Abstract
Study Objectives
To define sleep-related movements in healthy adults according to sex and age.
Methods
Sleep-related movements from 50 video-polysomnography (vPSG) recordings of 27 men and 23 women, from 20 to 70 years old, were classified according to International classification of sleep disorders (ICSD-3-TR) and American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) criteria (codified movements); the remaining movements (non-codified movements) were described according to type (elementary movements-EMs or complex movements-CMs), topography (focal, segmental, multifocal or generalized) and, if present, were assigned to motor patterns (MPs).
Results
Of 4057 movements analyzed, 54.6% (2216/4057) were non-codified (1861 CMs, 355 EMs) and 1841 were codified. CMs were mainly generalized (70%) while EMs were multifocal (40%) or focal (30%). The median movement index (MI; movement/hour) was 11 and the median duration was 4 seconds. MI decreased from stages N1/REM > N2 > N3; men showed a higher MI. An MP was assigned to 2204 codified and non-codified movements, mainly stretching (50%) and scratching (30%). Stretching increased in REM sleep while food-carrying behaviors increased in N2. Men showed more food-carrying behaviors, changes of body positions, and comfort movements while stretching was more common in women. Younger participants exhibited more food-carrying behaviors, while scratching and stretching were more prevalent in the middle-aged group. Older participants showed more changes in body positions and comfort movements.
Conclusions
In total, 54.6% of sleep-related movements in healthy participants were non-codified and characterized by motor sequences that can configure MPs. Our comprehensive classification method allows a detailed description of the physiological movements underlying differential motor control during sleep stages influenced by age and sex.
Keywords: aging, behavioral sleep medicine, healthy participants, motor pattern, non-codified, periodic limb movements, sleep in women, sleep-related movements, sleep–wake physiology, video-polysomnography
Graphical Abstract
Statement of Significance.
Not all movements observed during sleep in healthy participants correspond to the motor events defined in the International Classification of Sleep Disorders and American Academy of Sleep Medicine or commonly described in the literature. Our study proposed a three-step video classification method aimed at analyzing the heterogeneous sleep-related physiological movements combining movement type and topography with the identification of specific motor patterns. The latter may facilitate the differential diagnosis between physiological and pathological phenomena, especially in patients with nocturnal epilepsy or parasomnias where physiological movements could be misinterpreted as minor pathological events. We applied this method to 50 participants of different ages and found significant differences according to sleep stages, sex, and age. These findings highlight how these factors can influence motor control during sleep.
A wide range of motor events can be observed during normal sleep. The different expressions of motor activity depend on several elements within the sleep framework (sleep stage, delta power, and arousals) and on individual characteristics (i.e. age and habits) [1, 2]. Physiological movements during sleep have interested researchers since before the introduction of video-polysomnography (vPSG) through direct observation [3] or indirect signs, such as the electromyographic (EMG) signal, electroencephalographic (EEG) artifacts, and mechanical bedspring transducers [4–6]. By means of vPSG, the current gold standard for the detection and diagnosis of all sleep-related motor events, some physiological movements had already been characterized and included in the third edition of the International Classification of Sleep Disorders (ICSD-3-TR) and American Academy of Sleep Medicine (AASM) (i.e. limb movement- LM) [7, 8], while others were commonly reported in literature (i.e. neck myoclonus- NM) [9] or recently described as a new entity, such as large muscle group movements- LMMs [10]. The remaining sleep-related motor events are challenging to classify due to their multifaceted nature, thus there have been only a few attempts to categorize them in detail. Stefani et al. [11] proposed a classification based on movement complexity and the involvement of the different body parts while Brás et al. [12] focused on the motor patterns (MPs) associated with spontaneous motor arousals. The latter are sequences of movements, involving recurrent body areas probably produced by the activation during arousals of central pattern generators (CPGs) [13, 14]. The MPs range from simple, phylogenetically conserved automatic movements, such as swallowing, to more complex ones that resemble wakeful activities, such as head orientation. Physiological MPs were originally observed as incidental findings in vPSG recordings of patients with nocturnal epilepsy and NREM parasomnias but their recognition also in the healthy control participants suggested an intrinsic rather than a pathological sleep phenomenon [15–17]. This observation complicates the differential diagnosis between physiological and pathological motor events, as the boundaries between the two are still blurred in some cases. Thus, although many efforts have been made to describe and quantify movements during sleep in healthy individuals [18], the controversies in this field have not been fully resolved and a unified classification encompassing all features is currently lacking.
The aim of this study was to describe and quantify all physiological movements during sleep by combining their types, semiology, and topography with the identification of specific MPs through detailed video analysis. We assess this comprehensive classification method in different age groups to investigate possible differences depending on age, sex, and sleep stages.
Methods
Participants
Fifty healthy participants (10 for each decade, from 20 to 70 years of age) were recruited from a random population of caregivers of patients who were consecutively referred to Bellaria Hospital between 2017 and 2022.
After a first screening interview by a hospital neurologist, each healthy volunteer underwent a structured interview with a neurologist expert in sleep medicine.
The main inclusion criteria were whether participants perceived their sleep as restorative and Epworth Sleepiness Scale (ESS) score < 10. Exclusion criteria included the presence of any sleep disorder according to the ICSD-3-TR [7] as well as other neurological, psychiatric, cardiac, pulmonary, renal, hepatic, metabolic or oncological diseases, pregnancy, the use of psychoactive medication, regular alcohol and high caffeine consumption.
The study was approved by our local Ethical Committee (Code CE: 17176). The consent of healthy participants was obtained in agreement with the Convention of Helsinki.
Video-polysomnography
All participants underwent an in-lab or home 24-hour nocturnal home-vPSG (XLTEK Trex HD, Natus Medical Incorporated®, video-camera Handycam HDR-CX700, Sony, 12.3 Megapixel resolution). All participants were instructed to keep their habitual routine on the day preceding the exam. During the night of the recording, participants had to sleep without blankets to allow the full detection of all movements. Video-polysomnography (vPSG) recordings included 3 EEG channels (frontal, central, and occipital), electrocardiogram, electro-oculogram, chin and both anterior tibialis electromyography, thoracoabdominal respirogram, and synchronized audio–video recording. Sleep stages were scored in 30-second epochs according to the AASM criteria [8].
Movement identification and descriptors
All visible sleep-related movements were identified and classified according to type, topography and were eventually assigned to a MP (see below in the section “Movement Classification”) by a sleep medicine trainee (A.M.); the MP was reviewed by two sleep specialists (F.P., G.L.) and any disagreements were resolved through consensus at the time. To identify the movements, the time-synchronized video was analyzed if there was an increase in EMG activity with a duration of >100 milliseconds on the mentalis or limbs muscles channels [11] and/or a movement artifact on at least two channels [10]. We excluded movements arising from wake after sleep onset (WASO). The duration of the movements was calculated as the time between the exact onset and offset observed in the video. Video analysis was extended to at least 3 seconds mini-epoch before the motor event to determine the exact onset of the movement and the presence of possible external (i.e. ambient noise) or internal triggers (i.e. deep inspiration and cough). Two movements were considered separate if there was an interruption in the motor sequence lasting longer than 1 second [10] and the EEG between movements corresponded to a sleep stage.
For each movement, the temporal appearance during the night and the sleep stage of onset [10] were provided as descriptors.
Movement classification
A) Movements were classified according to (A) type and (B) topography (adapting a previous classification proposed by Stefany et al. [11]); additionally, if present, they were assigned to (C) a MP (Figure 1). Since the type and topography of the “codified movements” are well-known, we have described these two aspects (A and B) only for the “non-codified movements,” while for the recognition of MPs (C), we have included all identified movements to ensure that no movements are omitted. Movements corresponding to those listed in the ICSD-3-TR [7] and the AASM manuals [8] or commonly described and accepted in the literature (NM and oro-alimentary automatisms-OAs) [9, 11] were classified as “codified movements.” The remaining motor events, the “non-codified” movements, were divided into elementary movements (EMs) and complex movements (CMs). The former were simple motions involving one or more body parts simultaneously, while the latter corresponded to a sequence of movements, eventually evolving into organized actions. The EMs and CMs were further characterized as myocloniform and non-myocloniform, respectively, if they were the results of a sudden, brief, shock-like muscle contraction or not.
B) Non-codified movements were defined as focal, segmental, multifocal, or generalized according to the number of body parts involved, considering five regions (head, upper trunk, lower trunk, arms, and legs). Focal movements involved only 1 body part, segmental 2 contiguous body parts, multifocal 2 noncontiguous or 3 parts (contiguous or not); generalized movements involved more than 3 noncontiguous body parts. As the CMs consisted of motor sequences, they were analyzed both at the beginning (T0) and at the end of the movement (T1).
C) Codified and non-codified movements were reviewed and eventually assigned to specific MPs. The latter were constituted by the combination of simple movements into integrated behaviors (such as exploring the environment, defense behaviors, scratching, stretching, comfort movements, manipulative behaviors, and food-carrying behaviors) or simple changes in body position associated or not with vocalizations. Exploring the environment pattern was represented by the opening of the eyes and/or a slow head rotation (“orientation”). Defense behaviors were represented by a jerky flexion of the arm, bringing the hand to the face as if to protect it. Manipulative behaviors consisted of fine hand movements as if to manipulate something, in isolation or preceded by arm flexion to locate objects. We chose the term food-carrying behaviors because the association of OAs (chewing/tongue-snapping/swallowing) with arm or foot movements with or without head movements, might resemble animal behavior [19], where preparatory actions to assess the distance to the target often precede food intake [20]. The food-carrying behaviors of MPs were also observed in codified movements when an LM was associated with OAs.
Statistical analysis
In the descriptive analysis, the data were presented as the median and interquartile range (IQR) for continuous variables and with absolute (n) and relative frequency (%) for categorical variables. The movement index (MI) was defined as the sum of the number of movements divided by the hours of sleep (movements per hour) and was stratified by temporal distribution during the night (dividing the night into three parts), sleep stages, sex, and age of the participants. The Kruskal–Wallis test for continuous variables or Chi-square test for categorical variables were used for the group comparisons. Spearman’s Rho coefficients were used to evaluate the correlation between variables. The p-value was corrected for multiple comparisons (Bonferroni correction) and p-value < .05 was considered significant. Statistical analysis was performed using Stata SE 14.2.
Results
Participants and sleep macrostructure
We analyzed 50 vPSG of healthy participants (27 M, 23 F) with a median BMI of 23.5 (IQR 21.9–26.6). Participants were divided into five age groups: 20–29 years (four women); 30–39 years (five women); 40–49 years (four women); 50–59 years (six women); and 60–70 years (four women). The median age was 43 years (IQR 30–57). Fourteen patients presented with snoring and eight of them had also associated respiratory events, with a median apnea–hypopnea index of 1.3 (IQR 0.7–2.8) (Table 1).
Table 1.
Sleep parameters | Total | 20–29 years | 30–39 years | 40–49 years | 50–59 years | 60–70 years |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
TTS (min) | 395.0 (362.5–435.0) |
388.0 (362.0–409.0) |
408.0 (321.0–475.0) |
409.0 (377.0–436.0) |
394.3 (340.5–409.5) |
392.5 (369.5–425.0) |
SL (min) | 6.0 (3.8–10.2) |
11.0 (5.9–15.0) |
6.9 (3.4–15.0) |
4.1 (2.0–4.8) |
6.1 (4.2–8.9) |
6.5 (2.9–9.2) |
REM L (min) | 70.0 (59.0–83.0) |
88.5 (63.5–132.0) |
72.0 (66.0–83.0) |
64.0 (53.0–74.5) |
75.3 (66.0–82.0) |
59.3 (42.0–69.5) |
SE (min) | 90.7 (85.6–93.6) |
90.9 (88.4–93.6) |
91.0 (86.6–95.0) |
92.8 (91.0–96.4) |
87.0 (81.2–93.0) |
87.9 (85.0–91.3) |
N1 (%) | 6.0 (4.0–9.4) |
6.3 (4.1–11.7) |
4.0 (3.2–6.0) |
5.5 (3.7–8.4) |
5.8 (4.0–7.3) |
12.0 (7.7–19.4) |
N2 (%) | 44.9 (37.6–50.4) |
45.5 (37.6–50.7) |
44.5 (39.4–54.1) |
45.7 (44.3–51.5) |
47.2 (39.1–49.6) |
37.6 (32.4–38.2) |
N3 (%) | 26.3 (20.8–29.9) |
27.5 (23.1–29.5) |
26.2 (20.0–35.1) |
27.3 (21.0–29.0) |
27.3 (21.0–29.0) |
28.8 (20.8–29.9) |
REM (%) | 21.5 (17.3–26.0) |
17.4 (15.3–25.0) |
21.1 (13.0–24.0) |
24.5 (19.1–27.3) |
22.7 (17.7–28.0) |
21.9 (16.9–23.7) |
PLMIAAMS [8] | 1.3 (0.5–8.6) |
0.7 (0–4.8) |
1.6 (0.8–5.4) |
0.7 (0.6–7.3) |
0.7 (0–1.6) |
9.7 (3.2–24.7) |
PLMIWASM [39] | 0.9 (0–6.9) |
0.6 (0–2.7) |
1.2 (0.1–4.8) |
0.3 (0–6.5) |
0.4 (0–1.6) |
8.9 (2.9–24.7) |
Values are presented as the median and interquartile range (IQR). TTS, total sleep time; SL, sleep latency; REM L, rapid eye movement (REM) sleep latency; SE, sleep efficiency; PLMI, periodic limb movements (PLM) index.
Sleep-related movement analysis
Type of movements.
A total of 4057 movements were analyzed, 3488 (86%) spontaneous and 569 (14%) evoked (398, 70%, by an internal and 171, 30%, by an external stimulus). A proportion of 1841 (45,4%) movements fulfilled the ICSD-3-TR [7] or AASM [8] criteria or were commonly reported in the literature (codified movements). The remaining movements (non-codified movements) consisted of 1861 (45.9%) complex movements (CMs) and 355 (8.7%) elementary movements (EMs). Among the codified movements, we identified 1464 (79.5%) leg movements (LM), 131 (7.1%) isolated OAs, 120 (6.5%) NM, 38 (2.1 %) rhythmic movements, 25 (1.4%) hypnic jerks, 24 (1.3%) hypnagogic foot tremor, 23 (1.2%) alternating leg muscle activation, and 16 (0.9%) chewing-like movements associated with rhythmic masticatory muscle activity. Additionally, we observed OAs at the beginning (in 198 movements; 4.9%) or within the motor sequence (in 465 movements; 11.5%).
The median total MI was 11 (IQR 8–15), with a homogeneous distribution throughout the three-thirds of the night (first tertile: median 10, IQR 7–14; second tertile: median 10, IQR 6–16 and third tertile: median 11, IQR 7–18). MI significantly decreased with the deepening of sleep, from N1 > N2 > N3 stages, to increase again during REM sleep (Table 2 and Figure 2A). The median duration of all movements was 4 seconds (IQR 2–8) and the movement duration was inversely proportional to sleep depth (N3 median duration: 5 seconds, IRQ 2–12; N1 vs. N3 p = .015, N2 vs. N3 p < .001), reaching its minimum during REM sleep (REM median duration: 3 seconds, IRQ 2–7; REM vs. N1/N2/N3 p < .001).
Table 2.
Movement | Total | N1 | N2 | N3 | REM | P-value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
CODIFIED and NON-CODIFIED | 11 (8–15) | 15 (7–31) | 10 (6–16) | 5 (3–8) | 15 (10–24) |
N1 vs. N2 = .001
**
N1 vs. N3 < .001 *** N2 vs. N3 < .001*** N2 vs. REM = .001** N3 vs. REM < .001*** |
CODIFIED | 5 (2–7) | 5 (0–17) | 4 (1–8) | 1 (0–3) | 6 (3–9) |
N1 vs. N2 = .032
*
N1 vs. N3 < .001 *** N2 vs. N3 < .001 *** N2 vs. REM = .001 ** N3 vs. REM < .001*** |
NON-CODIFIED | 6 (4–8) | 8 (4–15) | 6 (4–9) | 3 (2–5) | 8 (4–13) |
N1 vs. N2 = .023
*
N1 vs. N3 < .001*** N2 vs. N3 < .001*** N2 vs. REM = .007** N3 vs. REM < .001*** |
Elementary | 1 (0–2) | 0 (0–3) | 1 (0–1) | 0 (0–1) | 1 (0–3) |
N2 vs. REM = .032*
N3 vs. REM < .001*** |
Myocloniform | 0 (0–0) | 0 (0–0) | 0 (0–0) | 0 (0–0) | 0 (0–1) |
N1 vs. REM = .001
**
N2 vs. REM = .004 ** N3 vs. REM = .010 * |
Non-myocloniform | 1 (0–1) | 0 (0–3) | 1 (0–1) | 0 (0–1) | 1 (0–2) |
N1 vs. N3 = .034*
N2 vs. N3 = .014* N3 vs. REM < .001*** |
Complex | 5 (4–7) | 7 (4–12) | 5 (3–7) | 3 (2–4) | 6 (4–11) |
N1 vs. N2 = .016
*
N1 vs. N3 < .001 *** N2 vs. N3 < .001 *** N2 vs. REM = .029 * N3 vs. REM < .001 *** |
Myocloniform | 1 (0–1) | 0 (0–0) | 0 (0–1) | 0 (0–1) | 1 (0–2) |
N1 vs. REM = .001
**
N2 vs. REM < .001 *** N3 vs. REM < .001 *** |
Non-myocloniform | 5 (3–6) | 6 (4–12) | 5 (3–6) | 2 (1–4) | 5 (3–9) |
N1 vs. N2 = .001
**
N1 vs. N3 < .001 *** N2 vs. N3 < .001 *** N3 vs. REM < .001 *** |
Focal | 0 (0–1) | 0 (0–2) | 0 (0–1) | 0 (0–0) | 0 (0–1) | n.s. |
Segmental | 0 (0–0) | 0 (0–0) | 0 (0–0) | 0 (0–0) | 0 (0–0) | n.s. |
Multifocal | 1 (0–2) | 2 (0–4) | 1 (0–2) | 1 (0–1) | 2 (1–3) |
N1 vs. N3 = .011
*
N2 vs. N3 = .022 * N3 vs. REM = .007 ** |
Generalized | 4 (2–5) | 4 (2–8) | 3 (2–6) | 2 (1–3) | 5 (3–9) |
N1 vs. N3 < .001
***
N2 vs. N3 < .001 *** N2 vs. REM = .007 ** N3 vs. REM < .001 *** |
Values are presented as the median and interquartile range (IQR). The latter are presented by type (elementary and complex), subtype (myocloniform and non-myocloniform), and topography of the entire motor sequence (focal, segmental, multifocal, and generalized). Significant p-value after post hoc Bonferroni correction are presented; n.s. = non-significant (p-value > .05). Italics* represent significant difference (p < .05). * means p-value < .05, ** means < .01 and *** means < .001.
In comparison to females, males showed a significantly higher MI, particularly in the first third of the night (male/female MI = 12.5/7; p = .002) and shorter movement duration (p = .030; Table 3 and Figure 2B). Age was not associated with significant differences in MI, whereas movement duration significantly declined in the 50–59 year age group (median duration in 50–59 age group: 3 seconds, IRQ 2–6; 20–29 vs. 50–59 < 0.001, 30-39 vs. 50–59 = 0.002, 40–49 vs. 50–59 < 0.001, 60–70 vs. 50–59 = 0.001; Table 4 and Figure 2C). Women had a higher number of non-codified movements compared to men. EMs and myocloniform movements showed a significant increase during REM sleep. CMs with myocloniform onset were elevated in young adults and peaked in the 30–39 age group. Isolated OAs showed no significant differences in their MI across sleep stages, sex, and age.
Table 3.
Movement | Women | Men | P-value |
---|---|---|---|
Codified and non-codified | 10(5–13) | 11.5(9–17.5) | .044* |
Codified | 3(2–6) | 5(3–8.5) | .039* |
Non-codified | 6(3–8) | 7(5.5–8.5) | .18 |
Elementary | 1(0–2) | 1(0–2) | 0.56 |
Myocloniform | 0(0–0) | 0(0–0) | .50 |
Non-myocloniform | 0(0–1) | 1(0–1) | .50 |
Complex | 5(3–6) | 6(4–7) | .13 |
Myocloniform | 1(0–1) | 1(0–1) | .92 |
Non-myocloniform | 4(2–5) | 5(3–6.5) | .12 |
Focal | 0(0–0) | 0(0–1) | .22 |
Segmental | 0(0–1) | 0(0–0) | .082 |
Multifocal | 1(0–1) | 1.5(1–3) | .072 |
Generalized | 3(2–5) | 4(3–5) | .14 |
Values are presented as the median and interquartile range (IQR). The latter are presented by type (elementary and complex), subtype (myocloniform and non-myocloniform), and topography of the entire motor sequence (focal, segmental, multifocal, and generalized). Significant p-value after post hoc Bonferroni correction are presented; italics* represent significant difference (p < .05). * means p-value < .05, ** means < .01 and *** means < .001.
Table 4.
Movement | 20–29 years | 30–39 years | 40–49 years | 50–59 years | 60–69 years | P-value |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Codified and non-codified | 12(8–15) | 11.5(7–21) | 10(6–16) | 10(9–11) | 11.5(8–12) | .87 |
Codified | 3(2–5) | 4(2–6) | 2(1–7) | 5.5(3–7) | 6(4–8) | .48 |
Non-codified | 8(6–9) | 7(4–12) | 7(5–7) | 5(3–6) | 6(4–7) | .31 |
Elementary | 2(1–2) | 1(0–3) | 0.5(0–1) | 1(0–2) | 1(0–1) | .25 |
Myocloniform | 0(0–1) | 0(0–0) | 0(0–0) | 0(0–1) | 0(0–0) | .10 |
Non-myocloniform | 1(0–1) | 0(0–3) | 1(0–1) | 0(0–1) | 1(0–2) | .44 |
Complex | 6(4–7) | 5(3–11) | 6.5(5–7) | 4.5(2–6) | 5(4–6) | .41 |
Myocloniform | 1(0–1) | 2(1–2)* | 0(0–1) | 0.5(0–1) | 0.5(0–1) | .015* |
Non-myocloniform | 5(4–7) | 4(2–8) | 6(4–7) | 3(2–5) | 5(3–5) | .26 |
Focal | 1(0–2)** | 0(0–0) | 0(0–1) | 0(0–1) | 0(0–0) | .002** |
Segmental | 0(0–0) | 0.5(0–1) | 0(0–0) | 0(0–0) | 0(0–0) | .26 |
Multifocal | 2(1–3) | 1(1–1) | 1.5(0–2) | 1(0–2) | 1(1–2) | .77 |
Generalized | 4(2–4) | 4.5(2–10) | 5(4–5) | 2.5(2–5) | 3.5(2–5) | .36 |
Values are presented as the median and interquartile range (IQR). The latter are presented by type (elementary and complex), subtype (myocloniform and non-myocloniform), and topography of the entire motor sequence (focal, segmental, multifocal, and generalized). Significant p-value after post hoc Bonferroni correction are presented; italics* represent significant difference (p < .05). * means p-value < .05, ** means < .01 and *** means < .001.
Topography of non-codified movements.
Among the non-codified movements, 1387 movements (62.7%) were generalized, 588 (26.5%) multifocal, 125 (5.6%) segmental, and 116 (5.2%) focal. Movement topography across sleep stages, sex, and age groups are presented in Tables 2, 3, and 4, respectively.
EMs were, in descending order of prevalence, multifocal (135; 38%), focal (105; 30%), segmental (67; 19%), and generalized (48; 13%; Figure 3, on the left). The onset (T0) of CMs was focal in more than one-third of cases; then movements generalized or became multifocal (T1), particularly when they began as myocloniform (Figure 3, on the right).
Motor patterns.
MPs were identified in 2204 (54.3%) of the total sleep-related movements: 2151 (97.6%) were non-codified movements (1843 CMs, 308 EMs) and 53 (2.4%) were codified movements. The latter consisted of LM associated with OAs and were assigned to the food-carrying behaviors MP. The most frequently observed MPs were stretching (50%) and scratching (25%; Table 5) both of which were commonly observed at the beginning of a motor sequence (in nearly 85% of the cases), while all the other MPs were usually observed within the motor sequence. Vocalizations were rare (15 CMs), mostly occurring within the motor sequence and consisting of unintelligible moans and snorts.
Table 5.
Motor pattern | Total number | Median age | P-value |
---|---|---|---|
Stretching (1) | 1081(49.0) | 43(30–58) |
vs. 5 < .001
***
vs. 2 < .001 *** |
Scratching (2) | 551(25.0) | 41(28–54) | vs. 1 < .001 *** |
Comfort movements (3) and change in body position (4) | 273(12.4) | 46(30–56) | vs. 5 = .002 ** |
Food-carrying behaviors (5) | 107(4.9) | 32(24–46) |
vs. 3 = .002
**
vs. 1 < .001 *** |
Stretching (1) and scratching (2) | 93(4.2) | 42(30–54) | n.s. |
Exploring the environment (6) | 63(2.9) | 44(27–54) | n.s. |
Manipulative behaviors (7) | 27(1.2) | 34(28–62) | n.s. |
Categorical data were presented as absolute number and percentage, continuous data were presented as median and interquartile range (IQR). Significant p-values after post hoc Bonferroni correction are presented; n.s. = non-significant (p-value > .05). Italics* represent significant difference (p < .05). * means p-value < .05, ** means < .01 and *** means < .001.
Food-carrying behaviors were more frequent (+25%) in N2 NREM stage while stretching increased (+8%) and scratching and food-carrying behaviors were reduced (−10% and −16%, respectively) in REM stage (Figure 4A). Exploring the environment MP was mostly observed in NREM stage (45; 67.2%) and, in one-fifth of the cases, it was evoked by triggers. One-third of the exploring the environment MPs began with the eye-opening whereas this MP appeared more frequently within the motor sequence (out of 93 CMs) while the participant maintained a neutral facial expression; exploring the environment MPs was rarely associated with a head lifting (15; 38.5%). Men showed a higher frequency of food-carrying behaviors (p = .003), change of body position, and comfort movements (p = .002), while women frequently showed stretching (p ≤ .001) and exploring the environment MPs (p = .35; Figure 4B). Younger participants (median 32 years) showed more food-carrying behaviors; in the middle age group (median 41 and 43 years, respectively) scratching and stretching were the most frequent MPs; older participants (median 46 years) showed more often change of body position and comfort movements (Table 5). In Supplementary Materials we include examples of the manipulative behaviors, food-carrying behaviors and exploring the environment MPs.
Discussion
In our cohort, we observed that more than half of the sleep-related physiological movements could not be assigned to any specific classified motor event according to ICSD-3-TR [7] or AASM [8] criteria or any other movement commonly described in literature. The challenge in classifying these non-codified movements lies in their variability within and between participants throughout the night. To overcome this, our classification method builds upon previous attempts and integrates various movement aspects of increasing complexity. We began with a dichotomous distinction of movements based on type, categorizing them as either isolated (EMs) or organized into motor sequences (CMs). Then, we focused on topography which depends on the number of body areas involved. Finally, the recurrent combination of body areas led to the identification of specific MPs, which sometimes also have behavioral significance.
We found that most of the non-codified movements were CMs that typically started as focal movements, often involving the face or leg, and subsequently involved the whole body, particularly when they began as myocloniform. Movements occurred mainly in N1 and REM stages; as NREM sleep deepened, MI decreased significantly while duration increased in line with previous data from the literature [2, 11, 18, 21]. Males showed more movements than females as previously described [11, 18] and of shorter duration. Although the differences in MI between age groups did not reach statistical significance as in other studies [11, 18], MI is higher in the age groups at the extremes (<39 years and >60 years) potentially reflecting a mechanism of maturation [2, 22] and aging-related changes [23, 24] of the central inhibitory system.
In over half of the sleep-related movements, an MP was assigned: stretching and scratching were the most observed MPs, particularly at the beginning of the motor sequence. Stretching, which tended to be briefer and simpler, was frequently identified during REM sleep where movements are typically short-lasting and jerky [11, 12, 18, 25]. Otherwise, longer motor sequences were associated with better organized MPs, which often occurred during NREM sleep such as food-carrying, scratching, and exploring the environment MPs. Accordingly, in a cohort of 25 healthy young adults, Brás et al. [12] observed a higher prevalence of chewing in N2 stage, while scratching and exploratory behaviors were frequently observed during N3 stage.
In males, comfort movements and changes of body position prevailed along with food-carrying behaviors, which possibly might reflect their ethologically conserved role in procuring food. Conversely, women exhibit more stretching movements, which in animals represent a defensive behavior and exploration of peri-personal space, possibly to protect offspring from dangers [26, 27]. In fact, stretching might represent an information-gathering activity that allows us to mentally visualize the body schema during the night, converting the multiple inputs from receptors and, at the same time, identifying potentially dangerous environments [28].
In the middle-aged group, we found mainly scratching and stretching, which seem to have the common function of stress reduction in several animal species. Scratching rate in fact increases with sympathetic activation and during conflict in several animal species such as chimpanzees, long-tailed macaques, and baboons [29, 30]. Scratching during sleep is frequently observed in patients with epilepsy [15, 31–33] where it can be triggered or not by the ictal discharge. However, scratching in healthy participants appeared slower, less repetitive, and stereotyped compared to epileptic patients [15]. Stretching could reduce stress, as it does during the day [34] and may also be the result of a proprioceptive feedback mechanism that benefits the musculoskeletal system, particularly triggered during sleep by prolonged immobility.
Although no clear sexual behaviors were observed in the healthy participants, the stretching MP, when it involved the hips, could resemble the “pelvic thrusting” observed in motor episodes associated with epilepsy [35, 36] and sexsomnia [37]. During a seizure, however, the hip-swinging tends to be more pronounced and repetitive. Furthermore, when the stretching involves the limbs, it may resemble a tonic or dystonic posture, raising suspicion of an epileptic manifestation [38], especially if it occurs early in the movement sequence. However, physiological stretching is characterized by less sustained contractions and a more natural and symmetric posture. Otherwise, exploring the environment MP poses a challenge for differential diagnosis with the Disorders of Arousal episodes (DoA). However, in healthy participants, this MP occurred frequently in the middle-aged group, was more commonly evoked by triggers and occurred later in the motor sequence. When occurring early, it was not associated with trunk elevation, as if trying to get out of bed, or with expressions of confusion as observed in DoA episodes [16]. Finally, in older participants, there is an increased prevalence of the simplest MPs, comfort movements and change of body positions, which may be associated with a reduced cortical inhibition and movement organization that follows the course of cortical involution.
One strength of this study lies in our extensive effort to describe a large number of sleep-related physiological movements. To this end, we examined video data in detail even in the presence of artifacts and instructed participants to sleep without blankets. Our work is in line with a prior study that proposed a classification system for the non-codified physiological movements during sleep, but we expanded this classification by also incorporating the recognition of specific MPs. The latter could ease the differential diagnosis between physiological and pathological motor events occurring in patients with parasomnias and nocturnal epilepsy, particularly in the presence of minor motor events which are the most challenging. Furthermore, we had carefully selected a homogeneous group of healthy participants, balanced in terms of age and sex.
Our study has also some limitations to disclose. Small movements of the upper limbs and trunk might be lost due to the methodology chosen to identify movements. The sample consists of a single ethnic group, which could limit the generalizability of our results. Additionally, we only observed one night per participant, which prevents the assessment of inter-night differences or adaptation effects. However, many of our participants had undergone home-based vPSG, which allowed them to be in a familiar environment. It is important to note that we did not focus on facial expressions as the single camera was not always directed toward the face. Nonetheless, we did not observe overt facial expressions, unlike episodes of parasomnias and epilepsy.
Conclusion
This is the first study that comprehensively classified the non-codified sleep-related movements in healthy participants, combining their type and topography with the recognition of specific MPs. The latter results from the observation of a recurring combination of different body parts that resemble behaviors that have been preserved throughout the phylogenetic scale. Thanks to this three-step approach, we were able to describe and quantify sleep-related movements that would otherwise have been neglected yet account for the majority of movements in healthy participants of all ages, particularly in the female population.
Compared to a previous study [11], of which we followed the nomenclature, we observed that most of the non-codified movements during sleep were complex, consisting of motor sequences with a focal onset and subsequently involving multiple body areas or becoming generalized. The topographic distribution also differs from that previous study [11], since we specifically focused on the non-codified movements.
The observed differences in movement features as a function of sleep stages, sex, and age in healthy adults suggest that sleep-related movements are controlled by different CNS generators.
In particular, the movement gate during sleep is related to sleep depth and CNS integrity, both of which vary with age. Younger individuals showed a higher rate of movements, mostly myocloniform, suggesting an ongoing maturation process. In middle age, there were fewer movements during sleep, especially stretching and scratching, which probably served to relieve stress and prolonged immobility. In older participants, reduced motor inhibition due to neurodegeneration could lead to simpler and non-myocloniform behavioral episodes, like comfort movements. The sex differences observed in sleep-related movements, although speculative due to the lack of data in the literature on this topic, appear to preserve behaviors that are ethologically and phylogenetically ingrained and may be related to evolutionary or hormonal differences, such as feeding behavior in males and defense against potential environmental threats in females.
The main strength of the study is the identification of specific MPs that might help clinicians by providing criteria for recognizing physiological movements during the nights of patients with sleep-related disorders. This becomes even more significant as home videos are gaining prominence, serving not merely as a screening tool. It also opens new windows in the field of research, where automatic video analysis of movements is emerging and has the potential to recognize and assess movement characteristics.
Supplementary Material
Supplementary material is available at SLEEP online.
Acknowledgments
The authors thank Cecilia Baroncini, MA, for English editing.
Contributor Information
Angelica Montini, Department of Biomedical and NeuroMotor Sciences (DIBINEM), University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy.
Giuseppe Loddo, Department of Primary Care, Azienda AUSL Bologna, Bologna, Italy.
Corrado Zenesini, IRCCS, Istituto delle Scienze Neurologiche di Bologna, UOC Clinica Neurologica Rete Metropolitana NEUROMET, Bellaria Hospital, Bologna, Italy.
Greta Mainieri, Department of Biomedical and NeuroMotor Sciences (DIBINEM), University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy; IRCCS, Istituto delle Scienze Neurologiche di Bologna, UOC Clinica Neurologica Rete Metropolitana NEUROMET, Bellaria Hospital, Bologna, Italy.
Luca Baldelli, Department of Biomedical and NeuroMotor Sciences (DIBINEM), University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy; IRCCS, Istituto delle Scienze Neurologiche di Bologna, UOC Clinica Neurologica Rete Metropolitana NEUROMET, Bellaria Hospital, Bologna, Italy.
Francesco Mignani, IRCCS, Istituto delle Scienze Neurologiche di Bologna, UOC Clinica Neurologica Rete Metropolitana NEUROMET, Bellaria Hospital, Bologna, Italy.
Susanna Mondini, IRCCS, Istituto delle Scienze Neurologiche di Bologna, UOC Clinica Neurologica Rete Metropolitana NEUROMET, Bellaria Hospital, Bologna, Italy.
Federica Provini, Department of Biomedical and NeuroMotor Sciences (DIBINEM), University of Bologna, Bologna, Italy; IRCCS, Istituto delle Scienze Neurologiche di Bologna, UOC Clinica Neurologica Rete Metropolitana NEUROMET, Bellaria Hospital, Bologna, Italy.
Author Contribution
Angelica Montini: Conceptualization, Methodology, Formal analysis, Data curation, Software, Investigation, Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing, Validation, Supervision, Visualization. Giuseppe Loddo: Conceptualization, Methodology, Investigation, Formal analysis, Writing—review & editing, Validation, Supervision, Project administration. Corrado Zenesini: Statistical analysis. Greta Mainieri: Conceptualization, Investigation, Formal analysis, Validation, Writing—review & editing. Luca Baldelli: Conceptualization, Investigation, Software, Formal analysis, Validation, Writing—review & editing. Francesco Mignani: Methodology, Formal analysis. Susanna Mondini: Conceptualization, Investigation, Formal analysis, Validation, Writing—review & Editing, Supervision, Project administration. Federica Provini: Conceptualization, Methodology, Investigation, Visualization, Formal analysis, Writing—original draft, Writing—review & editing, Validation, Supervision, Project administration.
Disclosure Statement
Financial disclosure statement: The authors declare that they have no known competing financial arrangements or connections that could have influenced the work reported in this paper. Nonfinancial disclosure statement: The authors declare that they have no known potential conflicts of interest that could have influenced the work reported in this paper.
Data Availability
The data underlying this article are available in the article and its online supplementary material.
The data on which this article is based were provided with the patients’ consent.
References
- 1. Kryger MH, Roth T DW.. Principles and Practice of Sleep Medicine. Edition 7th ed. Philadelphia, PA: Elsevier, 2021. [Google Scholar]
- 2. Fukumoto M, Mochizuki N, Takeishi M, Nomura Y, Segawa M.. Studies of body movements during night sleep in infancy. Brain Dev. 1981;3(1):37–43. doi: 10.1016/s0387-7604(81)80004-6 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3. De Lisi L. Su di un fenomeno motorio costante del sonno normale: le mioclonie ipniche fisiologiche. Riv Pat Nerv Ment. 1932;39:481–496. [Google Scholar]
- 4. Jacobson A, Kales A, Lehmann D, Hoedemaker FS.. Muscle tonus in human subjects during sleep and dreaming. Exp Neurol. 1964;10(5):418–424. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(64)90033-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Rechtschaffen A, Kales A.. A manual of standardized terminology, techniques and scoring system for sleep stages of human subjects. Los Angeles, CA: Brain Information Service, 1968. [Google Scholar]
- 6. Crisp AH, Stonehill E, Eversden ID.. The design of a motility bed including its calibration for the subject’s weight. Med Biol Eng. 1970;8(5):455–463. doi: 10.1007/BF02477182 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7. American Academy of Sleep Medicine. International Classification of Sleep Disorders. 3rd ed. Text Revision. Darien, IL: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2023. [Google Scholar]
- 8. Troester MM, Quan SF, Berry RB, et al. ; for the American Academy of Sleep Medicine. The AASM Manual for the Scoring of Sleep and Associated Events: Rules, Terminology and Technical Specifications. Version 3. Darien, IL.: American Academy of Sleep Medicine; 2023. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Frauscher B, Brandauer E, Gschliesser V, et al. A descriptive analysis of neck myoclonus during routine polysomnography. Sleep. 2010;33(8):1091–1096. doi: 10.1093/sleep/33.8.1091 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Ferri R, Delrosso LM, Provini F, Stefani A, Walters AS, Picchietti DL.. Scoring of large muscle group movements during sleep: an international restless legs syndrome study group position statement. Sleep. 2021;44(9). doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsab092 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Stefani A, Gabelia D, Mitterling T, Poewe W, Högl B, Frauscher B.. A prospective video-polysomnographic analysis of movements during physiological sleep in 100 healthy sleepers. Sleep. 2015;38(9):1479–1487. doi: 10.5665/sleep.4994 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Brás AB, Maranci JB, Yang Z, Chaumereuil C, Arnulf I.. Movements and behaviors during spontaneous arousals in healthy young adults: an intermediary stage between wakefulness and sleep? Sleep Med. 2022;89:93–96. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2021.11.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Tassinari CA, Rubboli G, Gardella E, et al. Central pattern generators for a common semiology in fronto-limbic seizures and in parasomnias. A neuroethologic approach. Neurol Sci. 2005;26(suppl 3):s225–s232. doi: 10.1007/s10072-005-0492-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Tassinari CA, Cantalupo G, Högl B, et al. Neuroethological approach to frontolimbic epileptic seizures and parasomnias: the same central pattern generators for the same behaviours. Rev Neurol (Paris). 2009;165(10):762–768. doi: 10.1016/j.neurol.2009.08.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Zucconi M, Oldani A, Ferini-Strambi L, Bizzozero D, Smirne S.. Nocturnal paroxysmal arousals with motor behaviors during sleep: frontal lobe epilepsy or parasomnia? J Clin Neurophysiol. 1997;14(6):513–522. doi: 10.1097/00004691-199711000-00008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Loddo G, Sessagesimi E, Mignani F, et al. Specific motor patterns of arousal disorders in adults: a video-polysomnographic analysis of 184 episodes. Sleep Med. 2018;41:102–109. doi: 10.1016/j.sleep.2017.08.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Baldini T, Loddo G, Sessagesimi E, et al. Clinical features and pathophysiology of disorders of arousal in adults: a window into the sleeping brain. Front Neurol. 2019;10:526. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2019.00526 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Ibrahim A, Ferri R, Cesari M, et al. Large muscle group movements during sleep in healthy people: normative values and correlation to sleep features. Sleep. 2023;46(8). doi: 10.1093/sleep/zsad129 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Wernecke KEA, Brüggemann J, Fendt M.. Predator odor exposure increases food-carrying behavior in rats. Physiol Behav. 2016;154:15–19. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.11.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Cardinal RN and Everitt BJ.. Neural systems of motivation. In: Koob G, Thompson R. Encyclopedia of Behavioral Neuroscience. New York, NY, USA. Elsevier, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 21. Gardner R, Grossman WI, Roffwarg HP, Weiner H.. The relationship of small limb movements during REM sleep to dreamed limb action. Psychosom Med. 1975;37(2):147–159. doi: 10.1097/00006842-197503000-00005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Scher MS. Ontogeny of EEG sleep from neonatal through infancy periods. In: Montagna P, Chokroverty S.eds Handbook of Clinical Neurology. Sleep Disorders Part I. Vol 98. Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier B.V; 2011. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Kronholm E, Alanen E, Hyyppa MT.. Nocturnal motor activity in a community sample. Sleep. 1993;16(6):565–571. doi: 10.1093/sleep/16.6.565 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Gori S, Ficca G, Giganti F, Di Nasso I, Murri L, Salzarulo P.. Body movements during night sleep in healthy elderly subjects and their relationships with sleep stages. Brain Res Bull. 2004;63(5):393–397. doi: 10.1016/j.brainresbull.2003.12.012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. Baldridge M, Whitman BJ, Kramer RM, The concurrence of fine muscle activity and rapid eye movements during sleep. Psychosom Med. 1965;27:19–26. doi: 10.1097/00006842-196501000-00003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26. Fanselow MS. Neural organization of the defensive behavior system responsible for fear. Psychon Bull Rev. 1994;1(4):429–438. doi: 10.3758/BF03210947 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27. Caroline Blanchard D, Hynd AL, Minke KA, Minemoto T, Blanchard RJ.. Human defensive behaviors to threat scenarios show parallels to fear- and anxiety-related defense patterns of non-human mammals. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2001;25(7–8):761–770. doi: 10.1016/s0149-7634(01)00056-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28. Blanchard DC, Defensor EB and Blanchard RJ.. Fear, anxiety, and defensive behaviors in animals. In: Koob G, Thompson R.eds Encyclopedia of Behavioral Neuroscience. NY, NY, USA: Elsevier, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 29. Meletti S, Cantalupo G, Stanzani-Maserati M, Rubboli G, Tassinari CA.. The expression of interictal, preictal, and postictal facial-wiping behavior in temporal lobe epilepsy: a neuro-ethological analysis and interpretation. Epilepsy Behav. 2003;4(6):635–643. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30. Anderson JR. Behavioral pathologies in nonhuman primates. In: Koob G, Thompson R, Le Moal M, eds. Encyclopedia of Behavioral Neuroscience. Elsevier: New York, NY, USA, 2010. [Google Scholar]
- 31. Montagna P, Sforza E, Tinuper P, Cirignotta F, Lugaresi E.. Paroxysmal arousals during sleep. Neurology. 1990;40(7):1063–1066. doi: 10.1212/wnl.40.7.1063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32. Oldani A, Zucconi M, Asselta R, et al. Autosomal dominant nocturnal frontal lobe epilepsy. A video-polysomnographic and genetic appraisal of 40 patients and delineation of the epileptic syndrome. Brain. 1998;121 ( Pt 2)(Pt 2):205–223. doi: 10.1093/brain/121.2.205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Giuliano L, Uccello D, Fatuzzo D, Mainieri G, Zappia M, Sofia V.. Electroclinical findings of minor motor events during sleep in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsia. 2017;58(7):1261–1267. doi: 10.1111/epi.13770 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Andersen SL, Robinson SR, Smotherman WP.. Ontogeny of the stretch response in the rat fetus: kappa opioid involvement. Behav Neurosci. 1993;107(2):370–376. doi: 10.1037//0735-7044.107.2.370 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Silvestri R, Walters AS.. Rhythmic movements in sleep disorders and in epileptic seizures during sleep. Sleep Sci Pract. 2020;4(1):1–9.32395635 [Google Scholar]
- 36. Montini A, Loddo G, Baldelli L, Cilea R, Provini F.. Sleep-Related hypermotor epilepsy vs disorders of arousal in adults: a step-wise approach to diagnosis. Chest. 2021;160(1):319–329. doi: 10.1016/j.chest.2021.01.059 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Toscanini AC, Marques JH, Hasan R, Schenck CH.. Sexsomnia: case based classification and discussion of psychosocial implications. Sleep Sci. 2021;14(2):175–180. doi: 10.5935/1984-0063.20200057 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Gibbs SA, Proserpio P, Francione S, et al. Clinical features of sleep-related hypermotor epilepsy in relation to the seizure-onset zone: a review of 135 surgically treated cases. Epilepsia. 2019;60(4):707–717. doi: 10.1111/epi.14690 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Ferri R, Fulda S, Manconi M, et al. World Association of Sleep Medicine (WASM) 2016 standards for recording and scoring leg movements in polysomnograms developed by a joint task force from the International and the European Restless Legs Syndrome Study Groups (IRLSSG and EURLSSG). Sleep Med. 2016;26:86–95. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
The data underlying this article are available in the article and its online supplementary material.
The data on which this article is based were provided with the patients’ consent.