Abstract
Introduction
This study explored whether engagement with Creating Arts and Crafting (CAC) predicted subjective wellbeing and loneliness, above and beyond known sociodemographic predictors.
Methods
Secondary data from 7,182 adults living in England from the Taking Part Survey (a 2019–2020 UK household survey of culture and sport participation) were analyzed. Hierarchical Linear regressions were used to explore the predictive effect of engagement with CAC on aspects of subjective wellbeing (anxiety, happiness, life satisfaction and a sense that life is worthwhile) and loneliness. Covariates included gender, Index of Multiple Deprivation (IMD), age group, health and employment status.
Results
Engaging in CAC significantly predicted increased life satisfaction, a sense that life is worthwhile and happiness, above and beyond known sociodemographic predictors.
Conclusion
Our study provides support for the wellbeing benefits of engagement with creating arts and crafting, and we suggest that this will be a useful tool at a public health level, noting that relative accessibility and affordability creating arts and crafting.
Keywords: wellbeing, arts and crafts, life satisfaction, happiness, worthwhile life
1. Introduction
Since the COVID-19 pandemic, mental health provisions are failing to meet the growing demand on their services from people in need (1). Wellbeing describes how positive an individual’s psychological state is, which encompasses both feeling well [positive emotions and life satisfaction; (2)] and doing well [a sense of fulfillment; (3)]. Positive wellbeing is associated with a multitude of beneficial outcomes, including higher educational achievement (4), better physical health outcomes (5), more positive physical health behaviors (6) and reduced mortality (7). Loneliness is a negative psychological experience as a consequence of a deficit in high quality social connection (8, 9), and is associated with lower quality of life (10), as well as negative health behaviors and outcomes, such as substance misuse, smoking, eating disorders, depression, stress and premature death (8, 9). Any interventions to combat the profound public health issues of low wellbeing and high loneliness could therefore have a significant impact.
One suggested tool to improve wellbeing and combat loneliness is engagement with Creating Arts and Crafting (CAC). CAC can refer to numerous activities, such as pottery, drawing, painting, knitting, sewing, and crochet. There is wide-scale public interest in CAC, with British TV programs, such as The Great British Sewing Bee and All that Glitters populating prime time television slots. Approximately 20% of the British population are estimated to engage in arts and crafts (11). The arts and crafts industry contributes about £3.4 billion to the UK economy (12) and is forecast to be worth $50.91 billion worldwide by 2024 (13). Given its popularity, the unique purpose of this paper is to address gaps in the literature and directly assess the benefits of engagement in CAC on subjective wellbeing and loneliness, above and beyond known sociodemographic variable predictors within the general population.
Engagement with CAC is not a new intervention to improve mental health and wellbeing in clinical populations [e.g., (14)]. Indeed, CAC has been found to help with mental health issues (14, 15), enhance subjective wellbeing (16–20) and reportedly reduce suicidal tendencies (21). Therefore, not only is engagement with CAC of public interest, but it may also bring profound wellbeing benefits. As such, it has potential to be an optimal wellbeing intervention. However, while a substantial body of work has demonstrated the positive outcomes of engaging with CAC, most studies have investigated therapeutic interventions in clinical populations, only explored specific types of crafts and/or utilized small sample sizes, often with a qualitative approach [e.g., (14, 22–28)]. Thus, the extent to which the broad spectrum of CAC activities quantitatively contributes to wellbeing and loneliness outcomes in the everyday lives of the general population is comparatively understudied, and is the focus of this paper.
Less studied is whether engagement with CAC might be associated with reduced loneliness. Mindfulness is known to be effective in reducing loneliness [e.g., (29)], and there are parallels between mindfulness and CAC; motivations to engage in creative activities, including crafting, often center around their use as a tool to disengage from unwanted thoughts and feelings and to direct and focus attention onto the task at hand (30). This has been applied in therapies that use arts to facilitate mindfulness (31, 32). Considering the parallels between mindfulness and people’s motivations to engage in creative tasks, we seek to investigate here whether engagement with CAC can be effective in reducing loneliness.
1.1. Research question, aims and hypothesis
The current study aims to address these gaps in the literature. To this end we utilize a dataset from the Taking Part survey, an annual survey conducted by the UK Department for Culture, Media and Sport, which assesses a sample of the general population’s engagement in cultural, digital and sporting activities. We aim to quantitatively investigate whether engagement with general (rather than specific) CAC acts as a protective factor to enhance subjective wellbeing and reduce loneliness in a general (rather than clinical) population in a large, representative sample.
The current study controls for sociodemographic variables that are already known to be linked to subjective wellbeing and loneliness outcomes (33): level of deprivation, gender, age group, health and employment status. For instance, lower wellbeing and greater loneliness have been linked to greater deprivation (34, 35), poorer health (36, 37), and unemployment (38, 39). Loneliness also increases with age (40–42), with subjective wellbeing showing a U-shaped curve in high income countries, rising from the 45–54 age range onwards (36). Studies on gender differences in wellbeing and loneliness are more mixed [e.g., (42–45)] and sometimes yield only small effect sizes [e.g., (44)]. We include these sociodemographic variables in our analyses to: (a) control for statistical relationships between these known sociodemographic predictors and wellbeing/loneliness, and (b) to assess the relationship between engagement with CAC and wellbeing/loneliness above and beyond these known sociodemographic predictors. The latter enables us to determine by how much engagement with CAC contributes to subjective wellbeing and loneliness, in comparison to known sociodemographic predictors and their effect sizes. This assessment of magnitude allows us to evaluate how beneficial engagement with CAC may be to wellbeing and loneliness in a real world setting.
Wellbeing is measured here through Subjective Wellbeing (SWB), which comprises the variables of happiness, anxiety, life satisfaction and a sense that life is worthwhile, in line with the UK Office for National Statistics [ONS; e.g., (46)], as these variables provide a good measure of both feeling well [positive emotions and life satisfaction; (2)] and doing well [a sense of fulfillment; (3)]. The ONS has previously outlined the rationale for using these questions to measure subjective wellbeing (47). Our key research question asks: Does engagement with CAC predict SWB and loneliness above and beyond sociodemographic predictors of gender, deprivation, age group, health and employment? We predict that engagement with creating arts and crafting (vs. no engagement) will be associated with greater SWB and lower loneliness, over and above known sociodemographic predictors.
2. Methods
2.1. Participants
A random representative sample of 7,182 individuals were derived from the Taking Part Survey, a face-to-face household survey undertaken prior to COVID, in Year 13 (April 2019–March 2020), by the Department of Digital, Culture, Media and Sport (48). This dataset is available from the UK Data Service (48). The participants were aged 16 and over and living in England. Although the original dataset has 7,502 participants, the final sample size used in the main analyses for the current study was 7,182 due to missing data on outcome variables and/or covariates from 320 participants. Table 1 reports participants’ characteristics.
Table 1.
Characteristic | N (%) (Total N = 7,182) |
Gender, N (%) | |
Female | 3,902 (54.3%) |
Male | 3,280 (45.7%) |
Age group, N (%) | |
Not reported | 36 (0.5%) |
16–19 | 21 (0.3%) |
20–24 | 172 (2.4%) |
25–34 | 925 (12.9%) |
35–44 | 1,229 (17.1%) |
45–54 | 1,291 (18%) |
55–64 | 1,285 (17.9%) |
65–74 | 1,223 (17%) |
75–84 | 756 (10.5%) |
85+ | 244 (3.4%) |
IMD decile (scale = 1–10), N (%) | |
1 (relatively most deprived decile) | 732 (10.2%) |
2 | 722 (10.1%) |
3 | 613 (8.5%) |
4 | 650 (9.1%) |
5 | 772 (10.7%) |
6 | 707 (9.8%) |
7 | 770 (10.7%) |
8 | 775 (10.8%) |
9 | 778 (10.8%) |
10 (relatively least deprived decile) | 663 (9.2%) |
Self-reported poor health (scale = 1–5), mean (SE) | 2.11 (0.012) |
In employment | |
Yes, N (%) | 3,995 (55.6%) |
No, N (%) | 3,187 (44.4%) |
Engagement with creating arts and crafting | |
Yes, N (%) | 2,689 (37.4%) |
No, N (%) | 4,493 (62.6%) |
Satisfaction (scale = 1–10), mean (SE) | 7.76 (0.022) |
Happy (scale = 1–10), mean (SE) | 7.64 (0.025) |
Worthwhile (scale = 1–10), mean (SE) | 8.00 (0.021) |
Anxiety (scale = 1–10), mean (SE) | 2.79 (0.035) |
Lonely (scale = 1–5), mean (SE) | 2.22 (0.014) |
Index of multiple deprivation (IMD), Number (N), Standard error (SE).
2.2. Measures
2.2.1. Sociodemographics
As per Table 1, participants reported sociodemographic data regarding their age (categorical), gender (binary: male vs. female) and home postcode. The latter informed an index of multiple deprivation (IMD (49, 50)). IMD ranges from 1 to 10 where 1 represents the relatively most and 10 least deprived deciles, respectively. Participants were also asked to self-report their health (‘How is your health in general?’, 1–5, higher scores indicate poor health) and employment status (‘Are you working?’, options: ‘working’ vs. ‘not working’).
2.2.2. Engagement with creating arts and crafting
To capture this concept, we compared participants who indicated they engaged in at least one of the following activities within the last 12 months (vs. none): ‘Painting, drawing, printmaking or sculpture’ (question artp13), ‘Photography as an artistic activity (no family or holiday ‘snaps’)’ (artp14), ‘Made films or videos as an artistic activity (not family or holidays)’ (artp15), ‘Used a computer to create original artworks or animation’ (artp16), ‘Textile crafts such as embroidery, crocheting or knitting’ (artp17), ‘Wood crafts such as wood turning, carving or furniture making’ (artp18) and ‘Other crafts such as calligraphy, pottery or jewellery for yourself’ (artp19). Specifically, we used the variable artp263Y13 (whereby participants were asked to indicate if they participated in ‘none of these’ activities) and reversed coded this variable for the current study, so that one indicated a participant had taken part in at least one craft activity, and zero indicated a participant had not taken part in any of these craft activities. Overall, 37.4% of participants in the survey had engaged in at least one craft activity within the past 12 months, while 62.6% had not.
2.2.3. Subjective wellbeing (SWB)
Subjective wellbeing was measured through four single-item questions, whereby each was assessed on 0–10 Likert scales and analysed individually following ONS guidance (44). These items were: life satisfaction: “Overall, how satisfied are you with your life nowadays” (0 = “not at all satisfied?” to 10 = “completely satisfied”), life being worthwhile: “To what extent do you feel that the things in your life are worthwhile?” (0 = “not at all worthwhile” to 10 = “completely”), happiness: “Taking all things together, how happy would you say you are?” (0 = “extremely unhappy” to 10 = “extremely happy”), and anxiety: “On a scale where 0 is “not at all anxious” and 10 is “completely anxious,” overall, how anxious did you feel yesterday?” (0 = “not at all anxious” to 10 = “completely anxious”).
2.2.4. Loneliness
A single item measured loneliness: “How often do you feel lonely?” where 1 = ‘often or always’, 2 = ‘some of the time’, 3 = ‘occasionally’, 4 = ‘hardly ever’ and 5 = ‘never’. This variable was reverse-coded for the current study, so that higher scores reflect greater loneliness.
3. Results
3.1. Analyses
To test our hypothesis, we ran hierarchical linear regressions, with engagement with CAC and sociodemographics (gender, level of deprivation, age, general health and employment status) as the predictor variables, and SWB (life satisfaction, life being worthwhile, happiness and anxiety) and loneliness as the outcome variables. Therefore, a total of five hierarchical regressions were conducted, one on each outcome variable. Sociodemographic variables were entered in Block 1, with engagement with CAC entered in Block 2. All analyses were conducted via Jamovi. The categorical predictor variables were dummy coded: engagement with CAC (reference: none of these), gender (reference: female), and employment status (reference: not working). There was no multicollinearity between the predictor variables, as assessed through VIFs ≤1.37.
3.1.1. Life satisfaction
Engagement with creating arts and crafting (CAC) significantly predicted participants’ reported life satisfaction, above and beyond the effects of age, gender, deprivation, poor health, and being in work. Inclusion of engagement with CAC in the model explained an additional 0.1% of variance in life satisfaction scores compared to the effects of a model including only age, gender, deprivation, poor health and being in work, F (1, 7,175) = 4.59, p = 0.032. Engagement with CAC predicted a greater increase in life satisfaction (β = 0.088) than the increase associated with living in a less deprived area (β = 0.018). In the final stage of the model, deprivation, age, poor health and engaging with CAC were all significant predictors of life satisfaction, with those in less deprived areas, older age groups, those in better health and those who had engaged in CAC in the last year all reporting higher life satisfaction scores. The final model accounted for 16.4% of the variance in life satisfaction scores, R2 = 0.164, F (6, 7,175) = 235, p < 0.001. These findings align with our hypothesis.
3.1.2. Sense of life being worthwhile
Engagement with CAC significantly predicted participants’ sense that life is worthwhile, above and beyond the effects of age, gender, deprivation, poor health and being in work. Indeed, inclusion of engagement in CAC in the model explains an additional 0.4% variance in participants’ sense that life is worthwhile compared to a model that only includes age, gender, deprivation, poor health and being in work, F (1, 7,175) = 27.5, p < 0.001. Participants’ engagement with CAC in the last 12 months had a larger effect on their sense that life is worthwhile (β = 0.218) than being in employment (β = 0.136), than aging by one decile (~ 20 years; β = 0.082) or than living in a less deprived area (β = 0.019). In the final model, all six variables significantly predicted participants’ sense that life is worthwhile, with women, those in older age groups, those living in less derived areas, those reporting better health, those in employment and those who had engaged with CAC in the last 12 months all reporting a greater sense that life is worthwhile. The final model accounted for 11.2% of variance in scores measuring a sense that life is worthwhile, R2 = 0.112, F (6, 7,175) = 150, p < 0.001. This is in line with our hypothesis.
3.1.3. Happiness
Participants’ reported happiness was significantly predicted by their engagement with CAC, above and beyond the effects of age, gender, deprivation, poor health and being in work. An additional 0.1% of variance in participants’ happiness scores could be explained by the inclusion of engagement with CAC in the model, compared to the effects of age, gender, deprivation, poor health or being in work, F (1, 7,175) = 6.71, p = 0.010. Engagement with CAC predicted a similar increase in happiness (β = 0.128) as aging by one decile (~ 20 years; β = 0.127). In the final stage of the model, gender, age, poor health and engagement with CAC all significantly predicted happiness scores, with women, those from an older age group, those reporting better health and those who had engaged with CAC in the last 12 months all reporting being happier. The final model accounted for 10.6% of variance in participants’ happiness scores, R2 = 0.106, F (6, 7,175) = 142, p < 0.001. These findings align with our hypothesis.
3.1.4. Anxiety
Contrary to our hypothesis, engagement with CAC did not provide additional predictive power for anxiety scores to the model (p = 0.108). The final model accounts for 7.1% variance in anxiety scores, with women, those in younger age groups, and those in poorer health reporting higher anxiety scores, R2 = 0.071, F (6, 7,175) = 91.5, p < 0.001.
3.1.5. Loneliness
Contrary to our hypothesis, a model including engagement with CAC did not predict significantly more variance in loneliness scores than a model including age, gender, deprivation, poor health and being in work alone (p = 0.805). The final model accounted for 7.7% of variance in loneliness scores, with men, older age groups, those living in less deprived areas, those in better health and those in employment all reporting being less lonely, R2 = 0.077, F (6, 7,175) = 99, p < 0.001.
3.2. Summary
Our findings support aspects of our hypothesis; specifically that engagement with CAC is associated with significantly higher life satisfaction, happiness, and a sense that life is worthwhile. Engagement with CAC is not associated with lower levels of anxiety or loneliness (Table 2).
Table 2.
Predictor variables | Satisfaction | Happiness | Worthwhile | Anxiety | Loneliness |
---|---|---|---|---|---|
Step 1 | |||||
Intercept | 8.694*** | 8.418*** | 8.682*** | 2.559*** | 2.183*** |
Gender | −0.071 | −0.145** | −0.234*** | −0.387*** | −0.209*** |
IMD decile | 0.019** | 0.012 | 0.021** | 0.021 | −0.019*** |
Age group | 0.100*** | 0.126*** | 0.082*** | −0.239*** | −0.048*** |
General health | −0.749*** | −0.697*** | −0.571*** | 0.753*** | 0.284*** |
In employment | 0.062 | 0.019 | 0.136** | 0.002 | −0.187*** |
R 2 | 0.163 | 0.105 | 0.108 | 0.071 | 0.077 |
F | 280*** | 169*** | 174*** | 109.3*** | 118.8*** |
Step 2 | |||||
Intercept | 8.653*** | 8.358*** | 8.580*** | 2.506*** | 2.187*** |
Gender | −0.063 | −0.128** | −0.205*** | −0.371*** | −0.210*** |
IMD decile | 0.018* | 0.011 | 0.019** | 0.020 | −0.019*** |
Age group | 0.100*** | 0.127*** | 0.083*** | −0.238*** | −0.048*** |
General health | −0.747*** | −0.695*** | −0.567*** | 0.755*** | 0.284*** |
In employment | 0.062 | 0.018 | 0.136** | 0.002 | −0.187*** |
ECAC | 0.088* | 0.128* | 0.218*** | 0.113 | −0.007 |
R 2 | 0.164 | 0.106 | 0.112 | 0.071 | 0.077 |
ΔR2 | 0.0005 | 0.0008 | 0.003 | 0.0003 | <0.0001 |
ΔF | 4.59* | 6.71* | 27.5*** | 2.59 | 0.061 |
*p < 0.05; **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001.
4. Discussion
Given the concern for improving the population’s wellbeing (1) and decreasing loneliness (51), this paper set out to uniquely and directly assess the benefits of engagement with CAC to combat poor wellbeing and loneliness. For the first time, our paper demonstrates that engaging in general (rather than specific) crafting and arts activities may be beneficial in improving subjective wellbeing, over and above one’s gender, health, age, employment status and affluence. Importantly, we show that this pattern holds for the general (rather than clinical) population using quantitative (rather than qualitative) methods, addressing several gaps in the literature.
Specifically, in line with our hypothesis, engagement with CAC was linked to significantly increased life satisfaction, a greater sense that life is worthwhile and increased happiness, above and beyond known sociodemographic predictors. While these findings are of a small-sized magnitude, they are comparable to the effects of known sociodemographic variables, and are of practical importance at a population level. For example, the predictive effect of engagement with CAC on life satisfaction that we observed is an order of magnitude greater than the effect of living in a less deprived area. Similarly, the predictive effect of engagement with CAC on people’s sense that life is worthwhile is greater than the individual effect of being in employment, greater than the effect of living in a less deprived area, and greater than the effect of aging by one decile (~ 20 years). The effect of engagement with CAC on happiness was of a similar magnitude as the effect of aging by one decile or of being female. That is, although variances explained by engaging with CAC were objectively low, they are relatively larger (for life satisfaction and life being worthwhile) or comparable (for happiness) than those explained by other sociodemographic variables known to impact wellbeing. The relationships between engagement with CAC and subjective wellbeing measures in relation to sociodemographic predictors is noteworthy, because unlike static sociodemographic variables, engagement with CAC is a variable that can be manipulated and is therefore open to intervention. Thus, not only is engagement with CAC possibly more effective than many sociodemographic variables on improving aspects of wellbeing, but it is also an easier variable to manipulate and influence. We note also that engagement with crafting or creating arts has a relatively low entry point; crafting and creating arts is an accessible and relatively affordable pastime, and a popular pursuit among the general population. This is in contrast to attending live sporting events, which has also been demonstrated to positively predict SWM in a large UK sample (33), though attending live sporting events predicted lower loneliness, whereas engagement with CAC did not.
The implications of these findings are particularly significant given the importance of wellbeing. Higher SWB predicts health (52) and longevity (53), with some evidence pointing to positive effects on those with certain illnesses (54). Diener and Chan (55) outline the causal evidence demonstrating the beneficial influence SWB has on health and longevity. They also suggest policy makers include interventions to improve SWB across the population, given the potential impact of small SWB increases. Engagement with CAC is related to three of the four SWB measures assessed in this study. So, while the effects may be small, the contribution to SWB as measured by life satisfaction, happiness and life being worthwhile may provide a meaningful influence across society as a whole.
Our hypothesis was not supported with regards to one component of SWB (anxiety) and loneliness, as engagement with CAC had no significant predictive effect on either, above and beyond other sociodemographic predictors. Arts and crafts activities included here (e.g., drawing, painting, knitting, sewing, and crochet) can be considered as relatively solitary activities. This likely explains why we did not observe an effect of engaging with CAC on reducing loneliness. Further research should unpick the importance of the social elements of engagement with CAC, especially given the rich literature on social support and wellbeing (56).
Regarding the role of sociodemographic factors on SWB and loneliness, we largely replicate findings from previous research, as expected. We found that loneliness and perceiving life as worthwhile were both predicted by all sociodemographic variables here (deprivation, age group, gender, general health and employment status). Life satisfaction was predicted by deprivation, age and general health, whereas happiness and anxiety were both predicted by gender, age and health. In general, better health (36, 37) and older age (36, 40–42) are predictive of decreased loneliness and higher SWB. Further, higher levels of life satisfaction and the sense that life is worthwhile were associated with living in less deprived areas, as shown in previous research (34, 35). Similarly, loneliness is reported as lower in less deprived areas (57, 58). Previous literature also supports employment status being linked to factors associated with enhanced SWB (38, 59) and decreased loneliness (39). We found gender was not associated with life satisfaction, and previous literature has also been mixed in this regard (42–44, 57).
While our study’s strengths include the use of a nationally representative sample, investigation of overall engagement in craft activities and the use of quantitative methods, it is not without limitations. Firstly, our list of arts and crafts is not exhaustive and other arts categories, such as metalworking and literary arts were not considered. We also cannot be sure that participants shared the same understanding of what the various crafts listed in the survey entail. Of interest, many of the crafts included here may be seen as stereotypically feminine and thus the choice of CAC activities may be confounded with gender as a sociodemographic. It is unclear whether different arts and crafts affect men and women differently. Literature on masculinity threat and precarious manhood suggests that crafts such as knitting may be seen as threatening for many men (60). This question awaits further investigation, in particular whether stereotypically ‘masculine’ crafts (e.g., carpentry, metalworking) may have a larger impact on men’s wellbeing (29, 30). Secondly, it is important to note that the present study was correlational and therefore while we can speculate on the impact of engaging with CAC on life satisfaction, causation cannot be determined. The next step for future research is to experimentally manipulate whether or not participants engage in CAC and for how long to measure the causal impact on life satisfaction.
Further studies should also focus on the mechanisms through which engagement with CAC relates to wellbeing, which were not measured here. For example, a key component of creativity is the experience of flow (61), an intrinsically rewarding state characterized by total immersion in an activity. Frequent flow experiences have been linked to better quality of life, in particular wellbeing, satisfaction and a sense of mastery (62, 63). Creative arts are also associated with improved self-esteem and emotion regulation, and provide a means of authentic self-expression (64–70). A range of studies show that engagement in art therapy results in a reduction in anxiety (71), depression (72) and stress (73, 74), and improvements in social connection, wellbeing and life satisfaction (27, 68, 75, 76). Determining the exact mechanisms through which engagement with CAC may be beneficial for well-being awaits further studies.
Overall, our study provides support for exploring the wellbeing benefits of engagement with creative arts and crafting as a useful strategy to improve wellbeing at a population level, with the positive effects observed here being comparable to or greater than known sociodemographic predictors of wellbeing. Being already popular (37.4% of the current national sample), relatively cheap and accessible, engagement with CAC activities lends itself to government support and public uptake. Increased funding for creative arts and crafting activities for the general population may benefit society as a whole by improving wellbeing in modern living.
Data availability statement
Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://beta.ukdataservice.ac.uk/datacatalogue/doi/?id=8745#!#0.
Ethics statement
Ethical approval was not required for the study involving humans in accordance with the local legislation and institutional requirements. Written informed consent to participate in this study was not required from the participants or the participants’ legal guardians/next of kin in accordance with the national legislation and the institutional requirements.
Author contributions
HK: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SG: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SF: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. NG: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft. AH: Writing – review & editing. EK: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. KM: Writing – review & editing. RO: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing. SR: Conceptualization, Investigation, Writing – original draft. MZ: Conceptualization, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing.
Funding Statement
The author(s) declare that no financial support was received for the research, authorship, and/or publication of this article.
Conflict of interest
The authors declare that the research was conducted in the absence of any commercial or financial relationships that could be construed as a potential conflict of interest.
Publisher’s note
All claims expressed in this article are solely those of the authors and do not necessarily represent those of their affiliated organizations, or those of the publisher, the editors and the reviewers. Any product that may be evaluated in this article, or claim that may be made by its manufacturer, is not guaranteed or endorsed by the publisher.
References
- 1.British Medical Association . Mental health pressures data analysis. The British Medical Association is the trade union and professional body for doctors in the UK. (2023) Available at: https://www.bma.org.uk/advice-and-support/nhs-delivery-and-workforce/pressures/mental-health-pressures-data-analysis. Accessed October 10, 2023
- 2.Martela F, Sheldon KM. Clarifying the concept of well-being: psychological need satisfaction as the common core connecting eudaimonic and subjective well-being. Rev Gen Psychol. (2019) 23:458–74. doi: 10.1177/1089268019880886 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Deci EL, Ryan RM. Hedonia, eudaimonia, and well-being: an introduction. J Happiness Stud. (2006) 9:1–11. doi: 10.1007/s10902-006-9018-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 4.du Toit A, Thomson R, Page A. A systematic review and meta-analysis of longitudinal studies of the antecedents and consequences of wellbeing among university students. Int J Wellbeing. (2022) 12:163–206. doi: 10.5502/ijw.v12i2.1897 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Ngamaba KH, Panagioti M, Armitage CJ. How strongly related are health status and subjective well-being? Systematic review and meta-analysis. Eur J Pub Health. (2017) 27:879–85. doi: 10.1093/eurpub/ckx081, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Kushlev K, Drummond DM, Diener E. Subjective well-being and health behaviors in 2.5 million Americans. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. (2019) 12:166–87. doi: 10.1111/aphw.12178, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Trudel-Fitzgerald C, Millstein RA, von Hippel C, Howe CJ, Tomasso LP, Wagner GR, et al. Psychological well-being as part of the public health debate? Insight into dimensions, interventions, and policy. BMC Public Health. (2019) 19:1712. doi: 10.1186/s12889-019-8029-x, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Shrum LJ, Fumagalli E, Lowrey TM. Coping with loneliness through consumption. J Consum Psychol. (2022) 33:441–65. doi: 10.1002/jcpy.1329 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Prohaska T, Burholt V, Burns A, Golden J, Hawkley L, Lawlor B, et al. Consensus statement: loneliness in older adults, the 21st century social determinant of health? BMJ Open. (2020) 10:e034967. doi: 10.1136/bmjopen-2019-034967, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Rumas R, Shamblaw AL, Jagtap S, Best MW. Predictors and consequences of loneliness during the COVID-19 pandemic. Psychiatry Res. (2021) 300:113934. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2021.113934, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Statista . Arts and crafts as free time activity by age in England 2016. Statista. (2023) Available at: https://www.statista.com/statistics/557600/free-time-arts-and-crafts-by-age-uk-england/. Accessed October 10, 2023
- 12.Creative Industries Council . Crafts: facts & figures. The creative industries. (2021) Available at: https://www.thecreativeindustries.co.uk/facts-figures/industries-craft-facts-figures#:~:text=Measuring%20the%20Craft%20Economy%20(Industry%20Data)&text=Key%20findings%20of%20this%20research,from%20the%20UK%20crafts%20sector. Accessed October 10, 2023
- 13.Tighe D. Arts and crafts market value forecast worldwide 2017-2024. Statista. (2022) Available at: https://www.statista.com/statistics/1017884/arts-and-crafts-market-value-forecast-worldwide/. Accessed October 10, 2023
- 14.Macpherson H, Hart A, Heaver B. Building resilience through group visual arts activities: findings from a scoping study with young people who experience mental health complexities and/or learning difficulties. J Soc Work. (2016) 16:541–60. doi: 10.1177/1468017315581772 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Van Lith T, Schofield MJ, Fenner P. Identifying the evidence-base for art-based practices and their potential benefit for mental health recovery: a critical review. Disabil Rehabil. (2012) 35:1309–23. doi: 10.3109/09638288.2012.732188 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Wheatley D, Bickerton C. Measuring changes in subjective well-being from engagement in the arts, culture and sport. J Cult Econ. (2019) 43:421–42. doi: 10.1007/s10824-019-09342-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Wheatley D, Bickerton C. Subjective well-being and engagement in arts, culture and sport. J Cult Econ. (2017) 41:23–45. doi: 10.1007/s10824-016-9270-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Pesata V, Colverson A, Sonke J, Morgan-Daniel J, Schaefer N, Sams K, et al. Engaging the arts for wellbeing in the United States of America: a scoping review. Front Psychol. (2022) 12:791773. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2021.791773 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Michalos AC, Kahlke PM. Impact of arts-related activities on the perceived quality of life. Soc Indic Res. (2008) 89:193–258. doi: 10.1007/s11205-007-9236-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Bone JK, Fancourt D, Fluharty ME, Paul E, Sonke JK, Bu F. Associations between participation in community arts groups and aspects of wellbeing in older adults in the United States: a propensity score matching analysis. Aging Ment Health. (2022) 27:1163–72. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2022.2068129, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Sonke J, Sams K, Morgan-Daniel J, Pumariega A, Mallick F, Pesata V, et al. Systematic review of arts-based interventions to address suicide prevention and survivorship in Australia, Canada, the United Kingdom, and the United States of America. Health Promot Pract. (2021) 22:53S–63S. doi: 10.1177/1524839921996350, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Williams E, Dingle GA, Jetten J, Rowan C. Identification with arts-based groups improves mental wellbeing in adults with chronic mental health conditions. J Appl Soc Psychol. (2018) 49:15–26. doi: 10.1111/jasp.12561 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Perruzza N, Kinsella EA. Creative arts occupations in therapeutic practice: a review of the literature. Br J Occup Ther. (2010) 73:261–8. doi: 10.4276/030802210x12759925468943 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Keisari S, Piol S, Elkarif T, Mola G, Testoni I. Crafting life stories in Photocollage: an online creative art-based intervention for older adults. Behav Sci. (2021) 12:1. doi: 10.3390/bs12010001, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Curtis A, Gibson L, O’Brien M, Roe B. Systematic review of the impact of arts for health activities on health, wellbeing and quality of life of older people living in care homes. Dementia. (2018) 17:645–69. doi: 10.1177/1471301217740960, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Burt EL, Atkinson J. The relationship between quilting and wellbeing. J Public Health. (2011) 34:54–9. doi: 10.1093/pubmed/fdr041, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Riley J, Corkhill B, Morris C. The benefits of knitting for personal and social wellbeing in adulthood: findings from an international survey. Br J Occup Ther. (2013) 76:50–7. doi: 10.4276/030802213x13603244419077 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Burns P, Van Der Meer R. Happy hookers: findings from an international study exploring the effects of crochet on wellbeing. Perspect Public Health. (2020) 141:149–57. doi: 10.1177/1757913920911961, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Kelly D, Steiner A. The impact of community Men’s sheds on the physical health of their users. Health Place. (2021) 71:102649. doi: 10.1016/j.healthplace.2021.102649, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Milligan C, Neary D, Payne S, Hanratty B, Irwin P, Dowrick C. Older men and social activity: a scoping review of men’s sheds and other gendered interventions. Ageing Soc. (2015) 36:895–923. doi: 10.1017/s0144686x14001524 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Coholic DA. Exploring the feasibility and benefits of arts-based mindfulness-based practices with young people in need: aiming to improve aspects of self-awareness and resilience. Child Youth Care Forum. (2011) 40:303–17. doi: 10.1007/s10566-010-9139-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Coholic DA, Hardy A, Patricia G, Stephanie M. Exploring the benefits of an arts-based mindfulness group intervention for teachers, university students, and women survivors of intimate partner violence. Diver Res Health J. (2021) 4:16. doi: 10.28984/drhj.v4i2.323 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Keyes H, Gradidge S, Gibson N, Harvey A, Roeloffs S, Zawisza M, et al. Attending live sporting events predicts subjective wellbeing and reduces loneliness. Front Public Health. (2023) 10:989706. doi: 10.3389/fpubh.2022.989706, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.De Koning JL, Stathi A, Richards S. Predictors of loneliness and different types of social isolation of rural-living older adults in the United Kingdom. Ageing Soc. (2016) 37:2012–43. doi: 10.1017/s0144686x16000696 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Bellis MA, Lowey H, Hughes K, Deacon L, Stansfield J, Perkins C. Variations in risk and protective factors for life satisfaction and mental wellbeing with deprivation: a cross-sectional study. BMC Public Health. (2012) 12:492. doi: 10.1186/1471-2458-12-492, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Steptoe A, Deaton A, Stone AA. Subjective wellbeing, health, and ageing. Lancet. (2015) 385:640–8. doi: 10.1016/s0140-6736(13)61489-0, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Jessen MA, Pallesen AV, Kriegbaum M, Kristiansen M. The association between loneliness and health – a survey-based study among middle-aged and older adults in Denmark. Aging Mental Health. (2017) 22:1338–43. doi: 10.1080/13607863.2017.1348480 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Bonanomi A, Rosina A. Employment status and well-being: a longitudinal study on young Italian people. Soc Indic Res. (2022) 161:581–98. doi: 10.1007/s11205-020-02376-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Morrish N, Medina-Lara A. Does unemployment lead to greater levels of loneliness? A systematic review. Soc Sci Med. (2021) 287:114339. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2021.114339, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Victor CR, Yang K. The prevalence of loneliness among adults: a case study of the United Kingdom. J Psychol. (2012) 146:85–104. doi: 10.1080/00223980.2011.613875, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 41.Hawkley LC, Buecker S, Kaiser T, Luhmann M. Loneliness from young adulthood to old age: explaining age differences in loneliness. Int J Behav Dev. (2020) 46:39–49. doi: 10.1177/0165025420971048, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Barreto M, Victor C, Hammond C, Eccles A, Richins MT, Qualter P. Loneliness around the world: age, gender, and cultural differences in loneliness. Personal Individ Differ. (2021) 169:110066. doi: 10.1016/j.paid.2020.110066, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Maes M, Qualter P, Vanhalst J, Van den Noortgate W, Goossens L. Gender differences in loneliness across the lifespan: a meta–analysis. Eur J Personal. (2019) 33:642–54. doi: 10.1002/per.2220 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Arrondo R, Cárcaba A, González E. Drivers of subjective well-being in Spain: are there gender differences? Appl Res Qual Life. (2020) 16:2131–54. doi: 10.1007/s11482-020-09862-x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Batz C, Tay L. Gender differences in subjective well-being In: Diener E, Oishi S, Tay L, editors. Handbook of well-being. Salt Lake City, UT, USA: DEF Publishers; (2018) [Google Scholar]
- 46.The Office for National Statistics . Personal well-being user guidance. Personal well-being user guidance - Office for National Statistics. (2018) Available at: https://www.ons.gov.uk/peoplepopulationandcommunity/wellbeing/methodologies/personalwellbeingsurveyuserguide. Accessed November 19, 2023
- 47.The Office for National Statistics . Measuring subjective wellbeing – Office for National Statistics. (2011) Available at: http://www.ons.gov.uk/ons/guide-method/user-guidance/well-being/wellbeing-knowledge-bank/understanding-wellbeing/measuring-subjective-well-being.pdf. Accessed June 18, 2024
- 48.Department for Digital, Culture, Media and Sport . Taking part: the National Survey of culture, leisure and sport, 2019-2020; adult and child data. UK data service. SN:8745 (2021). doi: 10.5255/UKDA-SN-8745-1 [DOI]
- 49.Noble S, McLennan D, Noble M, Plunkett E, Gutacker N, Silk M, et al. , The English indices of deprivation 2019. Ministry of Housing, communities and local government. (2019) Available at: https://assets.publishing.service.gov.uk/government/uploads/system/uploads/attachment_data/file/833947/IoD2019_Research_Report.pdf.
- 50.Smith T, Noble M, Noble S, Wright G, McLennan D, Plunkett E. The english indices of deprivation 2015: Research report – warrington. (2015) Available at: https://www.warrington.gov.uk/sites/default/files/2019-09/english-indices-of-deprivation-2015-research-report.pdf. Accessed November 19, 2023
- 51.Jeste DV, Lee EE, Cacioppo S. Battling the modern behavioral epidemic of loneliness. JAMA Psychiatry. (2020) 77:553–4. doi: 10.1001/jamapsychiatry.2020.0027, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Lyubomirsky S, King L, Diener E. The benefits of frequent positive affect: does happiness lead to success? Psychol Bull. (2005) 131:803–55. doi: 10.1037/0033-2909.131.6.803, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Howell RT, Kern ML, Lyubomirsky S. Health benefits: Meta-analytically determining the impact of well-being on objective health outcomes. Health Psychol Rev. (2007) 1:83–136. doi: 10.1080/17437190701492486 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Chida Y, Steptoe A. Positive psychological well-being and mortality: a quantitative review of prospective observational studies. Psychosom Med. (2008) 70:741–56. doi: 10.1097/psy.0b013e31818105ba, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Diener E, Chan MY. Happy people live longer: subjective well-being contributes to health and longevity. Appl Psychol Health Well Being. (2011) 3:1–43. doi: 10.1111/j.1758-0854.2010.01045.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Zee KS, Bolger N, Higgins ET. Regulatory effectiveness of social support. J Pers Soc Psychol. (2020) 119:1316–58. doi: 10.1037/pspi0000235 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Batz-Barbarich C, Tay L, Kuykendall L, Cheung HK. A meta-analysis of gender differences in subjective well-being: estimating effect sizes and associations with gender inequality. Psychol Sci. (2018) 29:1491–503. doi: 10.1177/0956797618774796, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.VanderWeele TJ, Hawkley LC, Cacioppo JT. On the reciprocal association between loneliness and subjective well-being. Am J Epidemiol. (2012) 176:777–84. doi: 10.1093/aje/kws173, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Dolan P, Peasgood T, White M. Do we really know what makes us happy? A review of the economic literature on the factors associated with subjective well-being. J Econ Psychol. (2008) 29:94–122. doi: 10.1016/j.joep.2007.09.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Bosson JK, Wilkerson M, Kosakowska-Berezecka N, Jurek P, Olech M. Harder won and easier lost? Testing the double standard in gender rules in 62 countries. Sex Roles. (2022) 87:1–19. doi: 10.1007/s11199-022-01297-y [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Huotilainen M, Rankanen M, Groth C, Seitamaa-Hakkarainen P, Mäkelä M. Why our brains love arts and crafts. FormAkademisk - forskningstidsskrift for design og designdidaktikk. (2018) 11, 1–18. doi: 10.7577/formakademisk.1908 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Nakamura J, Csikszentmihalyi M. Flow theory and research In: Snyder CR, Lope SJ, editors. The Oxford handbook of positive psychology. Oxford, MS: Oxford University Press; (2009). 194–206. [Google Scholar]
- 63.Isham A, Gatersleben B, Jackson T. Flow activities as a route to living well with less. Environ Behav. (2018) 51:431–61. doi: 10.1177/0013916518799826 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 64.Nazeri A, Ghamarani A, Darouei P, Ghasemi TG. The effect of expressive arts therapy on emotion regulation of primary school students. Quart J Child Mental Health. (2020) 7:132–43. doi: 10.29252/jcmh.7.2.12 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Morris JA, Urbanski J, Fuller J. Using poetry and the visual arts to develop emotional intelligence. J Manag Educ. (2005) 29:888–904. doi: 10.1177/1052562905277313 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Nan JKM, Hinz LD, Lusebrink VB. Clay art therapy on emotion regulation: research, theoretical underpinnings, and treatment mechanisms. Neurosci Depress. (2021):431–42. doi: 10.1016/b978-0-12-817933-8.00009-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Fancourt D, Garnett C, Spiro N, West R, Müllensiefen D. How do artistic creative activities regulate our emotions? Validation of the emotion regulation strategies for artistic creative activities scale (ERS-ACA). PLoS One. (2019) 14:e0211362. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0211362, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Reynolds F. ‘Colour and communion’: exploring the influences of visual art-making as a leisure activity on older women’s subjective well-being. J Aging Stud. (2010) 24:135–43. doi: 10.1016/j.jaging.2008.10.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Mak HW, Fancourt D. Arts engagement and self-esteem in children: results from a propensity score matching analysis. Ann N Y Acad Sci. (2019) 1449:36–45. doi: 10.1111/nyas.14056, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Fraser C, Keating M. The effect of a creative art program on self-esteem, hope, perceived social support, and self-efficacy in individuals with multiple sclerosis. J Neurosci Nurs. (2014) 46:330–6. doi: 10.1097/jnn.0000000000000094 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Jenabi E, Bashirian S, Ayubi E, Rafiee M, Bashirian M. The effect of the art therapy interventions on depression symptoms among older adults: a meta-analysis of controlled clinical trials. J Geriatr Psychiatry Neurol. (2022) 36:185–92. doi: 10.1177/08919887221130264, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Abbing A, Ponstein A, van Hooren S, de Sonneville L, Swaab H, Baars E. The effectiveness of art therapy for anxiety in adults: a systematic review of randomised and non-randomised controlled trials. PLoS One. (2018) 13:e0208716. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0208716, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Kaimal G, Ray K, Muniz J. Reduction of cortisol levels and participants’ responses following art making. Art Ther. (2016) 33:74–80. doi: 10.1080/07421656.2016.1166832, PMID: [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Abbott KA, Shanahan MJ, Neufeld RW. Artistic tasks outperform nonartistic tasks for stress reduction. Art Ther. (2013) 30:71–8. doi: 10.1080/07421656.2013.787214 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Liddle JLM, Parkinson L, Sibbritt DW. Purpose and pleasure in late life: Conceptualising older women’s participation in art and craft activities. J Aging Stud. (2013) 27:330–8. doi: 10.1016/j.jaging.2013.08.002, PMID: [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.Gorny-Wegrzyn E, Perry B. Creative art: connection to health and well-being. Open J Soc Sci. (2022) 10:290–303. doi: 10.4236/jss.2022.1012020 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Data Availability Statement
Publicly available datasets were analyzed in this study. This data can be found here: https://beta.ukdataservice.ac.uk/datacatalogue/doi/?id=8745#!#0.