Skip to main content
Communications Psychology logoLink to Communications Psychology
. 2024 Aug 28;2:82. doi: 10.1038/s44271-024-00124-2

A systematic review and Bayesian meta-analysis provide evidence for an effect of acute physical activity on cognition in young adults

Jordan Garrett 1,2,, Carly Chak 1,2, Tom Bullock 1,2, Barry Giesbrecht 1,2,
PMCID: PMC11358546  PMID: 39242965

Abstract

Physical exercise is a potential intervention for enhancing cognitive function across the lifespan. However, while studies employing long-term exercise interventions consistently show positive effects on cognition, studies using single acute bouts have produced mixed results. Here, a systematic review and meta-analysis was conducted to determine the impact of acute exercise on cognitive task performance in healthy young adults. A Bayesian hierarchical model quantified probabilistic evidence for a modulatory relationship by synthesizing 651 effect sizes from 113 studies from PsychInfo and Google Scholar representing 4,390 participants. Publication bias was mitigated using the trim-and-fill method. Acute exercise was found to have a small beneficial effect on cognition (g = 0.13 ± 0.04; BF = 3.67) and decrease reaction time. A meta-analysis restricted to executive function tasks revealed improvements in working memory and inhibition. Meta-analytic estimates were consistent across multiple priors and likelihood functions. Physical activities were categorized based on exercise type (e.g., cycling) because many activities have aerobic and anaerobic components, but this approach may limit comparison to studies that categorize activities based on metabolic demands. The current study provides an updated synthesis of the existing literature and insights into the robustness of acute exercise-induced effects on cognition. Funding provided by the United States Army Research Office.

Subject terms: Cognitive neuroscience, Human behaviour


A single instance of exercise improves cognitive task performance especially in regard to reaction time. Cycling and high-intensity interval training (HIIT) were found to be particularly beneficial.

Introduction

A single bout of exercise induces a cascade of neuromodulatory changes that influence multiple brain systems1,2. This includes an increase in the synthesis of neurotransmitters (e.g., acetylcholine, dopamine, GABA, glutamate) and neurotrophic factors (e.g., BDNF), which can occur in a brain-region-specific manner (see ref. 1 for review). Given these impacts on the brain, it would be reasonable to hypothesize that single brief bouts of exercise are associated with changes in performance across a range of cognitive domains. Consistent with this hypothesis, there is abundant evidence that attention36, working memory711, decision making12,13, and cognitive control14,15 are facilitated by brief bouts of physical exercise. However, there is also evidence suggesting that exercise has little or no effect on cognitive task performance. For instance, Komiyama et al.16 observed no difference in accuracy on a spatial delayed response task between exercise and rest conditions. Further, working memory performance has been shown to remain unchanged either during or after a single bout of exercise6. The discrepant pattern of results in the literature investigating the link between exercise and performance on cognitive tasks is surprising given the consistent and robust physiological effects of even brief bouts of physical activity. However, it is unclear whether this limited impact of exercise on performance reflects the true state of affairs or whether the apparent lack of robust influence is due to vast empirical discrepancies across studies in the literature. Studying the impact of single exercise sessions on cognition can provide insight into how changes in our body’s physiological state impact behavior. This understanding can then guide the creation of more effective longer-term exercise interventions, which essentially involve regularly repeating brief exercise sessions over an extended period.

Meta-analytic techniques are a set of powerful tools that can expose dominant trends within a methodologically heterogeneous literature. There is a consensus amongst narrative reviews and previous meta-analyses that an acute bout of exercise has a small positive influence on behavioral performance1,1723. The nature of this effect is moderated by exercise protocol, cognitive tasks, and participant characteristics. For instance, Lambourne & Tomporowski20 observed that task performance during exercise was dependent on exercise modality, the type of cognitive task, and when it was completed relative to exercise onset. Similarly, post-exercise performance was moderated by exercise modality and the type of cognitive task. Chang et al.18 reported that post-exercise cognitive performance was influenced by exercise intensity, duration, and the time of cognitive test relative to exercise cessation. Interestingly, the authors found that study sample age was a significant moderator, where larger positive effects were found for high school (14–17 years), adult (31–60 years), and older adult (>60 years) samples compared to elementary (6–13 years) and young adult (18–30 years) samples. Multiple meta-analyses have observed that the effect of exercise is dependent on cognitive domain, with measures of executive function, attention, crystallized intelligence, and information processing speed showing the largest gains18,19,2426. Further, there is evidence that exercise has a differential influence on the speed and accuracy of cognitive processes. McMorris et al.21 observed that acute, intermediate exercise facilitated response times on working memory tasks, while accuracy was compromised. In contrast, exercise has been shown to boost both the accuracy and speed of cognitive control23. Altogether, it is important to consider cognitive task, participant, and physical activity characteristics to develop a holistic model of the relationship between exercise and cognition.

While these earlier meta-analyses have provided unique insights into understanding the relationship between acute exercise and cognition, they have two major limitations. First, the most recent holistic quantitative synthesis of the extant literature was published over a decade ago18. Meanwhile, the exercise and cognition literature has grown drastically. According to the electronic database Web of Science, almost 6,000 articles associated with the search term “exercise and cognition” have been published since this last holistic meta-analysis. In addition, more recent meta-analyses have primarily focused on executive processes19,22,26,27. Thus, previous models may provide an outdated and limited account of exercise-induced influences on other aspects of cognition, such as perception, long-term memory, and learning. Second, previous meta-analytic approaches employed frequentist statistical methods, which are based on a decision threshold rather than a characterization of the relevant evidence. As a result, it is possible that acute exercise and moderator variables are deemed to have a significant influence on task performance despite the fact that there may only be a small degree of probabilistic evidence in favor of this notion. In addition, relying on a decision threshold prevents these models from conveying the likelihood that an exercise protocol elicits a change in cognitive task performance. Past frequentist meta-analytic models also treated heterogeneity parameters as a fixed quantity and utilize only a point estimate, which can lead to an underestimation of the variability either between or within studies2831. This is especially true when the number of modeled studies is low3234. When considered together, there is a clear need for an updated meta-analysis using an approach that addresses these limitations.

The current study addressed these limitations in two ways. First, a comprehensive literature search was conducted spanning the years 1995–2023. To quantify the influence of exercise on cognition in young healthy adults, the search was limited to non-clinical studies whose subjects were between 18–45 years old. The analysis focused on subjects within this age range since exercise research has predominantly been dedicated toward studying the effects in children and older adults35,36. Studies were required to be experimental in nature, and consist of both an acute exercise manipulation and cognitive task measurements. A broad range of cognitive domains encompassing tasks probing perception to executive function were included in the meta-analysis. Similarly, a wide range of exercise types and testing contexts were included. For example, traditional laboratory exposures to exercise (e.g., cycling, running) and sport activities in real-world settings were viable candidates for analysis. By casting a wide net, the current study provides a large scope and updated summary of the current state of the exercise and cognition literature.

Second, the current study uses a Bayesian meta-analytic approach to synthesize studies across the acute exercise and cognition literature. The Bayesian approach affords a flexible modeling framework that uses reported effect sizes to characterize the relative evidence in favor of a modulatory account. Inherently, a random effects meta-analytic model is hierarchical in nature, making it well suited for Bayesian methods. When utilized within this statistical framework, priors are placed on parameters at the highest level of the model such as the estimated pooled effect size and measures of heterogeneity. This approach has several advantages compared to its frequentist counterpart. First, the use of priors on heterogeneity parameters can attenuate the underestimation of variation both between and within studies37,38, leading to a clearer understanding of sources of heterogeneity and an increased precision when estimating the pooled effect size39. Furthermore, priors provide additional constraints on low-level parameter estimates and a greater degree of “shrinkage” of outliers towards the overall pooled effect size or mode(s) of grouping variables39,40. Therefore, a Bayesian meta-analysis is more robust to outliers and can be more conservative when proper priors are employed. Second, the method yields a posterior distribution for all parameter estimates. This grants the capability of directly modeling the degree of uncertainty in heterogeneity estimates37. Posterior distributions can be used to compute the probability that an exercise protocol elicits a change in task performance of a given magnitude (e.g., large effect size). Compared to the approximation of p-values and confidence intervals, which require additional assumptions for hierarchical models, calculating the high-density interval (HDI), which indicates the most credible outcomes in the posterior distribution, for complex hierarchical models is seamless39. Third, it is possible to incorporate knowledge from previous meta-analyses when constructing prior distributions. This affords the ability to quantitatively compare the observed data to the predictions of previous models.

Considering the results of past meta-analyses, exercise was expected to have a small positive influence on cognition. Cognitive task and exercise characteristics were anticipated to moderate this relationship, as evidenced by nonzero parameter estimates, reflecting the selective nature of exercise-induced effects. Model comparisons were conducted to evaluate how moderator inclusion improved predictive performance, and robustness of parameter estimates were determined by employing multiple priors and likelihood functions.

Methods

Literature search

Studies investigating the impact of an acute bout of exercise on cognition were obtained through searches of the electronic databases PsychInfo and Google Scholar according to the PRISMA guidelines41. On 09 September 2023, databases were queried using a search string that combined the following physical activity and cognitive domain keywords: [“exercise” OR “physical activity” OR “physical exertion” OR “physical fatigue”] AND [“perception” OR “attention” OR “working memory” OR “executive function” OR “memory” OR “decision making” OR “motor skill” OR “skill acquisition” OR “language” OR “reasoning”]. For the PsychInfo search, the filters “journal article”, “English”, “empirical study”, “human”, and “peer reviewed” were applied. Search results were limited to studies published between 1995 and 2023 and whose subjects were between 18 and 45 years of age. Note, this literature search and analysis were not preregistered, nor was a review protocol prepared prior to the literature search.

Eligibility criteria

Studies were deemed eligible for inclusion in the meta-analysis if they met all of the following criteria: assessed the influence of an acute bout of exercise on cognition, compared the effects of exercise with an active and/or passive control group(s), utilized cognitive tasks that measured reaction time (RT) and/or accuracy, tested cognition either during, pre-, or post-exercise and consisted of cognitively normal subjects. Note, an acute bout was defined as an instance of physical activity that occurred within a single 24-hour period18. Two researchers independently screened records based on their title, abstract, and full text. In the case of discrepancies, a third researcher resolved them by reading the full-text.

Data extraction and coding

Information concerning experimental design and procedures, exercise details (i.e., type, intensity, duration), and sample characteristics were extracted from the final list of studies by a single researcher. Means and standard deviations of accuracy and/or RT measures on all cognitive tasks were inserted into an electronic spreadsheet for the calculation of effect sizes. The primary outcome measures for each domain were inserted separately if a task assessed multiple cognitive domains. Regarding studies that probed cognition at multiple time points during or post-exercise, measures for each time point were also recorded separately. If the statistics necessary for calculating effect sizes were not reported in the full-text of the article, the authors were contacted and asked to provide them.

All effect sizes were categorized into one of seven cognitive domains that were generally based on the DSM-542: executive function, information processing, perception, attention, learning, motor skills, and memory. The classification criteria used for categorizing a cognitive task into a domain is provided in the Supplementary Table 1. To account for variability in the metric used to measure exercise intensity across studies (e.g., ventilatory threshold, heart rate), each intensity was labeled as either light, moderate, or vigorous according to the American College of Sports Medicine guidelines43. Exercise durations were grouped into one of five time bins: ≤16 minutes, 20–27 minutes, 30–35 minutes, 40–45 minutes, ≥60 minutes. In the event that a study did not provide the exercise duration, its time bin was labeled as “not provided”. Exercise types were based on the modality reported in each study, yielding the following categorizations: cycling, high intensity interval training (HIIT), running, walking, circuit training, resistance exercise, and sports activity. The latter category encompassed studies that used sports-related exercises that did not fit into the other labels, such as rock climbing or soccer. The time at which cognitive task performance was evaluated relative to exercise was categorized as either during exercise or 0, 15, 20–75, and ≥180 minutes after cessation. Lastly, effect sizes were also coded according to task performance dependent measures (i.e., RT vs accuracy). Note, the levels of each categorical moderator were chosen with the intention of achieving a balance between specificity and statistical power to yield reliable estimates that can inform the design of future exercise studies.

Calculating effect sizes

Cohen’s d effect sizes were calculated for studies that tested cognition pre-/post-exercise without a control condition by dividing the mean change in performance by the standard deviation of the pre-test. If the study included a control group (e.g., rest), the mean change of the control condition was subtracted from the mean change of the exercise condition and divided by the pooled standard deviation of pretest scores20,44. For studies that tested cognition during, or only after exercise, the mean of the control condition was subtracted from the mean of the exercise condition and divided by the standard deviation of the control condition21. All effect sizes were converted into the bias-corrected standardized mean difference, Hedge’s g, by multiplying them by the correction factor J=134df1 where df is the degrees of freedom45. The sign of effect sizes for RT and error were reversed to reflect a positive influence of exercise on cognitive task performance. Once effect sizes were extracted from each study, inspection of a funnel plot and Egger’s regression test were conducted to assess the risk of publication bias.

Bayesian hierarchical modeling

The overall effect of exercise on cognition was assessed using a Bayesian hierarchical model46,47, which was implemented through the R package brms48. In the first level of the model, a study’s observed effect size(s) θ^ik was assumed to be an estimate of the true effect size θk. The observed effect(s) θ^ik were modeled as being sampled from a normally distributed population underlying study k with a mean equivalent to the true effect and a variance of σk2. In the second level of the model, the true effect size θk was assumed to have been drawn from an overarching distribution whose mean represented the overall pooled effect μ, and whose variance depicted the degree of between-study heterogeneity τ2. The final level of the model contained weakly informative priors. A standard normal prior was used for the pooled effect, while the prior for τ2 was a Half-Cauchy distribution with location and scale parameters set to 0 and 0.5, respectively.

Following the main meta-analysis, subgroup analyses were conducted to determine potential moderators of the relationship between exercise and cognitive task performance. More specifically, we analyzed the influence of the following primary moderators: cognitive domain, time of cognitive test relative to exercise, task outcome measure, exercise intensity, duration, and type. The following secondary moderators were also analyzed to determine the influence of study and participant characteristics on the overall pooled effect size: average sample age, body mass index (BMI kg/m2), height (cm), weight (kg), VO2 max (ml/kg/min), percentage of female participants, within- vs between-study design, and publication year. With the exception of publication year and the percentage of female participants, all secondary moderators were mean centered for interpretability. A standard normal distribution was used as a weakly informative prior for the difference in effect sizes between subgroups. When reporting model parameter estimates, we use the [mode ± standard deviation] and the 89% HDI of posterior distribution.

Statistical inference

For all estimated effect sizes, Bayes Factors (BFs) were used to determine the degree of evidence in favor of a difference from zero. BFs were approximated using the reciprocal of the Savage-Dickey density ratio, which was implemented using the function bayesfactor_parameters from the bayestestR package49. This method involves dividing the height of the prior distribution for the null value by the height of the posterior distribution at the same value, and represents the credibility of the null value for a parameter once the data has been taken into consideration. BFs were also used to ascertain the predictive performance of subgroup models. After each model was compared to a null counterpart (i.e., moderator excluded) using the function bayesfactor_models, an inclusion BF (bayesfactor_inclusion) was estimated to determine if including a moderator improved predictive power50. To estimate stable BFs, a large number of sampling iterations (10,000) and warmup samples (2000) were used for each of four chains when estimating model parameters51. BFs were interpreted following the guidelines proposed by Jeffreys 52. A BF between 1 and 3 indicates “anecdotal” evidence for the alternative hypothesis, between 3 and 10 indicates “moderate” evidence, between 10 and 30 indicates “strong” evidence, and greater than 30 indicates “very strong” evidence39,5355. The reciprocal of these ranges signifies evidence in favor of the null hypothesis (e.g., 0.33-1 = anecdotal evidence). When conducting subgroup analyses with more than two factors, orthonormal coding was employed to ensure that an identical prior was used for each factor level and that estimated BFs were accurate56. Parameter estimates were extracted from all models using the R package emmeans.

Sensitivity analysis

A popular criticism of the Bayesian approach is that priors are chosen subjectively, which in turn can bias parameter estimates and their corresponding BFs40,57. Although utilizing weakly informative priors mitigates bias, a sensitivity analysis that evaluates the contribution of both priors and the likelihood function must be conducted to determine if the model results are robust38,5860. Thus, we replicated the previously described modeling approach with the exception of using two different priors for the overall pooled effect size. The first was a normal distribution with a mean of zero and standard deviation of ½. Since this prior adds greater weight to the probability that exercise has no influence on task performance, we denoted it as the no effect (NE) prior. The second prior was constructed by synthesizing estimates from previous meta-analyses on acute exercise and cognition18,2023, resulting in a normal distribution with a mean of 0.24 and standard deviation of 0.57. This prior was denoted as the positive effect (PE) prior.

The influence of the likelihood function was assessed by modeling study effect sizes as being sampled from a t-distribution. An advantage of using this likelihood function, compared to a normal distribution, is that model parameter estimates are influenced less  by outliers40. The Half-Cauchy prior was used for the scale of the distribution, while a standard normal prior was used for its mean. For its shape (i.e., degree of freedom) an exponential distribution with a rate equal to 1/29 served as a prior. To determine if meta-analytic estimates were robust across the alternative priors and likelihood function, we visually compared the posterior distributions across models for large deviations58.

Results

Description of studies

The literature search yielded 15,900 peer reviewed journal articles, and after removing duplicates 8295 remained. Subsequent an initial screening based off the titles and abstracts, 805 studies were identified as potential candidates for modeling. 113 of these studies were deemed eligible for inclusion in the meta-analysis according to their full-text contents (Fig. 1). In total, 642 effect sizes were extracted from these studies, representing data from 4390 subjects. A majority of the effects measured the influence of exercise on executive function (k = 434) and attention (k = 109). Fewer effects were measured during exercise (k = 82) relative to after the cessation of exercise (k = 560). Visual inspection of a funnel plot suggested that the effect sizes were distributed symmetrically (Fig. 2a), however there was very strong evidence for asymmetry according to Egger’s regression intercept (β = 1.18 ± 0.25; HDI = [0.78, 1.58]; BF = 253.24) suggesting the presence of publication bias. This was addressed by employing the trim and fill approach, which imputes low-precision effect sizes until the funnel plot is symmetrical61.

Fig. 1. PRISMA flow diagram of literature search results.

Fig. 1

A total of 113 studies were deemed eligible for meta-analytic modeling.

Fig. 2. Meta-analysis of the effect of acute exercise on general cognitive task performance.

Fig. 2

a Funnel plot of 642 study effect sizes (black circles). Imputed effect sizes after using the trim and fill method are represented by the unfilled circles (n=9). Vertical blue line indicates the estimated pooled effect sizes, while dashed black lines represent a pseudo 95% confidence limits. b Posterior distribution of estimated pooled effect. Horizontal black line indicates bounds of 89% HDI derived using n=651 effect sizes. c Empirical cumulative density function of distribution in b, where the dashed black line indicates the pooled effect. d Representation of using the Savage-Dickey ratio to calculate BFs. The density of the null value in the prior distribution (red) is divided by its density in the posterior distribution (blue) to yield probabilistic evidence in favor of the alternative hypothesis. e Posterior distributions of between and within study heterogeneity.

Overall effect

The meta-analysis indicated that there was moderate evidence for an acute bout of exercise to have a small positive influence on overall performance across cognitive domains (g = 0.13 ± 0.04; HDI = [0.06, 0.20]; BF = 3.67) (Fig. 2b, d). According to the posterior distribution, there was a low probability that the estimated pooled effect was less than or equal to zero (p = 0.01) and an 80% chance that the effect size fell between the range of 0 to 0.2 (Fig. 2c). There was a large amount of heterogeneity within (τwithin= 0.65 ± 0.03; HDI = [0.60, 0.70]; Iwithin2= 81.19%) and moderate amount between (τbetween= 0.29 ± 0.05; HDI = [0.20, 0.38]; Ibetween2 = 15.9%) studies (Fig. 2e). Effect size estimates for each individual study are presented in Table 1.

Table 1.

Study description and estimated effect sizes

Study N Experimental design Exercise type Exercise intensity Duration (min) Cognitive domain Test time N effects g 89% HDI
Hogervorst et al., 124 15 Within Cycling Vigorous 60 Inhibition Pre-Post 1 0.03 −0.39, 0.47
McMorris & Graydon, 1996 20 Within Cycling

Moderate

vigorous

2 Executive function During 6 0.11 −0.19, 0.45
Brisswalter et al., 1997 20 Within Cycling Vigorous 40 Information processing Pre-Post 4 0 −0.36, 0.35
Arcelin & Delignieres, 1998 22 Within Cycling Moderate 10 Information processing During 2 0.03 −0.35, 0.44
McMorris et al., 1999 9 Within Cycling

Moderate

vigorous

Task completion Executive function During 4 0.01 −0.34, 0.38
Collardeau et al., 2001 11 Within Running Vigorous 90 Perception Pre-Post 2 −0.05 −0.47, 0.34
Deuster et al., 2002 13 Within Running Vigorous Volitional Exhaustion

Attention

Learning

Executive function

Pre-Post 24 0.16 −0.05, 0.39
Hillman et al., 2003 19 Within Running Vigorous 30 Executive function Post 2 −0.08 −0.44, 0.35
Pesce et al., 125 16 Within Cycling Moderate Task completion Attention During 2 0.25 −0.15, 0.70
Davranche et al., 2005a 7 Within Cycling Maximal Volitional exhaustion Perception Pre-Post 2 0.08 −0.32, 0.49
Davranche & Pichon, 2005b 12 Within Cycling Maximal Volitional exhaustion Perception During 1 0.06 −0.37, 0.49
Pesce et al., 126 48 Within Cycling Vigorous Task duration Attention During 2 0.14 −0.24, 0.57
Vickers & Williams, 127 10 Within Cycling

Moderate

Vigorous

2 Motor Skills Pre-Post 4 −0.05 −0.43, 0.29
Lo Bue-Estes et al., 128 26 Between Running Vigorous 20

Executive function

Memory

Pre-Post 8 0.01 −0.28, 0.32
Coles & Tomporowski, 129 18 Within Cycling Moderate 40 Executive function Pre-Post 1 0 −0.41, 0.44
Fontana et al., 130 32 Within Running

Light

Moderate

Vigorous

4 Executive function During 6 0.07 −0.23, 0.40
Luft et al., 131 30 Within Running Vigorous 20

Attention

information processing

Memory

Pre-Post 10 −0.11 −0.39, 0.16
Pontifex et al., 132 21 Within

Running

Resistance

Vigorous 30 Executive function Pre-Post 4 −0.31 −0.69, 0.05
Srygley et al., 133 52 Within Walking Light Task Completion Executive function During 2 0.03 −0.4, 0.39
Thomson et al., 134 163 Within Treadmill Vigorous 27 Executive function Pre-Post 2 0.1 −0.3, 0.49
Lambourne et al., 6 19 Within Cycling Moderate 40 Executive function Pre, During, Post 4 −0.36 −0.76, 0
Norling et al., 135 121 Between Running

Light

Moderate

Vigorous

30 Attention Pre-Post 3 0.08 −0.28, 0.46
Young et al., 136 27 Within Running Moderate 7 Motor skills Pre-Post 1 −0.02 −0.47, 0.39
Chang et al., 137 42 Between Cycling Vigorous 30 Executive function Pre-Post 3 −0.02 −0.37, 0.35
Green & Helton, 138 12 Within Climbing Moderate 3 Memory During 1 −0.2 −0.71, 0.20
Ohlinger et al., 139 50 Within Walking Light Task completion

Attention

Executive function

During 3 −0.01 −0.38, 0.35
Hope et al., 140 52 Between Punching Vigorous Volitional exhaustion Memory Post 3 −0.42 −0.83, −0.02
Lambourne, 141 16 Within Cycling Vigorous 35 Executive function Pre-Post 1 −0.01 −0.46, 0.39
Moore et al., 142 30 Between Cycling Moderate 6–54

Perception

Executive function

Attention

Pre-Post 5 −0.12 −0.43, 0.23
Quelhas Martins et al., 9 24 Between Cycling Moderate 8 Executive function During 1 0.1 −0.33, 0.53
Roberts & Cole, 143 40 Within Circuit

Light

Moderate

1–5 Executive function Pre-Post 8 −0.05 −0.34, 0.25
Bullock & Giesbrecht, 144 26 Between Cycling Moderate 136 Perception Pre, Post 8 −0.14 −0.44, 0.15
Byun et al., 145 25 Within Cycling Light 10 Executive function Pre-Post 2 0.17 −0.23, 0.58
Darling & Helton, 146 12 Within Climbing Moderate 5 Memory During 2 −0.1 −0.51, 0.29
Nibbeling et al., 147 22 Between Running Vigorous 10

Motor Skills

Executive function

Memory

Attention

Post 5 −0.08 −0.43, 0.24
Pontifex et al., 148 34 Within Running Moderate 20 Attention Pre-Post 2 0 −0.39, 0.4
Schmidt-Kassow et al., 149 18; 31 Within Walking Light 30 Memory During 2 0.05 −0.36, 0.43
Tsai et al., 150 60 Between Resistance

Moderate

Vigorous

45 Executive function Pre-Post 4 0.02 −0.33, 0.36
Bantoft et al., 151 45 Within Walking Light <60

Executive function

Attention

Information processing

During 4 −0.1 −0.39, 0.17
Larson et al., 152 69 Between Walking Light 60 Executive function During 2 −0.03 −0.43, 0.35
Osgood, 153 86 Between Sport Activity Light 2 Attention Post 1 −0.06 −0.47, 0.37
Perciavalle et al., 154 30 Within Cycling Vigorous Volitional Exhaustion Executive function Pre-Post 4 0.22 −0.11, 0.61
Shia et al., 155 17 Within Cycling Vigorous 45 Attention Pre-Post 1 −0.11 −0.55, 0.3
Stevens et al., 156 35; 34 Between Cycling

Light

Moderate

15 Learning During 4 0.04 −0.31, 0.4
Weng et al., 157 26 Within Cycling Moderate 30 Executive function Pre-Post 4 0.29 −0.07, 0.67
Alloway et al., 158 72 Within Running Light 8 Executive function Pre-Post 1 −0.01 −0.43, 0.42
Ando et al., 159 14 Within Cycling

Moderate

Vigorous

6 Perception During 2 −0.08 −0.48, 0.31
Brush et al., 160 28 Within Resistance

Light

Vigorous

45 Executive function Post 60 −0.1 −0.26, 0.04
Connell et al., 161 24 Between Cycling Moderate 180 Attention Pre-Post 4 −0.21 −0.6, 0.12
Hsieh et al., 162 20 Within Resistance

Moderate

Vigorous

30 Executive function Pre-Post 1 0.04 −0.39, 0.46
Hsieh et al., 163 18 Within Resistance Moderate 30 Executive function Post 1 0.03 −0.39, 0.47
Komiyama et al., 16 10 Within Cycling Moderate 30 Executive function During 3 0.04 −0.33, 0.42
Lowe et al., 164 51 Within Walking

Light

Moderate

20 Executive function Pre-Post 4 0.01 −0.34, 0.35
Torbeyns et al., 165 23 Within Cycling Light 30

Memory

Executive function

Attention

During 6 −0.1 −0.42, 0.22
Tsukamoto et al.,166 12 Within Cycling

Moderate

Vigorous

40 Executive function Pre-Post 16 −0.19 −0.45, 0.07
Zach & Shalom, 167 20 Within

Running

Volleyball

Resistance

Vigorous 15; Game duration Executive function Pre-Post 6 0.37 0.02, 0.7
Chang et al., 168 30 Within Cycling Vigorous 30 Executive function Post 1 0.35 −0.07, 0.9
Crush & Loprinzi, 169 352 Between Running Moderate 10; 20; 30; 45; 60

Executive function

Attention

Post 75 −0.14 −0.28, 0.0
González Fernández et al., 170 18 Within Cycling Light 45 Attention During 1 0.02 −0.39, 0.45
Lindheimer et al.,171 60 Between Cycling Light 25 Executive function Pre-Post 16 −0.12 −0.35, 0.12
Lowe et al., 172 28 Within Walking

Light

Moderate

20 Executive function Pre-Post 4 −0.31 −0.68, 0.06
Luu & Hall, 173 31 Within Yoga Light 25 Executive function Pre-Post 2 −0.19 −0.62, 0.19
Randolph & O’Connor, 174 18 Within Walking Light 10

Attention

Executive function

Information processing

Pre-Post 28 −0.06 −0.25, 0.14
Slutsky et al., 175 24 Between Cycling Light 15

Attention

Executive function

Pre-Post 7 −0.18 −0.47, 0.14
Sudo et al., 176 32 Between Cycling Vigorous Volitional exhaustion Executive function Pre-Post 3 0.28 −0.07, 0.7
Cuttler et al., 177 120 Between

Resistance

walking

Light

moderate

30

Memory

Attention

Pre-Post 6 −0.61 −0.98, −0.25
Daikoku et al.,178 44 Between Cycling Light 3 Memory During 1 −0.04 −0.5, 0.35
Elkana et al., 179 69 Between Cycling Moderate 15 Executive function Pre-Post 1 −0.03 −0.5, 0.35
Fenesi et al., 180 77 Between Calisthenics Light 13

Learning

Memory

Post 2 0.16 −0.24, 0.55
Kendall, 181 60 Between HIIT Vigorous 20

Information processing

Learning

Cognitive Control

Post 8 0.82 0.42, 1.16
Legrand et al., 182 101 Between Running Moderate 15

Attention

Executive function

Pre-Post 2 0.05 −0.34, 0.44
Samani & Heath, 183 14 Within Cycling Vigorous 10 Executive function Pre-Post 1 0.08 −0.34, 0.52
Siddiqui & Loprinzi, 184 20 Within Walking Moderate 20

Memory

Executive function

Pre, During, Post 4 0.02 −0.31, 0.4
Sng et al., 185 80 Between Walking Light 15 Memory During, Post 4 −0.25 −0.62, 0.1
Wade & Loprinzi, 186 34 Between Walking Moderate 15 Memory Post 1 −0.13 −0.56, 0.3
Yamazaki et al.,187 30 Within Cycling

Light

Moderate

10

Attention

Executive function

Pre-Post 16 0.07 −0.16, 0.32
Baker et al., 188 19 Within Cycling Light 120

Attention

Executive function

During 3 −0.06 −0.44, 0.31
Du Rietz et al., 189 26 Within Cycling Vigorous 20

Attention

Executive function

Pre-Post 4 −0.06 −0.42, 0.28
Engeroff et al., 190 26 Within Resistance

Moderate

Vigorous

Maximal

60 Executive function Pre-Post 4 0.1 −0.26, 0.45
Haynes et al., 191 24 Within Walking Light 15 Memory During, Post 9 −0.04 −0.32, 0.24
Johnson & Loprinzi, 192 40 Within Walking

Moderate

Vigorous

15 Memory Post 2 −0.01 −0.04, 0.38
McGowan et al., 193 58 Within Running Moderate 20 Executive function Pre-Post 2 −0.02 −0.39, 0.39
Mehren et al., 107 31; 32 Within Cycling Vigorous 30 Executive function Post 8 0.15 −0.14, 0.45
Schmidt et al., 194 15; 19 Within Running Vigorous 38 Attention Pre-Post 6 −0.24 −0.57, 0.09
Stenling et al., 195 32 Within Walking Moderate 3

Executive function

Attention

Post 8 0 −0.29, 0.29
Wu et al., 196 35 Within

Cycling

Resistance

Light

Vigorous

30 Executive function Post 8 0.28 −0.03, 0.59
Zhou & Qin, 197 72 Between Cycling Light 25 Executive function Pre-Post 2 −0.05 −0.42, 0.37
Aly & Kojima, 198 40 Between Cycling Light 20 Executive function Pre-Post 2 0.07 −0.32, 0.47
Chacko et al., 199 15 Within Cycling Vigorous 30 Executive function Pre-Post 2 0.06 −0.32, 0.49
Kao et al., 8 23 Within Walking Light 20 Executive function Post 4 0.24 −0.10, 0.63
Morris et al., 200 14 Within Cycling Moderate 30

Attention

Executive function

Pre-Post 6 −0.01 −0.33, 0.3
Walsh et al., 201 15; 13 Within Walking Moderate 20

Executive function

Attention

Pre-Post 8 0.01 −0.31, 0.29
Kao et al., 108 36 Within

HIIT

Walking

Moderate 16 Executive function Post 2 0.32 −0.06, 0.78
Kim et al., 202 16 Within Walking Vigorous 10 Executive function Pre-Post 2 −0.03 −0.38, 0.41
Klatt & Smeeton, 203 27 Within Cycling Vigorous Task Completion Memory During 4 −0.15 −0.5, 0.21
Kuhne et al., 204 50 Between Cycling Moderate 40-55 Memory Post 2 −0.09 −0.49, 0.29
Manocchio & Lowe, 205 22 Within Walking

Light

Moderate

20

Attention

Executive function

Pre-Post 8 −0.06 −0.34, 0.25
Miyashiro et al., 206 17 Within Push-Ups Vigorous 20 Executive function Post 1 0 −0.45, 0.41
Trammell & Aguilar, 207 28 Within Running Vigorous 20

Memory

Executive function

Pre-Post 5 −0.11 −0.47, 0.2
Zhu et al., 208 16 Within

Cycling

Running

Moderate

Vigorous

40 Executive function Pre-Post 16 0.1 −0.14, 0.34
Aguirre-Loaiza et al., 209 19 Between Cycling Vigorous 20 Executive function Pre-Post 4 −0.06 −0.41, 0.3
Drollette & Meadows, 210 22 Within HIIT Vigorous 9 Executive function Post 6 0.06 −0.24, 0.39
Engeroff et al., 211 26 Within Resistance

Moderate

Vigorous

60 Attention Pre-Post 16 −0.17 −0.39, 0.08
Frith et al., 212 45 Within Running Moderate 15 Executive function During 1 0.06 −0.4, 0.46
Kao et al., 213 27 Within Running Moderate 24

Attention

Executive function

Pre-Post 5 −0.04 −0.35, 0.31
LaCount et al., 214 18 Within HIIT Vigorous 16

Attention

Executive function

Information processing

Post 3 −0.09 −0.46, 0.28
Loprinzi & Storm, 215 180; 225; 158 Between Treadmill

Moderate

Vigorous

25 Memory Post 6 −0.02 −0.33, 0.3
Shirzad et al., 216 28 Within Cycling Light 26 Executive function Pre-Post 2 −0.23 −0.64, 0.17
Zheng et al., 217 27 Within Cycling Moderate 15 Executive function During 2 0.27 −0.11, 0.72
Chueh et al., 218 30 Within Step Exercise Vigorous 20

Attention

Executive function

Pre-Post 8 0.15 −0.15, 0.44
Yamada et al., 219 85 Within Recumbent Bike Vigorous 20 Executive function Pre-Post 4 0.02 −0.31, 0.38
B. Zhang et al., 220 18 Within Cycling Moderate 25 Perception Post 4 0.29 −0.09, 0.65
D. Zhang et al., 221 76 Within Cycling Light 20 Attention Pre-Post 4 0.03 −0.33, 0.36

Subgroup analyses

Primary subgroup analyses revealed that acute exercise reduced RT on cognitive tasks (g = 0.27; HDI = [0.18, 0.36]; BF=6.71×103), but had no impact on accuracy (g = 0.04; HDI = [−0.04, 0.12]; BF =6.15×102) (Table 2) (Fig. 3a). Engaging in either cycling (g = 0.21; HDI = [0.11, 0.32]; BF = 14.74) or HIIT (g = 0.73; HDI = [0.40, 1.09]; BF =26.05) was found to have an enhancing effect on performance in cognitive tasks (Fig. 3b). In regard to cognitive domain, there was evidence that acute exercise has a positive influence on executive processes (g = 0.18; HDI = [0.10, 0.27]; BF =36.97). Furthermore, behavioral performance was found to improve immediately after exercise cessation (g = 0.16; HDI = [0.11, 0.30]; BF =4.03) and in response to vigorous intensity exercises (g = 0.19; HDI = [0.09, 0.28]; BF =5.03). Lastly, at least moderate evidence in favor of non-zero parameter estimates were observed for the secondary moderators publication year, within-subjects design, age, percentage of female participants, and weight (Table 3).

Table 2.

Primary moderator estimates

Exercise moderator variable N g 89% HDI BF10 Cognitive moderator variable N g 89% HDI BF10
Intensity Domain
 Light 167 0.10 −0.02, 0.22 0.13 Attention 109 0.06 −0.08, 0.17 0.08
 Moderate 222 0.07 −0.03, 0.18 0.09 Executive function 434 0.18 0.10, 0.27 36.97
 Vigorous 253 0.19 0.09, 0.28 5.03 Information processing 15 0.12 −0.17, 0.41 0.14
Learning 12 0.24 −0.11, 0.59 0.25
Duration (minutes) Memory 44 −0.06 −0.25, 0.13 0.08
 ≤16 161 0.14 0.02, 0.26 0.30 Motor skills 6 −0.03 −0.51, 0.49 0.20
 20–27 152 0.15 0.02, 0.26 0.32 Perception 22 0.13 −0.17, 0.44 0.15
 30–35 93 0.08 −0.08, 0.22 0.09
 40–45 113 0.04 −0.13, 0.21 0.09
 >60 48 −0.03 −0.21, 0.15 0.08 Task outcome
 Not provided 75 0.37 0.17, 0.57 6.21 Accuracy 377 0.04 −0.04, 0.12 6.15e-2
Reaction time 265 0.27 0.18, 0.36 6.71e3
Type circuit 8 0.08 −0.33, 0.53 0.26
 Cycling 204 0.21 0.11, 0.32 14.74 Task completion time (relative to exercise)
 HIIT 18 0.73 0.40, 1.09 26.05 During 82 0.02 −0.18, 0.13 0.09
 Resistance 97 −0.06 −0.29, 0.14 0.11 Immediately after 315 0.16 0.11, 0.30 4.03
 Running 172 0.05 −0.10, 0.19 0.08 20–75 min post 94 0.22 0.13, 0.44 0.76
 Sport activity 26 0.04 −0.23, 0.29 0.11 >180 min post 151 0.08 −0.03, 0.28 0.10
 Walking 117 0.04 −0.10, 0.19 0.07

Fig. 3. Subgroup analyses.

Fig. 3

Posterior distributions of a cognitive and b exercise moderators. Horizontal black line indicates the 89% HDI interval, while the black dot represents the mode of the posterior distribution. Intervals derived using n=651 effect sizes.

Table 3.

Secondary moderator estimates

Moderator variable N g 89% HDI BF10
Publication year 642 0.13 0.06, 0.20 3.25
Exp. design
Between 193 0.03 −0.11, 0.16 0.07
Within 449 0.17 0.08, 0.24 12.18
Age (years) (μ=22.49) 599 0.14 0.06, 0.21 3.13
% Female 577 0.12 0.05, 0.20 4.23
BMI (kg/m2) (μ=24.02) 378 0.20 0.08, 0.30 1.28
VO2 max (ml/kg/min) (μ=43.22) 299 0.19 0.05, 0.34 1.08
Height (cm) (μ=158.55) 275 0.21 0.09, 0.33 2.90
Weight (kg) (μ=65.02) 283 0.21 0.08, 0.33 4.28

To test for the possible contribution of a learning effect to the estimated overall pooled effect size, a separate meta-analysis was conducted on effects from studies employing a pre-/post-test design (N effect sizes = 298). Despite the estimated pooled effect size for this subset of data being nominally similar to the estimate for the entire dataset, there was anecdotal evidence in favor of the null hypothesis (g = 0.15 ± 0.06; HDI = [0.04, 0.24]; BF =0.95). Moderator analyses indicated that there was no credible evidence for a difference in this estimated pooled effect size as a function of whether or not a control group was included in the study (BFInclusion=0.12;w/control:g=0.18±0.10;HDI=[0.03,0.33];BF=0.51;w/ocontrol:g=0.11±0.13;HDI=0.03,0.26;BF=0.18), suggesting that the estimated influence of exercise on general cognitive performance is not driven by a learning effect.

Model comparisons

Model comparisons were performed to determine if including a moderator improved predictive performance. Only a model that included task performance measure as a moderator was more likely when compared to a null counterpart (BFInclusion =357.10) (Table 4). This is likely due to a number of factors. First, acute exercise had a negligible impact on a majority of the levels in each subgroup. Second, there was a high degree of uncertainty in estimated model coefficients, as evidenced by their wide HDI intervals. Third, Bayesian inference automatically penalizes model complexity and favors more parsimonious models. If a model has many parameters, but a majority of them are nonzero, then a simpler counterpart will be favored.

Table 4.

Subgroup model comparisons

Model BFInclusion
No moderators 1
Exercise intensity 2.16e-4
Exercise duration 9.35e-4
Exercise type 2.00e-2
Cognitive domain 4.97e-3
Task outcome 357.10
Task completion time 5.52e-4
Publication date 9.05e-4
Experimental design 0.04
 Age 0
 % Female 4.53e-3
 BMI 0.04
 VO2 8.40e-3
 Height 2.89e-3
 Weight 0.01

Interactions between moderators

An exploratory analysis was conducted to determine if the influence of moderator variables was contingent on one another. Due to the computationally intensive nature of Bayesian modeling, analyses were limited to the following pairs of moderators: (1) exercise intensity and type, (2) exercise intensity and duration, (3) exercise type and duration, (4) cognitive domain and exercise type, (5) cognitive domain and exercise intensity, (6) cognitive domain and task performance measure, (7) exercise type and task performance measure. Although none of the pairs of interaction models had more predictive power compared to a null counterpart (BFInclusion: Model 1 = 3.86×106; Model 2=1.66×108; Model 3=1.84×103; Model 4 = 1.31 × 10−4; Model 5 = 3.91×105; Model 6 = 7.1×103; Model 7 = 7.05×104), there were two that had nonzero parameter estimates.

The first model included an interaction between cognitive domain and exercise type. There was evidence in favor of cycling improving performance on tasks that probed attention (g = 0.34; HDI = [0.14, 0.56]; BF =3.05) and executive function (g = 0.28; HDI = [0.14, 0.40]; BF = 17.83). HIIT exercises were found to bolster executive function (g = 1.01; HDI = [0.61, 1.43]; BF =155.33), while resistance exercises had an aversive impact on attentional performance (g = −0.76; HDI = [−1.20, −0.38]; BF =18.07) (Fig. 4a). The second model included an interaction between cognitive domain and task performance measure and indicated that time-dependent measures of executive function are improved (g = 0.30; HDI = [0.19, 0.39]; BF =1.10×103) (Fig. 4b).

Fig. 4. Interactions between cognitive and exercise moderators.

Fig. 4

Posterior mode estimates of models including interactions between cognitive domain and a exercise type and b task outcome measure. Width of line represents 89% HDI derived using n=651 effect sizes.

Sensitivity analyses

The estimated overall effect of acute exercise on cognition was consistent across the NE prior (g = 0.13 ± 0.04; HDI = [0.06, 0.20]; BF = 6.52), PE prior (g = 0.12 ± 0.04; HDI = [0.06, 0.19]; BF =6.51), and t likelihood function (g = 0.12 ± 0.04; HDI = [0.06, 0.18]; BF =8.77) (Fig. 5a). Interestingly, there was anecdotal-to-moderate evidence in favor of the synthesized estimate from previous meta-analyses (i.e., g = 0.24) across the PE (BF =3.19), NE (BF =2.78), and standard normal (BF =5.27) priors. Estimates of between-study heterogeneity were also robust across the NE prior (τbetween= 0.29 ± 0.05; HDI = [0.20, 0.37]), the PE prior (τbetween= 0.29 ± 0.05; HDI = [0.21, 0.37]), and t likelihood function (τbetween= 0.31 ± 0.03; HDI = [0.26, 0.38]) (Fig. 5b). In contrast, within study heterogeneity was estimated to be lower when using the t likelihood function (τwithin= 0.17 ± 0.02; HDI = [0.13, 0.19]) relative to the NE (τwithin= 0.65 ± 0.03; HDI = [0.61, 0.70]) and PE (τwithin= 0.65 ± 0.02; HDI = [0.60, 0.70]) priors (Fig. 5c). Note, this reduction reflects the diminished influence of outliers on variance estimates by the inclusion of the shape parameter for the t distribution (v = 1.52 ± 0.14; HDI = [1.30; 1.73]). In addition to testing the robustness of parameter estimates, a model comparison was conducted to determine if either the null or positive effect prior was more probable given the data. The t-likelihood function was not included in this comparison since it would only indicate if effect sizes were more likely to have been drawn from either a normal or t-distribution. When compared to a standard normal prior, there was anecdotal evidence in favor of both the PE (BF =2.56) and NE (BF =1.48) priors. Relative to the PE prior, there was anecdotal evidence against the NE prior (BF =0.73). Altogether, parameter estimates were not biased by the prior or likelihood function.

Fig. 5. Sensitivity analyses.

Fig. 5

Estimates for the a overall pooled effect size, b between- and c within-study heterogeneity parameters across the t-likelihood function (TL), weakly informed, null effect (NE), and positive effect (PE) priors. Color dots represent mode of posterior distributions, while color horizontal line depicts the 89% HDI derived using n=651 effect sizes.

Executive function meta-analysis

Considering that the majority of the effect sizes were from tasks that probed executive function, and that this cognitive domain encompasses multiple sub-domains, a separate meta- analysis and set of meta-regressions were conducted on this subset of data. Categorization criteria from previous meta-analyses and systematic reviews17,24,26 were used to classify effect sizes into the following sub-domains of executive function: working memory, cognitive control, decision making, planning, and inhibition. For completeness, the primary moderators used in the main meta-analysis were also tested.

The results were similar to the main meta-analysis. There was very strong evidence in favor of exercise having a small positive influence on overall task performance (g = 0.20 ± 0.06; HDI = [0.12, 0.30]; BF =29.57), and a moderate degree of heterogeneity both within (τwithin= 0.51 ± 0.03; HDI = [0.47, 0.57]) and between studies (τbetween= 0.40 ± 0.06; HDI = [0.30, 0.48]). Subgroup analyses indicated that a model including the moderator task outcome measure had more predictive power relative to a null counterpart (BFInclusion =48.43). Paralleling the main meta-analysis, there was very strong evidence that acute exercise improved RT on executive function tasks (g = 0.32; HDI = [0.21, 0.42]; BF =748.18), but no credible evidence was observed for an effect on accuracy (g = 0.13; HDI = [0.04, 0.23]; BF =0.63) (Table 5). Furthermore, there was moderate evidence in favor of a positive impact of exercise on inhibition (g = 0.21; HDI = [0.09, 0.33]; BF =3.14) and working memory (g = 0.22; HDI = [0.11, 0.34]; BF =6.89) (Fig. 6). Yet, a model including executive function sub-domain as a moderator did not improve model performance (BFInclusion =7.52×104), nor did models including interactions between moderators.

Table 5.

Executive function moderator estimates

Exercise moderator variable N g 89% HDI BF10 Cognitive moderator variable N g 89% HDI BF10
Intensity Sub-domain
 Light 107 0.23 0.08, 0.37 1.71 Cognitive control 55 0.23 0.06, 0.39 0.73
 Moderate 151 0.15 0.01, 0.28 0.30 Decision making 28 0.10 −0.16, 0.39 0.14
 Vigorous 175 0.24 0.12, 0.36 13.49 Inhibition 153 0.21 0.09, 0.33 3.14
Planning 18 0.14 −0.12, 0.38 0.15
Duration (minutes) Working memory 179 0.22 0.11, 0.34 6.89
 ≤16 93 0.25 0.09, 0.40 1.76
 20–27 99 0.13 −0.02, 0.27 0.19 Task outcome
 30–35 68 0.19 0.03, 0.36 0.44 Accuracy 253 0.13 0.04, 0.23 0.63
 40–45 100 0.13 −0.06, 0.32 0.16 Reaction time 180 0.32 0.21, 0.42 749.18
 >60 25 0.11 −0.12, 0.32 0.13
 Not provided 48 0.53 0.29, 0.83 24.40 Type
Circuit 8 0.15 −0.31, 0.65 0.31
Task completion time (relative to exercise) Cycling 133 0.28 0.15, 0.42 19.58
 During 39 0.23 0.004, 0.44 0.45 HIIT 12 0.96 0.56, 1.38 71.23
 Immediately after 197 0.21 0.10, 0.31 4.17 Resistance 90 −0.07 −0.33, 0.17 0.12
 20–75 min post 72 0.31 0.13, 0.47 4.11 Running 116 0.06 −0.12, 0.25 0.10
 >180 min post 125 0.13 −0.03, 0.29 0.17 Sport activity 11 0.30 −0.07, 0.68 0.35
Walking 63 0.14 −0.05, 0.34 0.18

Fig. 6. Subgroup analyses of effects from tasks testing executive function.

Fig. 6

Posterior distributions for executive function sub-domain. Horizontal black line indicates the 89% HDI interval, while the black dot represents the mode of the posterior distribution, which was derived using n=433 effect sizes.

Discussion

A large corpus of empirical work has examined how a single bout of acute exercise modulates activity within multiple brain systems that underly cognition. Despite inconsistencies in results across empirical studies, there is consensus amongst previous reviews and meta-analyses that acute exercise impacts behavioral performance18,20,22 and that this relationship is moderated by both exercise protocol and behavioral task characteristics. The goal of the present work was to address two key limitations of previous meta-analyses. First, recent meta-analyses have a narrower focus, often limited to a single cognitive domain or a specific subset of domains. In contrast, the current meta-analysis presents an updated synthesis of the literature spanning a much wider range of cognitive domains. Second, in contrast to previous frequentist approaches, a Bayesian framework was adopted allowing for the quantification of the degree of evidence in favor of the hypothesis that acute exercise influences cognition in young healthy adults. The current meta-analysis observed that acute exercise has a small positive influence on overall cognitive task performance, and sensitivity analyses indicated that the alternative hypothesis was 6.51–8.77 times more likely than the null across multiple priors and likelihood functions. The magnitude and directionality of this effect were consistent with the results of previous meta-analyses on acute exercise and cognition18,21,22,62. Subgroup analyses suggested that this relationship is moderated by task performance measure, cognitive domain, exercise type and intensity, and the time of task completion relative to exercise cessation. Model comparison results indicated that accounting for variations amongst moderator levels did not improve predictive performance. Given our eligibility criteria, these results are limited to healthy individuals between the ages of 18–45 years old.

Similar to McMorris et al.63, acute exercise was found to improve RT but no credible evidence was observed for an influence on accuracy. A possible explanation for this differential impact on task outcome measures is that exercise modulates primary motor cortex (M1) excitability64. There is accumulating evidence that acute exercise increases M1 intracortical facilitation6568 and inhibition69,70. Yamazaki et al.68 observed that the intracortical circuits of both exercised (i.e., legs) and non-exercised (i.e., hand) effectors are modulated by an acute bout of low intensity pedaling. Thus, alterations in the activity of excitatory or inhibitory circuits of non-exercised cortical representations may promote faster RT on cognitive tasks. However, the lack of concurrent changes in corticospinal excitability or motor-evoked potentials suggests that this explanation is not a viable account of a mechanism that engenders faster RTs. An alternative explanation is that exercise increases peripheral and central concentrations of catecholamines, such as norepinephrine, epinephrine, and dopamine, which in turn improves the speed of cognition1,71,72. Indeed, acute exercise has been found to improve response time on choice RT, decision-making, and interference tasks18,73,74. Yet, it is unclear as to why changes in neurochemical levels would facilitate RT but have no impact on accuracy. Considering that physical activity modulates population-level tuning in the sensory areas of nonhuman animals and invertebrates7581, along with sensory responses in humans8284, it stands to reason that the fidelity of stimulus representations would also be impacted, resulting in changes in accuracy. Changes in the fidelity of feature selective stimulus representations can be determined by applying encoding models to recorded neural activity83,8590. For instance, Garrett et al.91 applied an inverted encoding model to topographical patterns of alpha band activity, recorded at the scalp, while subjects completed a spatial working memory task both at rest and during a bout of moderate intensity cycling. Notably, it was possible to reconstruct spatially selective responses during exercise, and the selectivity of these responses decreased during exercise relative to rest. Therefore, encoding models can be a powerful tool for future research to demystify how the precision of task-relevant representations is influenced by exercise. It is also important to keep in mind that many psychological tasks are relatively simple to do, which can lead to ceiling effects that may mask the influence of exercise on accuracy measures. Lastly, the differential impact of exercise on accuracy and RT may be due to the relative sensitivities of these dependent measures to modulations of different stages of information processing. For example, there is evidence that in near-threshold tasks accuracy is sensitive to perceptual manipulations, whereas in supra-threshold (i.e., perceptually easy tasks, including many of those used in the studies in this meta-analysis) RT is sensitive to modulations in both perceptual and post-perceptual processes92,93. Indeed, Davranche et al.73 utilized a drift diffusion model to determine which aspects of decision-making are modulated by HIIT. Importantly, drift rate and decision response boundary size increased significantly after exercise relative to before, while non-decision time decreased. This suggests there was an improvement in perceptual discrimination, the efficiency of non-decisional processes (e.g., motor execution), and the adoption of a more conservative criterion. Future research employing computational models of response time and representational fidelity is needed to develop a comprehensive understanding of the selective influence exercise on information processing speed and accuracy.

Parameter estimates of a model including exercise modality as a moderator suggested that engaging in cycling or HIIT may beneficially impact cognition, especially on attentional and executive processes. Cycling is a commonly used modality in exercise and cognition research. Numerous empirical studies have found that a bout of cycling benefits inhibition, as measured using either the Stroop or Eriksen Flanker task15,9499. Improvements in planning94,100, task-switching27,101,102, and the speed of decision making103 have also been reported. In contrast to the ubiquity of cycling, the use of HIIT workouts in exercise and cognition research is a relatively recent practice, hence the small number of effect sizes from studies using this modality compared to other types of exercise. The number of effect sizes is important because low-level parameters in a hierarchical model are influenced both by the subset of data directly dependent on the low-level parameter, and by high-level parameter estimates that rely on all of the data. This makes low-level parameter estimates indirectly dependent on the entire dataset, and causes shrinkage in estimates at all levels of the model. In other words, the estimated relationship between HIIT and behavioral performance is derived directly from the few representative effect sizes and indirectly from the rest of the data. The observed positive effect of HIIT on cognition corroborates previous findings. For example, Alves et al.3 observed that the time to complete a Stroop Task decreased after ten 1-minute bouts of exercising at 80% heart rate reserve relative to a control condition. Improvements in time-dependent measures on interference tasks (i.e., Stroop and flanker) have been correlated with an increase in left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex activity, as measured with functional near infrared spectroscopy (fNIRS), and a decrease in P3 latency measured with EEG104. Furthermore, enhancements have also been shown to coincide with an increase in peripheral levels of neural growth factors and lactate105. Lastly, a recent meta-analysis on elite athletes observed that HIIT team-based sports had a positive impact on cognitive task performance25. Interestingly, because of the small number of published studies in the literature, it is currently unclear if the type of exercise modality used for HIIT workouts (e.g., cycling, sprinting, resistance) differentially impacts cognition.

Behavioral task performance was found to be improved by engaging in vigorous intensity exercise. These results are surprising, considering that exercise intensity is believed to have an inverted-U relationship with performance; where moderate intensity exercise elicits the greatest enhancements while more intense, fatiguing exercise imposes decrements18,62,63,71,106,107. This effect could be driven by HIIT workouts, but may also depend on multiple cognitive task and exercise protocol characteristics. For instance, Chang et al.18 observed that exercise intensity was only a significant moderator when cognition was tested post-exercise. Similarly, Oberste et al.23 found that exercise intensity influenced time-dependent measures of interference control but not accuracy. When considering these results, one must also consider that both aforementioned meta-analyses included studies whose subjects were children, adolescents, and older adults. In contrast, the current study was limited to young adults, and there is evidence that the effect of exercise on cognition is comparatively smaller in this age group18,23. Thus, a model containing an interaction between cognitive domain, task outcome measure, and age groups across the lifespan may be required to observe evidence for an effect of intensity. In addition, there was evidence for the enhancing effects of exercise post-cessation, corroborating previous research1,18,94. Interestingly, in the current meta-analysis cognition was not found to be impacted during exercise. Prior meta-analytic findings on cognition during exercise are mixed, with some reporting that it is exacerbated20, while others that find evidence for an enhancement18.

Given that the majority of the effect sizes were from tasks that probed executive function, a separate meta-analysis was conducted on this subset of data. This analysis revealed that exercise has a small positive impact on RT measures of executive processes. When looking at model parameters, there was evidence in favor of exercise-enhancing inhibition and working memory. Behavioral research has shown that both the accuracy9 and speed of working memory108,109 are facilitated by an instance of physical activity. What remains to be determined is the neural mechanisms that engender these behavioral effects. Kao et al.108 observed that a reduction in RT on the Sternberg task post-HIIT corresponded to an increase in frontal alpha desynchronization during encoding, maintenance, and retrieval periods when working memory load is high. Neuroimaging studies have also found evidence for changes in the activation levels of frontal areas110 and their connectivity with the intraparietal sulcus post-exercise111. These changes in neural activity were not accompanied by a change in behavior, suggesting that more research is needed to demystify the neuromodulatory effect of acute exercise on working memory.

Engaging in repeated bouts of acute exercise over a long period of time can have lasting changes on baseline neurochemical levels, cortical volume, and structural/functional connectivity, which can alter cognitive task performance1,112115. Research investigating the influence of these long-term interventions on cognition has primarily focused on children or older adults. Systematic reviews and meta-analyses suggest that exercise has a small to moderate beneficial impact on general task performance for both of these age groups, with the largest effect sizes observed for measures of executive function, attention, and academic performance35. Despite the relative paucity of meta-analyses on how exercise interventions impact cognition in healthy young adults, recent work suggests that it may have a similar beneficial effect. Indeed, a recent meta-analysis, conducted by Ludyga et al.116, indicated that long-term exercise interventions have a small positive influence on general cognition regardless of age. The magnitude of this effect was dependent on the interaction between intervention length and exercise duration, with longer interventions and sessions producing greater benefits. Integrating these findings with the current meta-analysis, there is support for the notion that the beneficial impact of long-term interventions on cognition may be a product of repeated exposure to acute exercise induced effects.

There are a number of possible explanations as to why exercise induced effects are small. One possibility is that cognitive function is at its peak during young adulthood, leaving little room for improvements in task performance. Indeed, previous reviews and meta-analyses have observed that the effect of exercise is moderated by age35, with the greatest benefits observed for preadolescent children and older adults18,23,26. Contrary to this account, though, the largest exercise induced effects were observed for executive processes, which are believed to be at peak efficiency during this period in the lifespan117,118. Furthermore, there was moderate evidence that the impact of exercise increased as the average age of sampled young adults also increased. Another explanation may be that cognition is resilient to slight or modest perturbations in overall global state. For example, Bullock et al.119 demonstrated there was no change in accuracy or RT on a target detection task during experimentally induced hypoxia, hypercapnia, hypocapnia, and normoxia. Meta-analytic modeling of the influence of acute stress on executive function revealed that stress has a small negative impact on working memory and cognitive flexibility, but no impact on inhibition120. This suggests that cognition is able to selectively adapt to changes in physiological state caused by various types of stressors, including exercise. A final more intriguing and functional explanation for exercise having a small impact on cognition is that experimental protocols do not typically require the engagement of the body to execute the cognitive task, but rather have people engage in a cognitive task while exercising (or shortly thereafter). This experimental design contrasts real-world tasks that require engagement of the body in the service of the cognitive task. When components of the exercise are incorporated into task goals, then larger changes in performance may be observed. Empirical research investigating how exercise influences task performance in embodied settings versus classic laboratory settings (see121 for review) is necessary to test the plausibility of this explanation. In addition, the notion that the integrated action of the body and the mind are required to produce the largest effects of exercise on cognition is consistent with a recent evolutionary account of the link between cognition and exercise122.

The discrepancy in moderator results between the current meta-analysis and previous meta-analyses could be due to differences in the statistical approach. Frequentist methods typically conduct an omnibus test to determine if levels of a moderator are significantly different from one another and as a measure of a model’s goodness of fit. In contrast, the Bayesian approach determines how likely the observed effect sizes are under a model that includes a moderator and if predictive power is increased. There are a few key advantages to using the Bayesian approach compared to classical frequentist methods. First, it models the uncertainty involved in estimates of between- and within-study heterogeneity and returns a full posterior distribution for both parameters123. With these posterior distributions, one can simulate possible pooled effect sizes across credible levels of heterogeneity and develop an informed hypothesis for a subsequent meta-analysis. Similarly, the posterior distributions of effect size estimates can be used as well-informed prior distributions for new data. Importantly, this facilitates the updating of meta-analyses as new research is published. It should be mentioned that the degree of between-study heterogeneity was numerically similar to previous meta-analyses18,22, implying that they did not suffer from an issue of underestimation by assuming heterogeneity to be a fixed quantity. Second, the Bayesian approach permits the inclusion of prior knowledge. Across all tested priors, there was evidence in favor of a pooled effect derived from averaging the reported estimates of previous meta-analyses. When comparing a prior distribution based on this knowledge to a null effect prior, the former was found to be more probable. Lastly, the posterior distribution of parameter estimates can be used to ascertain the likelihood that one will observe an effect size of a given magnitude for an exercise protocol and cognitive task combination. For example, a researcher could compute the probability that the influence of a bout of cycling on cognitive control will fall within the range of large effect sizes, even if that range does not encompass the maximum a posteriori probability estimate. In contrast, the frequentist approach only produces the maximum likelihood estimate and an interval around it based on fictitious repeats of the meta-analysis. Therefore, the Bayesian approach provides more information for designing future exercise and cognition studies.

Limitations

A potential limitation in the current meta-analysis is the categorization of exercise type using the activity reported in each study. An alternative approach is to categorize exercise based on the theoretical and physiological distinctions between aerobic and anaerobic exercise. We did not adopt this approach here because many activities used in the literature typically include aerobic and anaerobic components, and basing their classification on what authors reported provides insights into the exercise modalities that have been predominantly used in the literature. Another limitation is the schema used to categorize exercise duration. In the event that a study did not report how long participants engaged in exercise, these effects were classified as “not provided”, rendering them as uninterpretable. Lastly, sensitivity analyses were not conducted for moderator parameter estimates due to the high degree of computational demands. However, considering that the pooled effect size estimate was robust across multiple priors and likelihood functions, it is likely that moderator parameter estimates are also consistent.

Conclusions

In summary, the current meta-analytic examination has shown that there is moderate evidence for an acute bout of aerobic exercise inducing a small enhancement in overall performance on cognitive tasks, especially on those that probe executive function and measure response time. Incorporating computational models of decision-making processes, such as drift-diffusion or signal detection models, into exercise research may provide useful insights into the nature of speeded executive processes. Furthermore, testing performance in a real-world setting where individuals typically engage in physical activity may amplify exercise-induced effects.

Supplementary information

Peer Review File (3.6MB, pdf)
Supplementary Material (104.3KB, pdf)

Acknowledgements

This work was funded by the U.S. Army Combat Capabilities Development Command Soldier Center Measuring and Advancing Soldier Tactical Readiness and Effectiveness (MASTR-E) program through award W911NF-19F-0018 under US Army Research Office contract W911NF-19-D-0001 for the Institute for Collaborative Biotechnologies. The views expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not reflect the official policies or positions of the Department of Army, the Department of Defense, or any other department or agency of the U.S. government. The funders had no role in study design, data collection, and analysis, the decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript. Thank you to Grace Giles, Ph.D., Julie Cantelon, Ph.D., and Neil Dundon, Ph.D., for providing guidance in conducting analyses and interpreting results. Lastly, thank you to Emily Machniak and Riddhima Chandra for assisting in data collection efforts.

Author contributions

J.G., C.C., T.B., and B.G., designed the study, created the literature search criterion, and performed initial article screenings. J.G., C.C., and B.G. performed screenings of articles based on their full text. J.G., extracted effect sizes, conducted statistical analyses, and wrote the initial draft of the manuscript. B.G., T.B., and C.C. provided edits to the manuscript.

Peer review

Peer review information

Communications Psychology thanks Sebastian Ludyga and Nicole Logan for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Primary Handling Editors: Daniel Quintana, Antonia Eisenkoeck and Jennifer Bellingtier. A peer review file is available.

Data availability

Data have been made publicly available at https://github.com/jggarrett23/PACMAn.

Code availability

Code has been made publicly available at https://github.com/jggarrett23/PACMAn.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

Jordan Garrett, Email: jordangarrett@ucsb.edu.

Barry Giesbrecht, Email: giesbrecht@ucsb.edu.

Supplementary information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s44271-024-00124-2.

References

  • 1.Basso, J. C. & Suzuki, W. A. The effects of acute exercise on mood, cognition, neurophysiology, and neurochemical pathways: a review. Brain Plast.2, 127–152 (2017). 10.3233/BPL-160040 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Hillman, C. H., Erickson, K. I. & Kramer, A. F. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat. Rev. Neurosci.9, 58–65 (2008). 10.1038/nrn2298 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Alves, C. R. R. et al. Influence of acute high-intensity aerobic interval exercise bout on selective attention and short-term memory tasks. Percept. Mot. Skills118, 63–72 (2014). 10.2466/22.06.PMS.118k10w4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Chang, Y. K., Pesce, C., Chiang, Y. T., Kuo, C. Y. & Fong, D. Y. Antecedent acute cycling exercise affects attention control: an ERP study using attention network test. Front. Hum. Neurosci.9, 156 (2015). 10.3389/fnhum.2015.00156 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Davranche, K. & Audiffren, M. Facilitating effects of exercise on information processing. J. Sports Sci.22, 419–428 (2004). 10.1080/02640410410001675289 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Lambourne, K., Audiffren, M. & Tomporowski, P. D. Effects of acute exercise on sensory and executive processing tasks. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc42, 0–00 (2010). 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181cbee11 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Chen, A. G., Zhu, L. N., Yan, J. & Yin, H. C. Neural basis of working memory enhancement after acute aerobic exercise: FMRI study of preadolescent children. Front. Psychol.7, 1804 (2016). 10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01804 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Kao, S. C., Wang, C. H. & Hillman, C. H. Acute effects of aerobic exercise on response variability and neuroelectric indices during a serial n-back task. Brain Cogn.138, 105508 (2020). 10.1016/j.bandc.2019.105508 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Quelhas Martins, A., Kavussanu, M., Willoughby, A. & Ring, C. Moderate intensity exercise facilitates working memory. Psychol. Sport Exerc.14, 323–328 (2013). 10.1016/j.psychsport.2012.11.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Roig, M., Nordbrandt, S., Geertsen, S. S. & Nielsen, J. B. The effects of cardiovascular exercise on human memory: a review with meta-analysis. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.37, 1645–1666 (2013). 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.06.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Schaefer, S., Lövdén, M., Wieckhorst, B. & Lindenberger, U. Cognitive performance is improved while walking: differences in cognitive–sensorimotor couplings between children and young adults. Eur. J. Dev. Psychol.7, 371–389 (2010). 10.1080/17405620802535666 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Kamijo, K. et al. Differential influences of exercise intensity on information processing in the central nervous system. Eur. J. Appl. Physiol.92, 305–311 (2004). 10.1007/s00421-004-1097-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Kamijo, K. & Takeda, Y. General physical activity levels influence positive and negative priming effects in young adults. Clin. Neurophysiol.120, 511–519 (2009). 10.1016/j.clinph.2008.11.022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Hillman, C. H. et al. The effect of acute treadmill walking on cognitive control and academic achievement in preadolescent children. Neuroscience159, 1044–1054 (2009). 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2009.01.057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Kamijo, K., Nishihira, Y., Higashiura, T. & Kuroiwa, K. The interactive effect of exercise intensity and task difficulty on human cognitive processing. Int. J. Psychophysiol.65, 114–121 (2007). 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2007.04.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Komiyama, T. et al. Cognitive function at rest and during exercise following breakfast omission. Physiol. Behav.157, 178–184 (2016). 10.1016/j.physbeh.2016.02.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Cantelon, J. A. & Giles, G. E. A review of cognitive changes during acute aerobic exercise. Front. Psychol.12, 653158 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 18.Chang, Y. K., Labban, J. D., Gapin, J. I. & Etnier, J. L. The effects of acute exercise on cognitive performance: a meta-analysis. Brain Res.1453, 87–101 (2012). 10.1016/j.brainres.2012.02.068 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Haverkamp, B. F. et al. Effects of physical activity interventions on cognitive outcomes and academic performance in adolescents and young adults: a meta-analysis. J. Sports Sci.38, 2637–2660 (2020). 10.1080/02640414.2020.1794763 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Lambourne, K. & Tomporowski, P. The effect of exercise-induced arousal on cognitive task performance: a meta-regression analysis. Brain Res.1341, 12–24 (2010). 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.03.091 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.McMorris, T., Sproule, J., Turner, A. & Hale, B. J. Acute, intermediate intensity exercise, and speed and accuracy in working memory tasks: a meta-analytical comparison of effects. Physiol. Behav.102, 421–428 (2011). 10.1016/j.physbeh.2010.12.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Moreau, D. & Chou, E. The acute effect of high-intensity exercise on executive function: a meta-analysis. Perspect. Psychol. Sci.14, 734–764 (2019). 10.1177/1745691619850568 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Oberste, M. et al. Effects and moderators of acute aerobic exercise on subsequent interference control: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol.10, 2616 (2019). 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02616 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Ishihara, T., Drollette, E. S., Ludyga, S., Hillman, C. H. & Kamijo, K. The effects of acute aerobic exercise on executive function: a systematic review and meta-analysis of individual participant data. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.128, 258–269 (2021). 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.06.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Logan, N. E., Henry, D. A., Hillman, C. H. & Kramer, A. F. Trained athletes and cognitive function: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol.21, 725–749 (2023). 10.1080/1612197X.2022.2084764 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Ludyga, S., Gerber, M., Brand, S., Holsboer-Trachsler, E. & Pühse, U. Acute effects of moderate aerobic exercise on specific aspects of executive function in different age and fitness groups: a meta-analysis. Psychophysiology53, 1611–1626 (2016). 10.1111/psyp.12736 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Oberste, M., Sharma, S., Bloch, W. & Zimmer, P. Acute exercise-induced set shifting benefits in healthy adults and its moderators: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Front. Psychol.12, 2 (2021). 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.528352 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T. & Rothstein, H. R. A basic introduction to fixed-effect and random-effects models for meta-analysis. Res. Synth. Methods1, 97–111 (2010). 10.1002/jrsm.12 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Hackenberger, B. K. Bayesian meta-analysis now – let’s do it. Croat. Med. J.61, 564 (2020). 10.3325/cmj.2020.61.564 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Sutton, A. J. et al. Methods for meta-analysis in medical research methods for meta-analysis in medical research contents preface acknowledgements part a: meta-analysis methodology: the basics. https://www.researchgate.net/publication/236905532 (2000).
  • 31.Sutton, A. J. & Abrams, K. R. Bayesian methods in meta-analysis and evidence synthesis. Stat. Methods Med. Res.10, 277–303 (2001). 10.1177/096228020101000404 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Chung, Y., Rabe-Hesketh, S. & Choi, I. H. Avoiding zero between-study variance estimates in random-effects meta-analysis. Stat. Med.32, 4071–4089 (2013). 10.1002/sim.5821 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Kontopantelis, E., Springate, D. A. & Reeves, D. A re-analysis of the Cochrane Library data: the dangers of unobserved heterogeneity in meta-analyses. PloS one8, e69930 (2013). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 34.Sidik, K. & Jonkman, J. N. A comparison of heterogeneity variance estimators in combining results of studies. Stat. Med.26, 1964–1981 (2007). 10.1002/sim.2688 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Erickson, K. I. et al. Physical activity, cognition, and brain outcomes: a review of the 2018 physical activity guidelines. Med Sci. Sports Exerc.51, 1242–1251 (2019). 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001936 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Stillman, C. M., Esteban-Cornejo, I., Brown, B., Bender, C. M. & Erickson, K. I. Effects of exercise on brain and cognition across age groups and health states. Trends Neurosci.43, 533–543 (2020). 10.1016/j.tins.2020.04.010 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Thompson, S. G. & Sharp, S. J. Explaining heterogeneity in meta-analysis: a comparison of methods. Stat. Med.18, 2693–2708 (1999). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Williams, D. R., Rast, P. & Bürkner, P.-C. Bayesian meta-analysis with weakly informative prior distributions. 10.31234/OSF.IO/7TBRM (2018).
  • 39.Kruschke, J. K. & Liddell, T. M. The Bayesian new statistics: hypothesis testing, estimation, meta-analysis, and power analysis from a bayesian perspective. Psychon. Bull. Rev.25, 178–206 (2018). 10.3758/s13423-016-1221-4 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Kruschke, J. K. Doing Bayesian data analysis: a tutorial with R, JAGS, and Stan, 2nd Edn. 10.1016/B978-0-12-405888-0.09999-2 (Elsevier Science, 2014).
  • 41.Moher, D. et al. Preferred reporting items for systematic review and meta-analysis protocols (PRISMA-P) 2015 statement. Syst. Rev.20, 148–160 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Sachdev, P. S. et al. Classifying neurocognitive disorders: the DSM-5 approach. Nat. Rev. Neurol.10, 634–642 (2014). 10.1038/nrneurol.2014.181 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Garber, C. E. et al. American College of Sports Medicine position stand. Quantity and quality of exercise for developing and maintaining cardiorespiratory. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc.43, 1334–1359 (2011). 10.1249/MSS.0b013e318213fefb [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Hedges, L. V. & Olkin, I. Statistical methods for meta-analysis. 10.1016/c2009-0-03396-0 (Elsevier, 1985).
  • 45.Borenstein, M., Hedges, L. V., Higgins, J. P. T. & Rothstein, H. Introduction to meta-analysis. (Wiley, 2021).
  • 46.Higgins, J. P. T., Thompson, S. G. & Spiegelhalter, D. J. A re-evaluation of random-effects meta-analysis. J. R. Stat. Soc. Ser. A, (Stat. Soc.)172, 137 (2009). 10.1111/j.1467-985X.2008.00552.x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Röver, C. Bayesian random-effects meta-analysis using the Bayesmeta R package. J. Stat. Softw.93, 1–51 (2020).
  • 48.Bürkner, P. C. Advanced Bayesian multilevel modeling with the R package brms. R. J.10, 395–411 (2018). 10.32614/RJ-2018-017 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Makowski, D., Ben-Shachar, M. & Lüdecke, D. bayestestR: describing effects and their uncertainty, existence and significance within the bayesian framework. J. Open Source Softw.4, 1541 (2019). 10.21105/joss.01541 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Hinne, M., Gronau, Q. F., van den Bergh, D. & Wagenmakers, E.-J. A conceptual introduction to bayesian model averaging. Adv. Methods Pract. Psychol. Sci.3, 200–215 (2020). 10.1177/2515245919898657 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Schad, D. J., Betancourt, M. & Vasishth, S. Toward a principled Bayesian workflow in cognitive science. Psychol. Methods26, 103–126 (2021). 10.1037/met0000275 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Jeffreys, H. Theory of probability. 3rd Ed., Clarendon Press, Oxford. (Oxford University Press, 1961).
  • 53.Dienes, Z. How Bayes factors change scientific practice. J. Math. Psychol.10.1016/j.jmp.2015.10.003 (2016).
  • 54.Kass, R. E. & Raftery, A. E. Bayes factors. J. Am. Stat. Assoc.10.1080/01621459.1995.10476572 (1995).
  • 55.Wetzels, R. et al. Statistical evidence in experimental psychology: an empirical comparison using 855 t tests. Perspect. Psychol. Sci.6, 291–298 (2011) [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 56.Rouder, J. N., Morey, R. D., Speckman, P. L. & Province, J. M. Default Bayes factors for ANOVA designs. J. Math. Psychol.56, 356–374 (2012). 10.1016/j.jmp.2012.08.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Goldstein, M. Subjective bayesian analysis: principles and practice. Bayesian Anal. 1, 403–420 (2006).
  • 58.Depaoli, S., Winter, S. D. & Visser, M. The importance of prior sensitivity analysis in bayesian statistics: demonstrations using an interactive shiny app. Front. Psychol.11, 3271 (2020). 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.608045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Lambert, P. C., Sutton, A. J., Burton, P. R., Abrams, K. R. & Jones, D. R. How vague is vague? A simulation study of the impact of the use of vague prior distributions in MCMC using WinBUGS. Stat. Med.24, 2401–2428 (2005). 10.1002/sim.2112 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.McElreath, R. Statistical rethinking: a Bayesian course with examples in R and Stan. 10.1201/9781315372495 (2018).
  • 61.Duval, S. & Tweedie, R. Trim and fill: a simple funnel-plot-based method of testing and adjusting for publication bias in meta-analysis. Biometrics56, 455–463 (2000). 10.1111/j.0006-341X.2000.00455.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Etnier, J. et al. The influence of physical fitness and exercise upon cognitive functioning: a meta-analysis. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol.19, 249–277 (1997).
  • 63.McMorris, T. & Hale, B. J. Differential effects of differing intensities of acute exercise on speed and accuracy of cognition: a meta-analytical investigation. Brain Cogn.80, 338–351 (2012). 10.1016/j.bandc.2012.09.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Neva, J. L. et al. Acute exercise modulates the excitability of specific interneurons in human motor cortex. Neuroscience475, 103–116 (2021). 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2021.08.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Lulic, T., El-Sayes, J., Fassett, H. J. & Nelson, A. J. Physical activity levels determine exercise-induced changes in brain excitability. PLoS One12, e0173672 (2017). 10.1371/journal.pone.0173672 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Neva, J. L., Brown, K. E., Mang, C. S., Francisco, B. A. & Boyd, L. A. An acute bout of exercise modulates both intracortical and interhemispheric excitability. Eur. J. Neurosci.45, 1343–1355 (2017). 10.1111/ejn.13569 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Singh, A. M., Duncan, R. E., Neva, J. L. & Staines, W. R. Aerobic exercise modulates intracortical inhibition and facilitation in a nonexercised upper limb muscle. BMC Sports Sci. Med. Rehabil.6, 23 (2014). 10.1186/2052-1847-6-23 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Yamazaki, Y. et al. Acute low-intensity aerobic exercise modulates intracortical inhibitory and excitatory circuits in an exercised and a non-exercised muscle in the primary motor cortex. Front. Physiol.10, 1361 (2019). 10.3389/fphys.2019.01361 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Mooney, R. A. et al. Acute aerobic exercise modulates primary motor cortex inhibition. Exp. Brain Res.234, 3669–3676 (2016). 10.1007/s00221-016-4767-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Smith, A. E., Goldsworthy, M. R., Garside, T., Wood, F. M. & Ridding, M. C. The influence of a single bout of aerobic exercise on short-interval intracortical excitability. Exp. Brain Res.232, 1875–1882 (2014). 10.1007/s00221-014-3879-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.McMorris, T., Tomporowski, P. D. & Audiffren, M. Exercise and cognitive function. Wiley Blackwell, 1–377 10.1002/9780470740668 (2009)
  • 72.McMorris, T. & Hale, B. J. Is there an acute exercise-induced physiological/biochemical threshold which triggers increased speed of cognitive functioning? A meta-analytic investigation. J. Sport Health Sci.4, 4–13 (2015). 10.1016/j.jshs.2014.08.003 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Davranche K., Giraud D., Hays A., Gajdos T. Exploring Decision-making Processes: The Impact of Acute High-Intensity Aerobic Exercise. bioRxiv; 10.1101/2023.02.14.528466 (2023).
  • 74.Tomporowski, P. D. Effects of acute bouts of exercise on cognition. Acta Psychol.112, 297–324 (2003). 10.1016/S0001-6918(02)00134-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Ayaz, A., Saleem, A. B., Schölvinck, M. L. & Carandini, M. Locomotion controls spatial integration in mouse visual cortex. Curr. Biol.23, 890–894 (2013). 10.1016/j.cub.2013.04.012 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Fu, Y. et al. A cortical circuit for gain control by behavioral state. Cell156, 1139–1152 (2014). 10.1016/j.cell.2014.01.050 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 77.Kaneko, M., Fu, Y. & Stryker, M. P. Locomotion induces stimulus-specific response enhancement in adult visual cortex. J. Neurosci.37, 3532–3543 (2017). 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3760-16.2017 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Keller, G. B., Bonhoeffer, T. & Hübener, M. Sensorimotor mismatch signals in primary visual cortex of the behaving mouse. Neuron10.1016/j.neuron.2012.03.040 (2012) [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 79.Maimon, G., Straw, A. D. & Dickinson, M. H. Active flight increases the gain of visual motion processing in Drosophila. Nat. Neurosci.13, 393–399 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 80.Niell, C. M. & Stryker, M. P. Modulation of visual responses by behavioral state in mouse visual cortex. Neuron10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.033 (2010) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 81.Polack, P.-O., Friedman, J. & Golshani, P. Cellular mechanisms of brain state-dependent gain modulation in visual cortex. Nat. Neurosci.16, 1331–1339 (2013). 10.1038/nn.3464 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Bullock, T., Cecotti, H. & Giesbrecht, B. Multiple stages of information processing are modulated during acute bouts of exercise. Neuroscience307, 138–150 (2015). 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2015.08.046 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Bullock, T., Elliott, J. C., Serences, J. T. & Giesbrecht, B. Acute exercise modulates feature-selective responses in human cortex. J. Cogn. Neurosci.29, 605–618 (2017). 10.1162/jocn_a_01082 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Cao, L. & Händel, B. Walking enhances peripheral visual processing in humans. PLOS Biol.17, e3000511 (2019). 10.1371/journal.pbio.3000511 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Brouwer, G. J. & Heeger, D. J. Decoding and reconstructing color from responses in human visual cortex. J. Neurosci.29, 13992–14003 (2009). 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3577-09.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Bullock, T. et al. Eye movements disrupt EEG alpha-band coding of behaviorally relevant and irrelevant spatial locations held in working memory. J. Neurophysiol.129, 1191–1211 (2023). 10.1152/jn.00302.2021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Foster, J. J., Sutterer, D. W., Serences, J. T., Vogel, E. K. & Awh, E. The topography of alpha-band activity tracks the content of spatial working memory. J. Neurophysiol.115, 168–177 (2016). 10.1152/jn.00860.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.MacLean, M. H., Bullock, T. & Giesbrecht, B. Dual process coding of recalled locations in human oscillatory brain activity. J. Neurosci.39, 6737–6750 (2019). 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.0059-19.2019 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Samaha, J., Sprague, T. C. & Postle, B. R. Decoding and reconstructing the focus of spatial attention from the topography of alpha-band oscillations. J. Cogn. Neurosci.28, 1090–1097 (2016). 10.1162/jocn_a_00955 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Sprague, T. C., Saproo, S. & Serences, J. T. Visual attention mitigates information loss in small- and large-scale neural codes. Trends Cogn. Sci.19, 215–226 (2015). 10.1016/j.tics.2015.02.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Garrett, J., Bullock, T. & Giesbrecht, B. Tracking the contents of spatial working memory during an acute bout of aerobic exercise. J. Cogn. Neurosci.33, 1271–1286 (2021). 10.1162/jocn_a_01714 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Mordkoff, J. T. & Egeth, H. E. Response time and accuracy revisited: converging support for the interactive race model. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.19, 981–991 (1993). 10.1037/0096-1523.19.5.981 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Santee, J. L. & Egeth, H. E. Do reaction time and accuracy measure the same aspects of letter recognition? J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform.8, 489–501 (1982). 10.1037/0096-1523.8.4.489 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Basso, J. C., Shang, A., Elman, M., Karmouta, R. & Suzuki, W. A. Acute exercise improves prefrontal cortex but not hippocampal function in healthy adults. J. Int. Neuropsychol. Soc.21, 791–801 (2015). 10.1017/S135561771500106X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Chang, Y. K. et al. Effect of acute aerobic exercise on cognitive performance: role of cardiovascular fitness. Psychol. Sport Exerc.15, 464–470 (2014). 10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.04.007 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.Davranche, K., Hall, B. & McMorris, T. Effect of acute exercise on cognitive control required during an Eriksen Flanker task. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol.31, 628–639 (2009). 10.1123/jsep.31.5.628 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 97.Faulkner, J., Lambrick, D., Kaufmann, S. & Stoner, L. Effects of upright and recumbent cycling on executive function and prefrontal cortex oxygenation in young healthy men. J. Phys. Act. health13, 882–887 (2016). 10.1123/jpah.2015-0454 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 98.Kunzler, M. R. & Carpes, F. P. Intense cycling exercise improves acute cognitive responses. Int. J. Sports Med.41, 879–884 (2020). 10.1055/a-1114-6170 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 99.Yanagisawa, H. et al. Acute moderate exercise elicits increased dorsolateral prefrontal activation and improves cognitive performance with Stroop test. NeuroImage50, 1702–1710 (2010). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2009.12.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 100.Hung, T. M., Tsai, C. L., Chen, F. T., Wang, C. C. & Chang, Y. K. The immediate and sustained effects of acute exercise on planning aspect of executive function. Psychol. Sport Exerc.14, 728–736 (2013). 10.1016/j.psychsport.2013.05.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 101.Bae, S. & Masaki, H. Effects of acute aerobic exercise on cognitive flexibility required during task-switching paradigm. Front. Hum. Neurosci.13, 1–9 (2019). 10.3389/fnhum.2019.00260 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 102.Pesce, C. & Audiffren, M. Does acute exercise switch off switch costs? A study with younger and older athletes. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol.33, 609–626 (2011). 10.1123/jsep.33.5.609 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 103.McMorris, T. Exercise and decision-making in team games. Exercise and cognitive function 179–192 10.1002/9780470740668.CH9 (2009)
  • 104.Kao, S. C., Drollette, E. S., Ritondale, J. P., Khan, N. & Hillman, C. H. The acute effects of high-intensity interval training and moderate-intensity continuous exercise on declarative memory and inhibitory control. Psychol. Sport Exerc.38, 90–99 (2018). 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.05.011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 105.Kujach, S. et al. Acute sprint interval exercise increases both cognitive functions and peripheral neurotrophic factors in humans: the possible involvement of lactate. Front. Neurosci.13, 1455 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 106.Dietrich, A. & Audiffren, M. The reticular-activating hypofrontality (RAH) model of acute exercise. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.35, 1305–1325 (2011). 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2011.02.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 107.Mehren, A. et al. Intensity-dependent effects of acute exercise on executive function. Neural Plast.2019, 1–17 (2019). 10.1155/2019/8608317 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 108.Kao, S.-C., Wang, C.-H., Kamijo, K., Khan, N. & Hillman, C. Acute effects of highly intense interval and moderate continuous exercise on the modulation of neural oscillation during working memory. Int. J. Psychophysiol.160, 10–17 (2021). 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2020.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 109.Rattray, B. & Smee, D. J. The effect of high and low exercise intensity periods on a simple memory recognition test. J. Sport Health Sci.5, 342 (2016). 10.1016/j.jshs.2015.01.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 110.Li, L. et al. Acute aerobic exercise increases cortical activity during working memory: a functional MRI study in female college students. PLoS One9, e99222 (2014). 10.1371/journal.pone.0099222 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 111.Weng, T. B. et al. The acute effects of aerobic exercise on the functional connectivity of human brain networks. Brain Plast.2, 171–190 (2017). 10.3233/BPL-160039 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 112.Baniqued, P. L. et al. Brain network modularity predicts exercise-related executive function gains in older adults. Front. Aging Neurosci.9, 426 (2018). 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00426 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 113.Firth, J. et al. Effect of aerobic exercise on hippocampal volume in humans: a systematic review and meta-analysis. NeuroImage166, 230–238 (2018). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 114.Voss, M. W., Nagamatsu, L. S., Liu-Ambrose, T. & Kramer, A. F. Exercise, brain, and cognition across the life span. J. Appl. Physiol.111, 1505–1513 (2011). 10.1152/japplphysiol.00210.2011 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 115.Voss, M. W. et al. Neurobiological markers of exercise-related brain plasticity in older adults. Brain Behav. Immun.28, 90–99 (2013). 10.1016/j.bbi.2012.10.021 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 116.Ludyga, S., Gerber, M., Pühse, U., Looser, V. N. & Kamijo, K. Systematic review and meta-analysis investigating moderators of long-term effects of exercise on cognition in healthy individuals. Nat. Hum. Behav.4, 603–612 (2020) [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 117.Ferguson, H. J., Brunsdon, V. E. A. & Bradford, E. E. F. The developmental trajectories of executive function from adolescence to old age. Sci. Rep.11, 1382 (2021). 10.1038/s41598-020-80866-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 118.Hartshorne, J. K. & Germine, L. T. When does cognitive functioning peak? The asynchronous rise and fall of different cognitive abilities across the lifespan. Psychol. Sci.26, 433–443 (2015). 10.1177/0956797614567339 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 119.Bullock, T., Giesbrecht, B., Beaudin, A. E., Goodyear, B. G. & Poulin, M. J. Effects of changes in end-tidal PO2 and PCO2 on neural responses during rest and sustained attention. Physiol. Rep.9, e15106 (2021). 10.14814/phy2.15106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 120.Shields, G. S., Sazma, M. A. & Yonelinas, A. P. The effects of acute stress on core executive functions: a meta-analysis and comparison with cortisol. Neurosci. Biobehav Rev.68, 651–668 (2016). 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2016.06.038 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 121.Gordon, J. et al. The road towards understanding embodied decisions. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev.131, 722–736 (2021). 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2021.09.034 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 122.Raichlen, D. A. & Alexander, G. E. Adaptive capacity: an evolutionary-neuroscience model linking exercise, cognition, and brain health. Trends Neurosci.40, 408–421 (2017). 10.1016/j.tins.2017.05.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 123.Raudenbush, S. W. & Bryk, A. S. Hierarchical linear models: applications and data analysis methods, 485 (2002).
  • 124.Hogervorst, E., Riedel, W., Jeukendrup, A. & Jolles, J. Cognitive performance after strenuous physical exercise. Percept. Mot. Skills83, 479–488 (1996). 10.2466/pms.1996.83.2.479 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 125.Pesce, C., Capranica, L., Tessitore, A. & Figura, F. Focusing of visual attention under submaximal physical load. Int. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol.1, 275–292 (2003). 10.1080/1612197X.2003.9671719 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 126.Pesce, C., Tessitore, A., Casella, R., Pirritano, M. & Capranica, L. Focusing of visual attention at rest and during physical exercise in soccer players. J. Sports Sci.25, 1259–1270 (2007). 10.1080/02640410601040085 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 127.Vickers, J. N. & Williams, A. M. Performing under pressure: the effects of physiological arousal, cognitive anxiety, and gaze control in biathlon. J. Mot. Behav.39, 381–394 (2007). 10.3200/JMBR.39.5.381-394 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 128.Lo Bue-Estes, C. et al. Short-term exercise to exhaustion and its effects on cognitive function in young women. Percept. Mot. Skills107, 933–945 (2008). 10.2466/pms.107.3.933-945 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 129.Coles, K. & Tomporowski, P. D. Effects of acute exercise on executive processing, short-term and long-term memory. J. Sports Sci.26, 333–344 (2008). 10.1080/02640410701591417 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 130.Fontana, F. E., Mazzardo, O., Mokgothu, C., Furtado, O. & Gallagher, J. D. Influence of exercise intensity on the decision-making performance of experienced and inexperienced soccer players. J. Sport Exerc Psychol.31, 135–151 (2009). 10.1123/jsep.31.2.135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 131.Luft, C. D. B., Takase, E. & Darby, D. Heart rate variability and cognitive function: effects of physical effort. Biol. Psychol.82, 186–191 (2009). 10.1016/j.biopsycho.2009.07.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 132.Pontifex, M. B., Hillman, C. H., Fernhall, B., Thompson, K. M. & Valentini, T. A. The effect of acute aerobic and resistance exercise on working memory. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc.41, 927–934 (2009). 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181907d69 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 133.Srygley, J. M., Mirelman, A., Herman, T., Giladi, N. & Hausdorff, J. M. When does walking alter thinking? Age and task associated findings. Brain Res.1253, 92–99 (2009). 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.11.067 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 134.Thomson, K., Watt, A. P. & Liukkonen, J. Differences in ball sports athletes speed discrimination skills before and after exercise induced fatigue. J. Sports Sci. Med.8, 259–264 (2009). [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 135.Norling, J. C., Sibthorp, J., Suchy, Y., Hannon, J. C. & Ruddell, E. The benefit of recreational physical activity to restore attentional fatigue: the effects of running intensity level on attention scores. J. Leis. Res.42, 135–152 (2010). 10.1080/00222216.2010.11950198 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 136.Young, W. et al. Acute effect of exercise on kicking accuracy in elite Australian football players. J. Sci. Med. Sport13, 85–89 (2010). 10.1016/j.jsams.2008.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 137.Chang, Y.-K. et al. Effects of acute exercise on executive function: a study with a tower of London task. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol.33, 847–865 (2011). 10.1123/jsep.33.6.847 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 138.Green, A. L. & Helton, W. S. Dual-task performance during a climbing traverse. Exp. Brain Res.215, 307–313 (2011). 10.1007/s00221-011-2898-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 139.Ohlinger, C. M., Horn, T. S., Berg, W. P. & Cox, R. H. The effect of active workstation use on measures of cognition, attention, and motor skill. J. Phys. Act. Health8, 119–125 (2011). 10.1123/jpah.8.1.119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 140.Hope, L., Lewinski, W., Dixon, J., Blocksidge, D. & Gabbert, F. Witnesses in action: the effect of physical exertion on recall and recognition. Psychol. Sci.23, 386–390 (2012). 10.1177/0956797611431463 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 141.Lambourne, K. The effects of acute exercise on temporal generalization. Q J. Exp. Psychol.65, 526–540 (2012). 10.1080/17470218.2011.605959 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 142.Moore, R. D., Romine, M. W., O’connor, P. J. & Tomporowski, P. D. The influence of exercise-induced fatigue on cognitive function. J. Sports Sci.30, 841–850 (2012). 10.1080/02640414.2012.675083 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 143.Roberts, A. P. J. & Cole, J. C. The effects of exercise and body armor on cognitive function in healthy volunteers. Mil. Med.178, 479–486 (2013). 10.7205/MILMED-D-12-00385 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 144.Bullock, T. & Giesbrecht, B. Acute exercise and aerobic fitness influence selective attention during visual search. Front. Psychol.5, 1290 (2014). 10.3389/fpsyg.2014.01290 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 145.Byun, K. et al. Positive effect of acute mild exercise on executive function via arousal-related prefrontal activations: an fNIRS study. NeuroImage98, 336–345 (2014). 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2014.04.067 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 146.Darling, K. A. & Helton, W. S. Dual-task interference between climbing and a simulated communication task. Exp. Brain Res.232, 1367–1377 (2014). 10.1007/s00221-014-3855-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 147.Nibbeling, N., Oudejans, R. R. D., Ubink, E. M. & Daanen, H. A. M. The effects of anxiety and exercise-induced fatigue on shooting accuracy and cognitive performance in infantry soldiers. Ergonomics57, 1366–1379 (2014). 10.1080/00140139.2014.924572 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 148.Pontifex, M. B., Parks, A. C., Henning, D. A. & Kamijo, K. Single bouts of exercise selectively sustain attentional processes. Psychophysiology52, 618–625 (2015). 10.1111/psyp.12395 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 149.Schmidt-Kassow, M. et al. Treadmill walking during vocabulary encoding improves verbal long-term memory. Behav. Brain Funct.10, 24 (2014). 10.1186/1744-9081-10-24 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 150.Tsai, C. L. et al. Executive function and endocrinological responses to acute resistance exercise. Front. Behav. Neurosci.8, 1–12 (2014). 10.3389/fnbeh.2014.00262 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 151.Bantoft, C. et al. Effect of standing or walking at a workstation on cognitive function: a randomized counterbalanced trial. Hum. Factors58, 140–149 (2016). 10.1177/0018720815605446 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 152.Larson, M. J. et al. Slow walking on a treadmill desk does not negatively affect executive abilities: an examination of cognitive control, conflict adaptation, response inhibition, and post-error slowing. Front. Psychol.6, 723 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 153.Osgood, J. M. Acute cardiovascular exercise counteracts the effect of ego-depletion on attention: how ego-depletion increases boredom and compromises directed attention. Int. J. Psychol. Stud.7, 85–96 (2015). 10.5539/ijps.v7n3p85 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 154.Perciavalle, V., Maci, T., Perciavalle, V., Massimino, S. & Coco, M. Working memory and blood lactate levels. Neurol. Sci.36, 2129–2136 (2015). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 155.Shia, R. M. et al. Individual differences in biophysiological toughness: sustaining working memory during physical exhaustion. Mil. Med.180, 230–236 (2015). 10.7205/MILMED-D-14-00363 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 156.Stevens, D. J., Arciuli, J. & Anderson, D. I. Concurrent movement impairs incidental but not intentional statistical learning. Cogn. Sci.39, 1081–1098 (2015). 10.1111/cogs.12180 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 157.Weng, T. B., Pierce, G. L., Darling, W. G. & Voss, M. W. Differential effects of acute exercise on distinct aspects of executive function. Med. Sci. Sports Exerc.47, 1460–1469 (2015). 10.1249/MSS.0000000000000542 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 158.Alloway, R. G., Alloway, T. P., Magyari, P. M. & Floyd, S. An exploratory study investigating the effects of barefoot running on working memory. Percept. Mot. Skills122, 432–443 (2016). 10.1177/0031512516640391 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 159.Ando, S. et al. Effects of strenuous exercise on visual perception are independent of visual resolution. Physiol. Behav.106, 117–121 (2012). 10.1016/j.physbeh.2012.01.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 160.Brush, C. J. et al. Dose–response and time course effects of acute resistance exercise on executive function. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol.38, 396–408 (2016). 10.1123/jsep.2016-0027 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 161.Connell, C. J. W., Thompson, B., Kuhn, G. & Gant, N. Exercise-induced fatigue and caffeine supplementation affect psychomotor performance but not covert visuo-spatial attention. PLoS One11, e0165318 (2016) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 162.Hsieh, S.-S., Chang, Y.-K., Hung, T.-M. & Fang, C.-L. The effects of acute resistance exercise on young and older males’ working memory. Psychol. Sport Exerc.22, 286–293 (2016). 10.1016/j.psychsport.2015.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 163.Hsieh, S.-S. Acute resistance exercise facilitates attention control in adult males without an age-moderating effect. J. Sport Exerc. Psychol.38, 247–254 (2016). 10.1123/jsep.2015-0282 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 164.Lowe, C. J., Kolev, D. & Hall, P. A. An exploration of exercise-induced cognitive enhancement and transfer effects to dietary self-control. Brain Cogn.110, 102–111 (2016). 10.1016/j.bandc.2016.04.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 165.Torbeyns, T. et al. Cycling on a bike desk positively influences cognitive performance. PloS One10.1371/journal.pone.0165510 (2016) [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 166.Tsukamoto, H. et al. Greater impact of acute high-intensity interval exercise on post-exercise executive function compared to moderate-intensity continuous exercise. Physiol. Behav.155, 224–230 (2016). 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.12.021 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 167.Zach, S. & Shalom, E. The influence of acute physical activity on working memory. Percept. Mot. Skills122, 365–374 (2016). 10.1177/0031512516631066 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 168.Chang, Y.-K. et al. Acute exercise has a general facilitative effect on cognitive function: a combined ERP temporal dynamics and BDNF study. Psychophysiology54, 289–300 (2017). 10.1111/psyp.12784 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 169.Crush, E. A. & Loprinzi, P. D. Dose-response effects of exercise duration and recovery on cognitive functioning. Percept. Mot. Skills124, 1164–1193 (2017). 10.1177/0031512517726920 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 170.González Fernández, F. T., Etnier, J., Zabala, M. & Sanabria, D. Vigilance performance during acute exercise. Int. J. Sport Psychol.48, (2017).
  • 171.Lindheimer, J. B., OʼConnor, P. J., McCully, K. K. & Dishman, R. K. The effect of light-intensity cycling on mood and working memory in response to a randomized, placebo-controlled design. Psychosom. Med.79, 243–253 (2017). 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000381 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 172.Lowe, C. J., Staines, W. R. & Hall, P. A. Effects of moderate exercise on cortical resilience: a transcranial magnetic stimulation study targeting the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Psychosom. Med.79, 143–152 (2017). 10.1097/PSY.0000000000000361 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 173.Luu, K. & Hall, P. A. Examining the acute effects of hatha yoga and mindfulness meditation on executive function and mood. Mindfulness8, 873–880 (2017). 10.1007/s12671-016-0661-2 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 174.Randolph, D. D. & O’Connor, P. J. Stair walking is more energizing than low dose caffeine in sleep deprived young women. Physiol. Behav.174, 128–135 (2017). 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.03.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 175.Slutsky, A. B. et al. The effects of low-intensity cycling on cognitive performance following sleep deprivation. Physiol. Behav.180, 25–30 (2017). 10.1016/j.physbeh.2017.07.033 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 176.Sudo, M. et al. Executive function after exhaustive exercise. Eur. J. Appl Physiol.117, 2029–2038 (2017). 10.1007/s00421-017-3692-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 177.Cuttler, C., Connolly, C. P., LaFrance, E. M. & Lowry, T. M. Resist forgetting: effects of aerobic and resistance exercise on prospective and retrospective memory. Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol.7, 205 (2017). [Google Scholar]
  • 178.Daikoku, T., Takahashi, Y., Tarumoto, N. & Yasuda, H. Auditory statistical learning during concurrent physical exercise and the tolerance for pitch, tempo, and rhythm changes. Mot. Control22, 233–244 (2018). 10.1123/mc.2017-0006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 179.Elkana, O. et al. Conscientiousness is associated with improvement in visuospatial working memory and mood following acute physical exercise: a randomized controlled trial. Pers. Individ. Differ.132, 126–132 (2018). 10.1016/j.paid.2018.05.018 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 180.Fenesi, B., Lucibello, K., Kim, J. A. & Heisz, J. J. Sweat so you don’t forget: exercise breaks during a university lecture increase on-task attention and learning. J. Appl. Res. Mem. Cogn.7, 261–269 (2018). 10.1016/j.jarmac.2018.01.012 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 181.Kendall, B. The effects of acute exercise on postural control, information processing, motor skill acquisition, and executive function. Wayne State University Dissertations (2018).
  • 182.Legrand, F. D. et al. Brief aerobic exercise immediately enhances visual attentional control and perceptual speed. Testing the mediating role of feelings of energy. Acta Psychol. (Amst.)191, 25–31 (2018). 10.1016/j.actpsy.2018.08.020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 183.Samani, A. & Heath, M. Executive-related oculomotor control is improved following a 10-min single-bout of aerobic exercise: evidence from the antisaccade task. Neuropsychologia108, 73–81 (2018). 10.1016/j.neuropsychologia.2017.11.029 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 184.Siddiqui, A. & Loprinzi, P. D. Experimental investigation of the time course effects of acute exercise on false episodic memory. J. Clin. Med.7, 157 (2018). 10.3390/jcm7070157 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 185.Sng, E., Frith, E. & Loprinzi, P. D. Temporal effects of acute walking exercise on learning and memory function. Am. J. Health Promot.32, 1518–1525 (2018). 10.1177/0890117117749476 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 186.Wade, B. & Loprinzi, P. D. The experimental effects of acute exercise on long-term emotional memory. J. Clin. Med.7, 486 (2018). 10.3390/jcm7120486 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 187.Yamazaki, Y. et al. Inter-individual differences in working memory improvement after acute mild and moderate aerobic exercise. PLoS One13, e0210053 (2018). 10.1371/journal.pone.0210053 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 188.Baker, R., Coenen, P., Howie, E., Williamson, A. & Straker, L. The musculoskeletal and cognitive effects of under-desk cycling compared to sitting for office workers. Appl. Ergon.79, 76–85 (2019). 10.1016/j.apergo.2019.04.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 189.Du Rietz, E. et al. Beneficial effects of acute high-intensity exercise on electrophysiological indices of attention processes in young adult men. Behav. Brain Res.359, 474–484 (2019). 10.1016/j.bbr.2018.11.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 190.Engeroff, T., Niederer, D., Vogt, L. & Banzer, W. Intensity and workload related dose-response effects of acute resistance exercise on domain-specific cognitive function and affective response – a four-armed randomized controlled crossover trial. Psychol. Sport Exerc.43, 55–63 (2019). 10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.12.009 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 191.Haynes, J. T., Frith, E., Sng, E. & Loprinzi, P. D. Experimental effects of acute exercise on episodic memory function: considerations for the timing of exercise. Psychol. Rep.122, 1744–1754 (2019). 10.1177/0033294118786688 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 192.Johnson, L. & Loprinzi, P. D. The effects of acute exercise on episodic memory function among young university students: moderation considerations by biological sex. Health Promot. Perspect.9, 99–104 (2019). 10.15171/hpp.2019.14 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 193.McGowan, A. L., Chandler, M. C. & Brascamp, J. W. & Pontifex, M. B. Pupillometric indices of locus-coeruleus activation are not modulated following single bouts of exercise. Int. J. Psychophysiol.140, 41–52 (2019). 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.04.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 194.Schmidt, S. L. et al. Decrease in attentional performance after repeated bouts of high intensity exercise in association-football referees and assistant referees. Front. Psychol.10, 2014 (2019). 10.3389/fpsyg.2019.02014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 195.Stenling, A., Moylan, A., Fulton, E. & Machado, L. Effects of a brief stair-climbing intervention on cognitive performance and mood states in healthy young adults. Front. Psychol.10, 2300 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 196.Wu, C.-H. et al. Effects of acute aerobic and resistance exercise on executive function: an ERP study. J. Sci. Med. Sport22, 1367–1372 (2019). 10.1016/j.jsams.2019.07.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 197.Zhou, F. & Qin, C. Acute moderate-intensity exercise generally enhances attentional resources related to perceptual processing. Front. Psychol.10, 2547 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 198.Aly, M. & Kojima, H. Acute moderate-intensity exercise generally enhances neural resources related to perceptual and cognitive processes: a randomized controlled ERP study. Ment. Health Phys. Act.19, 100363 (2020). 10.1016/j.mhpa.2020.100363 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 199.Chacko, S. C. et al. A single bout of vigorous-intensity aerobic exercise affects reactive, but not proactive cognitive brain functions. Int. J. Psychophysiol.147, 233–243 (2020). 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2019.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 200.Morris, T. P. et al. Light aerobic exercise modulates executive function and cortical excitability. Eur. J. Neurosci.51, 1723–1734 (2020). 10.1111/ejn.14593 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 201.Walsh, J. J. et al. Acute aerobic exercise impairs aspects of cognitive function at high altitude. Physiol. Behav.223, 112979 (2020). 10.1016/j.physbeh.2020.112979 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 202.Kim, H. J., Bae, S., Huh, J. H., Lee, J. W. & Han, D. H. Hemodynamic changes in response to aerobic exercise: near-infrared spectroscopy study. Int. J. Sports Med.42, 377–385 (2021). 10.1055/a-1198-8465 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 203.Klatt, S. & Smeeton, N. J. Attentional and perceptual capabilities are affected by high physical load in a simulated soccer decision-making task. Sport Exerc. Perform. Psychol.10, 205–216 (2021). [Google Scholar]
  • 204.Kuhne, L. A. et al. The effects of acute cardiovascular exercise on memory and its associations with exercise-induced increases in neurotrophic factors. Front. Aging Neurosci.13, 750401 (2021). 10.3389/fnagi.2021.750401 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 205.Manocchio, F. & Lowe, C. J. Investigating cortical buffering effects of acute moderate intensity exercise: a cTBS study targeting the left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex. Front. Hum. Neurosci.15, 645326 (2021). 10.3389/fnhum.2021.645326 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 206.Miyashiro, S. et al. Activation of the orbitofrontal cortex by both meditation and exercise: a near-infrared spectroscopy study. PLoS One16, e0247685 (2021). 10.1371/journal.pone.0247685 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 207.Trammell, J. P. & Aguilar, S. C. Natural is not always better: the varied effects of a natural environment and exercise on affect and cognition. Front. Psychol.11, 575245 (2020). 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.575245 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 208.Zhu, Y., Sun, F., Chiu, M. M. & Siu, A. Y.-S. Effects of high-intensity interval exercise and moderate-intensity continuous exercise on executive function of healthy young males. Physiol. Behav.239, 113505 (2021). 10.1016/j.physbeh.2021.113505 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 209.Aguirre-Loaiza, H. et al. Effect of acute physical exercise on inhibitory control in young adults: high-intensity indoor cycling session. Physiol. Behav.254, 113902 (2022). 10.1016/j.physbeh.2022.113902 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 210.Drollette, E. S. & Meadows, C. C. The effects of acute high-intensity interval exercise on the temporal dynamics of working memory and contralateral delay activity. Psychophysiology59, e14112 (2022). 10.1111/psyp.14112 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 211.Engeroff, T., Banzer, W. & Niederer, D. The impact of regular activity and exercise intensity on the acute effects of resistance exercise on cognitive function. Scand. J. Med. Sci. Sports32, 94–105 (2022). 10.1111/sms.14050 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 212.Frith, E., Miller, S. E. & Loprinzi, P. D. Effects of verbal priming with acute exercise on convergent creativity. Psychol. Rep.125, 375–397 (2022). 10.1177/0033294120981925 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 213.Kao, S.-C. et al. Acute effects of aerobic exercise on conflict suppression, response inhibition, and processing efficiency underlying inhibitory control processes: an ERP and SFT study. Psychophysiology59, e14032 (2022). 10.1111/psyp.14032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 214.LaCount, P. A. et al. Acute effects of physical exercise on cognitive and psychological functioning in college students with attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder. Ment. Health Phys. Act.22, 100443 (2022). 10.1016/j.mhpa.2022.100443 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 215.Loprinzi, P. D. & Storm, B. C. Acute exercise on memory: application of the retrieval-induced forgetting paradigm. Psychol. Res.87, 1417–1428 (2023). 10.1007/s00426-022-01758-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 216.Shirzad, M. et al. Passive exercise increases cerebral blood flow velocity and supports a postexercise executive function benefit. Psychophysiology59, e14132 (2022). 10.1111/psyp.14132 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 217.Zheng, K. et al. Changes in working memory performance and cortical activity during acute aerobic exercise in young adults. Front. Behav. Neurosci.16, 884490 (2022). 10.3389/fnbeh.2022.884490 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 218.Chueh, T.-Y., Hung, C.-L., Chang, Y.-K., Huang, C.-J. & Hung, T.-M. Effects of cognitive demand during acute exercise on inhibitory control and its electrophysiological indices: a randomized crossover study. Physiol. Behav.265, 114148 (2023). 10.1016/j.physbeh.2023.114148 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 219.Yamada, Y. et al. Improved interference control after exercise with blood flow restriction and cooling is associated with but not mediated by increased lactate. Physiol. Behav.270, 114291 (2023). 10.1016/j.physbeh.2023.114291 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 220.Zhang, B., Meng, X., Yu, Y., Han, Y. & Liu, Y. Effects of moderate-to-vigorous acute exercise on conscious perception and visual awareness. J. Mot. Behav.55, 262–268 (2023). 10.1080/00222895.2023.2168602 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 221.Zhang, D., Jin, X., Wang, L. & Jin, Y. Form and color visual perception in green exercise: positive effects on attention, mood, and self-esteem. J. Environ. Psychol.88, 102028 (2023). 10.1016/j.jenvp.2023.102028 [DOI] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Peer Review File (3.6MB, pdf)
Supplementary Material (104.3KB, pdf)

Data Availability Statement

Data have been made publicly available at https://github.com/jggarrett23/PACMAn.

Code has been made publicly available at https://github.com/jggarrett23/PACMAn.


Articles from Communications Psychology are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES