Skip to main content
Springer logoLink to Springer
. 2024 Aug 29;29(1):54. doi: 10.1007/s40519-024-01682-4

Early adaptive schemas, emotional regulation, and cognitive flexibility in eating disorders: subtype specific predictors of eating disorder symptoms using hierarchical linear regression

J S Mitchell 1,, T Huckstepp 1, A Allen 1,2, P J Louis 3, T E Anijärv 1,4, D F Hermens 1
PMCID: PMC11362190  PMID: 39210038

Abstract

Purpose

Understanding how early adaptive schemas, cognitive flexibility, and emotional regulation influence eating disorder (ED) symptoms, and whether this differs across diagnostic subtypes is critical to optimising treatment. The current study investigated the relationship between these variables and ED symptomology in individuals self-reporting an ED diagnosis and healthy controls.

Methods

A dataset of 1576 online survey responses yielded subsamples for anorexia nervosa (n = 155), bulimia nervosa (n = 55), binge eating disorder (n = 33), other specified feeding or eating disorder (n = 93), and healthy participants (n = 505). The hierarchical linear regression analysis included Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire 6.0 Global Score as the dependent variable; Young Positive Schema Questionnaire, Emotional Regulation Questionnaire, and Cognitive Flexibility Inventory subscale scores as the independent variables; and demographic measures as the covariates.

Results

The number of significant predictors varied considerably by ED sub-group. Amongst the anorexia nervosa, bulimia nervosa, and healthy subsamples, the adaptive schema Self-Compassion and Realistic Expectations was associated with lower ED symptom severity. In comparison, age and body mass index were the strongest predictors for binge eating disorder, whilst the Expressive Suppression (a subscale of the Emotional Regulation Questionnaire) was the strongest predictor for other specified feeding or eating disorders.

Conclusion

Early adaptive schemas, cognitive flexibility, and emotional regulation vary across ED subtype, suggesting the need for tailored treatment that disrupts the self-reinforcing cycle of ED psychopathology. Future research investigating how early adaptive schemas may predict or be associated with treatment response across diagnostic subtypes is needed.

Level of evidence: Level IV, evidence obtained from multiple time-series with or without the intervention, such as case studies.

Supplementary Information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1007/s40519-024-01682-4.

Keywords: Eating disorders, Early adaptive schemas, Cognitive flexibility, Emotional regulation

Introduction

Background

Eating disorders (EDs) comprise both clinically defined and related syndromes characterised by maladaptive eating behaviours, altered body image-related perceptions, and impaired behavioural regulation [1, 2]. Whilst ED subtypes display characteristic symptoms: Anorexia Nervosa (AN), restrictive eating; Bulimia Nervosa (BN), binge eating followed by purging behaviours; Binge Eating Disorder (BED), recurrent episodes of overeating without compensatory behaviours; and Other Specified Feeding or Eating Disorders (OSFED), a range of disordered eating patterns not meeting other diagnostic criteria, there is growing evidence to suggest early maladaptive schemas are a key transdiagnostic risk and maintenance factor across EDs [3].

Schema therapy and early maladaptive schemas

Accordingly, there is interest in using schema therapy to identify and challenge early maladaptive schemas related to body-image perfectionism for the treatment of EDs [4, 5]. Young [6] defines early maladaptive schemas as themes or patterns consisting of emotions, cognitions, memories, and bodily sensations that distort one’s view of oneself and one’s relationship with others. These patterns are often rigid and pervasive during one’s lifetime, comparable to personality disorder features [7]. Early maladaptive schemas develop when early caregivers do not adequately meet a child’s core emotional needs [6]. This often includes trauma and mistreatment [8], but can also include overprotection from parents [9]. Other contributing factors to early maladaptive schemas include a child’s temperament, culture, and environment [6]. The specific unmet needs are associated with the type of early maladaptive schemas that may develop [10] which, in turn, is linked with the development of an ED [11]. When an early maladaptive schema is triggered, an individual uses a range of coping mechanisms to reduce the associated emotional pain. For the ED populations, such coping mechanisms include constant self-criticism and holding one’s body image to a high standard at the expense of one’s mental well-being [12].

Early adaptive schemas

In contrast to early maladaptive schemas, early adaptive schemas have been theorised to develop when a child’s core emotional needs are met adequately early in life by primary caregivers [13, 14]. Early maladaptive schemas may influence the development and maintenance of EDs with the number, and content of early maladaptive schemas shown to be associated with disordered eating behaviours and their severity [11, 15]. Crucially, schema therapy treatment includes not just confronting the rigid early maladaptive schemas, but also helping to increase the strengths of early adaptive schemas. These include a broad range of adaptive capacities including that of being compassionate towards and accepting of oneself. Consistent with this, schema therapy treatment has produced positive responses even in severe ED presentations [4, 5]. However, additional research is required to investigate the link between early adaptive schemas and ED symptoms, and whether this varies across ED subtypes as this may inform treatment approach.

In one recent study investigating the relationship between early adaptive schemas and the Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire 6.0 (EDE-Q) subscales [15], healthy boundaries, emotional openness, and spontaneity, social belonging, and self-care were identified as ‘protective’ against ED symptoms, whilst increased optimism predicted greater ED pathology. Providing an invaluable preliminary investigation, the studied sample (N = 388) was clinically heterogenous (50% reported current ED diagnosis). However, part (semi-partial) correlations were not reported, and correlations between predictors in the model were high (r > 0.7). Thus, there is a need to (1) explore the associations between early adaptive schemas and ED symptoms across (self-reported) clinical ED subtypes, and (2) apply more stringent multicollinearity thresholds; as regression coefficients and standard errors estimates can be biased even at conventionally acceptable thresholds (i.e. variance inflation factor = 5 or Pearson correlation coefficient = 0.7; [16, 17]. Moreover, the regression models in this study accounted for limited (16–31%) variance in ED symptoms, warranting further investigation of new models incorporating relevant variables to more accurately approximate variability in ED symptoms.

Emotion regulation

Emotion regulation is defined by Gross [18] as the process which individuals regulate and express their emotions. The two broadest categories of emotion regulation are cognitive reappraisal and expressive suppression. Cognitive appraisal is the process of modifying the meaning of a situation to change the emotional impact, whilst expressive suppression involves inhibiting emotion-expressive behaviour [18, 19]. Individuals with EDs experience higher levels of emotional intensity, have less awareness of their emotions, and use less adaptive emotion regulation strategies compared with healthy populations [2022], and clinical populations with other non-ED mental health diagnoses [23]. Furthermore, greater emotion dysregulation in individuals with EDs is also associated with more severe clinical features, such as repetitive negative thinking, dysfunctional metacognitions, and greater ED symptomatology [21, 24].

There is some evidence to indicate emotional dysregulation profiles vary by ED subtype. For instance, Mallorquí-Bagué et al. [22] found that individuals with BN, BED, and OSFED exhibit poorer emotional regulation than those with AN. Using an ecological momentary assessment to measure daily emotion dynamics, Williams-Kerver et al. [24] found individuals with AN and BN experienced more intense negative affect than those with BED, whilst, the BN and BED groups showed greater daily emotional fluctuation compared to the AN group. Furthermore, they identified that individuals with BN demonstrated a better ability to differentiate emotions, whilst the AN group had the lowest ability to do so. However, two recent meta-analyses indicate emotion regulation difficulties, particularly awareness, acceptance, rumination and reappraisal, are comparable across the spectrum of EDs, and that their role in EDs may be more nuanced and require a consideration of mediating and moderating variables [25, 26].

Emerging evidence suggests the relationship between early maladaptive schemas and disordered eating may be mediated through emotion dysregulation [27]. For example, in a sample of young Lebanese adults, Gerges and colleagues showed that only one schema domain, namely Disconnection and Rejection (consisting of the negative schemas: Defectiveness, Emotional Deprivation, Emotional Inhibition, Social Isolation, Negative Pessimism and Mistrust/Abuse), had a direct effect on disordered eating behaviour, whilst the other four schema domains (i.e. the impaired autonomy/performance, impaired limits, other-directedness, and over vigilance/inhibition schema) had a significant effect only when mediated through emotional dysregulation.

Cognitive flexibility

Cognitive flexibility is the ability to modify thinking strategies and behaviour in response to changing environmental stimuli. Dennis and Vander Wal [28] emphasise two distinct aspects of cognitive flexibility that contribute to adaptive thinking as measured by the two subscales of the Cognitive Flexibility Inventory. The first is the tendency to perceive problems as controllable (i.e. Control) and the second is the ability to generate multiple alternative explanations and solutions to difficult situations (i.e. Alternatives). Across ED subtypes there is evidence for impaired cognitive flexibility compared to their healthy counterparts [29, 30], being less able to efficiently switch between tasks. There is limited and conflicting evidence that cognitive inflexibility more pronounced in certain subtypes, with evidence for greater difficulties in AN [31], and BED [32]. Of note, there is evidence that cognitive flexibility may constitute an endophenotype of AN, with no significant difference in cognitive flexibility between individuals in the acute phase of AN and those in the recovered stage [30, 33].

Aim and hypotheses

Taken together, this evidence highlights the converging (and seemingly inter-related) influence that early adaptive schemas, emotion regulation, and cognitive flexibility has in EDs. However, the degree to which these factors predict ED symptom severity and whether this varies between ED subtypes has yet to be determined. As such, the aim of the current study was to investigate the variability in ED symptomology accounted for by early adaptive schemas, cognitive flexibility, and emotional regulation, in individuals whom self-reported a current clinical diagnosis of AN, BN, BED or OSFED. Understanding whether the contributions of these factors differ across diagnostic subtypes may have implications for how schema therapy is utilised. The current study adhered to a nine-factor structure of the Young Positive Schema Questionnaire as this was previously validated with the current sample [1].

As per the pre-registered protocol (see “Protocol Registration”), it was predicted the inclusion of the early adaptive schemas would account for the most significant proportion of variance in ED symptoms. Specifically, congruent with previous findings [15], we predicted the early adaptive schema’s Self-Compassion and Realistic Expectations, Healthy Self-Control, Emotional Openness and Spontaneity, and Social Belonging would each independently predict lower eating disorder symptomatology across ED subtypes (i.e. act as protective factors).

Methods

Protocol registration

The analysis and reporting protocol for this manuscript was registered on the Open Science Framework (OSF; https://doi.org/10.17605/OSF.IO/7SDUZ). Post hoc changes to the statistical analyses include: (1) inclusion of all predictor variables in the hierarchical linear regression model regardless of whether they were significantly correlated with the criterion, (2) univariate outliers were removed on a variable-by-variable basis, and (3) the Shapiro–Wilk test was used instead of Kolmogorov–Smirnov test for the normality of residuals assessment.

Participants

Instruments

Demographic measures

Age, weight, and height were reported by manual entry, whilst education, nationality, and sex were selected from a list (manual entry required for ‘other’). Body-mass-index (BMI) values were calculated using reported weight and height values.

Diagnosis

Participants self-reported if they had a current mental health diagnosis and/or ED diagnosis from a health professional (type not specified) and to indicate the specific diagnosis (i.e. AN, BN, BED, or OSFED).

Questionnaire measures

Young Positive Schema Questionnaire: early adaptive schemas were measured using a reduced item (48-items) and factor structure (9 factors) version of the Young Positive Schema Questionnaire [13, 34]. The original 56-item questionnaire has strong reliability (α = 0.72–0.89) and validity in psychiatric samples [35]. The reliability and validity of the reduced item and factor version used here has previously been shown with this sample [1], with internal consistency matching previous reports (α = 0.77 and 0.92).

Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire 6.0 (EDE-Q): The EDE-Q Global Score (average of subscales) was used to measure ED symptomatology [36], with higher scores indicating greater ED symptom severity. The EDE-Q Global Score has good internal consistency (α = 0.90; [37].

Emotional Regulation Questionnaire (ERQ): Cognitive Reappraisal and Expressive Suppression scores were determined by the ERQ [19]. Total scores are calculated by adding relevant subscale items, with higher scores representative of coping strategy use. The reliability of the two subscales in ED samples has previously been established (Cognitive Reappraisal: α = 0.87; Expressive Suppression: α = 0.80; [38].

Cognitive Flexibility Inventory (CFI): Cognitive flexibility scores were measured using the CFI [28], with two subscales, Alternatives and Control, calculated. The CFI subscales have demonstrated good internal consistency (Alternatives: α = 0.91; Control: α = 0.85[28].

Procedure

Individuals aged ≥ 18 years were recruited through social networking sites (e.g. Facebook) and online forums (e.g. Reddit) via the distribution of a survey using the Qualtrics platform between September 2020 and May 2021. Advertisement of the survey was targeted at ED-related pages and forums to increase the chance of capturing individuals with ED symptoms. The survey was completed online, requiring approximately 25 min. A detailed information sheet including a description of the study and its aims was presented on the first page of the electronic survey. Individuals were informed that their data would be recorded anonymously and participation in the research was voluntary, and they could exit the survey at any time. Consent was obtained via participants selecting a checkbox prior to beginning the survey. After the survey, participants were provided with contact details for support services, and they were also given the opportunity to enter a draw to win one of four $50 AUD Amazon vouchers which were drawn at the completion of the project. The study was approved by the University of the Sunshine Coast Ethics Committee (S201469).

Statistical approach

The nature of missing data was evaluated using Little MCAR’s test in Statistical Package for the Social Sciences (SPSS), with missing values subsequently imputed using the miceforest package [39]. Data formatting, scale coding, univariate outlier detection, and descriptive analysis was performed in Python using the Pandas [40], Numpy [41], MatPlotLib, Seaborn [42], Pinqouin [43], Statannotations [44], and SciKit [45] packages, with multivariate outlier detection conducted in R studio using previously published code [46]. Univariate outliers were determined using the median absolute deviation procedure (median ± 2.5 (median absolute deviation * 1.4826); [47]), and multivariate outliers were identified using the Mahalanobis Distance variant—Minimum Covariance Distance (see [46]). Descriptive statistics were tabulated for continuous and categorical variables. Correlations were quantified using a Pearson’s correlation coefficient and presented in the Supplementary Materials.

Inferential statistics (and associated assumption checks) were conducted using the Pingouin [43], and HLR [48] packages in Python. HLR package outputs reported in text were verified against Statistical Package for the Social Sciences outputs to contribute to the package’s validation (see Supplementary Materials for example analysis). Group differences in continuous demographic variables were assessed with a one-way Welch ANOVA to account for unequal variances and sample sizes, with Games–Howell test post hoc comparisons (Games–Howell corrected p values).

The hierarchical linear regression analysis included EDE-Q Global Score as the dependent (or criterion) variable; Young Positive Schema Questionnaire, ERQ, and CFI subscale scores as the independent (or predictor) variables; and age, body mass index, and sex as the covariates. Each of the covariates were entered in model one, cognitive flexibility (Alternatives and Control subscales) and emotional regulation (Cognitive Reappraisal and Expressive Suppression subscales) entered in model two, and the nine-factor structure of the Young Positive Schema Questionnaire (Emotional Fulfilment and Stable Attachment, Success, Empathic Consideration, Optimism, Emotional Openness, Self-Compassion and Realistic Expectations, Developed Self, Social Belonging, and Healthy Self-Control) entered in model three. P values (< 0.05, uncorrected) and confidence intervals (95%) were reported, with confidence intervals interpreted as per recent discussions (see [49, 50]); confidence intervals represent the probability the range around the point-estimate contains the ‘true’ population effect estimate with repeated sampling (i.e. in the long run), not the range of values with 95% probability (this interpretation is reserved for Bayesian credible intervals).

Post hoc power analysis (see Supplementary Materials G) was conducted using G-power (v.3.1) with significance level α = 0.05 as recommended for the use in the clinic research [51].

Results

Missing data, dataset splitting and outlier removal

Of the submitted responses (n = 1576), 175 participants (11.1%) had some missing data. At a questionnaire level, 0.8–5.6% of responses were missing across EDE-Q questions, 9.5% for ERQ questions, and 7.4% for CFI questions. Missing values were imputed using Multiple Imputation by Chained Equations (MICE; [52]), as there was greater than 5% of participants missing data, which was not missing completely at random (Little MCARs X2 [1762] = 2084.73, p < 0.001).

After missing value imputation, the complete dataset (n = 1576) was split according to self-reported ED diagnosis subtype, resulting in datasets for AN (n = 174), BN (n = 76), BED (n = 47), OSFED (n = 108). Participants who reported more than one ED diagnosis subtype (n = 100) were not included in the analysis. For participants who did not report a current ED diagnosis (n = 1063), a subsample reported a previous ED diagnosis (n = 190), and another reported a current mental health diagnosis with no previous ED diagnosis (n = 315). The remaining participants were deemed the healthy control sample (n = 558).

Across samples, univariate outliers for age and questionnaire scales were present; however, these values were retained as they were within the range of plausible values. Univariate outliers detected for BMI (being > 2.5 * median absolute deviations above the median per sub-group) were removed listwise from the AN (n = 11), BN (n = 4), BED (n = 3), OSFED (n = 6), and healthy (n = 31) samples. Similarly, multivariate outliers across samples (AN [n = 5], BN [n = 15], BED [n = 11], OSFED [n = 7], and healthy [n = 15]) were removed listwise. Finally, participants who did not disclose their sex (AN [n = 3], BN [n = 2], OSFED [n = 2], and healthy [n = 7]) were removed listwise as sex was included as a model covariate (i.e. not disclosed is uninterpretable). This resulted in reduced final samples for AN (n = 155), BN (n = 55), BED (n = 33), OSFED (n = 93), and healthy (n = 505).

Assumptions checks

Using a combination of statistical and visual checks, the assumptions of homoscedasticity and normality of model residuals were largely satisfied, with QQ-plots showing minor deviations from normality in the tails only. Whilst not all predictors were significantly correlated with the dependent variable, all displayed a linear relationship, hence, all predictors were retained in the model. Independence of residuals was only met for the BN dataset, with positive autocorrelation of residuals (Durbin–Watson < 1.5) present in the AN, OSFED and healthy datasets, and negative autocorrelation (Durbin–Watson > 2.5) in the BED dataset. Whilst the data were not of a time-series, we suspected autocorrelation could represent spatial autocorrelation due to hierarchical structuring of the data (i.e. clustering). To verify this, we shuffled the data (by row (i.e. subject)) and recalculated the Durbin–Watson statistics. In each case, the independence of residuals was met (see Supplementary Materials), suggesting some spatial clustering in responses. As the autocorrelation did not change the model and effect estimates, we utilised the unshuffled data, noting the autocorrelation as a limitation of the data collection process.

Across the AN, BN, OSFED, and healthy datasets, multicollinearity did exceed conventional thresholds set out in the protocol [16, 17, 53], and therefore, all variables were retained in the model. Severe multicollinearity was detected in the BED dataset. Consequently, those scales with the highest variance inflation factor values (i.e. Young Positive Schema Questionnaire; Openness, Emotional Fulfilment and Stable Attachment, Success, and Social Belonging) were removed from the model.

Preliminary analysis

Descriptive statistics

A summary of sample demographics is provided in Table 1, with group comparisons for continuous variables. The mean and standard deviations for each Young Positive Schema Questionnaire, ERQ, and CFI subscale are provided in the supplementary materials (Table E1. Across subtypes, mean age varied from 25 to 30 years old, with females accounting for greater than 80% of participants (range = 81.82–95.48%). Post hoc comparisons (see supplementary materials table E2-3) found the healthy sample was significantly older than the AN, BN, and OSFED, but not BED sample. BMI varied from 18.5 to 27, with the AN sample reporting the lowest BMI (M = 18.56, SD = 2.69), which was significantly lower than all other groups. In contrast, the BED sample reported the highest BMI (M = 27.03, SD = 5.61), which was a significantly greater than all other groups. We note the elevated AN BMI is suggestive of a heterogenous sample including more mild-to-moderate cases and/or atypical presentations.

Table 1.

Continuous and categorical descriptive statistics for each eating disorder subtype

Anorexia nervosa Bulimia nervosa Binge eating disorder OSFED Healthy Comparisons
Mean (SD/range)
 Age 25.79 (7.06/18–56) 25.56 (5.22/18–41) 30.55 (11.2/18–54) 25.54 (5.21/18–39) 29.59 (12.38/18–78)

AN < healthy ***

BN < healthy ***

OSFED < healthy ***

 BMI 18.56 (2.69/13.13—26.3) 23.4 (4.61/16.3–34.16) 27.03 (5.61/18.01–38.78) 23.54 (4.93/15.83–39.01) 23.51 (4.89/13.18–38.87)

AN < BED***

AN < BN***

AN < healthy ***

AN < OSFED ***

BN < BED *

Healthy < BED *

OSFED < BED *

Count
 Sex
  Male 7 (4.51) 4 (7.27) 6 (18.18) 7 (7.53) 51 (10.1)
  Female 148 (95.48) 51 (92.73) 27 (81.82) 86 (92.47) 454 (89.9)
 Education
   < Grade 10 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 2 (6.06) 0 (0.0) 1 (0.2)
  Grade 10 3 (1.94) 1 (1.82) 0 (0.0) 7 (7.53) 21 (4.16)
  Grade 12 48 (30.97) 11 (20.0) 8 (24.24) 30 (32.26) 123 (24.36)
  VET 20 (12.9) 11 (20.0) 8 (24.24) 15 (16.13) 81 (16.04)
  Bachelor’s 62 (40.0) 19 (34.55) 12 (36.36) 28 (30.11) 201 (39.8)
  Masters 20 (12.9) 10 (18.18) 1 (3.03) 12 (12.9) 70 (13.86)
  Phd 2 (1.29) 3 (5.45) 2 (6.06) 1 (1.08) 8 (1.58)
 Nationality
  Australian 11 (7.1) 4 (7.27) 5 (15.15) 11 (11.83) 166 (32.87)
  New Zealand 5 (3.23) 2 (3.64) 1 (3.03) 0 (0.0) 8 (1.58)
  North American 87 (56.13) 29 (52.73) 19 (57.58) 54 (58.06) 156 (30.89)
  South American 1 (0.65) 2 (3.64) 1 (3.03) 2 (2.15) 18 (3.56)
  British 19 (12.26) 5 (9.09) 1 (3.03) 9 (9.68) 44 (8.71)
  Chinese 1 (0.65) 0 (0.0) 0 (0.0) 1 (1.08) 5 (0.99)
  Other 23 (14.84) 12 (21.82) 4 (12.12) 11 (11.83) 98 (19.41)
  Canadian 8 (5.16) 1 (1.82) 2 (6.06) 5 (5.38) 10 (1.98)
 Mental health diagnosis
  Major depressive disorder 74 (47.74) 28 (50.91) 15 (45.45) 35 (37.63) 0 (0.00)
  Bipolar disorder 15 (9.68) 4 (7.27) 5 (15.15) 14 (15.05) 0 (0.00)
  Social anxiety disorder 19 (12.26) 8 (14.55) 8 (24.24) 16 (17.2) 0 (0.00)
  Generalised anxiety disorder 79 (50.97) 25 (45.45) 15 (45.45) 43 (46.24) 0 (0.00)
  Panic disorder 16 (10.32) 3 (5.45) 5 (15.15) 7 (7.53) 0 (0.00)
  Post-traumatic stress disorder 40 (25.81) 10 (18.18) 7 (21.21) 33 (35.48) 0 (0.00)
  Substance use disorder 11 (7.1) 6 (10.91) 3 (9.09) 4 (4.3) 0 (0.00)
  Obsessive compulsive disorder 27 (17.42) 6 (10.91) 7 (21.21) 15 (16.13) 0 (0.00)
  Body dysmorphic disorder 37 (23.87) 12 (21.82) 5 (15.15) 14 (15.05) 0 (0.00)
  Borderline personality disorder 18 (11.61) 7 (12.73) 2 (6.06) 12 (12.9) 0 (0.00)
  Attention-deficit hyperactivity disorder 22 (14.19) 10 (18.18) 7 (21.21) 17 (18.28) 0 (0.00)
  Other 11 (7.1) 3 (5.45) 0 (0.0) 12 (12.9) 0 (0.00)

p < 0.05*, p < 0.01**, p < 0.001***

OSFED other specific feeding or eating disorder, PhD Philosophy higher degree, SD standard deviation, VET vocational education training

Correlations

Pearson correlations between continuous descriptive statistics and model variables are reported in the supplementary material as a function of ED subtype (Tables B1-5).

Primary analysis

Here, we report the final model (step 3) output (see Tables 2, 3, 4, 5, and 6 for full output).

Table 2.

Anorexia nervosa hierarchical multiple regression output

Variable B SE B β T-statistic p value Unique (semi-partial-squared) variance (%) R2 Adj R2 Δ R2 F-change (df)
Step 1 0.057 0.038 0.057 3.022 (3,151)*
 Constant 5.285 [3.051, 7.52] 1.131 4.673  < 0.001
 Age 0.011 [−0.015, 0.037] 0.013 0.069 0.871 0.385 0.47
 BMI −0.097 [−0.165, −0.029] 0.034 −0.222 −2.813 0.006 4.94
 Sex 0.29 [-0.591, 1.17] 0.446 0.051 0.650 0.517 0.26
Step 2 0.156 0.116 0.099 4.317 (4,147)**
 Constant 4.974 [2.571, 7.377] 1.216 4.091  < 0.001
 Age 0.021 [−0.005, 0.047] 0.013 0.125 1.581 0.116 1.44
 BMI −0.077 [−0.144, −0.01] 0.034 −0.176 −2.277 0.024 2.98
 Sex 0.175 [−0.678, 1.029] 0.432 0.031 0.406 0.685 0.09
 Cognitive reappraisal −0.197 [−0.378, −0.015] 0.092 −0.194 −2.136 0.034 2.62
 Expressive suppression 0.105 [−0.036, 0.246] 0.071 0.117 1.477 0.142 1.25
 Alternate 0.011 [−0.006, 0.028] 0.009 0.11 1.273 0.205 0.93
 Control −0.026 [−0.051, −0.001] 0.013 −0.188 −2.087 0.039 2.50
Step 3 0.376 0.304 0.220 5.410 (9,138)***
 Constant 4.412 [2.026, 6.798] 1.207 3.656  < 0.001
 Age 0.021 [−0.002, 0.045] 0.012 0.128 1.780 0.077 1.43
 BMI 0.424 [−0.106, 0.021] 0.032 −0.097 −1.316 0.190 0.78
 Sex −0.042 [−0.365, 1.214] 0.399 0.075 1.062 0.290 0.51
 Cognitive reappraisal −0.055 [−0.229, 0.12] 0.088 −0.054 −0.621 0.536 0.17
 Expressive suppression 0.104 [−0.067, 0.276] 0.087 0.116 1.203 0.231 0.65
 Alternate 0.008 [−0.009, 0.024] 0.008 0.076 0.890 0.375 0.36
 Control −0.004 [−0.029, 0.021] 0.013 −0.029 −0.325 0.745 0.06
 YPSQ_EFSA −0.022 [−0.208, 0.163] 0.094 −0.025 −0.237 0.813 0.03
 YPSQ_S −0.143 [−0.317, 0.032] 0.088 −0.144 −1.616 0.108 1.18
 YPSQ_EC 0.03 [−0.156, 0.216] 0.094 0.025 0.320 0.749 0.05
 YPSQ_O −0.158 [−0.347, 0.031] 0.095 −0.151 −1.656 0.100 1.24
 YPSQ_EOS 0.196 [−0.003, 0.395] 0.101 0.199 1.947 0.054 1.71
 YPSQ_SCRE −0.469 [−0.712, −0.225] 0.123 −0.356 −3.804  < 0.001 6.54
 YPSQ_DS −0.058 [−0.185, 0.068] 0.064 −0.07 −0.916 0.361 0.38
 YPSQ_SB −0.112 [−0.299, 0.075] 0.095 −0.114 −1.182 0.239 0.63
 YPSQ_HSC 0.131 [−0.027, 0.288] 0.080 0.149 1.639 0.103 1.22

p < 0.05*, p < 0.01**, p < 0.001***

B unstandardised beta, β standardised beta, DS developed self, EC empathic consideration, EFSA emotional fulfilment and stable attachment, EOS emotional openness, HSC healthy self-control, O optimism, S success, SB social belonging, SCRE self-compassion and realistic expectations, SE standard error, YPSQ Young Positive Schema Questionnaire, Δ change in

Table 3.

Bulimia nervosa hierarchical multiple regression output

Variable B SE B β T-statistic p value Unique (semi-partial-squared) variance (%) R2 Adj R2 Δ R2 F-change (df)
Step 1 0.201 0.154 0.201 4.281 (3,51)**
 Constant 2.271 [−0.729, 5.271] 1.494 1.520 0.135
 Age −0.051 [−0.109, 0.007] 0.029 −0.241 -1.754 0.085 4.82
 BMI 0.041 [−0.022, 0.104] 0.031 0.171 1.302 0.199 2.65
 Sex 1.271 [0.157, 2.385] 0.555 0.302 2.291 0.026 8.22
Step 2 0.473 0.394 0.271 6.045 (4,47)**
 Constant 1.165 [−1.99, 4.32] 1.568 0.743 0.461
 Age −0.042 [−0.098, 0.013] 0.028 −0.199 −1.525 0.134 2.61
 BMI 0.036 [−0.021, 0.093] 0.028 0.151 1.282 0.206 1.84
 Sex 0.791 [−0.188, 1.77] 0.487 0.188 1.625 0.111 2.96
 Cognitive reappraisal −0.174 [−0.492, 0.144] 0.158 −0.137 −1.099 0.277 1.36
 Expressive suppression 0.194 [−0.014, 0.401] 0.103 0.22 1.880 0.066 3.97
 Alternate 0.036 [0.008, 0.063] 0.014 0.307 2.620 0.012 7.7
 Control −0.034 [−0.07, 0.002] 0.018 −0.258 −1.898 0.064 4.04
Step 3 0.772 0.675 0.299 5.526 (9,38)***
 Constant 4.653 [1.822, 7.483] 1.398 3.328 0.002
 Age −0.004 [−0.053, 0.045] 0.024 -0.018 −0.162 0.872 0.02
 BMI −0.005 [−0.054, 0.045] 0.024 −0.019 −0.189 0.851 0.02
 Sex 0.49 [−0.286, 1.266] 0.383 0.117 1.279 0.209 0.98
 Cognitive reappraisal −0.286 [−0.563, -0.009] 0.137 −0.226 −2.087 0.044 2.62
 Expressive suppression −0.003 [−0.222, 0.215] 0.108 −0.004 −0.031 0.976 0
 Alternate 0.026 [−0.002, 0.054] 0.014 0.223 1.906 0.064 2.18
 Control −0.001 [−0.034, 0.032] 0.016 −0.007 −0.055 0.957 0
 YPSQ_EFSA −0.133 [−0.388, 0.121] 0.126 −0.164 −1.063 0.295 0.68
 YPSQ_S 0.231 [0.001, 0.462] 0.114 0.265 2.033 0.049 2.48
 YPSQ_EC 0.05 [−0.271, 0.37] 0.158 0.03 0.314 0.755 0.06
 YPSQ_O −0.127 [−0.385, 0.13] 0.127 −0.137 −1.000 0.324 0.6
 YPSQ_EOS −0.228 [−0.453, -0.004] 0.111 −0.248 −2.062 0.046 2.56
 YPSQ_SCRE −0.848 [−1.245, -0.451] 0.196 −0.6 −4.322  < 0.001 11.23
 YPSQ_DS 0.015 [−0.162, 0.193] 0.088 0.021 0.176 0.861 0.02
 YPSQ_SB 0.179 [−0.055, 0.413] 0.115 0.213 1.549 0.13 1.44
 YPSQ_HSC −0.048 [−0.235, 0.138] 0.092 −0.057 −0.525 0.603 0.17

p < 0.05*, p < 0.01**, p < 0.001***

B unstandardised beta, β standardised beta, DS developed self, EC empathic consideration, EFSA emotional fulfilment and stable attachment, EOS emotional openness, HSC healthy self-control, O optimism, S success, SB social belonging, SCRE self-compassion and realistic expectations, SE standard error, YPSQ Young Positive Schema Questionnaire, Δ change in

Table 4.

Binge eating disorder hierarchical multiple regression output

Variable B SE B β T-statistic p value Unique (semi-partial-squared) variance (%) R2 Adj R2 Δ R2 F-change (df)
Step 1 0.542 0.494 0.542 11.427 (3,29)***
 Constant 0.864 [−2.287, 4.015] 1.541 0.561 0.579
 Age −0.063 [−0.1, −0.025] 0.018 −0.439 −3.389 0.002 18.15
 BMI 0.161 [0.088, 0.235] 0.036 0.567 4.488 0.000 31.83
 Sex −0.023 [−1.106, 1.06] 0.530 −0.006 −0.043 0.966 0
Step 2 0.722 0.644 0.180 4.060 (4,25)*
 Constant 0.132 [−3.724, 3.988] 1.872 0.071 0.944
 Age −0.063 [−0.098, −0.027] 0.017 −0.439 −3.666 0.001 14.93
 BMI 0.11 [0.039, 0.18] 0.034 0.385 3.204 0.004 11.41
 Sex 0.3 [−0.795, 1.395] 0.532 0.074 0.564 0.578 0.35
 Cognitive reappraisal −0.446 [−0.752, −0.14] 0.148 −0.367 −3.006 0.006 10.04
 Expressive suppression 0.183 [−0.138, 0.504] 0.156 0.163 1.175 0.251 1.53
 Alternate 0.05 [0.014, 0.087] 0.018 0.338 2.832 0.009 8.91
 Control −0.028 [−0.087, 0.031] 0.029 −0.129 −0.990 0.332 1.09
Step 3 0.773 0.638 0.051 0.913 (5,20)
 Constant −2.177 [−7.089, 2.734] 2.355 −0.925 0.366
 Age −0.075 [−0.117, −0.033] 0.020 −0.523 −3.701 0.001 15.49
 BMI 0.161 [0.054, 0.268] 0.051 0.566 3.127 0.005 11.05
 Sex 0.594 [−0.6, 1.788] 0.572 0.146 1.038 0.312 1.22
 Cognitive reappraisal −0.6 [−1.067, −0.133] 0.224 −0.493 −2.679 0.014 8.11
 Expressive suppression 0.204 [−0.35, 0.757] 0.265 0.181 0.767 0.452 0.67
 Alternate 0.061 [0.016, 0.106] 0.022 0.411 2.822 0.011 9.01
 Control −0.038 [−0.111, 0.034] 0.035 −0.175 −1.099 0.285 1.37
 YPSQ_EC −0.154 [−0.666, 0.358] 0.246 −0.094 −0.626 0.538 0.44
 YPSQ_EOS 0.112 [−0.45, 0.675] 0.269 0.082 0.417 0.681 0.2
 YPSQ_SCRE 0.298 [−0.291, 0.887] 0.282 0.263 1.057 0.303 1.26
 YPSQ_DS −0.159 [−0.504, 0.187] 0.166 −0.146 −0.956 0.351 1.03
 YPSQ_HSC 0.234 [−0.281, 0.749] 0.247 0.175 0.949 0.354 1.02

p < 0.05*, p < 0.01**, p < 0.001***

B unstandardised beta, β standardised beta, DS developed self, EC empathic consideration, EOS emotional openness, HSC healthy self-control, SCRE self-compassion and realistic expectations, YPSQ Young Positive Schema Questionnaire, Δ change in

Table 5.

OSFED hierarchical multiple regression output

Variable B SE B β T-statistic p value Unique (semi-partial-squared) variance (%) R2 Adj R2 Δ R2 F-change (df)
Step 1 0.039 0.007 0.039 1.212 (3,89)
Constant 2.18 [−0.574, 4.934] 1.386 1.573 0.119
Age 0.001 [−0.061, 0.062] 0.031 0.003 0.031 0.976 0
BMI 0.057 [−0.008, 0.122] 0.033 0.191 1.752 0.083 3.31
Sex 0.213 [−0.94, 1.365] 0.580 0.038 0.367 0.715 0.15
Step 2 0.186 0.118 0.146 3.817 (4,85)**
Constant 1.285 [−1.815, 4.384] 1.559 0.824 0.412
Age −0.006 [−0.065, 0.053] 0.030 −0.02 −0.187 0.852 0.03
BMI 0.052 [−0.01, 0.113] 0.031 0.173 1.674 0.098 2.68
Sex 0.048 [−1.08, 1.177] 0.568 0.009 0.085 0.932 0.01
Cognitive reappraisal −0.239 [−0.514, 0.037] 0.138 −0.195 −1.723 0.088 2.85
Expressive suppression 0.336 [0.119, 0.552] 0.109 0.325 3.078 0.003 9.08
Alternate 0.014 [−0.012, 0.04] 0.013 0.118 1.056 0.294 1.07
Control 0 [−0.038, 0.038] 0.019 0 −0.003 0.997 0
Step 3 0.384 0.254 0.198 2.719 (9,76)**
Constant 2.345 [−1.156, 5.845] 1.758 1.334 0.186
Age −0.04 [−0.1, 0.019] 0.030 −0.142 −1.349 0.181 1.48
BMI 0.066 [0.004, 0.127] 0.031 0.22 2.117 0.038 3.63
Sex −0.481 [−1.695, 0.733] 0.610 −0.087 −0.789 0.432 0.5
Cognitive reappraisal −0.155 [−0.465, 0.155] 0.156 −0.127 −0.996 0.323 0.8
Expressive suppression 0.324 [0.035, 0.613] 0.145 0.314 2.235 0.028 4.05
Alternate 0.001 [−0.025, 0.027] 0.013 0.006 0.054 0.957 0
Control 0.006 [−0.037, 0.05] 0.022 0.039 0.293 0.771 0.07
YPSQ_EFSA −0.164 [−0.568, 0.241] 0.203 −0.132 −0.806 0.423 0.53
YPSQ_S −0.075 [−0.38, 0.229] 0.153 −0.064 −0.494 0.623 0.2
YPSQ_EC 0.102 [−0.196, 0.4] 0.150 0.076 0.683 0.497 0.38
YPSQ_O −0.246 [−0.617, 0.126] 0.187 −0.196 −1.315 0.192 1.4
YPSQ_EOS 0.314 [−0.055, 0.684] 0.186 0.263 1.694 0.094 1.48
YPSQ_SCRE −0.328 [−0.711, 0.056] 0.193 −0.258 −1.702 0.093 0.5
YPSQ_DS 0.264 [0.01, 0.518] 0.127 0.243 2.074 0.041 3.63
YPSQ_SB −0.006 [−0.365, 0.352] 0.180 −0.006 −0.034 0.973 0.8
YPSQ_HSC 0.196 [−0.093, 0.485] 0.145 0.165 1.354 0.180 4.05

p < 0.05*, p < 0.01**, p < 0.001***

B unstandardised beta, β standardised beta, DS developed self, EC empathic consideration, EFSA emotional fulfilment and stable attachment, EOS emotional openness, HSC healthy self-control, O optimism, S success, SB social belonging, SCRE self-compassion and realistic expectations, SE standard error, YPSQ Young Positive Schema Questionnaire, Δ change in

Table 6.

Healthy sample hierarchical multiple regression output

Variable B SE B β T-statistic P value Unique (semi-partial-squared) variance (%) R2 Adj R2 Δ R2 F-change (df)
Step 1 0.145 0.14 0.145 28.239 (3,501)***
 Constant 1.712 [0.529, 2.895] 0.602 2.843 0.005
 Age −0.055 [−0.067, −0.043] 0.006 −0.395 -8.686  < 0.001 12.88
 BMI 0.074 [0.042, 0.105] 0.016 0.209 4.587  < 0.001 3.59
 Sex 0.577 [0.109, 1.045] 0.238 0.101 2.425 0.016 1
Step 2 0.386 0.378 0.242 48.923 (4,497)***
 Constant 1.489 [0.036, 2.942] 0.740 2.013 0.045
 Age −0.03 [−0.041, −0.019] 0.006 −0.215 −5.266  < 0.001 3.42
 BMI 0.075 [0.048, 0.102] 0.014 0.212 5.472  < 0.001 3.7
 Sex 0.61 [0.204, 1.016] 0.207 0.107 2.952 0.003 1.08
 Cognitive reappraisal −0.326 [−0.443, −0.209] 0.059 −0.23 −5.493  < 0.001 3.73
 Expressive suppression 0.32 [0.229, 0.412] 0.047 0.261 6.884  < 0.001 5.85
 Alternate 0.014 [0.001, 0.026] 0.006 0.081 2.096 0.037 0.54
 Control −0.046 [−0.062, −0.03] 0.008 −0.247 −5.580  < 0.001 3.84
Step 3 0.513 0.497 0.126 14.044 (9,488)***
 Constant 2.608 [1.165, 4.05] 0.734 3.552  < 0.001
 Age −0.019 [−0.03, −0.008] 0.005 −0.135 −3.442 0.001 1.18
 BMI 0.071 [0.046, 0.096] 0.013 0.201 5.600  < 0.001 3.13
 Sex 0.518 [0.149, 0.886] 0.188 0.09 2.756 0.006 0.76
 Cognitive reappraisal −0.125 [−0.237, −0.012] 0.057 −0.088 −2.169 0.031 0.47
 Expressive suppression 0.185 [0.072, 0.298] 0.057 0.151 3.224 0.001 1.04
 Alternate 0.013 [0.001, 0.025] 0.006 0.078 2.132 0.034 0.45
 Control −0.019 [−0.035, −0.002] 0.009 −0.1 −2.173 0.030 0.47
 YPSQ_EFSA −0.284 [−0.414, −0.154] 0.066 −0.213 −4.293  < 0.001 1.84
 YPSQ_S −0.008 [−0.135, 0.12] 0.065 −0.005 −0.123 0.902 0
 YPSQ_EC −0.022 [−0.142, 0.099] 0.061 −0.012 −0.352 0.725 0.01
 YPSQ_O −0.112 [−0.251, 0.027] 0.071 −0.083 −1.580 0.115 0.25
 YPSQ_EOS 0.072 [−0.057, 0.2] 0.065 0.055 1.099 0.273 0.12
 YPSQ_SCRE −0.437 [−0.574, −0.301] 0.070 −0.325 −6.291  < 0.001 3.95
 YPSQ_DS 0.08 [−0.019, 0.179] 0.050 0.062 1.579 0.115 0.25
 YPSQ_SB 0.044 [−0.069, 0.156] 0.057 0.034 0.760 0.447 1.18
 YPSQ_HSC −0.006 [−0.117, 0.105] 0.057 −0.004 −0.106 0.916 0.76

p < 0.05*, p < 0.01**, p < 0.001***

B unstandardised beta, β standardised beta, DS developed self, EC empathic consideration, EFSA emotional fulfilment and stable attachment, EOS emotional openness, HSC healthy self-control, O optimism, S success, SB social belonging, SCRE self-compassion and realistic expectations, SE standard error, YPSQ Young Positive Schema Questionnaire, Δ change in

Anorexia nervosa

The final model including early adaptive schemas explained an additional 20% of the variance in EDE-Q Global Score (Adj. R2 = 30.4%), F-change (9, 138) = 5.410, p < 0.001. Of the predictors, only Self-Compassion and Realistic Expectations accounted for a significant proportion of variance (β = −0.356, p < 0.001), explaining 6.54% of unique variance in EDE-Q Global Score. Specifically, increased Self-Compassion and Realistic Expectations were associated with decreased ED symptomology.

Bulimia nervosa

Model three accounted an additional 27% of the variance (Adj. R2 = 67.5%) in EDE-Q Global Score, F-change (9, 38) = 5.526, p < 0.001. Analysis of individual scales showed success (β = 0.265, p = 0.049) was the only significant positive predictor of Global Score, whilst Cognitive Reappraisal (β =−0.226, p = 0.044), Emotional Openness (β =−0.248, p = 0.046), and Self-Compassion and Realistic Expectations (β =−0.600, p < 0.001) were significant negative predictors. Higher levels the Success schema was associated with greater ED symptomology, whilst stronger Cognitive Reappraisal, Emotional Openness, Self-Compassion and Realistic Expectations were associated with decreased ED symptoms. Amongst these significant predictors, Self-Compassion and Realistic Expectations accounted for the most (11.23%) unique variance in Global Score, followed by Cognitive Reappraisal (2.62%), Emotional Openness (2.56%), and Success (2.48%).

Binge eating disorder

The addition of early adaptive schemas in model three did not account for additional variance (Adj. R2 = 63.8%) in EDE-Q Global Score, F-change (6, 19) = 0.896, p = 0.518. Analysis of individual scales showed Alternatives (β = 0.411, p = 0.011) and BMI (β = 0.566, p = 0.005) remained significant positive predictors of Global Score, whilst Cognitive Reappraisal (β =−0.493, p = 0.014), and age (β =−0.523, p = 0.001) remained significant negative predictors. Specifically, higher values of BMI and Alternatives scores were associated with greater ED symptomology, whilst increased age and Cognitive Reappraisal predicted decreased ED symptoms. Amongst these significant predictors, age accounted for the most (15.49%) unique variance in global score, followed by BMI (11.05%), Alternatives (9.01%), and Cognitive Reappraisal (8.11%).

Other specified feeding or eating disorders

Model three accounted for an additional 19.8% of the variance in EDE-Q Global Score (Adj. R2 = 25.4%), F-change (9, 76) = 2.719, p = 0.008. Expressive Suppression (β = 0.314 p = 0.028) retained its significant positive association, however, the inclusion of early adaptive schemas led to BMI (β = 0.220, p = 0.038) and Developed Self (β = 0.243, p = 0.041) being significant positive predictors. Explicitly, increased Expressive Suppression, Developed Self, and BMI were associated with greater ED symptomology. Amongst these significant predictors, Expressive Suppression (4.05) and Healthy Self-control (4.05%) accounted for the most unique variance, followed by BMI (3.63%), and Developed Self (3.63%).

Healthy

Model three accounted for an additional 12.6% of the variance in EDE-Q Global Score (Adj. R2 = 49.7%), F-change (9, 488) = 14.044, p < 0.001. The addition of the early adaptive schemas decreased the significance level of Expressive Suppression (β = 0.151 p = 0.001), Control (β =−0.100, p = 0.030), and Cognitive Reappraisal (β = 0.090, p = 0.031), although they remained significant predictors, as did age (β =−0.135, p < 0.001), BMI (β = 0.201 p < 0.001), sex (β = 0.090, p = 0.006), and Alternatives (β = 0.078, p = 0.034). Emotional Fulfilment and Stable Attachment (β =−0.213, p < 0.001), and Self-Compassion and Realistic Expectations (β =−0.325, p < 0.001) were the strongest negative predictors, accounting for 1.84 and 3.95% of unique variance in Global Score, respectively. Of the positive predictors, BMI (3.13%) accounted for the most unique variance, followed by Expressive Suppression (1.04%).

Discussion

Summary

Overall, these findings indicate the relationship between early adaptive schemas, cognitive flexibility, emotional regulation, and ED symptoms varies considerably across diagnostic subtypes. In line with the hypotheses, the inclusion of early adaptive schemas accounted for the greatest proportion of variance in ED symptoms in all samples, except for BED. In the case of BED, the model including the CFI and ERQ subscales accounted for the most variance. Contrary to predictions, Healthy Self-Control, Emotional Openness and Spontaneity, and Social Belonging did not independently predict decreased ED symptoms across samples. However, Emotional Openness and Spontaneity did significantly predict lower ED symptoms in BN. Lastly, the hypothesised relationship between Self-Compassion and Realistic Expectations and decreased ED symptoms across diagnoses was partially supported, insofar as it was the strongest predictor of decreased ED symptoms in the AN, BN and healthy samples. In contrast, age and BMI were the strongest predictors for BED, whilst Expressive Suppression was the strongest predictor for OSFED.

Notably, the covariates of age, sex, and BMI displayed ED subtype specific relationships. Age was only a significant predictor of ED symptoms in the BED and healthy control groups, and in both cases, increasing age was associated with lower ED symptoms. Sex was only a significant predictor of ED symptoms in the healthy control group, such that females had significantly greater ED symptoms. BMI was a significant predictor of ED symptoms in the BED, OSFED, and healthy control groups, which predicted greater ED symptoms in all cases.

Strengths and limits

In assimilating these findings within the existing literature, several limitations must be considered. First, the self-reported nature of the data is a key limitation, and hence, pause is justified when considering the degree of bias inherent in the data, and the overall reliability and validity of the inferences drawn from it. Second, the sample sizes for BN, BED, and OSFED were small compared to the AN and healthy samples, which may contribute to imprecise parameter estimates. Further to this point, a significant number (n = 100) of survey respondents who reported multiple EDs were not included in the analysis as concurrent ED diagnoses cannot occur. Third, the values reported suggest the sub-groups somewhat departed from a homogenous grouping. For instance, the healthy sample reported a mean EDE-Q global score of 2.91, which, when compared to previous validation studies in community samples (EDE-Q Global Score = 1.4–1.5; [54]), indicates this sample likely included individuals with undiagnosed EDs. Relatedly, the AN sub-groups BMI was elevated (M = 18.56, SD = 2.69), indicating the sample included mild-to-moderate cases and/or atypical presentations.

Considering the survey was primarily posted on ED-related pages and forums, this is to be expected. Although this may speak to the prevalence of subclinical ED symptoms and heterogeneity of presentations in the population, it introduces a substantial confound, and therefore, we exercised (and recommend) caution when interpreting the results. Finally, the use of a 9-factor structure of the Young Positive Schema Questionnaire was used (see [1]), complicating direct comparisons to studies using the previously validated 14-factor structure [13]. Further studies using populations in Australia should test out the 14-factor structure and test its robustness.

All observed whole and local effects had large effect sizes according to Cohen [55] excluding local effects for OSFED and the Healthy group (see Supplementary Materials G for power analysis). The sample sizes used provided post hoc statistical power values above 0.8 at significance level, α = 0.05 for almost all models except for OSFED. Therefore, a limitation here was that the sample size for the investigation of OSFED was not large enough and should be considered in future studies. Another limitation was the use of linear approximation even in cases where conditions for this were violated. However, no non-linear terms were included in the models in this study to reduce model complexity given the limited sample sizes. The use of larger sample sizes in future studies will open possibilities for considering non-linear relationships between predictors and outcomes.

Despite these limitations, the current study provides an informative investigation of the variability in ED symptomology accounted for by early adaptive schemas, cognitive flexibility, and emotional regulation across ED subtypes. Furthermore, this study provides a robust and transparent application of the HLR package (with available code) in an effort to promote open-access alternatives to conducting this method of statistical analysis.

What is already known?

Early adaptive schemas [11], emotion dysregulation [24, 25], and cognitive flexibility [30, 33] have each independently been associated with ED symptom presentation and severity. Emerging research has highlighted their inter-dependence [27, 30, 56], however, limited research has evaluated which factors account for the most variance in ED symptoms and whether this differs across diagnostic subtypes. A recent examination of early adaptive schemas and the Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire 6.0 (EDE-Q) subscales [15] found healthy boundaries, emotional openness, and spontaneity, social belonging, and self-care were ‘protective’ against ED symptoms, whilst increased optimism predicted greater ED pathology. However, the studied sample (N = 388) was clinically heterogenous (50% reported current ED diagnosis).

What does this study add?

The current findings support previous research indicating an association between ED symptom severity and emotion regulation [21, 22], cognitive flexibility [33], and early adaptive schemas [15]. Specifically, our findings are in accordance with existing literature indicating BED, BN, and AN display a similar emotional regulation profile [20, 22], whilst BED displays a unique profile. For example, we found the ERQ (specifically Cognitive Reappraisal) and CFI (specifically Alternatives) accounted for a significant portion of variance in BED symptom severity, whilst early adaptive schemas showed no such association. It is worth noting that Svaldi et al. [23] previously proposed that individuals with BED might possess superior abilities to recognise and regulate emotions compared to other ED subtypes. In addition, individuals with BED have been observed to use both positive and negative metacognitive beliefs regarding binge eating as a coping strategy to reduce worry and unpleasant emotions [57]. In terms of cognitive flexibility, several previous studies have not included BED in comparative analyses with other ED types [29, 58]. However, in support of the present finding, one study found that individuals with BED exhibited poorer cognitive flexibility than those with AN [32].

Relatedly, our findings suggest that early adaptive schemas may serve as maintenance factors for eating psychopathology in AN and BN, but not in BED. This suggests the potential for disorder-specific treatment strategies, individuals with AN and BN may benefit from attachment-based therapies targeting core emotional needs. Consistent with the ‘protective’ role of Healthy Boundaries reported by Maher [15], Self-Compassion and Realistic Expectations was associated with decreased ED symptom severity in AN, BN, and healthy participants. This contributes to the robust evidence base highlighting perfectionism (i.e. Unrelenting Standards, the antithesis of Realistic Expectations) as a key factor in maintaining ED pathology [3]. Notably, several studies have found an association between perfectionism and low self-compassion [59, 60], a particularly important association in the context of EDs. Individuals with EDs who fear self-compassion tend to have worse treatment outcomes [61], which has been previously associated with two factors [62]. First is a fear of losing personal standards, including perceptions of weakness, fear of reduced motivation, and concerns about being seen as failures. Second, is a fear of emotional vulnerability and feeling unworthy of compassion. This fear and self-compassion can be explained by the challenges faced by individuals with histories of trauma, abuse, neglect, or unmet emotional needs in forming attachment relationships. During therapy, efforts to encourage healthy attachments can reactivate their attachment system, triggering grief and unpleasant emotions they have learned to avoid. The re-emergence of these difficult feelings can pose a barrier to recovery.

In terms of treatment implications, schema therapy presents as a promising of option for anorexia nervosa (AN) and bulimia nervosa (BN), as it can progressively challenge high standards and self-criticism whilst promoting self-compassion and realistic expectations. Although research supports schema therapies effectiveness [5], its specific impact on enhancing self-compassion across different ED presentations requires further investigation. For instance, cognitive flexibility and emotion regulation appear to influence binge eating disorder (BED) more than early adaptive schemas, suggesting that therapies like metacognitive therapy [63], and dialectical behavioural therapy [64], respectively. However, whilst there exists some emerging evidence comparing these therapies to cognitive behaviour therapy [65], the comparative effectiveness with respect schema therapy for EDs remains a matter for future research.

Conclusion

The current study provides preliminary evidence that early adaptive schemas, cognitive flexibility, and emotional regulation are associated with ED symptoms in a subtype specific manner. Future experimental research is needed to better understand the relationship between these factors and EDs symptoms, and the role they may have in guiding schema-focussed therapies.

Supplementary Information

Below is the link to the electronic supplementary material.

Supplementary material 1. (623.1KB, docx)
Supplementary material 2. (489.9KB, pdf)

Acknowledgements

We wish to acknowledge the participant who contributed their time and shared their experiences to inform this research.

Author contributions

J.M conceptualised the paper, developed the original draft, was responsible for the data analysis, development of associated visuals, in addition to the writing, review, and editing of the finalised manuscript. T.E.A developed the analysis package. T.H and A.A conceptualised the original study, and T.H. was responsible for data collection. D.H provided supervision of the corresponding author and facilitated access to resources. All the authors contributed to the review and editing of the finalised manuscript. All the authors have read and approved the final version of the manuscript.

Funding

Open Access funding enabled and organized by CAUL and its Member Institutions. This work forms part of the corresponding author’s PhD supported by the Australian Government RTP scholarship.

Availability of data and materials

An example of the study analysis code is provided in the supplementary materials. The package used for the primarily analysis can be accessed at https://github.com/teanijarv/HLR, and the code to reproduce the supplementary material example analysis can be found at https://github.com/JulesMitchell/SchemaHLR. The (de-identified) data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.

Declarations

Ethics approval and consent to participate

The study was approved by the University of the Sunshine Coast Ethics Committee (S201469). Consent was provided by participants via selecting a checkbox prior to beginning the survey. See the original paper by Huckstepp et al. [1]

Informed consent

Informed consent was obtained from all individual participants included in the study.

Competing interests

John P. Louis receives revenues from his parenting programme that is taught, and books sold, worldwide. All the other authors declare they have no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher's Note

Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

References

  • 1.Huckstepp TJ et al (2023) Factor structure of the Young Positive Schema Questionnaire in an eating disorder sample. Eat Weight Disord Stud Anorexia Bulimia Obesity 28(1):13. 10.1007/s40519-023-01549-0 10.1007/s40519-023-01549-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Treasure J, Duarte TA, Schmidt U (2020) Eating disorders. Lancet 395:899–911 10.1016/S0140-6736(20)30059-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Stackpole R et al (2023) The association between eating disorders and perfectionism in adults: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Eat Behav 50:101769 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2023.101769 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Pugh M (2015) A narrative review of schemas and schema therapy outcomes in the eating disorders. Clin Psychol Rev 39:30–41 10.1016/j.cpr.2015.04.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Joshua PR et al (2023) Is schema therapy effective for adults with eating disorders? A systematic review into the evidence. Cogn Behav Ther. 10.1080/16506073.2022.2158926 [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 6.Young JE, Klosko JS, Weishaar ME (2003) Schema therapy: a practitioner’s guide. Guildford Press, New York, p 436 [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Riso LP et al (2006) The long-term stability of early maladaptive schemas. Cogn Ther Res 30(4):515–529 10.1007/s10608-006-9015-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Messman-Moore TL, Coates AA (2007) The impact of childhood psychological abuse on adult interpersonal conflict: the role of early maladaptive schemas and patterns of interpersonal behavior. J Emot Abus 7(2):75–92 10.1300/J135v07n02_05 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Bruysters NA-O, Pilkington PD (2023) Overprotective parenting experiences and early maladaptive schemas in adolescence and adulthood: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Psychopathol 30:10–23 10.1002/cpp.2776 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Pilkington PA-O, Bishop A, Younan R (2021) Adverse childhood experiences and early maladaptive schemas in adulthood: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Clin Psychol Psychopathol 28:569 10.1002/cpp.2533 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Maher AL et al (2022) Early maladaptive schemas in eating disorders: a systematic review. Eur Eat Disord Rev 30(1):3–22 10.1002/erv.2866 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Thimm JC, Chang M (2022) Early maladaptive schemas and mental disorders in adulthood: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Cogn Ther 15(4):371–413 10.1007/s41811-022-00149-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Louis JP et al (2018) Positive clinical psychology and Schema Therapy (ST): the development of the Young Positive Schema Questionnaire (YPSQ) to complement the Young Schema Questionnaire 3 Short Form (YSQ-S3). Psychol Assess 30(9):1199–1213 10.1037/pas0000567 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Lockwood G, Perris P (2012) A new look at core emotional needs. In: Vreeswijk M, Broersen J, Nadort M (eds) The Wiley-Blackwell handbook of schema therapy: theory, research, and practice. Wiley Blackwell, Hoboken, pp 41–66 [Google Scholar]
  • 15.Maher AL et al (2022) Exploring the association between early adaptive schemas and self-reported eating disorder symptomatology. Clin Psychol Psychother. 10.1002/cpp.2789 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 16.Dormann CF et al (2013) Collinearity: a review of methods to deal with it and a simulation study evaluating their performance. Ecography 36(1):27–46 10.1111/j.1600-0587.2012.07348.x [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Vatcheva KP et al (2016) Multicollinearity in regression analyses conducted in epidemiologic studies. Epidemiology (Sunnyvale) 6(2):227 10.4172/2161-1165.1000227 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Gross JJ (1998) The emerging field of emotion regulation: an integrative review. Rev Gen Psychol 2(3):271–299 10.1037/1089-2680.2.3.271 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Gross JJ, John OP (2003) Individual differences in two emotion regulation processes: implications for affect, relationships, and well-being. J Pers Soc Psychol 85(2):348–362 10.1037/0022-3514.85.2.348 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Ruscitti C et al (2016) Difficulties in emotion regulation in patients with eating disorders. Borderline Pers Disord Emot Dysregul 3(1):3 10.1186/s40479-016-0037-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Palmieri S et al (2023) Emotion dysregulation in patients with eating disorders: the role of metacognitions and repetitive negative thinking. Cogn Ther Res 47(4):655–668 10.1007/s10608-023-10398-1 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Mallorquí-Bagué N et al (2018) Emotion regulation as a transdiagnostic feature among eating disorders: cross-sectional and longitudinal approach. Eur Eat Disord Rev 26(1):53–61 10.1002/erv.2570 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Svaldi J et al (2012) Emotion regulation deficits in eating disorders: a marker of eating pathology or general psychopathology? Psychiatry Res. 10.1016/j.psychres.2011.11.009 [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 24.Williams-Kerver GA et al (2020) Differences in affective dynamics among eating disorder diagnostic groups. Clin Psychol Sci 8(5):857–871 10.1177/2167702620917196 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Prefit A-B, Cândea DM, Szentagotai-Tătar A (2019) Emotion regulation across eating pathology: a meta-analysis. Appetite 143:104438 10.1016/j.appet.2019.104438 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Leppanen J et al (2022) The role of emotion regulation in eating disorders: a network meta-analysis approach. Front Psychiatry. 10.3389/fpsyt.2022.793094 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 27.Gerges S et al (2022) Maladaptive cognitive schemas as predictors of disordered eating: examining the indirect pathway through emotion regulation difficulties. Int J Environ Res Public Health 19(18):11620 10.3390/ijerph191811620 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Dennis JP, Vander Wal JS (2010) The cognitive flexibility inventory: instrument development and estimates of reliability and validity. Cogn Ther Res 34(3):241–253 10.1007/s10608-009-9276-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Perpiñá C, Segura M, Sánchez-Reales S (2017) Cognitive flexibility and decision-making in eating disorders and obesity. Eat Weight Disord Stud Anorexia Bulimia Obesity 22(3):435–444 10.1007/s40519-016-0331-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Tchanturia K et al (2011) Cognitive flexibility and clinical severity in eating disorders. PLoS ONE. 10.1371/journal.pone.0020462 10.1371/journal.pone.0020462 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Tchanturia K et al (2012) Poor cognitive flexibility in eating disorders: examining the evidence using the Wisconsin Card Sorting Task. PLoS ONE 7(1):e28331 10.1371/journal.pone.0028331 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Aloi M et al (2015) Decision making, central coherence and set-shifting: a comparison between binge eating disorder, anorexia nervosa and healthy controls. BMC Psychiatry 15(1):6 10.1186/s12888-015-0395-z [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Roberts ME et al (2007) A systematic review and meta-analysis of set-shifting ability in eating disorders. Psychol Med. 10.1017/S0033291707009877 10.1017/S0033291707009877 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Louis JP, Louis KM, Young AM (2022) Positive schemas: replication, associations with negative schemas, and the dark triad. Psychol Rep. 10.1177/00332941221100445 [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 35.Paetsch A et al (2022) Psychometric properties of the German Version of the Young Positive Schema Questionnaire (YPSQ) in the general population and psychiatric patients. J Pers Assess 104(4):522–531 10.1080/00223891.2021.1966020 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Fairburn CG, Beglin SJ. Eating disorder examination questionnaire. Cognitive Behavior Therapy and Eating Disorders. 2008; 309–313.
  • 37.Peterson CB et al (2007) Psychometric properties of the eating disorder examination-questionnaire: factor structure and internal consistency. Int J Eat Disord 40(4):386–389 10.1002/eat.20373 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Danner UN, Sternheim L, Evers C (2014) The importance of distinguishing between the different eating disorders (sub)types when assessing emotion regulation strategies. Psychiatry Res 215(3):727–732 10.1016/j.psychres.2014.01.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Von Wilson S et al. (2022). AnotherSamWilson/miceforest: Release for Zenodo DOI (DOI). Zenodo. 10.5281/zenodo.7428632
  • 40.The pandas development team. pandas-dev/pandas: Pandas (v2.0.0rc0). 2023.
  • 41.Harris CR et al (2020) Array programming with NumPy. Nature 585(7825):357–362 10.1038/s41586-020-2649-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Waskom ML (2021) seaborn: statistical data visualization. J Open Sour Softw 6(60):3021. 10.21105/joss.03021
  • 43.Vallat R (2018) Pingouin: statistics in Python. J Open Sour Softw 3(31):1026 10.21105/joss.01026 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Charlier F et al. trevismd/statannotations (v0.5) 2022. Zenodo. 10.5281/zenodo.8396665
  • 45.Pedregosa F et al (2011) Scikit-learn: machine learning in python. J Mach Learn Res 12:2825–2830 [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Leys C et al (2018) Detecting multivariate outliers: use a robust variant of the Mahalanobis distance. J Exp Soc Psychol 74:150–156 10.1016/j.jesp.2017.09.011 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Leys C et al (2013) Detecting outliers: do not use standard deviation around the mean, use absolute deviation around the median. J Exp Soc Psychol 49(4):764–766 10.1016/j.jesp.2013.03.013 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Anijärv TE, Boyle R. teanijarv/HLR: v0.1.4. 2023; v0.1.4.
  • 49.Wagenmakers E-J et al (2018) Bayesian inference for psychology. Part I: theoretical advantages and practical ramifications. Psychonomic Bull Rev 25(1):35–57 10.3758/s13423-017-1343-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Morey RD et al (2016) The fallacy of placing confidence in confidence intervals. Psychon Bull Rev 23(1):103–123 10.3758/s13423-015-0947-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Hanley J, Moodie E (2011) Sample size, precision and power calculations: a unifed approach. J Biometr Biostat 2(5):1–9 [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Papageorgiou G et al (2018) Statistical primer: how to deal with missing data in scientific research? Interact Cardiovasc Thorac Surg 27(2):153–158 10.1093/icvts/ivy102 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Tabachnick BG, Fidell LS (2019) Using multivariate statistics, 7th edn. Pearson, London [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Mond JM et al (2004) Validity of the Eating Disorder Examination Questionnaire (EDE-Q) in screening for eating disorders in community samples. Behav Res Ther 42(5):551–567 10.1016/S0005-7967(03)00161-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Cohen J (1988) Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences, 2nd edn. Routledge, New York [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Nicol A et al (2022) Emotion regulation as a mediator between early maladaptive schemas and non-suicidal self-injury in youth. J Behav Cogn Ther 32(3):161–170 10.1016/j.jbct.2022.05.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Palmieri SA-O et al (2021) Metacognitive beliefs across eating disorders and eating behaviours: a systematic review. Clin Psychol Psychoth. 10.1002/cpp.2573 10.1002/cpp.2573 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Wang SA-O et al (2019) Cognitive rigidity and heightened attention to detail occur transdiagnostically in adolescents with eating disorders. Eat Disord. 10.1080/10640266.2019.1656470 10.1080/10640266.2019.1656470 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Ferrari MA-O et al (2018) Self-compassion moderates the perfectionism and depression link in both adolescence and adulthood. PLoS ONE. 10.1371/journal.pone.0192022 10.1371/journal.pone.0192022 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Linnett RJ, Kibowski F (2019) A multidimensional approach to perfectionism and self-compassion. Self and Identity 19(7):757–783 10.1080/15298868.2019.1669695 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Kelly AC et al (2013) Self-compassion and fear of self-compassion interact to predict response to eating disorders treatment: a preliminary investigation. Psychother Res. 10.1080/10503307.2012.717310 10.1080/10503307.2012.717310 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 62.Geller J et al (2019) Barriers to self-compassion in the eating disorders: the factor structure of the fear of self-compassion scale. Eat Behav. 10.1016/j.eatbeh.2019.101334 [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 63.Mansueto G et al (2024) A systematic review of the relationship between generic and specific metacognitive beliefs and emotion dysregulation: a metacognitive model of emotion dysregulation. Clin Psychol Psychother 31(1):e2961 10.1002/cpp.2961 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Rozakou-Soumalia N, Dârvariu Ş, Sjögren JA-O (2021) Dialectical behaviour therapy improves emotion dysregulation mainly in binge eating disorder and bulimia nervosa: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Pers Med. 10.3390/jpm11090931 10.3390/jpm11090931 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Lammers MA-O et al. Dialectical behavior therapy compared to cognitive behavior therapy in binge-eating disorder: an effectiveness study with 6-month follow-up. Int J Eat Disord 55(7):902–913. 10.1002/eat.23750 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary material 1. (623.1KB, docx)
Supplementary material 2. (489.9KB, pdf)

Data Availability Statement

An example of the study analysis code is provided in the supplementary materials. The package used for the primarily analysis can be accessed at https://github.com/teanijarv/HLR, and the code to reproduce the supplementary material example analysis can be found at https://github.com/JulesMitchell/SchemaHLR. The (de-identified) data that support the findings of this study are available from the corresponding author upon request.


Articles from Eating and Weight Disorders are provided here courtesy of Springer

RESOURCES