Abstract
Background
Sleep, or a lack thereof, strongly relates to mood dysregulation. Although considerable research uses symptom scales to examine this relation, few studies use longitudinal, real-time methods focused on pediatric irritability. This study leveraged an ecological momentary assessment (EMA) protocol, assessing bidirectional associations between momentary irritability symptoms and daily sleep duration in a transdiagnostic pediatric sample enriched for irritability.
Methods
A total of N=125 youth (Mage=12.58 years, SD=2.56 years; 74% male; 68.8% White) completed digital, in-vivo surveys three times a day for seven days. For a subset of youth, their parents also completed the EMA protocol. Trait irritability was measured using youth-, parent-, and clinician-report to test its potential moderating effect on the association between sleep duration and momentary irritability.
Results
Results from multilevel modelling dynamically linked sleep to irritability. Specifically, according to youth- and parent-report, decreased sleep duration was associated with increased morning irritability (bs≤ −.09, ps< .049). A bidirectional association between parent-reported nightly sleep duration and anger was found − increased evening anger related to decreased nightly sleep duration, and decreased sleep duration related to increased morning anger (bs≤ −.17, ps< .019). Trait irritability moderated this association, which was stronger for more irritable youth (b= −.03, p<.027).
Conclusions
This study adds to the literature and suggests sleep-irritability dynamics as a potential treatment target.
Keywords: Irritability, sleep, temporal links, anger, ecological momentary assessment
Introduction
Adequate sleep supports optimal psychological functioning in youth (Goldstein & Walker, 2014; Wheaton & Claussen, 2021). Yet, many youth fail to obtain sufficient sleep (Crowley, Wolfson, Tarokh, & Carskadon, 2018; Wheaton & Claussen, 2021). Sleep impacts emotion regulation and affective processes, including negative mood and anger (Baum et al., 2014; Konjarski, Murray, Lee, & Jackson, 2018; Kouros, Keller, Martín-Piñón, & El-Sheikh, 2022; Palmer & Alfano, 2017; Short, Booth, Omar, Ostlundh, & Arora, 2020; Short & Louca, 2015; Tomaso, Johnson, & Nelson, 2021). Inadequate sleep duration relates specifically to mood dysregulation (Asarnow & Mirchandaney, 2021; Short et al., 2020) and a range of psychiatric diagnoses (Craig, Weiss, Hudec, & Gibbins, 2020; Gregory & Sadeh, 2012). However, at this time, much of the research on youth focuses on unidirectional, retrospective relations between sleep and mood dysregulation (Asarnow & Mirchandaney, 2021; Short et al., 2020). Furthermore, there is a lack of work investigating reciprocal, prospective associations between sleep and pediatric irritability, a phenotype of mood dysregulation.
Irritability, an increased proneness to anger relative to peers, is a common transdiagnostic symptom (Brotman, Kircanski, Stringaris, Pine, & Leibenluft, 2017; Evans, Corteselli, Edelman, Scott, & Weisz, 2022). Children and adolescents with high levels of irritability tend to be easily angered and annoyed, have decreased thresholds for frustration, and exhibit grouchy mood. Understanding the role of sleep in irritability has clinical implications, as it is an easily targeted and modifiable behavior (Short et al., 2020; Whiting, Bellaert, Deveney, & Tseng, 2023). Limited previous work has studied sleep problems (e.g., insufficient duration, daytime sleepiness, delayed onset) and irritability in typically developing youth (Baum et al., 2014; Beebe et al., 2008; Rubens, Evans, Becker, Fite, & Tountas, 2017; Tamura, Komada, Inoue, & Tanaka, 2022) or in the context of one specific clinical diagnosis (e.g., anxiety disorders or depression or attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder [ADHD] or autism spectrum disorder; Loram et al., 2021; Mazurek & Sohl, 2016; Mulraney et al., 2017; Poznanski et al., 2018). Two studies have investigated sleep behaviors in the context of youth with disruptive mood dysregulation disorder (DMDD; Delaplace et al., 2018; Estrada-Prat et al., 2017), a diagnosis characterized by severe and chronic irritability. Results indicated fragmented sleep (i.e., nighttime awakenings, increased motor activity) in this clinical sample (Delaplace et al., 2018; Estrada-Prat et al., 2017). Few of the studies that investigated sleep and irritability implemented a validated irritability-specific measure (Loram et al., 2021; Rubens et al., 2017; Whiting et al., 2023). Additionally, much of the prior research on sleep and irritability used retrospective methods, while including one specific clinical group (Loram et al., 2021; Mazurek & Sohl, 2016; Mulraney et al., 2017; Poznanski et al., 2018), representing a critical gap in the literature.
Hence, in the current study we used ecological momentary assessment (EMA) to investigate relations between sleep duration and irritability that are measured in real-time, in-vivo, and prospectively in a transdiagnostic pediatric clinical sample. Variables were measured multiple times over a week, allowing us to test the reciprocity and directionality of daily sleep and momentary irritability associations. First, we hypothesized daily sleep duration and momentary irritability symptoms would manifest a bidirectional association. Specifically, we hypothesized that decreased nightly sleep duration would predict increased morning irritability, and vice versa, that increased evening irritability would predict decreased nightly sleep duration. Second, we hypothesized that these real-time associations would manifest more strongly among youth with higher levels of trait irritability.
Methods
Participants
Data were collected from a transdiagnostic sample of 125 youth 8–18 years old with varying levels of irritability (Mage=12.58 years, SD=2.56 years, 74% male, 68.8% White, 9.6% Black, 83.2% Not Hispanic/Latino; see Table 1 for additional demographic data). Recruitment posters was delivered via mail or online platforms. Participants had a primary diagnosis of either DMDD (n=37), ADHD (n=33), anxiety disorders (ANX; n=28), or were categorized as typically developing youth (TD; n=27). Diagnoses were determined based on a semi-structured clinical interview (Kiddie Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia [K-SADs], Kaufman et al., 1997). Exclusion criteria broadly was an IQ <70 assessed with the Wechsler Abbreviated Intelligence Scale (Wechsler, 1999) and substance use during the past three months (See Naim et al., 2021b for all inclusion/exclusion criteria). Of note, the current sample overlaps with previously published data (Naim et al., 2021b). However, this study includes an additional 16 participants and the questions on sleep duration as a potential factor associated with momentary irritability symptoms were not examined before.
Table 1.
Demographic information for the sample.
N=125 | M (SD) or % (n) |
---|---|
Demographics | |
Age | 12.58 (2.56) |
Sex (male) | 74.4% (93) |
IQ* | 113.29 (12.77) |
Race | |
Black | 9.6% (12) |
White | 68.8% (86) |
Asian or Asian American | 3.2% (4) |
American Indian or Alaskan Native | 2.4% (3) |
Multiple Races | 12.0% (15) |
Not reported | 4.0% (5) |
Ethnicity | |
Not Hispanic or Latino | 83.2% (104) |
Latino or Hispanic | 11.2% (14) |
Not reported | 5.6% (7) |
Symptom Measures | |
Youth-reported ARI | 3.44 (3.30) |
Parent-reported ARI | 3.93 (3.75) |
CL-ARI | 27.98 (18.57) |
Note: Affective Reactivity Index=ARI; CL-ARI = Clinician Affective Reactivity Index
IQ data are missing for 20 participants
CL-ARI data are missing for 58 participants
Ethical Considerations
Informed assent and consent were obtained from all youth participants and their parents. This study was approved by the National Institute of Mental Health institutional review board.
Procedure
Using a previously validated EMA protocol (Naim et al., 2021b), youth completed surveys three times a day (morning, afternoon, evening) for seven days (21 total surveys). Youth were asked about their wake and bedtimes in the morning and momentary irritability symptoms during each time window. Parents of youth with DMDD and ADHD also completed this EMA protocol, providing information about their child’s wake and bedtimes, and daily irritability symptoms. Youth’s trait irritability was assessed within two to three days upon completing EMA, using validated metrics obtained from several informants (self-, parent-, and clinician-report; see below for more information), capturing multiple perspectives and mitigating self-report biases (Kircanski et al., 2018; Smith, 2007). Compensation was provided with a bonus for completing 75% or more of the surveys.
Measures
EMA Ratings for Sleep Duration
Each morning survey included the following items: “Last night, I fell asleep at/This morning, I woke up at” or “Last night, my child fell asleep at/This morning, my child woke up at.” Participants selected a time from a dropdown menu, rounding to the nearest hour and selecting whether it was “AM” or “PM”. Daily sleep duration was calculated as the difference between each day’s reported bed and wake times. To control for outliers, we only included sleep duration times that fell within three standard deviations of the sample’s mean (less than 2% of reported timepoints were removed from the dataset).
EMA Ratings for Momentary Irritability Symptoms
Momentary irritability symptoms were assessed through four items including questions on anger, frustration, grouchiness, and mood change (Naim et al., 2021b; Naim et al., 2022; Tseng et al., 2023). All items were assessed using 5-point Likert scales. Participants were presented with the following items: anger: “At the time of the beep, I felt annoyed or angry” (1=not at all; 5=extremely), frustration: “Since the last beep, I felt frustrated” (1=not at all; 5=extremely), grouchiness: “Since the last beep, aside from being really, really angry and out of control, I was feeling generally grouchy or cranky” (1=none of the time, 5=the whole time), and mood change: “Since the last beep, my mood changed a lot” (1=not at all; 5=extremely; Naim et al., 2021b).
Self- and Parent-Reported Trait Irritability
The Affective Reactivity Index (ARI) was obtained from each parent-youth dyad (Stringaris et al., 2012). The ARI is a well validated six-item scale that assesses youth trait irritability based on the prior six months. Respondents report irritability symptoms based on 3-point Likert scales (0–2), with higher total scores indicating higher levels of irritability (Range: 0–12). The ARI has well established reliability and construct validity (Stringaris et al., 2012).
Clinician-Reported Trait Irritability
The Clinician Affective Reactivity Index (CL-ARI) is a 12-item semi-structured interview that assesses trait irritability in several domains (home, school, with peers; Haller et al., 2020). Each item is scored on four-to-six-point Likert scales and combined to create three subscales: temper outbursts, irritable mood, impairment. The frequency of temper outbursts is scored on a 5-point Likert scale (0= “none,” 4= “more than one outburst everyday”) and the duration on a 6-point Likert scale (0= “none,” 5= “60 minutes”). The frequency of irritable mood is scored on a 4-point Likert scale (0= “none,” 3= “four or more days”) and the severity is scored on a 6-point Likert scale (0= “not present,” 5= “severe”). The duration item for irritable mood is only completed if the frequency is reported as “four or more days” and is a binary (yes/no) outcome, probing whether irritable mood was present for at least half of the day. Lastly, the impairment subscale assesses irritability in three different domains (home, school, with peers) using 6-point Likert scales (0= “none,” 5= “severe”). The measure’s three subscales are weighted to create a total score (range: 0–100). The CL-ARI has established psychometric properties, demonstrating good test-retest reliability and convergent validity (Haller et al., 2020).
Data Analysis
We conducted multilevel modeling (MLM; Goldstein, 1995) using HLM software (Version 8; Raudenbush et al., 2019) to examine daily in-vivo associations between sleep duration and momentary irritability symptoms. Within-person continuous EMA variables of irritability symptoms (morning and evening) and nightly sleep duration for each of the seven EMA weekdays were included at level 1, person-centered. To model trait irritability and to account for age differences, ARI scores and age were entered as between-person variables at level 2, grand-mean centered. Age was included in all models. ARI scores were included in three separate models for each informant (see model below for details). Evening irritability symptoms (T-1) were cross-day lagged to analyze with that night’s reported sleep duration (T). Missing data were removed via HLM using listwise deletion at Level 1, meaning removed per each analysis, maximizing available data points for each participant across the models. Of note, compliance rates were relatively high with 80.2% for youth-report and 84.4% for parent-report, consistent with previous EMA studies (Heron, Everhart, McHale, & Smyth, 2017; Wen, Schneider, Stone, & Spruijt-Metz, 2017; Wrzus & Neubauer, 2023). A Missing Completely at Random (Little’s MCAR; Little, 1988) test was conducted including all EMA items of interest showing no significant effect, indicating that the missing data were observed at random.
For our primary question on whether nightly sleep duration would predict morning irritability symptoms, models included each of the irritability items measured in the morning (i.e., anger, frustration, grouchiness, mood change) as the outcome, and nightly sleep duration as the predictor (see below for a sample equation of sleep duration predicting morning anger). Level 1 continuous variables were standardized to enable coefficients comparison (Snijders & Bosker, 2012).
Level-1 Model
Level-2 Model
denotes the association between sleep duration at the night prior and the morning anger in following day at prompt , for participant . At level 2, denotes the association between these variables across the age range.
For the reverse question on evening irritability predicting nightly sleep duration, MLMs were similar, but nightly sleep duration was entered as the outcome variable and evening irritability symptoms were entered as the predictors (see below for a sample equation of evening anger predicting sleep duration).
Level-1 Model
Level-2 Model
denotes the association between evening anger for participant , at prompt (i.e., at the evening) and that following night’s sleep duration, for participant , as reported at prompt (i.e., the morning after). At level 2, denotes the association between these variables across the age range.
To examine the potential moderating effect of trait irritability, youth-, parent-, and clinician-ARI were each entered separately at level 2 and were grand centered. Youth-ARI was entered for youth-report models, parent-ARI was included for parent-report models, and clinician-ARI was run for both youth and parent-reported models.
Level-2 Model
Notably, models based on youth-report included the full sample. However, since parent-reported EMA data were collected only for DMDD and ADHD groups, models based on parent-report included a subsample of DMDD/ADHD participants. Findings are reported accordingly.
Supplementary Analyses
Our supplementary analyses included three additional models. First, we tested the specificity of our results by assessing whether nightly sleep duration predicted other negative (e.g., sadness) and positive (e.g., happiness) emotional states. More information on the analyses and findings is reported in the Supporting Information; findings overall indicated specificity for sleep-irritability associations (see Appendix S1). Second, in an exploratory manner, we tested bidirectional associations between sleep duration and daily lability of irritability symptoms (i.e., we used all three data points of daily EMA to calculate symptom variability). No significant findings emerged. More information on these models and findings are reported in the Supporting Information (see Appendix S1). Lastly, we examined the effect of sex and ethnicity on sleep duration, as well as the impact of sex and ethnicity on the associations between sleep duration and irritability (See Appendix S1). No significant findings emerged.
Results
Sample Characteristics
Sleep Duration
Average nightly sleep duration in the current sample was 8.74 hours (SD= 1.64) according to youth-report. Similar sleep duration was found according to parent-report in the DMDD/ADHD subsample (M= 9.22 hours; SD= 1.34). Overall, participants obtained adequate sleep as recommended for their respective age group (Paruthi et al., 2016; See Table 2). Sleep duration did not significantly differ between diagnostic groups for child-report (DMDD: M= 8.94, SD= 2.24; ADHD: M= 8.66, SD= 2.52; ANX: M= 8.56, SD= 2.02; TD: M= 8.58, SD= 2.81) or parent-report (DMDD: M= 9.40 SD = 1.59; ADHD: M= 9.02, SD = 1.86). Moderate parent-youth agreement was found for nightly sleep duration (ICC= 0.43; see Table 2 for all EMA variable statistics).
Table 2.
Average EMA-rated irritability and sleep duration for youth- and parent-report.
M (SD) | ||
---|---|---|
Youth-Report | Parent-Report | |
Sleep Duration | 8.74 (1.64) | 9.22 (1.34) |
Anger | 1.46 (.92) | 1.73 (1.06) |
Frustration | 1.68 (1.08) | 2.10 (1.21) |
Grouchiness | 1.50 (.85) | 1.92 (.99) |
Mood Change | 1.73 (1.07) | 1.83 (1.11) |
Trait Irritability
Average youth-reported ARI was 3.44 (SD= 3.30), with higher scores, as expected, in the DMDD group (M= 5.84, SD= 3.33) compared to the other diagnostic groups (ADHD: M= 3.00, SD= 2.81; ANX: M= 2.96, SD= 3.02; TD: M= 1.19, SD= 1.84). Notably, the ranges of youth-ARI scores across the different clinical diagnostic groups were similar (0–12 for DMDD; 0–11 for ADHD; 0–11 for ANX), speaking to the transdiagnostic characteristic of irritability. Parent-reported ARI for the DMDD/ADHD subsample was 3.93 (SD= 3.75). Parent- and youth-reported ARI agreement was ICC= 0.55.
Nightly Sleep Duration Predicting Waking Irritability
Decreased nightly sleep duration predicted higher morning grouchiness and frustration, as reported by youth across the entire sample (Grouchiness b=−.12, SE=.05, p=.018; Frustration b=−.09, SE=.05, p=.049) and parents for the DMDD/ADHD subsample (Grouchiness b=−.10, SE=.05, p=.044; Frustration b=−.16, SE=.06, p=.005; see Table 3 and 4 for all results). Decreased nightly sleep duration predicted higher morning anger and an increased mood change only by parent report for the DMDD/ADHD subsample (Anger b=−.26, SE=.05, p<.001; Mood Change b=−.21, SE=.05, p<.001).
Table 3.
Youth-reported nightly sleep duration predicting morning irritability symptoms and with youth-report ARI and Cl-ARI as moderators.
Outcome: | β (SE) | ||
---|---|---|---|
Full sample | Youth-reported ARI | CL-ARI | |
Anger | −.06 (.05) | −.00 (.01) | .01 (.00) |
Frustration | −.09 (.05)* | .00 (.01) | −.00 (.00) |
Grouchiness | −.12 (.05)* | .01 (.01) | .00 (.00) |
Mood Change | −.05 (.05) | −.01 (.01) | −.00 (.00) |
Note: Affective Reactivity Index=ARI; Clinician Affective Reactivity Index= CL-ARI; Age was included in all models as a covariate
p<.05
Table 4.
Parent-reported nightly sleep duration predicting morning irritability symptoms and with parent-report ARI and Cl-ARI as moderators.
Outcome: | β (SE) | ||
---|---|---|---|
Full sample | Parent-reported ARI | CL-ARI | |
Anger | −.26 (.05)** | −.03 (.01)* | −.01 (.00)* |
Frustration | −.16 (.06)* | −.02 (.01) | −.00 (.00) |
Grouchiness | −.10 (.05)* | −.00 (.01) | −.00 (.00) |
Mood Change | −.21 (.05)** | −.01 (.01) | −.01 (.00) |
Note: Affective Reactivity Index=ARI; Clinician Affective Reactivity Index= CL-ARI; Age was included in all models as a covariate
p<.05
p<.001
As hypothesized, the association between parent-reported nightly sleep duration and anger was moderated by parent-reported ARI and CL-ARI scores, for the DMDD/ADHD subsample (Parent-report ARI b= −.03, SE=.01, p=.027; CL-ARI b=−.01, SE=.00, p=.014). Specifically, the impact of decreased sleep the night prior on morning anger was stronger in DMDD/ADHD youth with higher trait irritability. All other models were non-significant.
Evening Irritability Predicting Nightly Sleep Duration
Higher parent-reported evening anger predicted decreased nightly sleep duration for the DMDD/ADHD subsample (b=−.17, SE=.07, p=.019; see Table 5 for full results). No other significant findings emerged (see Appendix S2 Table S2 for full youth-report results).
Table 5.
Parent-reported evening irritability symptoms predicting nightly sleep duration for the full sample and with parent-reported ARI and Cl-ARI as moderators.
Outcome: | β (SE) | ||
---|---|---|---|
Full sample | Parent-reported ARI | CL-ARI | |
Anger | −.17 (.07)* | −.02 (.02) | −.01 (.00) |
Frustration | −.04 (.06) | −.01 (.02) | −.01 (.00) |
Grouchiness | −.03 (.06) | .01 (.02) | −.01 (.00) |
Mood Change | −.06 (.05) | −.01 (.01) | −.01 (.00) |
Note: Affective Reactivity Index=ARI; Clinician Affective Reactivity Index= CL-ARI; Age was included in all models as a covariate
p<.05
Age as a Moderator
The association between youth- and parent-reported decreased nightly sleep duration and increased morning grouchiness was moderated by age, showing significantly stronger associations in younger participants (Full sample youth-report model: b=.04, SE=.02, p=.025; DMDD/ADHD subsample parent-report model: b=.06, SE=.02, p=.004). In addition, for the DMDD/ADHD subsample, the association between decreased nightly sleep duration and increased morning mood change, as reported by parents, was moderated by age (Age b=.05, SE=.02, p=.039), indicting a stronger association within young participants.
Discussion
The current study examined associations between nightly sleep duration and daily irritability symptoms in a transdiagnostic pediatric sample. Three main findings arose. First, bidirectional associations were found, showing that decreased nightly sleep duration predicted increased morning irritability symptoms according to both youth and parents, and vice versa, evening anger predicted decreased nightly sleep duration, according to parent-report. Second, the parent-reported association between sleep and morning anger was moderated by trait irritability and, therefore, was stronger for youth with elevated levels of irritability. Third, the parent-reported associations between sleep and daily irritability symptoms of grouchiness and mood change were moderated by age and specifically stronger for younger participants.
Our findings demonstrating a negative association between nightly sleep duration and morning anger were robust and consistent across all informants (i.e., youth, parents). These results are in line with existing published data linking sleep and anger in both adults (Krizan, Miller, & Hisler, 2020; Zlatan Krizan & Hisler, 2019) and youth (Baum et al., 2014; Kenny, Dooley, & Fitzgerald, 2016; Short & Louca, 2015). Our findings add to this existing literature by indicating bidirectionality, with evening anger also impacting nightly sleep duration in our DMDD/ADHD subsample based on parent-report. This observed effect of anger on sleep could be explained by its impact on physiological (e.g., cardiac activity) and cognitive (e.g., rumination) arousal (Hisler & Krizan, 2017; Spielberger, Reheiser, & Sydeman, 1995). Increased pre-sleep arousal impacts sleep throughout the night, inhibiting an individual’s ability to fall asleep, as well as impacting sleep quality for the rest of the night (Hisler & Krizan, 2017; Pillai, Steenburg, Ciesla, Roth, & Drake, 2014). Future studies should incorporate objective measures of physiological arousal (e.g., electrocardiogram, electrodermal activity) and sleep (e.g., actigraphy) to investigate the interplay between anger, sleep, and increased arousal in youth with irritability.
Interestingly, our findings suggest specificity for sleep-irritability associations, as similar patterns did not emerge with other negative (e.g., sadness) or positive (e.g., happiness) mood symptoms (see Appendix S1 for full results), suggesting potential causality between daily sleep duration and momentary irritability symptoms in youth, that should be explored further.
Overall, findings were more statistically robust for the parent-report models based on the DMDD/ADHD subsample rather than for the youth-report models that were based on the full sample. One potential explanation could be higher irritability levels in the DMDD/ADHD subsample, diagnoses that are typically enriched with irritability, compared to the full sample that also included ANX and TD groups. Alternatively, this discrepancy could also be driven by youth reporting lower levels of irritability compared to their parents in this study (see Table 2), a common pattern found in previous studies on youth psychopathology (Kircanski et al., 2018; Mallidi et al., 2023; Stoddard et al., 2014). Future studies aiming to replicate current findings may help delineate whether this discrepancy is driven by informant effects or symptom severity by including EMA data for parents and youth across several clinical diagnoses.
Age emerged as a significant moderator showing that the impact of decreased sleep on morning irritability is stronger for younger participants. As emotion regulatory and general emotional processes develop from middle childhood throughout adolescence, this age effect is not surprising (Zeman, Cassano, Perry-Parrish, & Stegall, 2006). This observed age effect also informs a critical and practical target for intervention, specifically in terms of providing education to parents and clinicians on how healthy sleep, particularly sufficient duration, can decrease next-day irritability symptoms earlier relative to later in childhood. Future studies could experimentally restrict sleep duration in a systematic way to examine sleep deprivation’s impact on irritability in youth. Other demographic variables, specifically sex and ethnicity, did not emerge as significant predictors of sleep duration, nor as moderators for the irritability-sleep associations (but see: e.g., Moore et al., 2011; See Appendix S1 for full results). These null findings might reflect the homogeneity of our sample, which was majority male and not Hispanic or Latino (See Table 1). Future work should test our findings in a more diverse sample to see if these effects emerge.
Our study has several strengths including its prospective, ecologically valid EMA protocol that collected naturalistic, in-vivo sleep-wake data and momentary irritability symptoms over the course of a week. Second, our study includes a transdiagnostic sample of youth with various clinical diagnoses. Third, data were obtained from youth, parents, and clinicians, providing a broader insight into the temporal dynamics of sleep duration and irritability symptoms, as perceived by different informants.
From a treatment perspective, our findings suggest that insufficient sleep could be a potential treatment target to improve irritability in youth, particularly in those experiencing clinically impairing levels of irritability as seen in ADHD and DMDD. Findings also highlight the potential clinical importance of decreasing evening irritability to improve sleep duration in this clinical population. While clinical irritability is a common and impairing symptom in pediatric psychopathology, effective treatments are still under development and testing (e.g., cognitive behavioral therapy [CBT]; Naim et al., 2021a). CBT for insomnia (CBT-I) protocols are efficacious for treating sleep disturbances in youth and clinical samples, and could be added as a supplement to other treatment protocols for youth with irritability delivered in both in-person and virtual settings (Taylor & Pruiksma, 2014; Werner-Seidler, Johnston, & Christensen, 2018). Sleep education and mindfulness protocols can be implemented in the community setting to improve sleep-related health outcomes, including general mood and irritability in youth (e.g., Lever et al., 2021; Tamura & Tanaka, 2014; Wolfson et al., 2015).
There are also several limitations in the current study. First, sleep duration was based on subjective estimation and rounded to the nearest hour. While daily subjective estimates of sleep are commonly used in research (e.g., Bauer & Blunden, 2008; Dietch et al., 2019; Gregory et al., 2008; Wolfson et al., 2003) and show sufficient agreement with actigraphy, particularly for reported bed and wake times, future work would benefit from integrating objective and passive measures (e.g., actigraphy) to control for mood-congruent biases (e.g., “I am in a bad mood so I must have gotten poor sleep”) and to further validate estimates of sleep timing (Konjarski et al., 2018; Mazza et al., 2020; Sadeh & Acebo, 2002; Sadeh, 2011). There are additional sleep indices (e.g., sleep variability) that were beyond the scope of the current study, that future research should investigate in relation to irritability given their relevance to well-being and affect in youth (Mathew et al., 2023; Moore et al., 2011). Objective estimates of sleep would also allow researchers to investigate other sleep indices, such as sleep onset latency and waking after sleep onset. Considering the available work suggesting sleep fragmentation in youth with DMDD (Delaplace et al., 2018), assessing these additional sleep behaviors is an important next step. Second, as irritability is frequently associated with ADHD (Cardinale et al., 2021; Karalunas, Gustafsson, Fair, Musser, & Nigg, 2019; Liu et al., 2019) and, as sleep problems are common in ADHD (Craig et al., 2020; Dimakos, Gauthier-Gagné, Lin, Scholes, & Gruber, 2021), disentangling the effects of irritability on sleep from those of ADHD symptoms is of value. Since ADHD symptoms were not included in our current EMA protocol, future work should investigate these pathways by modeling both ADHD-related symptomatology (e.g., hyperactivity) and irritability when testing associations with sleep. Third, EMA parent-report was limited to the DMDD/ADHD subsample. While sleep-irritability associations were found across the full sample, these were stronger and bidirectional in the DMDD/ADHD subsample, suggesting that youth with elevated irritability are potentially more sensitive to the effect of sleep on daily irritability, and vice versa, perpetuating a cycle. Research would benefit from future work that collects EMA sleep-irritability data across different informants and clinical groups to further elucidate the current study’s findings.
Conclusions
Overall, this work adds to the literature on sleep-mood dynamics showing significant and bidirectional associations between momentary irritability and sleep duration while measured ecologically in-vivo. Findings highlight sleep duration as a potential treatment target in the context of clinical irritability. Future work could integrate EMA to develop and deliver data-driven and personalized interventions by which patients improve their sleep habits in-vivo and in real-time.
Supplementary Material
Appendix S1 Table S1. Demographic information by diagnostic group.
Appendix S2 Table S2. Youth-reported evening irritability symptoms predicting nightly sleep duration and with the youth-reported ARI and Cl-ARI as moderators.
Key Points.
Insufficient sleep strongly relates to mood dysregulation. However, real-time reciprocal sleep-irritability associations are yet to be explored.
Using ecological momentary assessment, we tested daily relationships between irritability and nightly sleep duration in a transdiagnostic pediatric sample.
According to youth- and parent-report, decreased sleep duration was associated with increased morning irritability. A bidirectional association between parent-reported nightly sleep duration and anger was found − increased evening anger related to decreased nightly sleep duration, and decreased sleep duration related to increased morning anger.
Age and trait irritability moderated the effect of sleep duration on momentary irritability symptoms, which was stronger for younger youth with higher trait irritability.
Findings highlight sleep duration as a potential treatment target for clinical irritability in youth.
Acknowledgements
This work was supported by the Intramural Research Program (IRP) of the National Institute of Mental Health, National Institutes of Health (NIMH/NIH), ZIAMH002781 (Pine), ZIAMH002786 (Leibenluft), ZIAMH002778 (Leibenluft), and conducted under NIH Clinical Study Protocols 01-M-0192 and M-00-M-0021 (ClinicalTrials.gov identifiers: NCT00018057 (Pine) and NCT00025935 (Brotman)). The authors have declared that they have no competing or potential conflicts of interest.
Abbreviations
- ANX
Anxiety Disorders
- ARI
Affective Reactivity Index
- CL-ARI
Clinician Affective Reactivity Index
- TD
Typically Developing
Footnotes
Conflict of interest statement: No conflicts declared.
Supporting information
Additional supporting information may be found online in the Supporting Information section at the end of the article:
References
- Asarnow LD, & Mirchandaney R (2021). Sleep and mood disorders among youth. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics, 30, 251–268. Elsevier. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Bauer KM, & Blunden S (2008). How accurate is subjective reporting of childhood sleep patterns? A review of the literature and implications for practice. Current Pediatric Reviews, 4, 132–142. [Google Scholar]
- Baum KT, Desai A, Field J, Miller LE, Rausch J, & Beebe DW (2014). Sleep restriction worsens mood and emotion regulation in adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 55, 180–90. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Beebe DW, Fallone G, Godiwala N, Flanigan M, Martin D, Schaffner L, & Amin R (2008). Feasibility and behavioral effects of an at-home multi-night sleep restriction protocol for adolescents. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 49(9), 915–923. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Brotman MA, Kircanski K, Stringaris A, Pine DS, & Leibenluft E (2017). Irritability in youths: A translational model. American Journal of Psychiatry, 174, 520–532. American Psychiatric Publishing. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Cardinale EM, Freitag GF, Brotman MA, Pine DS, Leibenluft E, & Kircanski K (2021). Phasic versus tonic irritability: Differential associations with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder symptoms. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 60, 1513–1523. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Craig SG, Weiss MD, Hudec KL, & Gibbins C (2020). The functional impact of sleep disorders in children with ADHD. Journal of Attention Disorders, 24, 499–508. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Crowley SJ, Wolfson AR, Tarokh L, & Carskadon MA (2018). An update on adolescent sleep: New evidence informing the perfect storm model. Journal of Adolescence, 67, 55–65. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Delaplace R, Garny de La Rivière S, Bon Saint Come M, Lahaye H, Popov I, Rey N, Visticot A, et al. (2018). Sleep and disruptive mood dysregulation disorder: A pilot actigraphy study. Archives De Pediatrie: Organe Officiel De La Societe Francaise De Pediatrie, 25, 303–308. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dietch JR, Sethi K, Slavish DC, & Taylor DJ (2019). Validity of two retrospective questionnaire versions of the Consensus Sleep Diary: The whole week and split week self-assessment of sleep surveys. Sleep Medicine, 63, 127–136. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Dimakos J, Gauthier-Gagné G, Lin L, Scholes S, & Gruber R (2021). The associations between sleep and externalizing and internalizing problems in children and adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder: Empirical findings, clinical implications, and future research directions. Child and Adolescent Psychiatric Clinics of North America, 30, 175–193. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Estrada-Prat X, Álvarez-Guerrico I, Bleda-Hernández MJ, Camprodon-Rosanas E, Batlle-Vila S, Pujals-Altes E, Nascimento-Osorio MT, et al. (2017). Sleep study in disruptive mood dysregulation disorder and bipolar children. Actas Espanolas De Psiquiatria, 45, 12–20. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Evans SC, Corteselli KA, Edelman A, Scott H, & Weisz JR (2023). Is Irritability a Top Problem in Youth Mental Health Care? A Multi-informant, Multi-method Investigation. Child Psychiatry & Human Development, 54(4), 1027–1041. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Goldstein H (1995). Hierarchical data modeling in the social sciences. Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 20, 201–204. [American Educational Research Association, Sage Publications, Inc., American Statistical Association; ]. [Google Scholar]
- Gregory AM, Van der Ende J, Willis TA, & Verhulst FC (2008). Parent-reported sleep problems during development and self-reported anxiety/depression, attention problems, and aggressive behavior later in life. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine, 162, 330–335. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Haller SP, Kircanski K, Stringaris A, Clayton M, Bui H, Agorsor C, Cardenas SI, et al. (2020). The Clinician Affective Reactivity Index: Validity and reliability of a clinician-rated assessment of irritability. Behavior Therapy, 51, 283–293. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Heron KE, Everhart RS, McHale SM, & Smyth JM (2017). Using mobile-technology-based ecological momentary assessment (EMA) methods with youth: A systematic review and recommendations. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 42(10), 1087–1107. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Hisler G, & Krizan Z (2017). Anger tendencies and sleep: Poor anger control is associated with objectively measured sleep disruption. Journal of Research in Personality, 71, 17–26. [Google Scholar]
- Karalunas SL, Gustafsson HC, Fair D, Musser ED, & Nigg JT (2019). Do we need an irritable subtype of ADHD? Replication and extension of a promising temperament profile approach to ADHD subtyping. Psychological Assessment, 31, 236–247. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kaufman J, Birmaher B, Brent D, Rao U, Flynn C, Moreci P, Williamson D, et al. (1997). Schedule for Affective Disorders and Schizophrenia for School-Age Children-Present and Lifetime Version (K-SADS-PL): Initial reliability and validity data. Journal of the American Academy of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 36, 980–988. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kenny R, Dooley B, & Fitzgerald A (2016). Ecological Momentary Assessment of Adolescent Problems, Coping Efficacy, and Mood States Using a Mobile Phone App: An Exploratory Study. JMIR Mental Health, 3(4), e51. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kircanski K, White LK, Tseng W-L, Wiggins JL, Frank HR, Sequeira S, Zhang S, et al. (2018). A latent variable approach to differentiating neural mechanisms of irritability and anxiety in youth. JAMA Psychiatry, 75, 631–639. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Konjarski M, Murray G, Lee VV, & Jackson ML (2018). Reciprocal relationships between daily sleep and mood: A systematic review of naturalistic prospective studies. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 42, 47–58. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Kouros CD, Keller PS, Martín-Piñón O, & El-Sheikh M (2022). Bidirectional associations between nightly sleep and daily happiness and negative mood in adolescents. Child Development, 93, e547–e562. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Krizan Z, Miller A, & Hisler G (2020). Does losing sleep unleash anger? Sleep, 43(Supplement_1), A105. [Google Scholar]
- Krizan Zlatan, & Hisler G. (2019). Sleepy anger: Restricted sleep amplifies angry feelings. Journal of Experimental Psychology. General, 148, 1239–1250. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Lever JR, Murphy AP, Duffield R, & Fullagar HHK (2021). A combined sleep hygiene and mindfulness intervention to improve sleep and well-being during high-performance youth tennis tournaments. International Journal of Sports Physiology and Performance, 16, 250–258. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Little RJA (1988). A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. Journal of the American Statistical Association, 83(404), 1198–1202. Taylor & Francis. [Google Scholar]
- Liu L, Chen W, Vitoratou S, Sun L, Yu X, Hagger-Johnson G, Wu Z, et al. (2019). Is emotional lability distinct from “angry/irritable mood,” “negative affect,” or other subdimensions of oppositional defiant disorder in children with ADHD? Journal of Attention Disorders, 23, 859–868. SAGE Publications Inc. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Loram G, Silk T, Ling M, Fuller-Tyszkiewicz M, Hyde C, McGillivray J, & Sciberras E (2021). Associations between sleep, daytime sleepiness and functional outcomes in adolescents with ADHD. Sleep Medicine, 87, 174–182. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mallidi A, Meza-Cervera T, Kircanski K, Stringaris A, Brotman MA, Pine DS, Leibenluft E, et al. (2023). Robust caregiver-youth discrepancies in irritability ratings on the affective reactivity index: An investigation of its origins. Journal of Affective Disorders, 332, 185–193. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazurek MO, & Sohl K (2016). Sleep and behavioral problems in children with autism spectrum disorder. Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 46, 1906–1915. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mazza S, Bastuji H, & Rey AE (2020). Objective and subjective assessments of sleep in children: Comparison of actigraphy, sleep diary completed by children and parents’ estimation. Frontiers in Psychiatry, 11, 495. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Moore M, Kirchner HL, Drotar D, Johnson N, Rosen C, & Redline S (2011). Correlates of adolescent sleep time and variability in sleep time: The role of individual and health related characteristics. Sleep Medicine, 12(3), 239–245. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Mulraney M, Zendarski N, Mensah F, Hiscock H, & Sciberras E (2017). Do early internalizing and externalizing problems predict later irritability in adolescents with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder? The Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry, 51, 393–402. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naim R, Kircanski K, Gold A, German RE, Davis M, Perlstein S, Clayton M, et al. (2021a). Across-subjects multiple baseline trial of exposure-based cognitive-behavioral therapy for severe irritability: A study protocol. BMJ Open, 11, e039169. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naim R, Shaughnessy S, Smith A, Karalunas SL, Kircanski K, & Brotman MA (2022). Real-time assessment of positive and negative affective fluctuations and mood lability in a transdiagnostic sample of youth. Depression and Anxiety, 39(12), 870–880. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Naim R, Smith A, Chue A, Grassie H, Linke J, Dombek K, Shaughnessy S, et al. (2021b). Using ecological momentary assessment to enhance irritability phenotyping in a transdiagnostic sample of youth. Development and Psychopathology, 33, 1734–1746. Cambridge University Press. [Google Scholar]
- Palmer CA, & Alfano CA (2017). Sleep and emotion regulation: An organizing, integrative review. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 31, 6–16. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Paruthi S, Brooks LJ, D’Ambrosio C, Hall WA, Kotagal S, Lloyd RM, Malow BA, et al. (2016). Recommended amount of sleep for pediatric populations: A consensus statement of the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. Journal of Clinical Sleep Medicine, 12(6), 785–786. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Pillai V, Steenburg LA, Ciesla JA, Roth T, & Drake CL (2014). A seven day actigraphy-based study of rumination and sleep disturbance among young adults with depressive symptoms. Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 77, 70–75. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Poznanski B, Cornacchio D, Coxe S, Pincus DB, McMakin DL, & Comer JS (2018). The link between anxiety severity and irritability among anxious youth: Evaluating the mediating role of sleep problems. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 49, 352–359. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Raudenbush SW, Bryk AS, Cheong YF, & Congdon R (2019). HLM 8 for Windows. Skokie, IL: Scientific Software International, Inc. [Google Scholar]
- Rubens SL, Evans SC, Becker SP, Fite PJ, & Tountas AM (2017). Self-reported time in bed and sleep quality in association with internalizing and externalizing symptoms in school-age youth. Child Psychiatry and Human Development, 48(3), 455–467. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadeh A, & Acebo C (2002). The role of actigraphy in sleep medicine. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 6, 113–124. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Sadeh S (2011). The role and validity of actigraphy in sleep medicine: An update. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 15, 259–267. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Short MA, Booth SA, Omar O, Ostlundh L, & Arora T (2020). The relationship between sleep duration and mood in adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Sleep Medicine Reviews, 52, 101311. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Short MA, & Louca M (2015). Sleep deprivation leads to mood deficits in healthy adolescents. Sleep Medicine, 16, 987–993. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Smith SR (2007). Making sense of multiple informants in child and adolescent psychopathology: A guide for clinicians. Journal of Psychoeducational Assessment, 25, 139–149. [Google Scholar]
- Smith AR, Kircanski K, Brotman MA, Do QB, Subar AR, Silk JS, Engel S, et al. (2019). Advancing clinical neuroscience through enhanced tools: Pediatric social anxiety as an example. Depression and Anxiety, 36(8), 701–711. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Snijders T, & Bosker R (2012). Multilevel analysis: An introduction to Basic and advanced multilevel modeling (2nd ed.). Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage. [Google Scholar]
- Spielberger CD, Reheiser EC, & Sydeman SJ (1995). Measuring the experience, expression, and control of anger. Issues in Comprehensive Pediatric Nursing, 18, 207–232. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stoddard J, Stringaris A, Brotman MA, Montville D, Pine DS, & Leibenluft E (2014). Irritability in child and adolescent anxiety disorders. Depression and Anxiety, 31, 566–573. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Stringaris A, Goodman R, Ferdinando S, Razdan V, Muhrer E, Leibenluft E, & Brotman MA (2012). The Affective Reactivity Index: A concise irritability scale for clinical and research settings. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 53, 1109–1117. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamura N, Komada Y, Inoue Y, & Tanaka H (2022). Social jetlag among Japanese adolescents: Association with irritable mood, daytime sleepiness, fatigue, and poor academic performance. Chronobiology International, 39(3), 311–322. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tamura N, & Tanaka H (2014). Effects of sleep education with self-help treatment for elementary schoolchild with nocturnal lifestyle and irritability. Sleep and Biological Rhythms, 12. [Google Scholar]
- Taylor DJ, & Pruiksma KE (2014). Cognitive and behavioural therapy for insomnia (CBT-I) in psychiatric populations: A systematic review. International Review of Psychiatry (Abingdon, England), 26, 205–213. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tomaso CC, Johnson AB, & Nelson TD (2021). The effect of sleep deprivation and restriction on mood, emotion, and emotion regulation: Three meta-analyses in one. Sleep, 44, zsaa289. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Tseng W-L, Naim R, Chue A, Shaughnessy S, Meigs J, Pine DS, Leibenluft E, et al. (2023). Network analysis of ecological momentary assessment identifies frustration as a central node in irritability. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 64(8), 1212–1221. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wechsler D (1999). Manual for the Wechsler Abbreviated Intelligence Scale (WASI). San Antonio, TX: The Psychological Corporation. [Google Scholar]
- Wen CKF, Schneider S, Stone AA, & Spruijt-Metz D (2017). Compliance with mobile ecological momentary assessment protocols in children and adolescents: A systematic review and meta-analysis. Journal of Medical Internet Research, 19(4), e132. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Werner-Seidler A, Johnston L, & Christensen H (2018). Digitally-delivered cognitive-behavioural therapy for youth insomnia: A systematic review. Internet Interventions, 11, 71–78. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wheaton AG, & Claussen AH (2021). Short sleep duration among infants, children, and adolescents aged 4 months–17 years—United States, 2016–2018. MMWR. Morbidity and Mortality Weekly Report, 70, 1315–1321. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Whiting C, Bellaert N, Deveney C, & Tseng W-L (2023, March 23). Associations between sleep quality and irritability: Testing the mediating role of emotion regulation. SSRN Scholarly Paper, Rochester, NY. Retrieved June 15, 2023, from https://papers.ssrn.com/abstract=4386988 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfson AR, Carskadon MA, Acebo C, Seifer R, Fallone G, Labyak SE, & Martin JL (2003). Evidence for the validity of a sleep habits survey for adolescents. Sleep, 26, 213–216. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wolfson AR, Harkins E, Johnson M, & Marco C (2015). Effects of the Young Adolescent Sleep Smart Program on sleep hygiene practices, sleep health efficacy, and behavioral well-being. Sleep Health, 1, 197–204. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Wrzus C, & Neubauer AB (2023). Ecological Momentary Assessment: A meta-analysis on designs, samples, and compliance across research fields. Assessment, 30(3), 825–846. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- Zeman J, Cassano M, Perry-Parrish C, & Stegall S (2006). Emotion Regulation in Children and Adolescents. Journal of Developmental & Behavioral Pediatrics, 27(2), 155–168. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Appendix S1 Table S1. Demographic information by diagnostic group.
Appendix S2 Table S2. Youth-reported evening irritability symptoms predicting nightly sleep duration and with the youth-reported ARI and Cl-ARI as moderators.