Skip to main content
NIHPA Author Manuscripts logoLink to NIHPA Author Manuscripts
. Author manuscript; available in PMC: 2025 Aug 1.
Published in final edited form as: J Anxiety Disord. 2024 Jul 29;106:102911. doi: 10.1016/j.janxdis.2024.102911

Reward Processes in Extinction Learning and Applications to Exposure Therapy

Benjamin M Rosenberg 1, Nora M Barnes-Horowitz 1, Tomislav D Zbozinek 2,3, Michelle G Craske 1,2
PMCID: PMC11384290  NIHMSID: NIHMS2017660  PMID: 39128178

Abstract

Anxiety disorders are common and highly distressing mental health conditions. Exposure therapy is a gold-standard treatment for anxiety disorders. Mechanisms of Pavlovian fear learning, and particularly fear extinction, are central to exposure therapy. A growing body of evidence suggests an important role of reward processes during Pavlovian fear extinction. Nonetheless, predominant models of exposure therapy do not currently incorporate reward processes. Herein, we present a theoretical model of reward processes in relation to Pavlovian mechanisms of exposure therapy, including a focus on dopaminergic prediction error signaling, coinciding positive emotional experiences (i.e., relief), and unexpected positive outcomes. We then highlight avenues for further research and discuss potential strategies to leverage reward processes to maximize exposure therapy response, such as pre-exposure interventions to increase reward sensitivity or post-exposure rehearsal (e.g., savoring, imaginal recounting) strategies to enhance retrieval and retention of learned associations.

Keywords: Pavlovian fear extinction, exposure therapy, reward, positive affect

1. INTRODUCTION

Anxiety disorders are among the most common and most disabling mental health conditions (Yang et al., 2021). Psychological treatments for anxiety are widely studied and supported (Bandelow et al., 2018; Carpenter et al., 2018; Van Dis et al., 2020), with exposure therapy considered a gold-standard (Rauch et al., 2012; Abramowitz et al., 2019). However, despite the considerable effectiveness of exposure therapy, approximately 50% of individuals do not attain clinically significant treatment response (Loerinc et al., 2015). Understanding the factors associated with positive outcomes from exposure therapy may fuel future innovations in the treatment of anxiety disorders.

Current theories regarding exposure therapy derive, in part, from models of Pavlovian fear learning. As individuals learn that a neutral cue (conditional stimulus; CS) predicts an innately aversive outcome (unconditional stimulus; USav), conditional responses correspond with elevations in measures such as peripheral physiology and subjectively reported fear (Lonsdorf et al., 2017). The prediction that a CS will lead to a USav can lead to avoidance of the CS, which prevents new inhibitory learning from taking place and maintains CS-USav associations. Exposure therapy is designed to directly counteract this avoidance and enable individuals to learn that a feared CS does not lead to the USav as often as originally predicted, at least in the exposure therapy context (Craske et al., 2022).

Reward processes may further inform mechanisms of Pavlovian fear learning that are relevant to exposure therapy. For example, avoidance is thought to prompt a positive emotion of relief (Carver, 2009; Deutsch et al., 2015), which overlaps with dopaminergic mechanisms of reward signaling and may reinforce continued avoidance of the CS (San Martín et al., 2020; Vervliet et al., 2017). However, relief is also thought to occur when a CS surprisingly does not lead to a USav (i.e., extinction), thereby reinforcing approach to the CS (Vervliet et al., 2017). Furthermore, a feared CS may sometimes precede an innately appetitive (i.e., positive) outcome (USapp), which can similarly reinforce approach to the CS (i.e., counterconditioning, see Keller et al., 2019). Approach to the CS is critical to fear extinction, which by definition involves repeated nonreinforced “exposures” to the CS. Interventions to increase reward signaling and positive emotions (e.g., relief) may therefore have the potential to enhance exposure therapy treatment outcomes (Craske et al., 2016; Zbozinek & Craske, 2017a).

Nonetheless, current theories of Pavlovian fear learning and exposure therapy rarely incorporate reward processes. Herein, we argue for several mechanisms by which reward processes may influence Pavlovian fear learning and extinction. We discuss the relevance of these theories to exposure-based treatments of anxiety disorders, particularly considering growing evidence that individuals with both anxiety and depressive disorders (often comorbid with anxiety disorders; Kessler et al., 2012) exhibit low levels of positive affect, a correlate of reward hyposensitivity (Eisner et al., 2009; Grillo, 2016; Guineau et al., 2023; Kashdan, 2007; Vinograd et al., 2021; Winer et al., 2017). We then highlight potential modifications to existing exposure protocols, emphasizing avenues for leveraging reward processes to maximize Pavlovian extinction learning processes. We conclude by offering future research directions to test the theories presented in this paper.

2. PAVLOVIAN FEAR ACQUISITION AND EXTINCTION

Associative learning models can be used to describe how fear and anxiety are initially acquired (Pittig et al., 2018). This is demonstrated in experimental fear conditioning paradigms (Lonsdorf et al., 2017) where, during acquisition, presentation of a CS (e.g., a geometric shape) may be repeatedly followed by a USav (e.g., an electric shock). This forms an association (or a representation in memory) that the geometric shape leads to electric shock (CS-USav), which can result in conditional fear responding to the geometric shape and avoidance behavior.1

Following fear acquisition, one behavioral response is to prevent USav encounters (i.e., avoidance). This can occur prior to the presentation of a CS that predicts a USav or can occur after CS presentation but prior to the delivery of the USav. In the short-term, avoidance appears protective by preventing the USav from occurring. In the short- and long-term, avoidance prevents opportunities to learn whether a CS does or does not predict the USav (Lovibond, 2006). In the absence of such learning, the original excitatory CS-USav association, as well as the conditional fear response, is maintained (Lovibond et al., 2009; Pittig, 2019; Wong et al., 2022). With repetition, avoidance may become habitual or overgeneralized (across CSs or contexts) and continue even in situations where the CS no longer reliably predicts the USav (LeDoux et al., 2016) and when the conditional response has subsided. These effects can explain why avoidance can continue in the absence of fear.

In extinction learning, the feared CS is repeatedly presented in the absence of the USav, which leads to the formation of an inhibitory CS-noUSav association in memory. From the prior example, repeated presentation of the geometric shape without electric shock leads to a new memory representation of the geometric shape no longer being a predictor of electric shock, corresponding with reduced conditional fear responding to the geometric shape. A critical aspect of this learning process is prediction error, presented in the original Rescorla-Wagner model (Rescorla & Wagner, 1972). When the CS is presented, predictions are formed about the likelihood of USav occurrence. The prediction error that derives from the absence of the expected USav drives extinction learning.2 Mathematically, the Rescorla-Wagner model predicts that greater prediction error should correspond with greater associative learning (Rescorla & Wagner, 1972). In the inhibitory retrieval model of extinction learning (Bouton, 1993, 2002), the CS-noUSav association does not erase the original CS-USav association but is instead formed as an ‘inhibitory’ association that competes with the original association. This inhibitory association is contextually gated, meaning that the inhibitory CS-noUSav association is generally specific to the context in which it is formed (Bouton, 2004).3 Return of conditional fear can therefore occur when the CS is presented in a context that differs from the context in which the inhibitory association was formed, thereby reflecting that the original CS-USav association remains intact, even after successful extinction (Bouton, 2002, 2004). The combination of prediction error as the driver of extinction learning and competing inhibitory associations that are contextually gated forms the theoretical basis for the inhibitory retrieval model of exposure therapy (Craske et al., 2008; 2014; 2022).

One alternative is the latent cause model (Gershman & Niv, 2012), which proposes that distinct learning phases result in distinct memory states, whereas similar learning phases can be integrated into a single memory state. The latent cause model therefore argues that the original CS-USav association may be altered by making acquisition and extinction phases more similar, such as gradually reducing the rate of USav reinforcement during extinction (Niv, 2019). This has been shown to reduce spontaneous recovery and reinstatement relative to standard extinction in rodents (Gershman et al., 2013). Relatedly, there is some evidence to suggest that, consistent with both the inhibitory retrieval model and the latent cause model, occasional presentations of the USav during extinction (i.e., occasional reinforced extinction) may reduce spontaneous recovery (Culver et al., 2018; Thompson et al., 2018), though the effects are not always replicated (Quintero et al., 2022). The latent cause model may further explain individual differences in extinction learning. For example, individuals who assign acquisition and extinction trials to separate states are more likely to show spontaneous recovery than individuals who assign both acquisition and extinction to a single state (Gershman and Hartley, 2015).

While the inhibitory retrieval model has been applied in clinical contexts (Craske et al., 2022), direct clinical translation of the latent cause model remains untested. In the following sections, we therefore discuss extinction learning, reward processes, and clinical applications of these concepts from an inhibitory retrieval perspective, although we additionally consider areas for further study that align with a latent cause perspective.

3. REWARD PROCESSES DURING PAVLOVIAN FEAR LEARNING

The complex process of learning, remembering, and updating CS associations can be conceptualized according to a sequence of critical events (Figure 1). Below, we consider how reward processes relate to each event along this sequence.

Figure 1.

Figure 1.

Sequence of critical events during Pavlovian fear learning following an encounter with a CS. In a given encounter, the CS may terminate before, during, or after the US occurs.

3.1. Avoidance Behavior and Relief

Since avoidance can prevent extinction learning (Figure 1), it is critical to understand the mechanisms by which avoidance is reinforced. To address this need, recent studies have begun exploring relief, a positively valenced emotional construct that is likely to be influenced by many internal and external factors (Barrett & Westlin, 2021; Deutsch et al., 2015) and is closely tied to reward processes (e.g., self-reported reward-responsiveness, motivation, dopaminergic signaling) (Deutsch et al., 2015). In the context of Pavlovian fear learning, the absence of a USav following a CS is theorized to activate reward-related processes that correspond with the emotional output of relief (Deutsch et al., 2015)4. Clinically, leaving a social interaction where rejection is anticipated may lead to a self-reported experience of relief and thus reinforce continued avoidance of social interactions. In support of this theory, a recent study showed that relief is positively valenced and that relief response to threat omission elicited a similar pattern of reward response to monetary gain across behavioral, self-report, and physiological measures (Leng et al., 2023).

Indeed, studies using active avoidance conditioning paradigms (i.e., where during or after CS presentation participants may select an avoidance response to cancel USav delivery) have supported the theory of relief as a rewarding, positive reinforcer of avoidance. For example, avoidant choices during a CS presentation to prevent USav occurrence are associated with self-reported relief, with stronger expectancies of USav prior to avoidance leading to greater relief magnitude, also referred to as relief-pleasantness (San Martín et al., 2020; Vervliet et al., 2017). Moreover, using similar paradigms, relief-pleasantness following avoidance behavior has been shown to predict greater future avoidance behavior (Papalini et al., 2021). Thus, researchers have hypothesized that relief positively reinforces avoidance behavior (Papalini et al., 2021; San Martín et al., 2020; Vervliet et al., 2017), and thus may maintain the CS-USav relationship and thereby perpetuate fear of the CS (Lovibond et al., 2009; Pittig, 2019; Wong et al., 2022).

Other studies have linked the concept of relief to subjective perceptions of certainty and controllability. For instance, one study found that anxious individuals showed lower confidence in their avoidance responses and therefore experienced greater relief-pleasantness following successful avoidance (De Kleine et al., 2023). Similarly, another study found that limiting the number of avoidance responses available prior to a CS corresponded with lower confidence in avoidance responses, thereby increasing relief-pleasantness following the non-occurrence of the USav (Cobos et al., 2022). It may therefore be possible to reduce relief-related reinforcement of avoidance behaviors by 1) increasing the availability of avoidance behaviors throughout an encounter with a CS, and 2) increasing confidence that the available avoidance behaviors will be successful. By the same token, however, increasing avoidance opportunities and confidence in avoidance ability may serve as reliable safety signals (see 3.2.4 Incentives to Reduce Avoidance Behavior for further discussion), reduce US expectancy, and therefore obstruct inhibitory learning during extinction and exposure. Thus, these possibilities require additional research.

3.2. Fear Extinction

Herein we consider ways in which reward processes relate to US outcomes and subsequent prediction error signaling during fear extinction (Figure 1). These include 1) during USav omissions (CS-noUSav) that promote extinction learning, 2) during USav occurrences that are less aversive than expected, and 3) during USapp occurrences (CS-USapp) that may further promote extinction learning (e.g., counterconditioning). We then consider strategies for incentivizing approach behavior to the USav via simultaneous delivery of a USapp.

3.2.1. Prediction Error: Unexpected USav Omissions

Dopaminergic signaling has been shown to be critical to prediction error processes in appetitive conditioning paradigms, where the unconditional stimulus is rewarding (e.g., food, money). Specifically, the unexpected occurrence of a USapp is associated with dopamine signaling in the nucleus accumbens from neurons in the ventral tegmental area (Schultz, 2016). A number of rodent studies support the role of dopaminergic processes in fear extinction as well. Specifically, a dopaminergic response has been identified during omission of an expected USav in fear extinction (Kalisch et al., 2019; Gentry et al., 2019; Salinas-Hernández & Duvarci, 2021).5 Further, the magnitude of dopamine signaling has been associated with the strength of fear extinction learning, measured by conditional freezing behavior in mice (Salinas-Hernández et al., 2018). Inhibition of dopaminergic signaling impairs fear extinction, whereas activation accelerates fear extinction (Luo et al., 2018; Salinas-Hernández et al., 2018). Activation of dopaminergic signaling immediately after fear extinction has been associated with enhanced extinction memory storage and retrieval (Kalisch et al., 2019; Salinas-Hernández & Duvarci, 2021).

This set of animal findings suggests that omission of an expected USav during fear extinction shares biological processes with the unexpected occurrence of a USapp during appetitive conditioning (Gentry et al., 2019). Furthermore, the basolateral amygdala (BLA) receives dopaminergic inputs from the ventral tegmental area and substantia nigra (Rosenkrantz & Grace, 1999; Pezze & Feldon, 2004; Salinas-Hernández & Duvarci, 2021), and specific subpopulations of BLA neurons are associated with reward-seeking behaviors (Kim et al., 2016). In a study of fear extinction among mice, these same BLA neurons were further found to be both “necessary and sufficient” for storing of extinction memories, which has been posited as evidence that extinction memories may be stored as rewarding information (Zhang et al., 2020). Together, these findings suggest that reward and fear extinction processes may rely on common dopaminergic mechanisms of appetitive learning, and further support the assertion that relief during unexpected USav omission elicits a rewarding appetitive response (Leng et al., 2023). However, the directional nature of the relationship between prediction error learning and relief remains unclear. It is possible that 1) greater prediction error learning produces a greater positive emotional output (i.e., relief-pleasantness), 2) greater relief-pleasantness produces greater prediction error learning, or 3) the two processes are distinct but emerge from common dopaminergic mechanisms of extinction learning. Further research is needed to clarify the precise relationship among dopamine signaling, prediction error learning, and relief-pleasantness during unexpected USav omission.

In addition to the dopamine system, other neural systems associated with reward are implicated in fear extinction. For example, studies have identified overlap between endogenous opioids and fear learning and extinction (Meier et al., 2021). Inhibition of opioid receptors has been shown to sustain fear conditioned responses during fear acquisition (Eippert et al., 2009) and impair fear extinction learning (Bengoetxea et al., 2020; McNally, 2009), which suggests that activation of opioid receptors may be critical to the prediction error process during fear extinction (McNally, 2009). Opiodergic signaling has been implicated in studies of relief from physical pain, such that blocking endogenous opioids reduces self-reported relief-pleasantness (Sirucek et al., 2021) (although there is some evidence suggesting dopaminergic, rather than opiodergic, modulation, see Desch et al., 2023). It has been similarly posited that blocking opioid receptors prevents the rewarding experience of relief associated with unexpected USav omission (McNally & Westbrook, 2003), although empirical evidence during human fear extinction is needed. Additional research should evaluate the relationship between relief following USav omission and activation of the opioidergic system.

As previously discussed, the subjective experience of relief has most often been studied in relation to avoidance conditioning paradigms that result in prevention of potential USav delivery (Papalini et al., 2021; San Martín et al., 2020; Vervliet et al., 2017), and subjective relief correlates with and predicts future avoidance behavior (Papalini et al., 2021). However, when approaching the fearful CS corresponds with unexpected omission of a USav (i.e., fear extinction), this may similarly result in the subjective experience of relief. In support of this notion, several studies using both fear extinction and avoidance conditioning paradigms have shown that greater prediction error on a given trial corresponds with greater relief-pleasantness at the end of that trial (San Martín et al., 2020; Vervliet et al., 2017; Willems & Vervliet, 2021). Relatedly, as prediction error declines across fear extinction trials (an indication of successful extinction learning) or across repeated avoidance trials (an indication of successful learning of avoidance contingencies), self-reported relief-pleasantness also declines (San Martín et al., 2020; Vervliet et al., 2017).

Although avoidance conditioning paradigms share similarities with fear conditioning and extinction paradigms, there are key differences in the role of prediction error. In avoidance conditioning, the mismatch between an expected USav and its unexpected omission leads to learning that an active avoidance behavior (e.g., pressing a button) prevents USav occurrence, which can reinforce further avoidance behavior and prevent CS-USav extinction learning. In fear extinction, the mismatch between an expected USav and its unexpected omission leads to learning that approaching the CS does not lead to USav occurrence, which can reinforce further approach behavior and thus support extinction learning. For the purposes of clinical application, there is a need to find therapeutic strategies for 1) reducing the relief experienced during anticipatory or active avoidance, and 2) increasing the relief experienced at the point of unexpected US omission during an exposure.

3.2.2. Prediction Error: USav Aversiveness

Conditional fear following a CS is related to the aversiveness of a predicted USav (Hosoba et al., 2001) and reduces when USav intensity is reduced (Du et al., 2015). When an individual experiences a USav, this occurrence provides an opportunity for the individual to learn that the feared USav is not as bad as anticipated, thereby reducing conditional fear to the CS.6 On such occasions, individuals may experience partial USav omission (i.e., the USav was not fully omitted, but it did not fully occur as anticipated). Similar to other prediction error mechanisms of learning, it is possible that the mismatch between expected USav aversiveness and actual USav aversiveness corresponds with a rewarding relief signal to drive extinction learning.7 For example, if a socially anxious individual experiences rejection from their peers, and if that rejection was less aversive than expected, the individual may experience relief and begin to show reduced fear during future encounters with their peers. Of note, occasional exposure to the USav during extinction has been hypothesized to reduce USav aversiveness (Culver et al., 2018), which has been shown to reduce reacquisition of fear in some studies (Craske et al., 2022) and may relate to prediction error mechanisms. Additional research is needed to characterize the extent to which prediction error mechanisms of USav aversiveness relate to relief signaling and therefore support fear extinction.

3.2.3. Prediction Error: Counterconditioning

Counterconditioning involves repeated pairing of the CS that had been originally paired with a USav with a USapp instead and has been shown to lessen conditional fear responses (de Jong et al., 2000). Counterconditioning with a USapp is likely to involve reward-related processes, although other processes may exist as well (e.g., formation of associations that inhibit original excitatory fear associations; Keller et al., 2020). Specifically, pairing a CS that is expected to predict a USav with USapp may boost reward processes because of augmentation of the mismatch between expected and actual (USav omission and USapp receipt) outcomes.

Yet, evidence for the efficacy of counterconditioning of fear conditioning is mixed (for a review, see Keller et al., 2020). On the one hand, some evidence suggests that counterconditioning may lead to better extinction learning and less spontaneous recovery of fear (i.e., return of fear with the passage of time) compared to standard extinction procedures. For example, counterconditioning has been associated with greater reduction of USav expectancies during extinction and reduced spontaneous recovery (Kang et al., 2018), as well as weaker conditional (physiological) responses to the extinguished CS during extinction recall (Keller & Dunsmoor, 2020). However, other evidence has shown that, compared to standard extinction, counterconditioning does not reduce spontaneous recovery or reinstatement of fear (i.e., return of fear following uncued US occurrence) (van Dis et al., 2019; Quintero et al., 2023; Chen et al., 2022), and some evidence even suggests that conditional fear is more readily renewed following counterconditioning compared with standard extinction (Holmes et al., 2016). This is thought to occur because counterconditioning is more context-dependent than standard extinction (Holmes et al., 2016). Additionally, these studies demonstrated mixed results regarding valence ratings to the CS, with one study finding increased positive valence of the CS directly following counterconditioning (van Dis et al., 2019) and another study finding no change to CS valence (Kang et al., 2018). Methodological differences may account for discrepant findings, as studies have used a range of appetitive unconditional stimuli (e.g., sucrose, cartoons, positive film clips, pleasant music) and aversive unconditional stimuli (e.g., electric shocks, aversive sound). It may be particularly important to consider the aversiveness of the USav and positivity of the USapp in counterconditioning experiments to ensure sufficient contrast between the stimuli (Quintero et al., 2023).

Whereas both extinction and counterconditioning appear to recruit the dopamine system, no studies to our knowledge have examined the connection between counterconditioning and subjective reporting of positive emotion. While prediction error during CS-noUSav learning is associated with the positive emotion of relief, counterconditioning may similarly relate to positive emotions associated with the receipt of rewards (i.e., enjoyment). It is conceivable that equivocal findings in the counterconditioning literature are attributable to variations in USapp stimuli and associated dopaminergic reward signaling. It may be possible for counterconditioning to outperform extinction (i.e., lead to greater reductions in spontaneous recovery and reinstatement) if the occurrence of an unexpected USapp activates positive emotional states over and above relief. Additional research is needed to explore the role of inter-individual differences in reward signaling and subjective experiences of positive emotion during counterconditioning.

3.2.4. Incentives to Reduce Avoidance Behavior

Prevention of avoidance is crucial as it is necessary for extinction learning and exposure therapy. Thus, research on explicit prevention of avoidance may lead to strategies for maximizing learning in extinction and exposure therapy. Several studies have examined co-occurring rewards to incentivize approach behavior (Dibbets & Fonteyne, 2015; Pittig, Hengen, et al., 2018; Pittig & Dehler, 2019). For example, participants are more likely to approach an aversive CS (e.g., image of a spider) in the presence of monetary or social rewards (Pittig et al., 2018; Pittig & Dehler, 2019). Notably, the receipt of reward does not appear to reduce fear in response to the CS (Pittig & Dehler, 2019), and reward magnitude appears to be weighted less heavily than threat probability during approach-avoidance decisions (Boschet et al., 2022). Nonetheless, in a fear extinction paradigm where the CS is no longer paired with the USav, such increased approach behavior can facilitate extinction by creating more prediction error learning opportunities. Furthermore, evidence suggests that use of external rewards to incentivize reduced avoidance during extinction may also lead to reduced future avoidance and return of fear compared to traditional extinction where avoidance options are removed (Pittig & Wong, 2022).

Although research is more limited, evidence suggests that external rewards can reduce the use of safety signals, which is another form of avoidance. Safety signals reduce USav expectancy or directly predict US absence when present (Salkovskis, 1991; Wells et al., 1995; Wong et al., 2022). For example, presence of an anxiolytic medication (safety signal) may reduce expectancies that sensations of lightheadedness (CS) will lead to passing out (USav), as is commonly feared in the case of panic disorder. Through this process, the feared CS-USav association is maintained as opportunities for extinction learning via prediction error are reduced or removed (Lovibond et al., 2000, 2009). As such, safety signals are generally discouraged during exposure therapy.8 It is possible that these principles may be leveraged to overcome safety signal use. For example, in a fear conditioning safety behavior experiment that tested the impact of a rewarding USapp, participants were less likely to select a safety button that blocked a USav when forgoing the safety behavior was paired with a USapp, which in turn facilitated more rapid extinction learning (Pittig, 2019). Considerations for clinical applicability are discussed below.

4. INDIVIDUAL DIFFERENCES: MODERATORS OF REWARD, LEARNING, AND MEMORY

Broadly speaking, reward processes are considered central to the experience of positive emotions (Becker et al., 2019). Initial genome-wide association studies have begun to identify associations between genetic markers of positive affect and reward processes. For example, genetic markers linked to trait positive emotion have been associated with greater activation of the ventral striatum in response to positive facial images (Wingo et al., 2016). Another study showed similar findings for receipt of reward during a probabilistic decision-making task but failed to replicate effects using positive food-related images (Lancaster et al., 2017). Thus, further study is needed (Bondy & Bogdan, 2022; Kujawa et al., 2020). Neural measures of reward sensitivity, including activation of the ventral striatum in response to reward anticipation and receipt of reward during a card-guessing task, also correlate with state positive affect (Forbes et al., 2010). Furthermore, individual differences in positive affect such as anhedonia (characterized by low positive affect and limited interest or joy in activities), are associated with deficits in reward processes including both reward motivation and consumption (Borsini et al., 2020; Huys et al., 2013; Slaney et al., 2022). The below sections detail potential avenues for understanding how individual differences in positive emotions, a correlate of reward processes, relate to fear extinction.

4.1. Positive Affect & Reward Processes during Extinction

As previously stated, unexpected omission of a USav following CS occurrence (e.g., engaging in a social interaction and not being rejected) is thought to trigger the positive emotional state of relief. As with avoidance behavior, the magnitude of this experience (i.e., relief-pleasantness) tracks closely with prediction error and is therefore considered central to the formation of CS-noUS associations as they begin to compete with CS-USav associations (Vervliet et al., 2017). For example, maximal relief-pleasantness is reported when there is greater certainty that a CS will lead to a USav, and no USav occurs (Willems & Vervliet, 2021). Reported relief-pleasantness then declines during subsequent extinction trials in parallel with decreases in CS-USav predictions across trials (Vervliet et al., 2017). The role of individual differences in relief-pleasantness is beginning to be explored. For example, elevated anhedonia is associated with reduced self-reported relief-pleasantness during avoidance conditioning (Leng et al., 2022, 2024). Furthermore, elevated anhedonia is associated with distinct patterns of brain activity during fear extinction, including regions of canonical threat neurocircuitry such as the dorsal anterior cingulate, anterior insula, and amygdala that may further relate to individual differences in relief-pleasantness (although this remains an area of ongoing research) (Rosenberg et al., 2022; Young et al., 2021). However, to further examine the directions of these relationships, future studies should specifically test whether individual differences in self-reported relief-pleasantness can predict subsequent learning (e.g., prediction error on subsequent extinction trials).

4.2. Positive Affect & Processes in Learning and Memory that Inform Fear Extinction

Existing studies have demonstrated a connection between positive affect and broader learning and memory principles, which may in turn be applied to extinction. For example, although not specifically tested in fear conditioning and extinction paradigms, positive affect has been associated with enhanced encoding, rehearsal, and retrieval processes (Zbozinek & Craske, 2017a), described in more detail in the following sections. Regarding extinction, these memory processes are essential to the formation and retention of inhibitory CS-noUS associations (Figure 1) and therefore suggest the potential importance of positive affect during extinction learning.

4.2.1. Encoding & Consolidation

Following the initial learning of associations (e.g., CS-USav, CS-noUSav, CS-USapp), newly learned information is encoded and consolidated in memory for later retrieval. Variability in how deeply associations are initially encoded influences how these associations are subsequently stored or retrieved (Craik, 2002). Shallow encoding typically involves sensory or surface features (e.g., color, brightness) whereas deep encoding typically involves more complex features (e.g., meaning, inference) and requires greater attentional processing of the encoded stimulus (Craik & Lockhart, 1972; Craik, 2002; Ekuni et al., 2011).

Research suggests that both negative and positive mood can influence encoding processes. Negative mood is associated with more shallow encoding and less cognitive effort expenditure during learning (Bolte et al., 2003; Ellis et al., 1984; Hartlage et al., 1993; Leight & Ellis, 1981) as well as a tendency to engage in referential processing (i.e., processing new information independently of prior knowledge) (Storbeck & Clore, 2008). This is relevant to fear extinction learning as more shallow encoding of CS-noUS relationships may impact subsequent retrieval of extinction. Furthermore, referential processing may disrupt the extinction learning process, as individuals may be slower to update predictions in light of newly learned information. Alternatively, positive mood is associated with semantic learning processes that increase the depth of encoding and enhance long-term retention of learned information (Federmeier et al., 2001; Hänze & Hesse, 2008; Isen, 1987), which may be particularly important for retention of new CS-noUS associations. Positive mood also enhances relational processing (i.e., the integration of new information with prior knowledge) (Clore & Huntsinger, 2007) (Storbeck & Clore, 2008) in ways that may improve updating of expectancies during extinction learning. One potential example relates to the valence of the CS. During fear acquisition, the CS tends to take on a negative valence, given its association with a USav (Hermans et al., 2002). During extinction, the induction of positive mood can increase positive valence of the CS (Zbozinek et al., 2015). Since the omission of a USav is encoded as rewarding, increased positive valence of the CS may serve to augment extinction learning as more positively valenced cues may be more readily associated with positive outcomes, in part due to enhanced relational processing.

4.2.2. Rehearsal & Retrieval

Rehearsal of learned information can enhance consolidation and retrieval of long-term memories. In the inhibitory retrieval model of exposure therapy, this strategy takes the form of verbally and imaginally rehearsing the absence of the expected USav (Craske et al., 2022; McGlade & Craske, 2021). Mental rehearsal of the CS-USav association can maintain conditional fear (Joos et al., 2012, 2013), whereas repeated mental rehearsal of the CS-noUS in exposure therapy can reduce fear (McGlade & Craske, 2021). Furthermore, rehearsing a subset of learned information has been shown to increase memory while decreasing memory for non-rehearsed information belonging to the same category (i.e., retrieval-induced forgetting) (Anderson, 2003; Anderson et al., 1994, 2000). Thus, by intentionally engaging in rehearsal of learned information, it may be possible to selectively strengthen the consolidation and later retrieval of specific memories, such as CS-noUSav. Similarly, such a strategy could be implemented for selective strengthening of an appetitive CS-USapp association.

Positive mood is thought to be associated with deeper mental rehearsal of stored information, thereby enhancing consolidation and retrieval of long-term memories (Craik, 2002; Craik & Lockhart, 1972). Positive mood has been found to enhance retrieval-induced forgetting effects (Bäuml & Kuhbandner, 2007), possibly because of more relational processing (i.e., relating new information to previously learned information) than negative mood. Furthermore, retrieval of stored memories is often congruent with positive or negative mood (Matt et al., 1992), such that depressed individuals tend to recall information that is depression-congruent (Watkins, 1992). Conversely, memory retrieval may be biased toward CS-noUSav or CS-USapp associations when mood is positive. Indirect support of this notion includes evidence for higher positive affect to be associated with less reacquisition (Dour et al., 2016; Zbozinek & Craske, 2017b) and reinstatement (Zbozinek et al., 2015) of conditional fear following extinction, although discrepant findings (e.g., van Dis et al., 2019) motivate further research in this area.

If the absence of the USav during fear extinction is encoded as positive and rewarding, then perhaps positive mood enhances mood-congruent rehearsal of extinction learning (i.e., CS-noUSav) relative to original fear memories (i.e., CS-USav). In turn, if positive mood enhances rehearsal of positive (i.e., CS-noUSav) but not negative (i.e., CS-USav) associations, positive mood may therefore facilitate a decreased strength of retrievability of negative fear associations over time via retrieval-induced forgetting mechanisms. Alternatively, if negative mood is associated with mood-congruent rehearsal of original fear memories (i.e., CS-USav) relative to extinction memories (i.e., CS-noUS), negative mood inductions may increase the retrievability of fear associations. These possibilities could be tested by implementing a positive versus negative mood induction between fear extinction and reacquisition or between fear extinction and extinction recall.

5. CLINICAL IMPLICATIONS

In the section that follows, we provide an outline for future study of potential clinical applications of the relationships between reward processes and extinction learning. Specifically, we identify potential methods to be tested that could maximize prediction error learning and target reward processes, rehearsal and retrieval, and positive affect to facilitate improvements in exposure therapy outcomes.

5.1. Exposure Therapy as an Application of Extinction Learning

The associative learning model of fear acquisition and extinction can be applied clinically to inform the treatment of anxiety disorders via exposure therapy (Craske et al., 2018; Mineka & Oehlberg, 2008). Exposure therapy techniques have evolved over the years from initial systematic desensitization approaches (Wolpe, 1961, 1968) to emotional processing theories (Foz & Kozak, 1986; 1996), and more recently, inhibitory retrieval models of exposure therapy that emphasize maximizing prediction error (i.e., expectancy violation: the violation of expected USav occurrence) (Craske et al., 2022). This is achieved through exposures designed to test out predictions and provide potent mismatches between expectancies and outcomes, drawing upon principles such as attentional salience, removal of conditional inhibitors or safety signals, deepened extinction, occasional reinforced extinction (when appropriate), and mental rehearsal for consolidation of extinction learning. Additional strategies are utilized to offset contextual specificity of extinction learning, such as stimulus and contextual variation throughout exposure (Craske et al., 2022).

5.2. Prediction Error during Exposure Therapy

Current exposure approaches based on the inhibitory retrieval model emphasize maximizing expectancy violation (the clinical application of prediction error) across exposures to promote inhibitory learning and methods for promoting generalization and retrieval of the learning across time and context (see Craske et al., 2022 for a review). Building upon this existing framework, strategies aimed at maximizing reward sensitivity may serve to further support expectancy violation, as well as generalization and retrieval of learning, during exposure therapy (Figure 2, Table 1).

Figure 2.

Figure 2.

Theoretical model of reward-related processes before and during exposure therapy.

Table 1.

Reward-focused interventions and their potential applications in exposure therapy.

Possible Strategies to Target Reward Processes Timing of Intervention
Prior to Exposure During Exposure Post Exposure
Positive Mood Induction (e.g., imagining of future positive outcomes, exposure to positive images, or exposure to positive memories) X
Positive Affect Treatments (e.g., PAT or APT to increase overall reward sensitivity and enhance subsequent learning and memory processes) X X
Reduce Reward Signal Associated with Avoidance (i.e., decreasing reinforcing strength of using safety signals) X*
Testosterone/Estradiol Administration X* X*
Selection of Potentially Rewarding Exposures (e.g., social encounters with possibility of deepening meaningful relationships) X
Therapist Reinforcement (e.g., in-session praise of approach behavior) X
Connecting Exposures to Eudaimonic Rewards (e.g., sense of achievement, mastery, values) X X
Positive-Focused Rehearsal Strategies (e.g., savoring, imaginal recounting to maximize extinction and retrieval) X X
*

As direct evidence is currently lacking, these approaches should be considered highly tentative at this time.

In exposure therapy, individuals are specifically instructed to reduce avoidance in order to engage in new learning (Craske et al., 2022). As described in 3.2.1 Prediction Error: Unexpected USav Omissions, relief following unexpected USav omissions may be central to prediction error mechanisms of extinction learning. If so, methods that maximize the relieving or rewarding experience associated with prediction error (i.e., absence of the feared outcome) could be explored. For example, savoring (i.e., using first-person, present-moment language to rehearse the most positive aspects of an event) (see Sandman & Craske, 2022) during the post-exposure consolidation phase may encourage re-experiencing of the moment at which the expected feared outcome did not occur (e.g., savoring the positive experience of relief during a public speaking exposure upon realizing that the audience was not rejecting). This process could deepen inhibitory CS-noUSav learning and therefore strengthen extinction learning during exposure therapy.

Relatedly, studies of depression have highlighted a tendency for depressed individuals to update their beliefs from moderate expectancy violations more so than from maximal expectancy violations (Kube et al., 2022). This suggests a non-linear relationship between US expectancy and expectancy violation among depressed individuals (Kube et al., 2020), which contrasts with traditional linear models of expectancy violation (e.g., Rescorla & Wagner, 1972). These results have been interpreted as depressed individuals tending to overlook or minimize positive information that disconfirms negative expectations (i.e., cognitive immunization theory) (Liknaitzky et al., 2017; Everaert et al., 2018; Kube & Glombiewski, 2021; Kube et al., 2022; Kube & Rozenkrantz, 2021). These findings are consistent with the observation that depressed individuals tend to show lower positive affect following surprising positive events as they occur naturalistically in daily life (Villano & Heller, 2024). Some evidence also supports a sigmoidal relationship between reward expectancy and reward positivity, which allows there to be little difference in the positivity of reward when reward is unexpected versus very unexpected (Williams et al., 2017). If the most extreme expectancy violations are not accompanied by similarly heightened reward signaling, which may be the case as a function of reward hyposensitivity that is common to depression (Wang et al., 2022) and anhedonia (Borsini et al., 2020; Nusslock & Alloy, 2017; Slaney et al., 2022), then these exposures may be most susceptible to cognitive immunization effects (e.g., “This is too good to be true”).

The latent cause model may offer one alternative explanation for these phenomena, as maximizing expectancy violation may prompt individuals to seek alternative explanations for USav non-occurrence (i.e., formation of a novel latent state) (Niv, 2019). If so, there may be an optimal level of expectancy violation that promotes new learning but is unlikely to create a novel latent state, therefore enabling effective extinction of the original CS-USav association. Furthermore, it is possible that individuals with depression (or elevated anhedonia symptoms) are particularly likely to seek latent causes for unexpected threat omissions, contributing to the cognitive immunization effects described above. Additional work is needed to test these possibilities and to identify other strategies for integrating these principles clinically (i.e., to enhance extinction mechanisms of exposure therapy) (Craske et al., 2022).

Alternatively, individuals with heightened anhedonia or comorbid anxiety and depressive disorders may be particularly likely to benefit from strategies designed to maximize positive emotions during exposures. This may counteract cognitive immunization effects by instead encouraging the capturing and maximizing of the reward signal as it occurs. One such strategy involves the previously described savoring of the absence of the aversive outcome or presence of a rewarding outcome in the post-exposure consolidation phase. Notably, experiential processing of positive autobiographical memories (first person, present tense, with a focus upon sensory and situational details) was found to increase positive affect and reduce tendencies to dampen or dismiss the positive experience relative to analytical or naturalistic processing (Sandman & Craske, under review). Such a strategy may disrupt cognitive immunization effects and promote expectancy violation. A second strategy entails positive mood inductions prior to exposure trials, such as by repeated imagining of future positive outcomes (Zbozinek et al., 2015), exposure to positive images (Zbozinek & Craske, 2017), or exposure to positive autobiographical memories (Sandman & Craske, under review). Though some of these strategies have been linked to improved long-term extinction learning (Zbozinek et al., 2015; Zbozinek & Craske, 2017), it is possible that positive mood induction also increases the reward signal during exposures (thereby deepening CS-noUSav learning). Third, therapeutic strategies designed to increase overall reward sensitivity, such as Positive Affect Treatment (PAT) (Craske et al., 2019; 2023), may increase all positive emotions, including relief, and therefore facilitate expectancy violation during exposure. Thus, first conducting treatments such as PAT – especially among individuals with anxiety and low reward processing and low positive affect – may enhance the effects of subsequent exposure therapy. Future research is needed to clarify the possible impact of reward- and positive affect-enhancing strategies before and during exposure therapy.

Finally, when a predicted USav does not occur and a USapp occurs instead (i.e., counterconditioning), the formation of this novel CS-USapp association may then further inhibit CS-USav associations during extinction. For example, in addition to the absence of rejection during a speech exposure, a person with social anxiety may receive positive feedback during their presentation. Although there is mixed evidence regarding counterconditioning effects versus standard extinction related to spontaneous recovery or reinstatement of fear (see 3.2.3 Prediction Error: Counterconditioning), it may nonetheless be beneficial for therapists to maximize USapp occurrences during therapy. For example, therapists may 1) aid in the selection of exposures that have an increased possibility of leading to a powerful USapp (e.g., attending a personally-meaningful social gathering), and 2) apply strategies to amplify reward responsiveness (e.g., PAT; Craske et al., 2019; Craske et al., 2023) following USapp occurrences. Future research is needed to determine optimal strategies for incorporating counterconditioning principles in exposure therapy.

5.3. Leveraging Reward Processes to Facilitate Exposures

Exposure to the CS is, of course, a necessary step of exposure therapy. Indeed, individuals with anxiety demonstrate greater relief following avoidance (De Kleine et al., 2023), underscoring the need for creative strategies for increasing approach behavior. Building upon prior work in laboratory studies (see 3.2.4 Incentives to Reduce Avoidance Behavior), external and internal rewards may be incorporated clinically to facilitate approach behavior in myriad ways.

It may be possible to leverage rewards during exposure to compete with USav occurrence. For example, therapists commonly apply positive reinforcement techniques (e.g., praise) to support therapeutic alliance and engagement (Tsascher et al., 2012). Such strategies may be especially applicable during exposures (e.g., praise for remaining in the threatening context without escaping, reducing reliance on safety behaviors, or maintaining attention to the CS). Praise can function as a competing USapp and reinforce future approach behaviors even when a USav has occurred (similar to Pittig & Dehler, 2019), thereby enabling future opportunities for CS-noUSav learning. Furthermore, social support figures have been theorized as “prepared fear suppressors” that can signal accessibility of survival resources and reduce aversiveness of the USav without acting as a safety signal (i.e., without reducing the likelihood of USav occurrence) (Hornstein et al., 2022). It is possible that, in addition to suppressing fear, social support figures may further support extinction by encouraging approach behaviors (i.e., social support as a USapp). The function of social support as a prepared fear suppressor, in addition to reinforcement of approach behavior, may help explain why it is associated with sustained fear extinction (Hornstein et al., 2022). Further research is needed to understand mechanisms of social support to enhance exposure, while minimizing social support as a safety signal.

Although initial CS-USapp associations typically involve hedonic rewards (i.e., short-term enjoyment or pleasure), another possibility is to incentivize engagement in exposures in service of longer-term outcomes (i.e., goals, values-based living, sense of achievement or mastery) (Papageorgiou & Karekla, 2023) that contribute to eudaimonic well-being, which in turn is associated with physical and psychological health benefits (Cole et al., 2015; Fredrickson et al., 2013, 2015; Kitayama et al., 2016). For example, individuals with social anxiety may continue to attend social engagements if socializing also enables the goal of building more positive, long-term relationships. There is some preliminary evidence for this notion, since greater personal values and self-efficacy were found to predict self-reported willingness to engage in social interactions despite fear or anxiety (Lee & Yeghiazarian, 2021). However, it remains unclear whether approaches that emphasize values and long-term goals are more effective than exposure approaches without that emphasis, and thus studies that directly compare such approaches are needed.

Although rewards have been shown to incentivize CS approach, even when the CS strongly predicts the USav (Pittig, 2019; Pittig & Dehler, 2019), it is important to note that these effects might be lessened in clinical anxiety, as individuals with elevated anxiety tend to avoid more than healthy control participants (Pittig et al., 2021; Pittig & Scherbaum, 2020) when presented with a competing USapp. Given the finding that reward information is weighed less than threat information in favor of avoidance during approach-avoidance conflicts (Boschet et al., 2022), perhaps anxious individuals show even greater avoidance in these circumstances due to the heightened rewarding value of the avoidance behavior itself (De Kleine et al., 2023). Thus, applying therapeutic strategies designed to enhance attention and sensitivity to reward in new contexts (e.g., PAT, as described above) prior to exposure may boost the potency of the competing USapp, thereby facilitating a greater willingness to approach exposures.

5.4. Rehearsal & Retrieval

Mental rehearsal of the inhibitory CS-noUSav association immediately following exposure and between exposure sessions has been shown to facilitate greater symptom improvement among a sample of individuals with spider phobia (McGlade & Craske, 2021). Building upon this work to incorporate reward-relevant processes, strategies could be implemented immediately following or between exposure sessions that emphasize rehearsal and retrieval of positively valenced exposure information. Examples of such strategies include imaginal reexperiencing and description of positive events during exposure trials, akin to strategies within PAT (Craske et al., 2019, 2023) and positive memory specificity training with regards to the experience during exposure (Arditte Hall et al., 2018; Chen et al., 2021). Imaginal recounting or memory specificity training could 1) enhance positive emotions following US non-occurrence or USapp delivery during the post-exposure consolidation phase, 2) serve as a form of mental rehearsal, thereby improving later retrieval of the newly learned inhibitory CS-noUS relationship, or 3) serve as a form of retrieval-induced forgetting, which could facilitate extinction of the CS-USav association and improve memory for the CS-noUS association. To test these hypotheses, future studies may examine whether positive-focused imaginal recounting immediately following exposure or repeatedly between exposure sessions leads to improved treatment response.

5.5. Positive Affect

As previously stated, positive affect is linked to basic encoding, rehearsal, and retrieval learning processes in myriad ways. Clinicians may be able to apply this in exposure therapy to specifically target extinction learning processes (Craske et al., 2016; Zbozinek & Craske, 2017a). The association between low positive mood and poorer treatment outcomes (Craske et al., 2016; Sandman & Craske, 2022) may occur, in part, because low mood is associated with reduced effort during learning and shallowly encoded associations (Bolte et al., 2003; Ellis et al., 1984; Hartlage, et al., 1993; Leight & Ellis, 1981). Furthermore, it is possible that negative mood may lead to recall of memories in line with the feared outcome (e.g., a memory of a social interaction that preceded rejection), whereas positive mood may instead lead to recall of memories in line with extinction (e.g., a memory of a social interaction that preceded a new friendship). Thus, interventions that generally aim to increase positive mood could be applied prior to or in conjunction with exposure therapy to 1) deepen encoding of new associations as they are learned, 2) augment retrieval of extinction-congruent memories, which are associated with positive emotional experiences (e.g., relief or reward consumption), and 3) impede retrieval of fear-congruent memories (i.e., retrieval-induced forgetting), which are associated with negative emotional experiences (e.g., disappointment or loneliness). Strategies can be drawn from existing interventions such as PAT, which aims to increase positive affect and reward sensitivity by increasing engagement in positive activities, increasing attention to positive experiences, and increasing cultivation and savoring of positive experiences (Craske et al., 2019, 2023), or Amplification of Positivity Treatment (APT), which aims to increase positive affect by increasing engagement in positive thinking, emotions, and behavior (Kryza-Lacombe et al., 2021; Taylor et al., 2017, 2020, 2024). For example, prior to a course of exposure therapy, clinicians may implement strategies to generally increase positive affect. During a course of exposure therapy, clients may be instructed to more specific strategies, such as 1) intentionally attending to the positive emotions of relief and enjoyment, 2) applying strategies to prolong or deepen these experiences (e.g., increasing the duration of the positive social interaction), or 3) limiting distractions from the positive experience (i.e., maintain absorption).

5.6. Additional Considerations – Sex Differences

Though findings are mixed, there is some evidence to suggest that sex assigned at birth is associated with differences in reward processes (Bangasser & Cuarenta, 2021; Dhingra et al., 2021; Warthen et al., 2020) and extinction learning (Velasco et al., 2023), which in turn may relate to exposure therapy. One possibility is that sex hormones modulate reward sensitivity and thereby have downstream impacts on extinction and exposure outcomes. For example, administration of testosterone has been associated with increased reward sensitivity (Hermans et al., 2010) and approach behavior (Enter et al., 2016). Clinically, testosterone administration has also been associated with heightened initial fear during an exposure, followed by greater fear reduction at a subsequent exposure for public speaking anxiety (Hutschemaekers et al., 2021). Findings are somewhat mixed for estradiol (Hornung et al., 2020), though some studies have found that elevated estradiol (Diekhof, 2018) and estradiol administration are associated with increased reward sensitivity (Bayer et al., 2020; Veselic et al., 2021). Clinically, low estradiol due to hormonal contraceptive usage was associated with greater fear and avoidance after an exposure for spider phobia (Graham et al., 2018) and poor extinction recall after exposure for women with obsessive-compulsive disorder only (Levy et al., 2024). However, Levy and colleagues (Levy et al., 2023) failed to find differences in exposure outcomes between women with low versus high estradiol.

Together, these findings may further support the notion that threat and reward processes rely on overlapping biological systems. Alternatively, sex hormones may be associated with both threat and reward processes through independent pathways, such that manipulations of sex hormones may selectively impact threat versus reward processing depending on the circumstance. Future research is needed to further explore the extent to which these systems overlap. Furthermore, research is needed to test the potential roles of sex assigned at birth, individual differences in sex hormones, and administration of sex hormones as moderators of exposure outcomes (Velasco et al., 2023). Given these studies have not yet been conducted, existing findings at least highlight the need for clinician consideration of potential individual differences in exposure outcomes based on the presence of sex hormones.

6. CONCLUSION

In summary, we have presented a theoretical model of how reward processes relate to Pavlovian fear learning and may be leveraged in the treatment of anxiety disorders. Rewards can incentivize approach behavior to feared stimuli, thereby facilitating opportunities for extinction. Additionally, positive emotional experiences during extinction, such as relief, are thought to rely on dopaminergic reward signaling during formation of inhibitory CS-noUSav or CS-USapp associations. Furthermore, individual differences in positive emotional experiences may moderate encoding, rehearsal, and retrieval of extinction memories (Zbozinek & Craske, 2017a). Given that reward deficits are associated with anxiety-related disorders (Eisner et al., 2009; Kashdan, 2007; Vinograd et al., 2021) and are associated with poor treatment outcomes (Craske et al., 2016; Sandman & Craske, 2022), we contend that reward processes remain understudied in exposure. Thus, future research is needed to elucidate how these processes may be leveraged during exposure therapy to maximize therapy effectiveness and inform treatment selection and personalization.

HIGHLIGHTS.

  • Pavlovian fear learning, and particularly extinction, involves reward processes.

  • Reward processes are rarely incorporated in conceptualizations of exposure therapy.

  • Incorporating reward strategies may maximize the effectiveness of exposure therapy.

  • Additional research at the intersection of threat and reward learning is warranted.

FUNDING:

Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute of Mental Health (NIMH) of the National Institutes of Health (NIH) under award number T32MH015750. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. This work was in part supported by Wellcome Leap #20222084:1.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

CONFLICTS OF INTEREST: The authors declare that there were no conflicts of interest with respect to authorship or the publication of this article.

1

A different CS could be paired with an innately appetitive outcome (e.g., food) to form a CS-USapp association (Fanselow & Wassum, 2016). This is similar to the formation of CS-USav associations and involves overlapping neurocircuitry (Klein et al., 2022). The strengthening of the CS-USapp association tends to correspond with increases in approach behavior.

2

Prediction error also drives learning about appetitive stimuli. For example, when an expected USapp does not occur, this experience forms an inhibitory CS-noUSav association that competes with the original CS-USapp association and reduces conditional responding to the appetitive CS. Likewise, during counterconditioning, discrepancy between a predicted USav and an unexpected USapp drives learning of a CS-USapp association that may also compete with the original CS-USav association.

3

While the Rescorla-Wagner model (Rescorla & Wagner, 1972) was not designed to predict context-specificity of associative learning (e.g., extinction context), more recent prediction-error models built upon the Rescorla-Wagner model are able to accurately account for context modulation of excitatory and inhibitory learning and therefore have greater clinical utility (Zbozinek, et al., 2022).

4

The concept of threat relief is also studied in terms of relief from physical pain when a painful US is terminated, thereby reinforcing termination of the USav when it is possible (e.g., Andreatta & Pauli, 2017). However, in the context of treatments for anxiety disorders, it is often difficult to control when a US will be terminated (e.g., terminating a social interaction may not terminate one’s experience of judgment). Instead, anxious individuals often avoid the CSav, given its predicted likelihood of leading to a USav, thereby preventing new learning from taking place. For these reasons, we have primarily focused the present review on theories of relief in relation to avoidance (i.e., prevention of USav occurrence), and fear extinction (i.e., unexpected absence of threat during CS-noUSav learning) as these mechanisms are more directly applicable to models of exposure therapy.

5

Relatedly, the unexpected omission of a USapp during extinction of appetitive learning corresponds with decreases in dopaminergic signaling (Schultz, 2016).

6

This could also provide the opportunity for the individual to learn that the feared USav was worse than expected, thereby increasing conditional fear to the CS. However, when fear is out of proportion to the true threat (e.g., clinical anxiety), such occurrences are less likely.

7

Of note, the mismatch between predicted and experienced aversiveness of the USav is not captured in the original Rescorla-Wagner model (Rescorla & Wagner, 1972).

8

While there is some debate in this area, additional research is needed to evaluate the impacts of safety signals across a range of inhibitory strengths on prediction error learning (Craske et al., 2022).

Declarations of interest: none

REFERENCES:

  1. Anderson MC (2003). Rethinking interference theory: Executive control and the mechanisms of forgetting. Journal of Memory and Language, 49(4), 415–445. 10.1016/J.JML.2003.08.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  2. Anderson MC, Bjork RA, & Bjork EL (1994). Remembering Can Cause Forgetting: Retrieval Dynamics in Long-Term Memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(5), 1063–1087. https://psycnet.apa.org/doi/10.1037/0278-7393.20.5.1063 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  3. Anderson MC, Bjorkand EL, & Bjork R (2000). Retrieval-induced forgetting: Evidence for a recall-specific mechanism. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 7(3), 522–530. 10.3758/BF03214366 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  4. Andreatta M, & Pauli P (2017). Learning mechanisms underlying threat absence and threat relief: Influences of trait anxiety. Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, 145, 105–113. 10.1016/j.nlm.2017.09.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  5. Arditte Hall KA, De Raedt R, Timpano KR, & Joormann J (2018). Positive memory enhancement training for individuals with major depressive disorder. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 47(2), 155–168. 10.1080/16506073.2017.1364291 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  6. Bandelow B, Sagebiel A, Belz M, Görlich Y, Michaelis S, & Wedekind D (2018). Enduring effects of psychological treatments for anxiety disorders: meta-analysis of follow-up studies. The British Journal of Psychiatry, 212(6), 333–338. 10.1192/BJP.2018.49 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  7. Bangasser DA, & Cuarenta A (2021). Sex differences in anxiety and depression: circuits and mechanisms. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 22(11), 674–684. 10.1038/s41583-021-00513-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  8. Barrett LF, & Westlin C (2021). Navigating the science of emotion. Emotion Measurement, 39–84. 10.1016/B978-0-12-821124-3.00002-8 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  9. Bäuml K-H, & Kuhbandner C (2007). Remembering Can Cause Forgetting-but Not in Negative Moods. Psychological Science, 18(2), 111. 10.1111/j.1467-9280.2007.01857.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  10. Bayer J, Rusch T, Zhang L, Gläscher J, & Sommer T (2020). Dose-dependent effects of estrogen on prediction error related neural activity in the nucleus accumbens of healthy young women. Psychopharmacology, 237, 745–755. 10.1007/s00213-019-05409-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  11. Bengoetxea X, Goedecke L, Blaesse P, Pape HC, & Jüngling K (2020). The μ-opioid system in midline thalamic nuclei modulates defence strategies towards a conditioned fear stimulus in male mice. Journal of Psychopharmacology, 34(11), 1288. 10.1177/0269881120940919 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  12. Bolte A, Goschke T, & Kuhl J (2003). Emotion and intuition: Effects of positive and negative mood on implicit judgments of semantic coherence. Psychological science, 14(5), 416–421. 10.1111/1467-9280014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  13. Bondy E, & Bogdan R (2022). Understanding anhedonia from a genomic perspective. Anhedonia: Preclinical, Translational, and Clinical Integration, 61–79. 10.1007/7854_2021_293 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  14. Borsini A, Wallis ASJ, Zunszain P, Pariante CM, & Kempton MJ (2020). Characterizing anhedonia: A systematic review of neuroimaging across the subtypes of reward processing deficits in depression. Cognitive, Affective and Behavioral Neuroscience, 20(4), 816–841. 10.3758/S13415-020-00804-6/TABLES/3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  15. Boschet JM, Scherbaum S, & Pittig A (2022). Costly avoidance of Pavlovian fear stimuli and the temporal dynamics of its decision process. Scientific Reports 2022 12:1, 12(1), 1–13. 10.1038/s41598-022-09931-1 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  16. Bouton ME (1993). Context, time, and memory retrieval in the interference paradigms of pavlovian learning. Psychological Bulletin, 114(1), 80–99. 10.1037/0Q33-2909.114.1.80 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  17. Bouton ME (2002). Context, ambiguity, and unlearning: sources of relapse after behavioral extinction. Biological Psychiatry, 52(10), 976–986. 10.1016/S0006-3223(02)01546-9 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  18. Bouton ME (2004). Context and behavioral processes in extinction. In Learning and Memory. 10.1101/lm.78804 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  19. Carpenter JK, Andrews LA, Witcraft SM, Powers MB, Smits JAJ, & Hofmann SG (2018). Cognitive behavioral therapy for anxiety and related disorders: A meta-analysis of randomized placebo-controlled trials. Depression and Anxiety, 35(6), 502–514. 10.1002/DA.22728 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  20. Carver CS (2009). Threat sensitivity, incentive sensitivity, and the experience of relief. Journal of personality, 77(1), 125–138. 10.1111/i.1467-6494.2008.00540.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  21. Chen K, Barnes-Horowitz N, Treanor M, Sun M, Young KS, & Craske MG (2021). Virtual Reality Reward Training for Anhedonia: A Pilot Study. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 3748. 10.3389/FPSYG.2020.613617/BIBTEX [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  22. Chen Y, Lin X, Ai S, Sun Y, Shi L, Meng S, … & Shi J (2022). Comparing three extinction methods to reduce fear expression and generalization. Behavioural Brain Research, 420, 113714. 10.1016/j.bbr.2021.l13714 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  23. Clore GL, & Huntsinger JR (2007). How emotions inform judgment and regulate thought. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 11(9), 393–399. 10.1016/J.TICS.2007.08.005 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  24. Cobos PL, Quintero MJ, Ruiz-Fuentes M, Vervliet B, & López FJ (2022). The role of relief, perceived control, and prospective intolerance of uncertainty in excessive avoidance in uncertain-threat environments. International Journal of Psychophysiology, 179, 89–100. 10.1016/j.ijpsycho.2022.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  25. Cole SW, Levine ME, Arevalo JM, Ma J, Weir DR, & Crimmins EM (2015). Loneliness, eudaimonia, and the human conserved transcriptional response to adversity. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 62, 11–17. 10.1016/i.psvneuen.2015.07.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  26. Craik FIM (n.d.). Levels of processing: Past, present… and future? 10.1080/09658210244000135 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  27. Craik FIM, & Lockhart RS (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11(6), 671–684. 10.1016/S0022-5371(72)80001-X [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  28. Craske MG, Hermans D, & Vervliet B (2018). State-of-the-art and future directions for extinction as a translational model for fear and anxiety. In Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences. 10.1098/rstb.2017.0025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  29. Craske MG, Kircanski K, Zelikowsky M, Mystkowski J, Chowdhury N, & Baker A (2008). Optimizing inhibitory learning during exposure therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 46(1), 5–27. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2007.10.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  30. Craske MG, Meuret AE, Ritz T, Treanor M, & Dour HJ (2016). Treatment for anhedonia: A neuroscience driven approach. Depression and Anxiety. 10.1002/da.22490 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  31. Craske MG, Meuret AE, Ritz T, Treanor M, Dour H, & Rosenfield D (2019). Positive affect treatment for depression and anxiety: A randomized clinical trial for a core feature of anhedonia. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 87(5), 457–471. 10.1037/ccp0000396 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  32. Craske MG, Meuret A, Echiverri-Cohen A, Rosenfield D, & Ritz T (2023). Positive affect treatment targets reward sensitivity: a randomized controlled trial. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 10.1037/ccp0000805 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  33. Craske MG, Treanor M, Zbozinek TD, & Vervliet B (2022). Optimizing exposure therapy with an inhibitory retrieval approach and the OptEx Nexus. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 104069. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2022.104069 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  34. Culver NC, Stevens S, Fanselow MS, & Craske MG (2018). Building physiological toughness: Some aversive events during extinction may attenuate return of fear. Journal of behavior therapy and experimental psychiatry, 58, 18–28. 10.1016/i.ibtep.2017.07.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  35. de Jong PJ, Vorage I, & van den Hout MA (2000). Counterconditioning in the treatment of spider phobia: Effects on disgust, fear and valence. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(11), 1055–1069. 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00135-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  36. Desch S, Schweinhardt P, Seymour B, Flor H, & Becker S (2023). Evidence for dopaminergic involvement in endogenous modulation of pain relief. Elife, 12, e81436. 10.7554/eLife.81436 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  37. Deutsch R, Smith KJM, Kordts-Freudinger R, & Reichardt R (2015). How absent negativity relates to affect and motivation: An integrative relief model. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 152. 10.3389/FPSYG.2015.0Q152/BIBTEX [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  38. Dhingra I, Zhang S, Zhornitsky S, Wang W, Le TM, & Li CSR (2021). Sex differences in neural responses to reward and the influences of individual reward and punishment sensitivity. BMC neuroscience, 22, 1–14. 10.1186/s12868-021-00618-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  39. Dibbets P, & Fonteyne R (2015). High spider fearfuls can overcome their fear in a virtual approach-avoidance conflict task. Journal of Depression & Anxiety , 4(2), 1–7. 10.4172/2167-1044.1000182 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  40. Diekhof EK (2018). Estradiol and the reward system in humans. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences, 23, 58–64. 10.1016/j.cobeha.2018.03.010 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  41. Dour H, Brown L, & Craske MG (2016). Positive valence reduces susceptibility to return of fear and enhances approach behavior. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 50, 277–282. 10.1016/J.JBTEP.2015.09.010 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  42. Du J, Zeng X, Zheng X, & Zhuang C (2015). The impact of unconditioned stimulus devaluation on conditional fear extinction. Acta Psychologica Sinica, 47(3), 344. [Google Scholar]
  43. Eippert F, Bingel U, Schoell E, Yacubian J, & Büchel C (2008). Blockade of endogenous opioid neurotransmission enhances acquisition of conditioned fear in humans. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(21), 5465–5472. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5336-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  44. Eisner L, Johnson S, & Carver C (2009). Positive affect regulation in anxiety disorders. Journal of Anxiety Disorders, 23(5), 645–649. 10.1016/J.JANXDIS.2009.02.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  45. Ekuni R, Vaz LJ, & Bueno OFA (2011). Levels of processing: The evolution of a framework. Psychology & Neuroscience, 4, 333–339. 10.3922/i.psns.201L3.006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  46. Ellis HC, Thomas RL, & Rodriguez IA (1984). Emotional mood states and memory: Elaborative encoding, semantics processing, and cognitive effort. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 10(3), 470–482. 10.1037/0278-7393.10.3.470 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  47. Enter D, Spinhoven P, & Roelofs K (2016). Dare to approach: single dose testosterone administration promotes threat approach in patients with social anxiety disorder. Clinical Psychological Science, 4(6), 1073–1079. 10.1177/2167702616631499 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  48. Everaert J, Bronstein MV, Cannon TD, & Joormann J (2018). Looking Through Tinted Glasses: Depression and Social Anxiety Are Related to Both Interpretation Biases and Inflexible Negative Interpretations. Clinical Psychological Science, 6(4), 517–528. 10.1177/2167702617747968 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  49. Fanselow MS, & Wassum KM (2016). The origins and organization of vertebrate Pavlovian conditioning. Cold Spring Harbor perspectives in biology, 8(1), a021717. 10.1101/cshperspect.a021717 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  50. Federmeier KD, Kirson DA, Moreno EM, & Kutas M (2001). Effects of transient, mild mood states on semantic memory organization and use: an event-related potential investigation in humans. Neuroscience Letters, 305(3), 149–152. 10.1016/S0304-3940(01)01843-2 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  51. Foa EB, & Kozak MJ (1986). Emotional Processing of Fear. Exposure to Corrective Information. Psychological Bulletin, 99( 1), 20–35. 10.1037/0033-2909.99.1.20 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  52. Forbes EE, Ryan ND, Phillips ML, Manuck SB, Worthman CM, Moyles DL, Tarr JA, Sciarrillo SR, & Dahl RE (2010). Healthy adolescents’ neural response to reward: associations with puberty, positive affect, and depressive symptoms. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry, 49(2), 162–172. 10.1016/j.jaac.2009.11.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  53. Fredrickson BL, Grewen KM, Algoe SB, Firestine AM, Arevalo JM, Ma J, & Cole SW (2015). Psychological well-being and the human conserved transcriptional response to adversity. PloS one, 10(3), e0121839. 10.1371/journal.pone.0121839 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  54. Fredrickson BL, Grewen KM, Coffey KA, Algoe SB, Firestine AM, Arevalo JM, … & Cole SW (2013). A functional genomic perspective on human well-being. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 110(33), 13684–13689. 10.1073/pnas.1305419110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  55. Gentry RN, Schuweiler DR, & Roesch MR (2019). Dopamine signals related to appetitive and aversive events in paradigms that manipulate reward and avoidability. Brain research, 1713, 80–90. 10.1016/j.brainres.2018.10.008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  56. Gershman SJ, Hartley CA Individual differences in learning predict the return of fear. Learning & behavior 43, 243–250 (2015). 10.3758/s13420-015-0176-z [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  57. Gershman SJ, & Niv Y (2012). Exploring a latent cause theory of classical conditioning. Learning & behavior, 40, 255–268. 10.3758/s13420-012-0080-8 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  58. Gershman SJ, Jones CE, Norman KA, Monfils MH, & Niv Y (2013). Gradual extinction prevents the return of fear: implications for the discovery of state. Frontiers in behavioral neuroscience, 7, 164. 10.3389/fnbeh.2013.00164 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  59. Graham BM, Li SH, Black MJ, & Öst LG (2018). The association between estradiol levels, hormonal contraceptive use, and responsiveness to one-session-treatment for spider phobia in women. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 90, 134–140. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2018.02.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  60. Grillo L (2016). A possible role of anhedonia as common substrate for depression and anxiety. Depression research and treatment, 2016. 10.1155/2016/1598130 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  61. Guineau MG, Ikani N, Rinck M, Collard RM, Van Eijndhoven P, Tendolkar I, … & Vrijsen JN (2023). Anhedonia as a transdiagnostic symptom across psychological disorders: A network approach. Psychological medicine, 53(9), 3908–3919. 10.1017/S0033291722000575 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  62. Hänze M, & Hesse FW (2008). Emotional influences on semantic priming. Cognition and Emotion, 7(2), 195–205. 10.1080/026999393084Q9184 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  63. Hartlage S, Alloy LB, Vázquez C, & Dykman B (1993). Automatic and Effortful Processing in Depression. Psychological Bulletin, 113(2), 247–278. 10.1037/0033-2909.113.2.247 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  64. Hermans D, Vansteenwegen D, Crombez G, Baeyens F, & Eelen P (2002). Expectancy-learning and evaluative learning in human classical conditioning: affective priming as an indirect and unobtrusive measure of conditioned stimulus valence. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 40(3), 217–234. 10.1016/S0005-7967(0n00006-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  65. Hermans EJ, Bos PA, Ossewaarde L, Ramsey NF, Fernández G, & van Honk J (2010). Effects of exogenous testosterone on the ventral striatal BOLD response during reward anticipation in healthy women. Neuroimage, 52(1), 277–283. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2010.04.019 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  66. Holmes NM, Leung HT, & Westbrook RF (2016). Counterconditioned fear responses exhibit greater renewal than extinguished fear responses. Learning & Memory, 23(4), 141–150. 10.1101/LM.040659.115 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  67. Hornstein EA, Craske MG, Fanselow MS, & Eisenberger NI (2022). Reclassifying the unique inhibitory properties of social support figures: A roadmap for exploring prepared fear suppression. Biological Psychiatry, 91(9), 778–785. 10.1016/i.biopsych.2021.11.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  68. Hornung J, Lewis CA, & Derntl B (2020). Sex hormones and human brain function.Handbook of clinical neurology, 175, 195–207. 10.1016/B978-0-444-64123-6.00014-X [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  69. Hosoba T, Iwanaga M, & Seiwa H (2001). The effect of UCS inflation and deflation procedures on ‘fear’ conditioning. Behaviour research and therapy, 39(4), 465–475. 10.1016/S0005-7967(00)00025-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  70. Huys QJ, Pizzagalli DA, Bogdan R, & Dayan P (2013). Mapping anhedonia onto reinforcement learning: a behavioural meta-analysis. Biology of Mood & Anxiety Disorders, 3(1), 1–16. 10.1186/2045-5380-3-12 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  71. Isen AM (1987). Positive Affect, Cognitive Processes, and Social Behavior. Advances in Experimental Social Psychology, 20(C), 203–253. 10.1016/S0065-2601(08)60415-3 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  72. Joos E, Vansteenwegen D, & Hermans D (2012). Post-acquisition repetitive thought in fear conditioning: An experimental investigation of the effect of CS-US-rehearsal. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 43(2), 737–744. 10.1016/J.JBTEP.2011.10.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  73. Joos E, Vansteenwegen D, Vervliet B, & Hermans D (2013). Repeated activation of a cs-us-contingency memory results in sustained conditioned responding. Frontiers in Psychology, 4(MAY), 305. 10.3389/FPSYG.2013.003Q5/BIBTEX [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  74. Kalisch R, Gerlicher AMV, & Duvarci S (2019). A Dopaminergic Basis for Fear Extinction. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 23(4), 274–277. 10.1016/J.TICS.2019.01.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  75. Kang S, Vervliet B, Engelhard IM, van Dis EAM, & Hagenaars MA (2018). Reduced return of threat expectancy after counterconditioning verus extinction. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 108, 78–84. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2018.06.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  76. Kashdan TB (2007). Social anxiety spectrum and diminished positive experiences: Theoretical synthesis and meta-analysis. In Clinical Psychology Review. 10.1016/j.cpr.2006.12.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  77. Keller NE, & Dunsmoor JE (2020). The effects of aversive-to-appetitive counterconditioning on implicit and explicit fear memory. Learning & Memory, 27(1), 12–19. 10.1101/LM.050740.119 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  78. Keller NE, Hennings AC, & Dunsmoor JE (2020). Behavioral and neural processes in counterconditioning: Past and future directions. Behaviour research and therapy, 125, 103532. 10.1016/j.brat.2019.103532 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  79. Kessler RC, Petukhova M, Sampson NA, Zaslavsky AM, & Wittchen HU (2012). Twelve-month and lifetime prevalence and lifetime morbid risk of anxiety and mood disorders in the United States. International journal of methods in psychiatric research, 21(3), 169–184. 10.1002/mpr.1359 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  80. Kim J, Pignatelli M, Xu S, Itohara S, & Tonegawa S (2016). Antagonistic negative and positive neurons of the basolateral amygdala. Nature neuroscience, 19(12), 1636–1646. 10.1038/nn.4414 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  81. Kitayama S, Akutsu S, Uchida Y, & Cole SW (2016). Work, meaning, and gene regulation: Findings from a Japanese information technology firm. Psychoneuroendocrinology, 72, 175–181. 10.1016/j.psyneuen.2016.07.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  82. Klein S, Kruse O, León IT, Van Oudenhove L, van’t Hof SR, Klucken T, … & Stark R (2022). Cross-paradigm integration shows a common neural basis for aversive and appetitive conditioning. NeuroImage, 263, 119594. 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2022.119594 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  83. Kryza-Lacombe M, Pearson N, Lyubomirsky S, Stein MB, Wiggins JL, & Taylor CT (2021). Changes in neural reward processing following Amplification of Positivity treatment for depression and anxiety: Preliminary findings from a randomized waitlist controlled trial. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 142, 103860. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2021.103860 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  84. Kube T, & Glombiewski JA (2021). How Depressive Symptoms Hinder Positive Information Processing: An Experimental Study on the Interplay of Cognitive Immunisation and Negative Mood in the Context of Expectation Adjustment. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 45(3), 517–528. 10.1007/S10608-020-10191-4 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  85. Kube T, Kirchner L, Lemmer G, & Glombiewski JA (2022). How the Discrepancy Between Prior Expectations and New Information Influences Expectation Updating in Depression—The Greater, the Better? Clinical Psychological Science, 10(3), 430–449. https://doi.org/10.1177/21677026211024644 [Google Scholar]
  86. Kube T, & Rozenkrantz L (2021). When Beliefs Face Reality: An Integrative Review of Belief Updating in Mental Health and Illness. Perspectives on Psychological Science : A Journal of the Association for Psychological Science, 16(2), 247–274. 10.1177/1745691620931496 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  87. Kube T, Schwarting R, Rozenkrantz L, Glombiewski JA, & Rief W (2020). Distorted Cognitive Processes in Major Depression: A Predictive Processing Perspective. Biological Psychiatry, 87(5), 388–398. 10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2019.07.017 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  88. Kujawa A, Klein DN, Pegg S, & Weinberg A (2020). Developmental trajectories to reduced activation of positive valence systems: A review of biological and environmental contributions. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 43, 100791. 10.1016/i.dcn.2020.100791 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  89. Lancaster TM, Ihssen N, Brindley LM, & Linden DEJ (2017). Further support for association between GWAS variant for positive emotion and reward systems. Translational Psychiatry, 7(1), e1018–e1018. 10.1038/tp.2016.289 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  90. Landy LN, Schneider RL, & Arch JJ (2015). Acceptance and commitment therapy for the treatment of anxiety disorders: a concise review. Current Opinion in Psychology, 2, 70–74. 10.1016/J.COPSYC.2014.11.004 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  91. LeDoux JE, Moscarello J, Sears R, & Campese V (2016). The birth, death and resurrection of avoidance: a reconceptualization of a troubled paradigm. Molecular Psychiatry 2016 22:1, 22(1), 24–36. 10.1038/mp.2016.166 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  92. Lee CS, & Yeghiazarian C (2021). Personal value, self-efficacy, and social acceptability of a social behavior as correlates of behavioral action in social anxiety. Trends in Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, 43(3), 217–224. 10.47626/2237-6089-2020-0129 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  93. Leight KA, & Ellis HC (1981). Emotional mood states, strategies, and state-dependency in memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 20(3), 251–266. 10.1016/S0022-5371(81)90406-0 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  94. Leng L, Beckers T, & Vervliet B (2022). No joy - why bother? Higher anhedonia relates to reduced pleasure from and motivation for threat avoidance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 159, 104227. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2022.104227 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  95. Leng L, Beckers T, & Vervliet B (2023). What a relief! The pleasure of threat avoidance. Emotion. 10.1037/emo0001312 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  96. Leng L, Beckers T, & Vervliet B (2024). Anhedonia influences threat avoidance and relief: A conceptual replication. Journal of Mood & Anxiety Disorders, 5, 100050. 10.1016/j.xjmad.2024.100050 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  97. Levy HC, Goldblum RS, Mullins J, & Tolin DF (2023). An investigation of the moderating role of estradiol in interoceptive exposure therapy for women with panic disorder. Journal of Cognitive Psychotherapy, 37(3), 252–265. 10.1891/JCP-2022-0012 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  98. Levy HC, Mullins J, & Tolin DF (2024). An investigation of the role of estradiol in fear reduction during a single session of exposure therapy. Cognitive Behaviour Therapy, 1–13. 10.1080/16506073.2024.2313743 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  99. Loerinc A, Meuret A, Twohig M, Rosenfield D, Bluett E, & Craske MG (2015). Response rates for CBT for anxiety disorders: Need for standardized criteria. Clinical Psychology Review, 42, 72–82. 10.1016/J.CPR.2015.08.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  100. Lonsdorf TB, Menz MM, Andreatta M, Fullana MA, Golkar A, Haaker J, Heitland I, Hermann A, Kuhn M, Kruse O, Meir Drexler S, Meulders A, Nees F, Pittig A, Richter J, Römer S, Shiban Y, Schmitz A, Straube B, … Merz CJ (2017). Don’t fear ‘fear conditioning’: Methodological considerations for the design and analysis of studies on human fear acquisition, extinction, and return of fear. In Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2017.02.026 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  101. Lovibond P. (2006). Fear and Avoidance: An Integrated Expectancy Model. In Craske MG, Hermans D, & Vansteenwegen D (Eds.), Fear and learning: From basic processes to clinical implications, 117–132. American Psychological Association. 10.1037/11474-006 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  102. Lovibond PF, Davis NR, & O’Flaherty AS (2000). Protection from extinction in human fear conditioning. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 38(10), 967–983. 10.1016/S0005-7967(99)00121-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  103. Lovibond PF, Mitchell CJ, Minard E, Brady A, & Menzies RG (2009). Safety behaviours preserve threat beliefs: Protection from extinction of human fear conditioning by an avoidance response. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 47(8), 716–720. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2009.04.013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  104. Luo R, Uematsu A, Weitemier A, Aquili L, Koivumaa J, McHugh TJ, & Johansen JP (2018). A dopaminergic switch for fear to safety transitions. Nature Communications, 9(1), 1–11. 10.1038/s41467-018-04784-7 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  105. Matt GE, Vázquez C, & Campbell WK (1992). Mood-congruent recall of affectively toned stimuli: A meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 12(2), 227–255. 10.1016/0272-7358(92)90116-P [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  106. McGlade AL, & Craske MG (2021). Optimizing exposure: Between-session mental rehearsal as an augmentation strategy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 139, 103827. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2021.103827 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  107. McNally GP (2009). The Roles of Endogenous Opioids in Fear Learning. International Journal of Comparative Psychology, 22(3). 10.46867/IJCP.2009.22.03.02 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  108. McNally GP, & Westbrook RF (2003). Opioid Receptors Regulate the Extinction of Pavlovian Fear Conditioning. Behavioral Neuroscience, 117(6), 1292–1301. 10.1037/0735-7044.117.6.1292 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  109. Meier IM, Eikemo M, & Leknes S (2021). The Role of Mu-Opioids for Reward and Threat Processing in Humans: Bridging the Gap from Preclinical to Clinical Opioid Drug Studies. Current Addiction Reports, 8(2), 306–318. 10.1007/S40429-021-00366-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  110. Mineka S, & Oehlberg K (2008). The relevance of recent developments in classical conditioning to understanding the etiology and maintenance of anxiety disorders. Acta Psychologica, 127(3), 567–580. 10.1016/J.ACTPSY.2007.11.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  111. Niv Y. (2019). Learning task-state representations. Nature neuroscience, 22(10), 1544–1553. 10.1038/s41593-019-0470-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  112. Nusslock R, & Alloy LB (2017). Reward processing and mood-related symptoms: An RDoC and translational neuroscience perspective. Journal of Affective Disorders, 216, 3–16. 10.1016/J.JAD.2017.02.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  113. Ostlund SB, & Balleine BW (2008). Differential involvement of the basolateral amygdala and mediodorsal thalamus in instrumental action selection. Journal of Neuroscience, 28(17), 4398–4405. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5472-07.2008 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  114. Papageorgiou D, & Karekla M (2023). Using values interventions to improve exposure therapy engagement in specific phobias. Journal of Psychotherapy Integration. 34(1), 75–87. 10.1037/int0000320 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  115. Papalini S, Ashoori M, Zaman J, Beckers T, & Vervliet B (2021). The role of context in persistent avoidance and the predictive value of relief. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 138, 103816. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2021.103816 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  116. Perez OD, & Dickinson A (2020). A theory of actions and habits: The interaction of rate correlation and contiguity systems in free-operant behavior. Psychological Review, 127(6), 945. 10.1037/rev0000201 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  117. Pezze MA, & Feldon J (2004). Mesolimbic dopaminergic pathways in fear conditioning. Progress in neurobiology, 74{5), 301–320. 10.1016/j.pneurobio.2004.09.004 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  118. Pittig A. (2019). Incentive-based extinction of safety behaviors: Positive outcomes competing with aversive outcomes trigger fear-opposite action to prevent protection from fear extinction. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 121, 103463. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2019.103463 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  119. Pittig A, Boschet JM, Glück VM, & Schneider K (2021). Elevated costly avoidance in anxiety disorders: Patients show little downregulation of acquired avoidance in face of competing rewards for approach. Depression and Anxiety, 38(3), 361–371. 10.1002/DA.23119 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  120. Pittig A, & Dehler J (2019). Same fear responses, less avoidance: Rewards competing with aversive outcomes do not buffer fear acquisition, but attenuate avoidance to accelerate subsequent fear extinction. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 112, 1–11. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2018.11.003 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  121. Pittig A, Hengen K, Bublatzky F, & Alpers GW (2018). Social and monetary incentives counteract fear-driven avoidance: Evidence from approach-avoidance decisions. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 60, 69–77. 10.1016/J.JBTEP.2018.04.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  122. Pittig A, & Scherbaum S (2020). Costly avoidance in anxious individuals: Elevated threat avoidance in anxious individuals under high, but not low competing rewards. Journal of Behavior Therapy and Experimental Psychiatry, 66, 101524. 10.1016/J.JBTEP.2019.101524 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  123. Pittig A, Treanor M, LeBeau RT, & Craske MG (2018). The role of associative fear and avoidance learning in anxiety disorders: Gaps and directions for future research. Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews, 88, 117–140. 10.1016/J.NEUBIOREV.2018.03.015 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  124. Pittig A, & Wong AH (2022). Reducing the return of avoidance and fear by directly targeting avoidance: Comparing incentive-based and instructed extinction of avoidance to passive fear extinction. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 13(4), 20438087221136424. 10.1177/20438087221136424 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  125. Quintero MJ, Flores A, Gutiérrez-Huerta MT, Molina-Guerrero P, López FJ, & Morís J (2022). Limitations of occasional reinforced extinction to alleviate spontaneous recovery and reinstatement effects: Evidence for a trial-signalling mechanism. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 75(5), 765–783. 10.1177/17470218211043434 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  126. Quintero MJ, Morís J, & López FJ (2023). Evaluating the effects of counterconditioning, novelty-facilitated, and standard extinction on the spontaneous recovery of threat expectancy and conditioned stimulus valence. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 17470218231165373. 10.1177/17470218231165373 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  127. Rauch SAM, & Ruzek J (2012). Review of exposure therapy: A gold standard for PTSD treatment Skills for Psychological Recovery (SPR): Field Operations Guide View project Neuroendocrine System in Trauma and PTSD View project. Article in The Journal of Rehabilitation Research and Development. 10.1682/JRRD.2011.08.0152 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  128. Rescorla RA, & Wagner AR (1972). A theory of Pavlovian conditioning: Variations in the effectiveness of reinforcement and non-reinforcement. Classical Conditioning II: Current Research and Theory, 2, 64–69. [Google Scholar]
  129. Rosenberg BM, Taschereau-Dumouchel V, Lau H, Young KS, Nusslock R, Zinbarg RE, & Craske MG (2021). A multivoxel pattern analysis of anhedonia during fear extinction: Implications for safety learning. Biological Psychiatry: Cognitive Neuroscience and Neuroimaging. 10.1016/j.bpsc.2021.12.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  130. Rosenkranz JA, & Grace AA (1999). Modulation of basolateral amygdala neuronal firing and afferent drive by dopamine receptor activation in vivo. Journal of Neuroscience, 19(24), 11027–11039. 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.19-24-11027.1999 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  131. Salinas-Hernández XI, & Duvarci S (2021). Dopamine in Fear Extinction. Frontiers in Synaptic Neuroscience, 13, 10. 10.3389/FNSYN.2021.635879/BIBTEX [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  132. Salinas-Hernández XI, Vogel P, Betz S, Kalisch R, Sigurdsson T, & Duvarci S (2018). Dopamine neurons drive fear extinction learning by signaling the omission of expected aversive outcomes. ELife, 7. 10.7554/ELIFE.38818 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  133. Salkovskis PM (1991). The Importance of Behaviour in the Maintenance of Anxiety and Panic: A Cognitive Account. Behavioural and Cognitive Psychotherapy, 19(1), 6–19. 10.1017/S0141347300011472 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  134. San Martín C, Jacobs B, & Vervliet B (2020). Further characterization of relief dynamics in the conditioning and generalization of avoidance: Effects of distress tolerance and intolerance of uncertainty. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 124, 103526. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2019.103526 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  135. Sandman CF, & Craske MG (2022). Psychological Treatments for Anhedonia. In Pizzagalli DA(Ed.), Anhedonia: Preclinical, Translational, and Clinical Integration (pp. 491–513). Springer International Publishing, 10.1007/7854_2021_291 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  136. Sandman CF, & Craske MG (revise and resubmit). Experiential processing increases positive affect and decreases dampening appraisals during autobiographical memory recall in an anhedonic sample. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  137. Schultz W. (2016). Dopamine reward prediction-error signalling: a two-component response. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 17(3), 183–195. 10.1038/nrn.2015.26 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  138. Sirucek L, Price RC, Gandhi W, Hoeppli ME, Fahey E, Qu A, … & Schweinhardt P (2021). Endogenous opioids contribute to the feeling of pain relief in humans. Pain, 162(12), 2821–2831. 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000002285 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  139. Slaney C, Perkins AM, Davis R, Penton-Voak T, Munafò MR, Houghton CJ, & Robinson ESJ (2022). Objective measures of reward sensitivity and motivation in people with high v. low anhedonia. Psychological Medicine, 1–9. 10.1017/S0033291722001052 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  140. Storbeck J, & Clore GL (2008). The affective regulation of cognitive priming. Emotion, 8(2), 208. https://doi.org/10.1037%2F1528-3542.8.2.208 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  141. Swain J, Hancock K, Hainsworth C, & Bowman J (2013). Acceptance and Commitment Therapy in the treatment of anxiety: A systematic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 33(8), 965–978. 10.1016/J.CPR.2013.07.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  142. Taylor CT, Lyubomirsky S, & Stein MB (2017). Upregulating the Positive Affect System in Anxiety and Depression: Outcomes of a Positive Activity Intervention. Depression and Anxiety, 34(3), 267. 10.1002/DA.22593 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  143. Taylor CT, Pearlstein SL, Kakaria S, Lyubomirsky S, & Stein MB (2020). Enhancing Social Connectedness in Anxiety and Depression Through Amplification of Positivity: Preliminary Treatment Outcomes and Process of Change. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 44(4), 788–800. 10.1007/S10608-020-10102-7/FIGURES/3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  144. Taylor CT, Stein MB, Simmons AN, He F, Oveis C, Shakya HB, Sieber WJ, Fowler JH, & Jain S (2024). Amplification of Positivity Treatment for Anxiety and Depression: A Randomized Experimental Therapeutics Trial Targeting Social Reward Sensitivity to Enhance Social Connectedness. Biological Psychiatry, 95(5), 434–443. 10.1016/J.BIOPSYCH.2023.07.024 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  145. Thompson A, McEvoy PM, & Lipp OV (2018). Enhancing extinction learning: Occasional presentations of the unconditioned stimulus during extinction eliminate spontaneous recovery, but not necessarily reacquisition of fear. Behaviour research and therapy, 108, 29–39. 10.1016/j.brat.2018.07.001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  146. Tschacher W, Junghan UM, & Pfammatter M (2014). Towards a taxonomy of common factors in psychotherapy—results of an expert survey. Clinical psychology & psychotherapy, 21(1), 82–96. 10.1002/cpp.1822 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  147. Van Dis EAM, Hagenaars MA, Bockting CLH, & Engelhard IM (2019). Reducing negative stimulus valence does not attenuate the return of fear: Two counterconditioning experiments. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 120, 103416. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2019.103416 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  148. Van Dis EAM, Van Veen SC, Hagenaars MA, Batelaan NM, Bockting CLH, Van Den Heuvel RM, Cuijpers P, & Engelhard IM (2020). Long-term Outcomes of Cognitive Behavioral Therapy for Anxiety-Related Disorders: A Systematic Review and Meta-analysis. JAMA Psychiatry, 77(3), 265–273. 10.1001/JAMAPSYCHIATRY.2019.3986 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  149. Velasco ER, Florido A, Perez-Caballero L, Marin I, & Andero R (2023). The Impacts of Sex Differences and Sex Hormones on Fear Extinction. In Fear Extinction: From Basic Neuroscience to Clinical Implications (pp. 105–132). Cham: Springer International Publishing, 10.1007/7854_2023_426 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  150. Vervliet B, Lange I, & Milad MR (2017). Temporal dynamics of relief in avoidance conditioning and fear extinction: Experimental validation and clinical relevance. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 96, 66–78. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2017.04.011 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  151. Veselic S, Jocham G, Gausterer C, Wagner B, Ernhoefer-Reßler M, Lanzenberger R, … & Vermeer AL (2021). A causal role of estradiol in human reinforcement learning. Hormones and Behavior, 134, 105022. 10.1016/j.yhbeh.2021.105022 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  152. Villano WJ, & Heller AS (2024). Depression is associated with blunted affective responses to naturalistic reward prediction errors. Psychological medicine, 1–9. 10.1017/S0033291724000047 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  153. Vinograd M, Stout DM, & Risbrough VB (2021). Anhedonia in Posttraumatic Stress Disorder: Prevalence, Phenotypes, and Neural Circuitry. In Current Topics in Behavioral Neurosciences (pp. 1–15). Springer Berlin Heidelberg. 10.1007/7854_2021_292 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  154. Warthen KG, Boyse-Peacor A, Jones KG, Sanford B, Love TM, & Mickey BJ (2020). Sex differences in the human reward system: convergent behavioral, autonomic and neural evidence. Social cognitive and affective neuroscience, 15(7), 789–801. 10.1093/scan/nsaa104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  155. Watkins P. (1992). Mood-Congruent Memory in Depression: Emotional Priming or Elaboration? Does gratitude predict future joy? View project Implicit Memory Bias in Depression View project. Article in Journal of Abnormal Psychology. 10.1037/0021-843X.101.3.581 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  156. Wells A, Clark DM, Salkovskis P, Ludgate J, Hackmann A, & Gelder M (1995). Social phobia: The role of in-situation safety behaviors in maintaining anxiety and negative beliefs. Behavior Therapy, 26(1), 153–161. 10.1016/S0005-7894(05)80088-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  157. Willems AL, & Vervliet B (2021). When nothing matters: Assessing markers of expectancy violation during omissions of threat. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 136, 103764. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2020.103764 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  158. Williams CC, Hassall CD, Trska R, Holroyd CB, & Krigolson OE (2017). When theory and biology differ: The relationship between reward prediction errors and expectancy. Biological Psychology, 129, 265–272. 10.1016/J.BIOPSYCHO.2017.09.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  159. Winer ES, Bryant J, Bartoszek G, Rojas E, Nadorff MR, & Kilgore J (2017). Mapping the relationship between anxiety, anhedonia, and depression. Journal of affective disorders, 221, 289–296. 10.1016/j.jad.2017.06.006 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  160. Wingo AP, Almli LM, Stevens JS, Jovanovic T, Wingo TS, Tharp G, Li Y, Lori A, Briscione M, Jin P, Binder EB, Bradley B, Gibson G, & Ressler KJ (2016). Genome-wide association study of positive emotion identifies a genetic variant and a role for microRNAs. Molecular Psychiatry, 22(5), 774–783. 10.1038/mp.2016.143 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  161. Wolpe J. (1961). The systematic desensitization treatment of neuroses. The Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease, 132(3), 189–203. 10.1097/00005053-196103000-00001 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  162. Wolpe J. (1968). Psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition. Conditional Reflex: A Pavlovian Journal of Research & Therapy, 3, 234–240. 10.1007/BF03000093 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  163. Wong AHK, Wirth FM, & Pittig A (2022). Avoidance of learnt fear: Models, potential mechanisms, and future directions. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 151, 104056. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2022.104056 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  164. Yang X, Fang Y, Chen H, Zhang T, Yin X, Man J, Yang L, & Lu M (2021). Global, regional and national burden of anxiety disorders from 1990 to 2019: results from the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Epidemiology and Psychiatric Sciences, 30, e36. 10.1017/S2045796021000275 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  165. Young KS, Bookheimer SY, Nusslock R, Zinbarg RE, Damme KS, Chat IKY, … & Craske MG (2021). Dysregulation of threat neurocircuitry during fear extinction: the role of anhedonia. Neuropsychopharmacology, 46(9), 1650–1657. 10.1038/s41386-021-01003-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  166. Zbozinek TD, & Craske MG (2017a). The Role of Positive Affect in Enhancing Extinction Learning and Exposure Therapy for Anxiety Disorders. Journal of Experimental Psychopathology, 8(1), 13–39. 10.5127/JEP.052615 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
  167. Zbozinek TD, & Craske MG (2017b). Positive affect predicts less reacquisition of fear: relevance for long-term outcomes of exposure therapy. Cognition and Emotion, 31(4), 712–725. 10.1080/02699931.2016.1142428 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  168. Zbozinek TD, Holmes EA, & Craske MG (2015). The effect of positive mood induction on reducing reinstatement fear: Relevance for long term outcomes of exposure therapy. Behaviour Research and Therapy, 71, 65–75. 10.1016/J.BRAT.2015.05.016 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  169. Zbozinek TD, Perez OD, Wise T, Fanselow M, & Mobbs D (2022). Ambiguity drives higher-order Pavlovian learning. PLOS Computational Biology, 18(9), e1010410. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  170. Zhang X, Kim J, & Tonegawa S (2020). Amygdala Reward Neurons Form and Store Fear Extinction Memory. Neuron, 105(6), 1077–1093.e7. 10.1016/J.NEURON.2019.12.025 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

RESOURCES