; Senn et al., 2014) or chemogenetics (Boender et al., 2014; Roth, 2016) in conjunction with single-unit recordings to fully characterize the dPAG-PVT-BLA circuitry’s (as opposed to other midline thalamic regions for controls) role in processing predatory threat-induced escape behavior. If inactivating the dPAG-PVT circuits reduces the BLA’s response to threats, this would highlight the central role of the dPAG-PVT pathway in this defense mechanism. Conversely, if the BLA’s response remains unchanged despite dPAG-PVT inactivation, it could suggest the existence of multiple pathways for antipredatory defenses.

Future studies will also need to delineate the downstream pathways emanating from the BLA that orchestrate goal-directed flight responses to external predatory threats as well as internal stimulations from the dPAG/BLA circuit. Potential key structures include the dorsal/posterior striatum, which has been associated with avoidance behaviors in response to airpuff in head-fixed mice (Menegas et al., 2018) and flight reactions triggered by auditory looming cues (Li et al., 2021). Additionally, the ventromedial hypothalamus (VMH) has been implicated in flight behaviors in mice, evidenced by responses to the presence of a rat predator (Silva et al., 2013) and upon optogenetic activation of VMH Steroidogenic factor 1 (Kunwar et al., 2015) or the VMH-anterior hypothalamic nucleus pathway (Wang et al., 2015). Investigating the indispensable role of these structures in flight behavior could involve lesion or inactivation studies. Such interventions are anticipated to inhibit flight behaviors elicited by amygdala stimulation and predatory threats, confirming their critical involvement. Conversely, activating these structures in subjects with an inactivated or lesioned amygdala, which would typically inhibit fear responses to external threats (Choi and Kim, 2010), is expected to induce fleeing behavior, further elucidating their functional significance.

The innate predator-fear functions of the dPAG and BLA are distinct from their learned fear functions, as observed in FC studies. In FC, the dPAG is proposed to play a role in ‘US-processing’, transmitting footshock (pain) information to the amygdala, thus enhancing the CS pathway’s ability to activate the amygdalar fear system (Fanselow, 1998; Maren, 2001; Almeida et al., 2004). This hypothesis is supported by research demonstrating that muscimol inactivation of the dPAG hinders FC to the footshock US (Johansen et al., 2010), while electrical stimulation of dPAG neurons effectively serves as a US surrogate for FC (Ballesteros et al., 2014; Kim et al., 2013). As conditioning progresses, amygdalar fear may suppress footshock-evoked dPAG’s teaching signals, thereby moderating FC in the amygdala, potentially through conditioned analgesia that dampens the footshock US’s painfulness (Fanselow, 1981; Fanselow and Bolles, 1979). Consistent with this amygdalar-dPAG negative feedback notion, research has revealed that as CS-evoked responses of amygdalar neurons increase with CS-US pairings, footshock-evoked neural activity in the dPAG decreases (Johansen et al., 2010; Ozawa et al., 2017; Quirk et al., 1995). It remains to be determined whether there are distinct populations of innate predator-responsive and pain-responsive dPAG neurons or if the same dPAG neurons react to a wide range of aversive stimuli. Furthermore, it is worth noting that the same dPAG stimulation resulted in an activity burst UR in a small operant chamber, as opposed to a goal-directed escape to a nest UR in a large foraging arena (Kim et al., 2013). Additionally, FC to shock or predator US does not readily occur in naturalistic settings (Zambetti et al., 2022).

Overall, this study enhances our understanding of the neural basis of antipredatory-defensive behaviors in rats, emphasizing the important roles of the dPAG and BLA in processing and responding to threat-related stimuli. The study also suggests the possibility of an innate fear mechanism, which may complement the commonly studied learned fear mechanism in the research of anxiety and fear-related disorders. Further research into these mechanisms could lead to the development of novel therapeutic interventions that target both innate and learned fear mechanisms, improving the treatment of anxiety and fear-related disorders.

Materials and methods

Subjects

Male and female Long–Evans rats (initial weight 325–350 g) were individually housed in a climate-controlled vivarium (accredited by the Association for Assessment and Accreditation of Laboratory Animal Care), with a reversed 12 hr light/dark cycle (lights on at 7 PM) and placed on a standard food-restriction schedule with free access to water to gradually reach ~85% normal body weights. All experiments were performed during the dark cycle in compliance with the University of Washington Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee guidelines (IACUC protocol #: 4040-01).

Surgery

Under anesthesia (94 mg/kg ketamine and 6 mg/kg xylazine, intraperitoneally), rats were mounted in a stereotaxic instrument (Kopf) and implanted with a Microdrive of tetrode bundles (formvar-insulated nichrome wires, 14 µm diameter; Kanthal) into the dPAG (AP, –6.8; ML, +0.6; from Bregma), or BLA (AP, –2.8; ML, +5.2; from Bregma). For optogenetic stimulation, anesthetized male rats were injected with adeno-associated viruses (AAVs; serotype 5) to express Channelrhodopsin-EYFP (AAV5-CaMKIIa-hChR2(H134R)-EYFP, UNC Vector Core) or EYFP only (AAV5-CaMKIIa-EYFP) in the dPAG at a rate of 0.05 µl/min (total volume of 0.5 µl) via a microinjection pump (UMP3-1, World Precision Instruments) with a 33-gauge syringe (Hamilton). Animals were randomly assigned to each group. To avoid backflow of the virus, the injection needle was left in place for 10 min. After virus injection, an optic fiber attached to ferrules (0.22 NA, 200 µm core; ferrule diameter: 2.5 mm; Doric Lenses) or an optrode was implanted 0.4 mm dorsal to the injection sites. The microdrive and/or ferrule were secured by Metabond and dental cement with anchoring screws. Behavioral and recording experiments started after at least 1 week of recovery while optogenetic stimulation sessions started after at least 4 weeks after the surgery to allow for sufficient viral expression. For tracing experiments, male and female rats were injected with AAV-CamKII-EYFP into the dPAG and CTB into the BLA > 5 weeks and 1 week prior to the predator testing, respectively.

Foraging apparatus

A custom-built foraging arena comprised a nest (29 cm × 57 cm × 60 cm height) and a foraging area (202 cm length × 58 cm width × 6 cm height) with a V-shaped gate connecting the two areas. The animals’ movement was automatically tracked via two video tracking systems (Neuralynx and Any-maze).

Behavioral procedures

Habituation

Animals were placed in the nest area, where they were acclimated to the experimental room and chamber and allowed to eat food pellets (0.5 g; F0171, Bio-Serv) for 30 min/day for two consecutive days.

Baseline foraging

After the animals were placed in the nest, the gate to the foraging area was opened, allowing them to explore the arena. The animals were gradually trained to acquire a food pellet from various distances (25 cm, 50 cm, and 75 cm from the nest for the behavior-only experiment; an additional 100 cm and 125 cm for the recording experiment) and then return to their nest to consume it. Once the rat returned to the nest with the pellet, the gate was closed. The behavior-only groups underwent 4 days of baseline foraging training. For the recording experiments, baseline sessions continued until unit activity in the dPAG or amygdala unit was successfully detected.

Testing

For dPAG recordings, single units were recorded across three successive sessions: pre-robot, robot, and post-robot (7–14 trials/session; 4–14 recording days; n = 5 rats, 96 units). The number of trials varied among subjects because trials were excluded if rats attempted to procure the pellet within 10 s post-dPAG stimulation or robot activation. This exclusion criterion was implemented to ensure an accurate characterization of unit responsiveness. In the pre- and post-robot trials, rats were permitted to freely procure the pellet. Before initiating the robot trials, a programmed robot (LEGO Mindstorms EV3 set) was placed at the end of the foraging arena. Upon the gate’s opening, whenever the rat came near (~25 cm) the pellet, the programmed robot would surge forward 23 cm, 60 cm, or 140 cm toward the animal at a velocity of ~75 cm/s, before returning to its original position. If the rat made another attempt to procure the pellet within 10 s after a robot activation, both the preceding and subsequent trials were excluded from the analyses to isolate and measure the unit properties accurately. For optical stimulation and behavioral experiments, the procedure included three baseline trials with the pellet placed 75 cm away, followed by three dPAG stimulation trials with the pellet locations sequentially set at 75 cm, 50 cm, and 25 cm. During each approach to the pellet, rats received 473 nm light stimulation (1–2 s, 20 Hz, 10 ms width, 1–3 mW) through a laser (Opto Engine LLC) and a pulse generator (Master-8; A.M.P.I.). Additional testing to examine the functional response curves was conducted over multiple days, with incremental adjustments to the stimulation parameters (intensity, frequency, duration) after confirming that normal baseline foraging behavior was maintained. For these tests, one parameter was adjusted incrementally while the others were held constant (intensity curve at 20 Hz, 2 s; frequency curve at 3 mW, 2 s; duration curve at 20 Hz, 3 mW). If the rat failed to procure the pellet within 3 min, the gate was closed, and the trial was concluded. For amygdala recordings with optical stimulation of the dPAG, pre-stim, stim, and post-stim sessions (5–15 trials/session) were carried out successively on the same day. These three consecutive phases mirrored the pre-robot, robot, and post-robot sessions, except that the dPAG light stimulation was given instead of the robot predator (6–18 recording days; n = 6 rats, 320 units). A subset of these animals (n = 3 out of 6 rats, 85 out of 320 units) also participated in a robot session following the post-stim session on selected days (1–16 recording days). Sessions involving the robot predator were repeated until habituation was observed or recordings were deemed invalid due to limitations with the microdrive or failure to detect units. Note that the mean success rate for the dPAG recordings was 2.8% ± 1.31, and animals subjected to the BLA recordings did not succeed in retrieving pellets during any of the robot trials.

Optrode recording under anesthesia

Upon optrode implantation, the animal was kept under anesthesia and moved to a unit recording setup (Neuralynx). The electrode was slowly lowered, and dPAG activity was monitored. Once dPAG activity was detected, eight light stimulations (2 s, 20 Hz, 10 ms width) were given with 30 s inter-stimulus intervals. In total, 10 and 14 stimulation sessions were repeated in the two anesthetized rats, respectively. After the recording was completed, the animal was immediately perfused under an overdose of Beuthanasia. Only the unit data collected in the dPAG were included in the analyses after histological verification.

Single-unit recording and analyses

Extracellular single-unit activity was recorded through a 24-channel microdrive array loaded with a bundle of six tetrodes. The electrode tip was gold-plated to 100–300 kΩ measured at 1 kHz. The signals were amplified (×10,000), filtered (600–6000 kHz), and digitized (32 kHz) using a Cheetah data acquisition system (Neuralynx). A spike-sorting program (SpikeSort 3D; Neuralynx) and additional manual cutting were used for cluster isolation. Peri-event time histograms (PETH) were generated using NeuroExplorer (version 5.030; Nex Technologies) and further analyzed with custom MATLAB programs. The unit and speed data were binned at 0.1 s or 1 s and aligned to the time when the robot was activated, when the pellet was procured, or when the rat turned its body immediately before fleeing to the nest. All PETH data were normalized (z-scored) to the pre-event baseline period (from –5 s to 0 s prior to pellet procurement or robot activation, across 50 bins). Food approaching was indicated by the animal entering a designated zone 19 cm from the pellet, a behavior driven by hunger. To classify the responsiveness of the unit responses, the first five bins (0.1 s bins) were analyzed. If one or more bins within the 500 ms period showed z-scores higher than 3 (z > 3) exclusively during the robot session, then the unit was classified as a ‘robot cell’. The rest of the cells were further classified as food-specific (z > 3 exclusively during pre-robot session), BOTH (z > 3 during both pre-robot and robot sessions), or none (non-responsive) cells. In the light stimulation and recording experiments, the same criteria were applied to the unit classification except that the stimulation-responsive units (stim cells) were defined as cells showing significant activity (z > 3) during the stim session when aligned to the stimulation onset instead of the robot activation.

Cross-correlation

BLA cells that were simultaneously recorded from rats undergoing the four successive sessions (pre-stim, stim, post-stim, and robot sessions) were analyzed to generate CCs. Units firing less than 0.1 Hz were excluded due to the possible false peaks in their CCs. The shift predictors (100 random trial shuffles) were subtracted from the raw CCs. The correlated firing (in 10 ms bins) during the 0–2 s period following robot activation was calculated, and the CCs that showed significant peaks (z > 3) within the 0–100 ms window during the robot session were further analyzed. The peak values of the CCs and the peak areas under CC curves during the 100 ms window were compared across sessions (pre-stim, stim, post-stim, and robot sessions).

Histology

To verify the electrode placement, rats were overdosed with Beuthanasia, given electrolytic currents (10 µA, 10 s) in the target regions through the tetrode tips, and perfused intracardially with 0.9% saline and then 10% formalin. For rats injected with the virus, phosphate-buffered saline (PBS) and 4% paraformaldehyde were used as perfusates. Extracted brains were stored in the fixative at 4°C overnight followed by 30% sucrose solution until they sank. Transverse sections (50 µm) were mounted on gelatin-coated slides and stained with cresyl violet and Prussian blue dyes to examine the recording sites. To confirm viral expression, 30 µm sections were cut, mounted, and cover-slipped with Flouromount-GTM with DAPI (eBioscience). Immunohistochemistry was performed on some of the sections to visualize the viral and c-Fos expressions. In short, the sections were washed with 0.1 M PBS for 10 min three times, followed by three rinses with PBS with Triton X-100 (PBST). After 2 hr of blocking (using normal goat serum), the sections were incubated in primary antibodies (1:500 mouse anti-GFP and rabbit anti-c-Fos; Abcam) overnight. The following day, additional PBST washes and secondary antibodies (1:250 anti-mouse Alexa 488 and anti-rabbit Alexa 405; Abcam) were applied. The sections were examined under a fluorescence microscope (Keyence BZ-X800E) and analyzed using ImageJ (NIH). Only rats with correct viral expression or electrode/optic fiber tip locations in the target structure(s) were included for the statistical analyses (Supplementary file 1A).

Statistical analyses

Based on normality tests (Kolmogorov–Smirnov test, p<0.01; Supplementary file 1B), non-normally distributed variables were analyzed using non-parametric tests, while parametric tests were applied to normally distributed variables. Statistical significance across sessions was determined using the Friedman test, followed by Dunn’s multiple-comparison test with correction when needed. Pearson’s correlation coefficients evaluated variable relationships, and chi-square tests compared percentages of distinct unit types. Group comparisons involved independent t-tests or Mann–Whitney U tests, and within-group comparisons employed paired t-tests or Wilcoxon signed rank tests. Statistical analyses and graph generation were performed using SPSS (ver. 19), custom MATLAB codes, GraphPad Prism (ver. 9.00), and NeuroExplorer (ver. 5.030).

Acknowledgements

This study was supported by the National Institutes of Health grants MH099073 (JJK), AG067008 (EJK), and F32MH127801 (MK), and the Ministry of Science and ICT through the National Research Foundation of Korea grant: Brain Science Research Program NRF-2022M3E5E8018421 (JC) and NRF-2022R1A2C2009265 (JC). Artworks in Figures 1E, 2D, 3B and F were created with BioRender.com, and published using a CC BY-NC-ND license with permission.

Funding Statement

The funders had no role in study design, data collection and interpretation, or the decision to submit the work for publication.

Contributor Information

Jeiwon Cho, Email: jelectro21@ewha.ac.kr.

Jeansok John Kim, Email: jeansokk@uw.edu.

Kate M Wassum, University of California, Los Angeles, United States.

Kate M Wassum, University of California, Los Angeles, United States.

Funding Information

This paper was supported by the following grants:

  • National Institutes of Health MH099073 to Jeansok John Kim.

  • National Institutes of Health F32MH127801 to Mi-Seon Kong.

  • National Research Foundation of Korea NRF-2022M3E5E8018421 to Jeiwon Cho.

  • National Research Foundation of Korea NRF-2022R1A2C2009265 to Jeiwon Cho.

  • National Institutes of Health AG067008 to Eun Joo Kim.

Additional information

Competing interests

No competing interests declared.

Author contributions

Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Funding acquisition, Validation, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Data curation, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Visualization, Methodology, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.

Conceptualization, Supervision, Funding acquisition, Validation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Project administration, Writing – review and editing.

Ethics

All experiments were performed during the dark cycle in compliance with the University of Washington Institutional Animal Care and Use Committee guidelines (IACUC protocol: #4040-01).

Additional files

Supplementary file 1. Reconstructions of histology and normality test results.

(A) Histological reconstructions of recording sites in the dPAG and BLA, and optic fiber locations in the dPAG. (A) Red bars show the trajectory of tetrode recording sites in the dPAG. (B) Red bars depict the trajectory of optrode recording sites in the dPAG. (B) Red and gray circles represent the optic fiber locations for ChR2 and EYFP rats, respectively. (D) Orange circles and red bars indicate the optic fiber locations in the dPAG (left) and recording trajectories in the BLA (right), respectively. Numerical values represent AP coordinates relative to Bregma. (B) Normality test. The normality of the variable distributions was assessed using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test (p<0.01). Depending on the results of this test, parametric tests were used for normally distributed variables, while nonparametric tests were employed for variables that were not normally distributed.

elife-88733-supp1.docx (132.2KB, docx)
MDAR checklist

Data availability

The data that support the findings of this study is available from the Dryad data repository at https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.stqjq2cbh. The customized analysis tools are deposited on GitHub: https://github.com/KimLab-UW/Velocity (copy archived at Park, 2024).

The following dataset was generated:

Kim EJ, Kong MS, Park S, Cho J, Kim JJ. 2024. Periaqueductal gray activates antipredatory neural responses in the amygdala of foraging rats. Dryad Digital Repository.

References

  1. Almeida TF, Roizenblatt S, Tufik S. Afferent pain pathways: a neuroanatomical review. Brain Research. 2004;1000:40–56. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2003.10.073. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  2. Amano K, Tanikawa T, Kawamura H, Iseki H, Notani M, Kawabatake H, Shiwaku T, Suda T, Demura H, Kitamura K. Endorphins and pain relief: further observations on electrical stimulation of the lateral part of the periaqueductal gray matter during rostral mesencephalic reticulotomy for pain relief. Applied Neurophysiology. 1982;45:123–135. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Bagley EE, Ingram SL. Endogenous opioid peptides in the descending pain modulatory circuit. Neuropharmacology. 2020;173:108131. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2020.108131. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Ballesteros CI, de Oliveira Galvão B, Maisonette S, Landeira-Fernandez J. Effect of dorsal and ventral hippocampal lesions on contextual fear conditioning and unconditioned defensive behavior induced by electrical stimulation of the dorsal periaqueductal gray. PLOS ONE. 2014;9:e83342. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0083342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Bandler R, Carrive P, Zhang SP. Integration of somatic and autonomic reactions within the midbrain periaqueductal grey: viscerotopic, somatotopic and functional organization. Progress in Brain Research. 1991;87:269–305. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(08)63056-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bandler R, Keay KA. Columnar organization in the midbrain periaqueductal gray and the integration of emotional expression. Progress in Brain Research. 1996;107:285–300. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(08)61871-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bindi RP, Guimarães CC, de Oliveira AR, Melleu FF, de Lima MAX, Baldo MVC, Motta SC, Canteras NS. Anatomical and functional study of the cuneiform nucleus: A critical site to organize innate defensive behaviors. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2023;1521:79–95. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14954. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Boender AJ, de Jong JW, Boekhoudt L, Luijendijk MCM, van der Plasse G, Adan RAH. Combined use of the canine adenovirus-2 and DREADD-technology to activate specific neural pathways in vivo. PLOS ONE. 2014;9:e95392. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0095392. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  9. Brunzell DH, Kim JJ. Fear conditioning to tone, but not to context, is attenuated by lesions of the insular cortex and posterior extension of the intralaminar complex in rats. Behavioral Neuroscience. 2001;115:365–375. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Cameron AA, Khan IA, Westlund KN, Cliffer KD, Willis WD. The efferent projections of the periaqueductal gray in the rat: a phaseolus vulgaris-leucoagglutinin study. i. ascending projections. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1995;351:568–584. doi: 10.1002/cne.903510407. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Cannon JT, Prieto GJ, Lee A, Liebeskind JC. Evidence for opioid and non-opioid forms of stimulation-produced analgesia in the rat. Brain Research. 1982;243:315–321. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(82)90255-4. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Carrive P. The periaqueductal gray and defensive behavior: functional representation and neuronal organization. Behavioural Brain Research. 1993;58:27–47. doi: 10.1016/0166-4328(93)90088-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Carrive P, Morgan MM. In: The Human Nervous System. Carrive P, editor. London: Academic Press; 2012. Periaqueductal gray; pp. 367–400. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  14. Choi J-S, Kim JJ. Amygdala regulates risk of predation in rats foraging in a dynamic fear environment. PNAS. 2010;107:21773–21777. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1010079108. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Choi EA, McNally GP. Paraventricular thalamus balances danger and reward. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2017;37:3018–3029. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3320-16.2017. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Choi EA, Jean-Richard-Dit-Bressel P, Clifford CWG, McNally GP. Paraventricular thalamus controls behavior during motivational conflict. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2019;39:4945–4958. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2480-18.2019. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Daviu N, Füzesi T, Rosenegger DG, Rasiah NP, Sterley T-L, Peringod G, Bains JS. Paraventricular nucleus CRH neurons encode stress controllability and regulate defensive behavior selection. Nature Neuroscience. 2020;23:398–410. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-0591-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. De Oca BM, DeCola JP, Maren S, Fanselow MS. Distinct regions of the periaqueductal gray are involved in the acquisition and expression of defensive responses. The Journal of Neuroscience. 1998;18:3426–3432. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.18-09-03426.1998. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Deng H, Xiao X, Wang Z. Periaqueductal gray neuronal activities underlie different aspects of defensive behaviors. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2016;36:7580–7588. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4425-15.2016. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Di Scala G, Mana MJ, Jacobs WJ, Phillips AG. Evidence of Pavlovian conditioned fear following electrical stimulation of the periaqueductal grey in the rat. Physiology & Behavior. 1987;40:55–63. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(87)90185-5. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Engelke DS, Zhang XO, O’Malley JJ, Fernandez-Leon JA, Li S, Kirouac GJ, Beierlein M, Do-Monte FH. A hypothalamic-thalamostriatal circuit that controls approach-avoidance conflict in rats. Nature Communications. 2021;12:2517. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-22730-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Fanselow MS, Bolles RC. Triggering of the endorphin analgesic reaction by a cue previously associated with shock: Reversal by naloxone. Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society. 1979;14:88–90. doi: 10.3758/BF03329408. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  23. Fanselow MS. Naloxone and pavlovian fear conditioning. Learning and Motivation. 1981;12:398–419. doi: 10.1016/0023-9690(81)90002-3. [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  24. Fanselow MS. Pavlovian conditioning, negative feedback, and blocking: mechanisms that regulate association formation. Neuron. 1998;20:625–627. doi: 10.1016/s0896-6273(00)81002-8. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Fields HL. Pain modulation: expectation, opioid analgesia and virtual pain. Progress in Brain Research. 2000;122:245–253. doi: 10.1016/s0079-6123(08)62143-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Furigo IC, de Oliveira WF, de Oliveira AR, Comoli E, Baldo MVC, Mota-Ortiz SR, Canteras NS. The role of the superior colliculus in predatory hunting. Neuroscience. 2010;165:1–15. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Gross CT, Canteras NS. The many paths to fear. Nature Reviews. Neuroscience. 2012;13:651–658. doi: 10.1038/nrn3301. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  28. Herry C, Johansen JP. Encoding of fear learning and memory in distributed neuronal circuits. Nature Neuroscience. 2014;17:1644–1654. doi: 10.1038/nn.3869. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Jenck F, Moreau JL, Martin JR. Dorsal periaqueductal gray-induced aversion as a simulation of panic anxiety: elements of face and predictive validity. Psychiatry Research. 1995;57:181–191. doi: 10.1016/0165-1781(95)02673-k. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Johansen JP, Tarpley JW, LeDoux JE, Blair HT. Neural substrates for expectation-modulated fear learning in the amygdala and periaqueductal gray. Nature Neuroscience. 2010;13:979–986. doi: 10.1038/nn.2594. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Kim JJ, Rison RA, Fanselow MS. Effects of amygdala, hippocampus, and periaqueductal gray lesions on short- and long-term contextual fear. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1993;107:1093–1098. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.107.6.1093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Kim EJ, Horovitz O, Pellman BA, Tan LM, Li Q, Richter-Levin G, Kim JJ. Dorsal periaqueductal gray-amygdala pathway conveys both innate and learned fear responses in rats. PNAS. 2013;110:14795–14800. doi: 10.1073/pnas.1310845110. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Kim JJ, Jung MW. Fear paradigms: the times they are a-changin. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2018;24:38–43. doi: 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.02.007. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Kim EJ, Kong M-S, Park SG, Mizumori SJY, Cho J, Kim JJ. Dynamic coding of predatory information between the prelimbic cortex and lateral amygdala in foraging rats. Science Advances. 2018;4:eaar7328. doi: 10.1126/sciadv.aar7328. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. Kong M-S, Kim EJ, Park S, Zweifel LS, Huh Y, Cho J, Kim JJ. “Fearful-place” coding in the amygdala-hippocampal network. eLife. 2021;10:e72040. doi: 10.7554/eLife.72040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Krout KE, Loewy AD. Periaqueductal gray matter projections to midline and intralaminar thalamic nuclei of the rat. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 2000;424:111–141. doi: 10.1002/1096-9861(20000814)424:1&#x0003c;111::aid-cne9&#x0003e;3.0.co;2-3. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Kunwar PS, Zelikowsky M, Remedios R, Cai H, Yilmaz M, Meister M, Anderson DJ. Ventromedial hypothalamic neurons control a defensive emotion state. eLife. 2015;4:e06633. doi: 10.7554/eLife.06633. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Lavoie A, Liu B-H. Canine adenovirus 2: a natural choice for brain circuit dissection. Frontiers in Molecular Neuroscience. 2020;13:9. doi: 10.3389/fnmol.2020.00009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Li Y, Hickey L, Perrins R, Werlen E, Patel AA, Hirschberg S, Jones MW, Salinas S, Kremer EJ, Pickering AE. Retrograde optogenetic characterization of the pontospinal module of the locus coeruleus with a canine adenoviral vector. Brain Research. 2016;1641:274–290. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2016.02.023. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  40. Li Z, Wei JX, Zhang GW, Huang JJ, Zingg B, Wang X, Tao HW, Zhang LI. Corticostriatal control of defense behavior in mice induced by auditory looming cues. Nature Communications. 2021;12:e248-7. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-21248-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  41. Lischinsky JE, Lin D. Looming danger: unraveling the circuitry for predator threats. Trends in Neurosciences. 2019;42:841–842. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2019.10.004. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Ma J, du Hoffmann J, Kindel M, Beas BS, Chudasama Y, Penzo MA. Divergent projections of the paraventricular nucleus of the thalamus mediate the selection of passive and active defensive behaviors. Nature Neuroscience. 2021;24:1429–1440. doi: 10.1038/s41593-021-00912-7. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  43. Magierek V, Ramos PL, da Silveira-Filho NG, Nogueira RL, Landeira-Fernandez J. Context fear conditioning inhibits panic-like behavior elicited by electrical stimulation of dorsal periaqueductal gray. Neuroreport. 2003;14:1641–1644. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200308260-00020. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Maren S. Neurobiology of pavlovian fear conditioning. Annual Review of Neuroscience. 2001;24:897–931. doi: 10.1146/annurev.neuro.24.1.897. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. McNally GP, Johansen JP, Blair HT. Placing prediction into the fear circuit. Trends in Neurosciences. 2011;34:283–292. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.03.005. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  46. Menegas W, Akiti K, Amo R, Uchida N, Watabe-Uchida M. Dopamine neurons projecting to the posterior striatum reinforce avoidance of threatening stimuli. Nature Neuroscience. 2018;21:1421–1430. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0222-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Mithen S. The Prehistory of the Mind: The Cognitive Origins of Art, Religion and Science. London: Thames & Hudson; 1999. [Google Scholar]
  48. Mobbs D, Petrovic P, Marchant JL, Hassabis D, Weiskopf N, Seymour B, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. When fear is near: threat imminence elicits prefrontal-periaqueductal gray shifts in humans. Science. 2007;317:1079–1083. doi: 10.1126/science.1144298. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  49. Mobbs D, Marchant JL, Hassabis D, Seymour B, Tan G, Gray M, Petrovic P, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. From threat to fear: the neural organization of defensive fear systems in humans. The Journal of Neuroscience. 2009;29:12236–12243. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2378-09.2009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Oishi N, Nomoto M, Ohkawa N, Saitoh Y, Sano Y, Tsujimura S, Nishizono H, Matsuo M, Muramatsu S-I, Inokuchi K. Artificial association of memory events by optogenetic stimulation of hippocampal CA3 cell ensembles. Molecular Brain. 2019;12:2. doi: 10.1186/s13041-018-0424-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Olds ME, Olds J. Approach-avoidance analysis of rat diencephalon. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1963;120:259–295. doi: 10.1002/cne.901200206. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Ozawa T, Ycu EA, Kumar A, Yeh L-F, Ahmed T, Koivumaa J, Johansen JP. A feedback neural circuit for calibrating aversive memory strength. Nature Neuroscience. 2017;20:90–97. doi: 10.1038/nn.4439. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Park S. Velocity. swh:1:rev:85415c2c9255bbe26a897bed05cff3cc42ba29f4Software Heritage. 2024 https://archive.softwareheritage.org/swh:1:dir:ab8f109fe41194195924a1f4c21604ba11212b9d;origin=https://github.com/KimLab-UW/Velocity;visit=swh:1:snp:f84538aea3bd85da00d0fc803511ee2c7a58d594;anchor=swh:1:rev:85415c2c9255bbe26a897bed05cff3cc42ba29f4
  54. Quirk GJ, Repa C, LeDoux JE. Fear conditioning enhances short-latency auditory responses of lateral amygdala neurons: parallel recordings in the freely behaving rat. Neuron. 1995;15:1029–1039. doi: 10.1016/0896-6273(95)90092-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Reis FM, Lee JY, Maesta-Pereira S, Schuette PJ, Chakerian M, Liu J, La-Vu MQ, Tobias BC, Ikebara JM, Kihara AH, Canteras NS, Kao JC, Adhikari A. Dorsal periaqueductal gray ensembles represent approach and avoidance states. eLife. 2021;10:e64934. doi: 10.7554/eLife.64934. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Reis FMCV, Mobbs D, Canteras NS, Adhikari A. Orchestration of innate and conditioned defensive actions by the periaqueductal gray. Neuropharmacology. 2023;228:109458. doi: 10.1016/j.neuropharm.2023.109458. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Rescorla RA, Wagner AR. In: In Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory. Black AH, Prokosy WF, editors. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts; 1972. A theory of pavlovian conditioning: variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and nonreinforcement; pp. 64–99. [Google Scholar]
  58. Ressler RL, Maren S. Synaptic encoding of fear memories in the amygdala. Current Opinion in Neurobiology. 2019;54:54–59. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2018.08.012. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Rhoades RW, Mooney RD, Rohrer WH, Nikoletseas MM, Fish SE. Organization of the projection from the superficial to the deep layers of the hamster’s superior colliculus as demonstrated by the anterograde transport of Phaseolus vulgaris leucoagglutinin. The Journal of Comparative Neurology. 1989;283:54–70. doi: 10.1002/cne.902830106. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Roth BL. DREADDs for Neuroscientists. Neuron. 2016;89:683–694. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2016.01.040. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Senn V, Wolff SBE, Herry C, Grenier F, Ehrlich I, Gründemann J, Fadok JP, Müller C, Letzkus JJ, Lüthi A. Long-range connectivity defines behavioral specificity of amygdala neurons. Neuron. 2014;81:428–437. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2013.11.006. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Silva BA, Mattucci C, Krzywkowski P, Murana E, Illarionova A, Grinevich V, Canteras NS, Ragozzino D, Gross CT. Independent hypothalamic circuits for social and predator fear. Nature Neuroscience. 2013;16:1731–1733. doi: 10.1038/nn.3573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Tsang E, Orlandini C, Sureka R, Crevenna AH, Perlas E, Prankerd I, Masferrer ME, Gross CT. Induction of flight via midbrain projections to the cuneiform nucleus. PLOS ONE. 2023;18:e0281464. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0281464. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Vianna DML, Brandão ML. Anatomical connections of the periaqueductal gray: specific neural substrates for different kinds of fear. Brazilian Journal of Medical and Biological Research = Revista Brasileira de Pesquisas Medicas e Biologicas. 2003;36:557–566. doi: 10.1590/s0100-879x2003000500002. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Walker DL, Davis M. Involvement of the dorsal periaqueductal gray in the loss of fear-potentiated startle accompanying high footshock training. Behavioral Neuroscience. 1997;111:692–702. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.111.4.692. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Wang L, Chen IZ, Lin D. Collateral pathways from the ventromedial hypothalamus mediate defensive behaviors. Neuron. 2015;85:1344–1358. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.025. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Wei P, Liu N, Zhang Z, Liu X, Tang Y, He X, Wu B, Zhou Z, Liu Y, Li J, Zhang Y, Zhou X, Xu L, Chen L, Bi G, Hu X, Xu F, Wang L. Processing of visually evoked innate fear by a non-canonical thalamic pathway. Nature Communications. 2015;6:6756. doi: 10.1038/ncomms7756. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Yeh L-F, Ozawa T, Johansen JP. Functional organization of the midbrain periaqueductal gray for regulating aversive memory formation. Molecular Brain. 2021;14:136. doi: 10.1186/s13041-021-00844-0. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Zambetti PR, Schuessler BP, Lecamp BE, Shin A, Kim EJ, Kim JJ. Ecological analysis of pavlovian fear conditioning in rats. Communications Biology. 2022;5:830. doi: 10.1038/s42003-022-03802-1. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Zhou Z, Liu X, Chen S, Zhang Z, Liu Y, Montardy Q, Tang Y, Wei P, Liu N, Li L, Song R, Lai J, He X, Chen C, Bi G, Feng G, Xu F, Wang L. A VTA GABAergic neural circuit mediates visually evoked innate defensive responses. Neuron. 2019;103:473–488. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.05.027. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

eLife assessment

Kate M Wassum 1

This study presents valuable findings describing how the midbrain periaqueductal gray matter and basolateral amygdala communicate when a predator threat is detected. Though the periaqueductal gray is usually viewed as a downstream effector, this work contributes to a growing body of literature from this lab showing that the periaqueductal gray produces effects by acting on the basolateral amygdala, the experimental design, data collection, and analysis methods provide solid evidence for the main claims. The anatomical and immediately early gene evidence that the paraventricular nucleus of the thalamus may serve as a mediator of dorsolateral periaqueductal gray to basolateral amygdala neurotransmission provides an impetus for future functional assessment of this possibility. This study will appeal to a broad audience, including basic scientists interested in neural circuits, basic and clinical researchers interested in fear, and behavioral ecologists interested in foraging.

Reviewer #1 (Public Review):

Anonymous

In the presence of predators, animals display attenuated foraging responses and increased defensive behaviors that serve to protect them from potential predatory attacks. Previous studies have shown that the basolateral nucleus of the amygdala (BLA) and the periaqueductal gray matter (PAG) are necessary for the acquisition and expression of conditioned fear responses. However, it remains unclear how BLA and PAG neurons respond to predatory threats when animals are foraging for food. To address this question, Kim and colleagues conducted in vivo electrophysiological recordings from BLA and PAG neurons and assessed approach-avoidance responses while rats searched for food in the presence of a robotic predator.

The authors observed that rats exhibited a significant increase in the latency to obtain the food pellets and a reduction in the pellet success rate when the predator robot was activated. A subpopulation of PAG neurons showing an increased firing rate in response to the robot activation didn't change their activity in response to food pellet retrieval during the pre- or post-robot sessions. Optogenetic stimulation of PAG neurons increased the latency to procure the food pellet in a frequency- and intensity-dependent manner, similar to what was observed during the robot test. Combining optogenetics with single-unit recordings, the authors demonstrated that photoactivation of PAG neurons increased the firing rate of 10% of BLA cells. A subsequent behavioral test in 3 of these same rats demonstrated that BLA neurons responsive to PAG stimulation displayed higher firing rates to the robot than BLA neurons nonresponsive to PAG stimulation. Next, because the PAG does not project monosynaptically to the BLA, the authors used a combination of retrograde and anterograde neural tracing to identify possible regions that could convey robot-related information from PAG to the BLA. They observed that neurons in specific areas of the paraventricular nucleus of the thalamus (PVT) that are innervated by PAG fibers contained neurons that were retrogradely labeled by the injection of CTB in the BLA. In addition, PVT neurons showed increased expression of the neural activity marker cFos after the robot test, suggesting that PVT may be a mediator of PAG signals to the BLA.

Overall, the idea that the PAG interacts with the BLA via the midline thalamus during a predator vs. foraging test is new and quite interesting. The authors have used appropriate tools to address their questions. However, there are some major concerns regarding the design of the experiments, the rigor of the histological analyses, the presentation of the results, the interpretation of the findings, and the general discussion that largely reduces the relevance of this study.

The authors have fully addressed all my concerns.

Reviewer #2 (Public Review):

Anonymous

The authors characterized the activity of the dorsal periaqueductal gray (dPAG) - basolateral amygdala (BLA) circuit. They show that BLA cells that are activated by dPAG stimulation are also more likely to be activated by a robot predator. These same cells are also more likely to display synchronous firing.

The authors also replicate prior results showing that dPAG stimulation evokes fear and the dPAG is activated by a predator.

Lastly, the report performs anatomical tracing to show that the dPAG may act on the BLA via the paraventricular thalamus (PVT). Indeed, the PVT receives dPAG projections and also projects to the BLA. However, the authors do not show if the PVT mediates dPAG to BLA communication with any functional behavioral assay.

The major impact in the field would be to add evidence to their prior work, strengthening the view that the BLA can be downstream of the dPAG.

Reviewer #3 (Public Review):

Anonymous

In the present study, the authors examined how dPAG neurons respond to predatory threats and how dPAG and BLA communicate threat signals. The authors employed single-unit recording and optogenetics tools to address these issues in an 'approach food-avoid predator' paradigm. They characterized dPAG and BLA neurons responsive to a looming robot predator and found that dPAG opto-stimulation elicited fleeing and increased BLA activity. Importantly, they found that dPAG stimulation produces activity changes in subpopulations of BLA neurons related to predator detection, thus supporting the idea that dPAG conveys innate fear signals to the amygdala. In addition, injections of anterograde and retrograde tracers into the dPAG and BLA, respectively, along with the examination of c-FOS activity in midline thalamic relay stations, suggest that the paraventricular nucleus of the thalamus (PVT) may serve as a mediator of dPAG to BLA neurotransmission. Of relevance, the study helps to validate an important concept that dPAG mediates primal fear emotion and may engage upstream amygdala targets to evoke defensive responses. The series of experiments provides a compelling case for supporting their conclusions. The study brings important concepts revealing dynamics of fear-related circuits particularly attractive to a broad audience, from basic scientists interested in neural circuits to psychiatrists.

eLife. 2024 Aug 12;12:RP88733. doi: 10.7554/eLife.88733.4.sa4

Author response

Eun Joo Kim 1, Mi-Seon Kong 2, Sanggeon Park 3, Jeiwon Cho 4, Jeansok John Kim 5

The following is the authors’ response to the previous reviews.

Public Reviews

Reviewer 1 summarized that: In this revised version of the manuscript, the authors have made important modifications in the text, inserted new data analyses, and incorporated additional references, as recommended by the reviewers. These modifications have significantly improved the quality of the manuscript.

We are grateful for the reviewer's positive recognition of our revisions.

Reviewer 2 noted that:

(1) The authors do not show if the PVT mediates dPAG to BLA communication with any functional behavioral assay.

We appreciate the reviewer’s suggestion to include a functional assay to investigate the role of the PVT in mediating communication between the dPAG and BLA. Our primary objective was to confirm the upstream role of the dPAG in processing and relaying naturalistic predatory threat information to the BLA, thereby broadening our current understanding of the dPAG-BLA relationship based on Pavlovian fear conditioning paradigms.

Given previous anatomical findings indicating the absence of direct monosynaptic projections from the dPAG to the BLA (Cameron et al. 1995, McNally, Johansen, and Blair 2011, Vianna and Brandao 2003), we employed both anterograde and retrograde tracers, supplemented by c-Fos expression analysis following predatory threats, to explore possible routes through which threat signals may be conveyed from the dPAG to the BLA. Our findings indicated significant activity within the midline thalamic regions, particularly the PVT as a mediator of dPAG-BLA interactions, corroborating the possibility of dPAGàBLA information flow.

Investigating the PVT's functional role appropriately would require single-unit recordings, correlation analysis of PVT neuronal responses with dPAG and BLA neuronal responses, and pathway-specific causal techniques, involving other midline thalamic regions for controls. This comprehensive study would represent an independent study.

In response to previous feedback, we have carefully revised our manuscript to moderate the emphasis on the PVT's role. Both the Abstract, Results, and Discussion refer more broadly to "midline thalamic regions" and “The midline thalamus” (subheading) rather than specifically to the PVT. In the Introduction, we mention that the PVT "may be part of a network that conveys predatory threat information from the dPAG to the BLA." Our conclusions about the functional interaction between the dPAG and BLA, which broaden the view of Pavlovian fear conditioning, are not contingent on confirming a specific intermediary role for the PVT.

(2) The author also do not thoroughly characterize the activity of BLA cells during the predatory assay.

Our previous studies have extensively detailed BLA cell firing characteristics, including their responsiveness to food and/or a robot predator during the predatory assay (Kim et al. 2018, Kong et al. 2021), and compared these findings to other predator studies (Amir et al. 2019, Amir et al. 2015). In the current study, out of 85 BLA cells, 3 were food-specific and 4 responded to both the pellet and the robot, with none of these 7 cells responding to dPAG stimulation.

Given our earlier findings of the immediate responses of BLA neurons to robot activation, we specifically examined whether robot-responsive BLA neurons receive signals from the dPAG. For this analysis, we excluded all food-related cells (pellet cells and BOTH cells) and focused on the time window immediately after robot activation (within 500 ms after robot onset). This approach enabled us to avoid potential confounds from residual effects of robot-induced immediate BLA responses during the animals’ flight and nest entry behaviors.

Furthermore, as previously described, the robot is programmed to move forward a fixed distance and then return, repeatedly triggering foraging behavior. This setup facilitates the analysis of neural changes during food approach and predator avoidance conflicts. However, animals quickly adapt to the robot, reducing freezing and stretch-attend behaviors, making time-stamped analysis of these behaviors unfeasible.

We would like to highlight that the present study explicitly focused on demonstrating whether BLA neurons that responded to intrinsic dPAG optogenetic stimulation also responded to extrinsic predatory robot activation, and compared their firing characteristics to those BLA neurons that did not respond to dPAG stimulation (Figure 3). This targeted analysis provides insights into the responsiveness of BLA neurons to both intrinsic and extrinsic stimuli, furthering our understanding of the dPAG-BLA interaction in the context of predatory threats.

Reviewer 3 also raised no concerns and stated that: The series of experiments provide a compelling case for supporting their conclusions. The study brings important concepts revealing dynamics of fear-related circuits particularly attractive to a broad audience, from basic scientists interested in neural circuits to psychiatrists.

We sincerely thank the reviewer for the positive feedback on our revisions.

Recommendations for the Authors

Reviewer 1: There are a few minor concerns that the authors may want to fix:

(1) Point (5) The sentence: "The complexity of targeting the dPAG, which includes its dorsomedial, dorsolateral, lateral, and ventrolateral subdivisions" is hard to follow because the ventrolateral subdivision is not part of the dPAG. The authors may want to say specific subregions of the PAG instead. It is also unclear why transgenic animals would be needed for this projection-defined manipulations. The combination of retrograde Cre-recombinase virus with inhibitory opsin or chemogenetic approach may be sufficient.

We appreciate the reviewer’s insightful feedback regarding our description of the dPAG and the use of transgenic mice in future studies. As suggested, we have corrected the manuscript to exclude the 'ventrolateral' subdivision from the dPAG description, now accurately aligning with pioneering studies (Bandler, Carrive, and Zhang 1991, Bandler and Keay 1996, Carrive 1993) that designated dPAG as including the dorsomedial (dmPAG), dorsolateral (dlPAG) and lateral (lPAG) regions, as cited in our revised manuscript.

We acknowledge the reviewer’s helpful suggestion regarding the use of retrograde Cre-recombinase virus with inhibitory opsins or chemogenetic approaches as viable alternatives. These methods have been incorporated into our discussion (pages 14-15): “While our findings demonstrate that opto-stimulation of the dPAG is sufficient to trigger both fleeing behavior and increased BLA activity, we have not established that the dPAG-PVT circuit is necessary for the BLA’s response to predatory threats. To establish causality and interregional relationships, future studies should employ methods such as pathway-specific optogenetic inhibition (using retrograde Cre-recombinase virus with inhibitory opsins; Lavoie and Liu 2020, Li et al. 2016, Senn et al. 2014) or chemogenetics (Boender et al. 2014, Roth 2016) in conjunction with single unit recordings to fully characterize the dPAG-PVT-BLA circuitry’s (as opposed to other midline thalamic regions for controls) role in processing predatory threat-induced escape behavior. If inactivating the dPAG-PVT circuits reduces the BLA's response to threats, this would highlight the central role of the dPAG-PVT pathway in this defense mechanism. Conversely, if the BLA's response remains unchanged despite dPAG-PVT inactivation, it could suggest the existence of multiple pathways for antipredatory defenses.”

This revision addresses the critique by clarifying the anatomical description of the dPAG and emphasizing the feasibility of using targeted viral approaches without the necessity for transgenic animals.

(2) Point (6e) The authors mentioned that "pellet retrieval" was indicated by the animal entering a designated zone 19 cm from the pellet, driven by hunger. Entering the area 19cm of distance should be labeled as food approaching rather then food retrieval because in many occasions the animals may be some seconds away of grabbing the pellet.

We agree and incorporate the change (pg. 22).

(3) Point (11) We would strongly recommend the authors to replace the terminology "looming" by "approaching" to avoid confusion with several previous studies looking at defensive behaviors in responses to looming induced by the shadow of an object moving closer to the eyes.

Done.

(4) Point (17) The authors mentioned that "A total of three rats were utilized for the robot testing experiments depicted in Fig. 2 G-J." However, the figure indicates a total of 9 ChR2 and 4 controls.

We apologize for the confusion in our previous author responses. To examine the optical stimulation effects on behavior in Fig. 2G-J, we used a total of 9 ChR2 and 4 EYFP rats. The experimental sequence is detailed in the previously revised manuscript (pg. 20): “For optical stimulation and behavioral experiments, the procedure included 3 baseline trials with the pellet placed 75 cm away, followed by 3 dPAG stimulation trials with the pellet locations sequentially set at 75 cm, 50 cm, and 25 cm. During each approach to the pellet, rats received 473-nm light stimulation (1-2 s, 20-Hz, 10-ms width, 1-3 mW) through a laser (Opto Engine LLC) and a pulse generator (Master-8; A.M.P.I.). Additional testing to examine the functional response curves was conducted over multiple days, with incremental adjustments to the stimulation parameters (intensity, frequency, duration) after confirming that normal baseline foraging behavior was maintained. For these tests, one parameter was adjusted incrementally while the others were held constant (intensity curve at 20 Hz, 2 s; frequency curve at 3 mW, 2 s; duration curve at 20 Hz, 3 mW). If the rat failed to procure the pellet within 3 min, the gate was closed, and the trial was concluded.”

This clarification ensures that the actual number of animals used is accurately reflected and aligns with the figure data, addressing the reviewer's concern.

Reviewer 2: The authors made important changes in the text to address study limitations, including citations requested by the Reviewers and additional discussions about how this work fits into the existing literature. These changes have strengthened the manuscript.

(1) However, the authors did not perform new experiments to address any of the issues raised in the previous round of reviews. For example, they did not make optogenetic manipulations of the pathway including the PVT, and did not add any loss of function experiments. The justification that these experiments are better suited for future reports using mice is not convincing, because hundreds of papers performing these types of circuit dissection assays have been performed in rats.

We appreciate the reviewer's comments regarding the experimental scope of our study. Our study’s primary objective was to explore the dPAG’s upstream functional role in processing and conveying naturalistic predatory threat information to the BLA, extending our current understanding of the dPAG-BLA relationship based on Pavlovian fear conditioning paradigms. We believe that our findings effectively address this goal.

Our use of anterograde and retrograde tracers, supplemented by c-Fos expression analysis in response to predatory threats, was primarily conducted to verify the possibility of the dPAGàBLA information flow during predator encounters. This involved exploring potential routes through which threat signals might be conveyed from the dPAG to the BLA, given the lack of direct monosynaptic projections from the dPAG to BLA neurons (Cameron et al. 1995, McNally, Johansen, and Blair 2011, Vianna and Brandao 2003). This methodology helped us identify a potential structure, PVT, for more in-depth future studies. A thorough examination of the PVT's role would require single-unit recordings and causal techniques, incorporating other midline thalamic regions as controls, representing a significant and separate study on its own.

In response to prior feedback, we have carefully revised our manuscript to generally address the role of "midline thalamic regions" rather than focusing specifically on the PVT. We wish to emphasize that our findings, which illustrate unique functional interactions between the dPAG and BLA in response to a predatory imminence, remain compelling and informative even without definitive evidence of the PVT’s involvement.

Reviewer 3: In the revised version of the manuscript, the authors addressed adequately all the concerns raised by the reviewers.

We thank the reviewer for the thoughtful feedback on the earlier version of our manuscript and for reexamining the revisions we have made.

References

Amir, A., P. Kyriazi, S. C. Lee, D. B. Headley, and D. Pare. 2019. "Basolateral amygdala neurons are activated during threat expectation." J Neurophysiol 121 (5):1761-1777.

Amir, A., S. C. Lee, D. B. Headley, M. M. Herzallah, and D. Pare. 2015. "Amygdala Signaling during Foraging in a Hazardous Environment." J Neurosci 35 (38):12994-3005.

Bandler, R., P. Carrive, and S. P. Zhang. 1991. "Integration of somatic and autonomic reactions within the midbrain periaqueductal grey: viscerotopic, somatotopic and functional organization." Prog Brain Res 87:269-305.

Bandler, R., and K. A. Keay. 1996. "Columnar organization in the midbrain periaqueductal gray and the integration of emotional expression." Prog Brain Res 107:285-300.

Boender, A. J., J. W. de Jong, L. Boekhoudt, M. C. Luijendijk, G. van der Plasse, and R. A. Adan. 2014. "Combined use of the canine adenovirus-2 and DREADD-technology to activate specific neural pathways in vivo." PLoS One 9 (4):e95392.

Cameron, A. A., I. A. Khan, K. N. Westlund, and W. D. Willis. 1995. "The efferent projections of the periaqueductal gray in the rat: a Phaseolus vulgaris-leucoagglutinin study. II. Descending projections." J Comp Neurol 351 (4):585-601.

Carrive, P. 1993. "The periaqueductal gray and defensive behavior: functional representation and neuronal organization." Behav Brain Res 58 (1-2):27-47.

Kim, E. J., M. S. Kong, S. G. Park, S. J. Y. Mizumori, J. Cho, and J. J. Kim. 2018. "Dynamic coding of predatory information between the prelimbic cortex and lateral amygdala in foraging rats." Sci Adv 4 (4):eaar7328.

Kong, M. S., E. J. Kim, S. Park, L. S. Zweifel, Y. Huh, J. Cho, and J. J. Kim. 2021. "'Fearful-place' coding in the amygdala-hippocampal network." Elife 10.

Lavoie, A., and B. H. Liu. 2020. "Canine Adenovirus 2: A Natural Choice for Brain Circuit Dissection." Front Mol Neurosci 13:9.

Li, Y., L. Hickey, R. Perrins, E. Werlen, A. A. Patel, S. Hirschberg, M. W. Jones, S. Salinas, E. J. Kremer, and A. E. Pickering. 2016. "Retrograde optogenetic characterization of the pontospinal module of the locus coeruleus with a canine adenoviral vector." Brain Res 1641 (Pt B):274-90.

McNally, G. P., J. P. Johansen, and H. T. Blair. 2011. "Placing prediction into the fear circuit." Trends Neurosci 34 (6):283-92.

Roth, B. L. 2016. "DREADDs for Neuroscientists." Neuron 89 (4):683-94.

Senn, V., S. B. Wolff, C. Herry, F. Grenier, I. Ehrlich, J. Grundemann, J. P. Fadok, C. Muller, J. J. Letzkus, and A. Luthi. 2014. "Long-range connectivity defines behavioral specificity of amygdala neurons." Neuron 81 (2):428-37.

Vianna, D. M., and M. L. Brandao. 2003. "Anatomical connections of the periaqueductal gray: specific neural substrates for different kinds of fear." Braz J Med Biol Res 36 (5):557-66.

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Data Citations

    1. Kim EJ, Kong MS, Park S, Cho J, Kim JJ. 2024. Periaqueductal gray activates antipredatory neural responses in the amygdala of foraging rats. Dryad Digital Repository. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

    Supplementary Materials

    Supplementary file 1. Reconstructions of histology and normality test results.

    (A) Histological reconstructions of recording sites in the dPAG and BLA, and optic fiber locations in the dPAG. (A) Red bars show the trajectory of tetrode recording sites in the dPAG. (B) Red bars depict the trajectory of optrode recording sites in the dPAG. (B) Red and gray circles represent the optic fiber locations for ChR2 and EYFP rats, respectively. (D) Orange circles and red bars indicate the optic fiber locations in the dPAG (left) and recording trajectories in the BLA (right), respectively. Numerical values represent AP coordinates relative to Bregma. (B) Normality test. The normality of the variable distributions was assessed using the Kolmogorov–Smirnov test (p<0.01). Depending on the results of this test, parametric tests were used for normally distributed variables, while nonparametric tests were employed for variables that were not normally distributed.

    elife-88733-supp1.docx (132.2KB, docx)
    MDAR checklist

    Data Availability Statement

    The data that support the findings of this study is available from the Dryad data repository at https://doi.org/10.5061/dryad.stqjq2cbh. The customized analysis tools are deposited on GitHub: https://github.com/KimLab-UW/Velocity (copy archived at Park, 2024).

    The following dataset was generated:

    Kim EJ, Kong MS, Park S, Cho J, Kim JJ. 2024. Periaqueductal gray activates antipredatory neural responses in the amygdala of foraging rats. Dryad Digital Repository.


    Articles from eLife are provided here courtesy of eLife Sciences Publications, Ltd

    RESOURCES