Abstract
An acute bout of exercise in the moments after learning benefits the retention of new memories. This finding can be explained, at least partly, through a consolidation account: exercise provides a physiological state that is conducive to the early stabilisation of labile new memories, which supports their retention and subsequent retrieval. The modification of consolidation through non-invasive exercise interventions offers great applied potential. However, it remains poorly understood whether effects of exercise translate from the laboratory to naturalistic settings and whether the intensity of exercise determines the effect in memory. To this end, adult endurance runners were recruited as participants and completed two study sessions spaced two weeks apart. In each session, participants were presented with a list of words and asked to recall them on three occasions: (i) immediately following their presentation, (ii) after a 30-minute retention interval, and (iii) after 24 hours. Crucially, the 30-minute retention interval comprised our experimental manipulation: higher intensity exercise (running) in the first session and lower intensity exercise (walking) in the second, both completed in a naturalistic setting around participants’ existing physical activity training programmes. Exertion was recorded through heart rate and rate of perceived exertion data. Alertness, mood, and arousal ratings were also collected before and after the 30-minute retention interval. Immediate memory for the two wordlists was matched, but participants retained significantly more words after 30 minutes and 24 hours when encoding was followed by higher than lower intensity exercise. Exertion data revealed that participants experienced vigorous and light exercise in the higher and lower intensity conditions, respectively. Significant improvements in alertness, mood, and arousal were observed following both exercise conditions, but especially in the higher intensity condition. These outcomes reveal that experiencing higher intensity physical activity in the field is conducive to declarative memory retention, possibly because it encourages consolidation.
Introduction
New memories are fragile upon their formation and vulnerable to disruption [1]. For labile memory traces to be transferred to a more enduring and stable form that can be used adaptively to guide behaviour, they must undergo a process of consolidation [1–3]. This memory stabilisation is proposed to occur through two interrelated processes: (i) early cellular-level consolidation, which is a hippocampal-dependent process that strengthens synaptic connectivity through the induction of long-term potentiation (LTP) in the immediate aftermath of encoding [4, 5] and (ii) later systems-level consolidation, which involves the strengthening of connections between the hippocampus and neocortical regions, facilitating the transition of memory representations into a hippocampal-independent state over the longer term [3, 6].
Contemporary theories propose that (at least) cellular consolidation is an automatic and opportunistic process that occurs especially during states of reduced sensory processing and physical activity, which would otherwise interfere [7–9]. This opportunistic-consolidation hypothesis is supported by (i) behavioural evidence demonstrating superior memory following periods of sleep and quiet rest [10–16], and (ii) neuroimaging and neuroscientific evidence demonstrating a greater magnitude of consolidation-related brain activity (e.g., neural “replay” of recently encoded memories) during periods of quiescence [12, 17–20]. Indeed, the disruption of such mechanisms, for example, through pharmaceutical interventions and periods of task engagement, is found to be detrimental to memory retention [16, 21, 22], especially in those with impaired memory systems [23–25]. These findings evidence the malleable nature of early consolidation processes, where the fate of new memories can be determined in the minutes immediately following their creation.
Converse to observations of superior memory following post-encoding quiescence [e.g., wakeful rest; 16], acute exercise in the post-encoding period can also be conducive to declarative memory consolidation [26–35]. A meta-analysis of published work determined that acute exercise during the consolidation period after encoding positively influences the retention of new episodic information, relative to control conditions including no exercise [36]. Importantly, the effect of post-encoding exercise is dissociable from the influence of exercise in encoding, for example, when exercise is experienced prior to or during the encoding of information [32]. Indeed, current data suggest that there is no additive benefit of experiencing both pre- and post-encoding acute exercise [37]. Still, it is possible that post-encoding exercise could positively affect both the consolidation and retrieval of encoded traces should a bout of acute exercise occur shortly following encoding and soon before memory is probed. However, a beneficial effect of acute exercise on consolidation can be isolated, for example, in cases where exercise occurs shortly following encoding, but recall takes place after an extended period (e.g., 24 hours) by which point residual effects of exercise (e.g., on underlying neurophysiology or state of arousal) would be expected to have elapsed [36].
To achieve a positive effect in memory, the duration of the acute post-encoding bout of acute exercise need not be excessive in duration: both short (e.g., 5 minutes) and longer-duration (e.g., 60 minutes) bouts of acute exercise can positively influence consolidation [38, 39], and these effects are not transient but long lasting and detectable at least 24-hours post-encoding [34, 35, 40]. Moreover, in keeping with the time-dependent nature of consolidation, such effects are not restricted to exercise in the immediate aftermath of encoding; an acute bout of exercise can benefit consolidation even when it occurs several hours after encoding [28, 31]. This resonates with the outcomes of the aforementioned meta-analysis, which observed an overall positive effect of acute exercise during early and late consolidation [36]. Intriguingly, the meta-analysis also revealed that the effects of exercise during consolidation appear to be moderated by a range of factors, including exercise intensity. Specifically, pooled evidence suggests that vigorous-intensity exercise is beneficial to consolidation, while light and moderate intensity are not [36]. This does, however, conflict with some data suggesting that higher-intensity exercise has minimal effect on consolidation when it occurs soon after encoding [41], possibly because of the induction of an excessive stress response in cases of high-intensity exercise, which is known to be detrimental to consolidation [42]. It is worth noting that research exploring the contribution of exercise intensity to consolidation remains in its infancy and few studies have compared the effects of lower and higher intensity exercise on consolidation directly, especially within naturalistic settings.
Rather than a reduction in interference [e.g., from sensory processing associated with task engagement; 16, 43], post-encoding acute exercise may benefit consolidation because it stimulates neurophysiological activity in the hippocampus and associated networks [26], including the induction of LTP and synaptic plasticity [44]. While the acute cerebrovascular response to a single bout of exercise–and the relationship with cognition–is poorly characterised [45], emerging evidence suggests that even a short period of moderate aerobic exercise lasting ~15–20 minutes can increase hippocampal blood flow significantly [46, 47]. There is also compelling evidence for a positive relationship between acute (and chronic) exercise and the stimulation of brain-derived neurotrophic growth factor (BNDF) [48, 49], which is a precursor of LTP and synaptic plasticity [50], and is implicated in the success of consolidation [51–53]. Independent studies have shown that acute bouts of moderate-intensity [54, 55] and high-intensity [40, 55–57] exercise encourage BNDF expression and support memory retention, but findings are mixed and further work is required to better characterise the specific contributions of exercise duration and intensity to BNDF expression and memory consolidation. Nevertheless, it is possible that neurophysiological and cerebrovascular dynamics might explain, at least partly, why higher intensity exercise in the post-encoding period is reported to benefit early consolidation, relative to light intensity exercise and control (e.g., wakeful rest) conditions [36].
Are there applied consequences for the benefit of acute exercise on consolidation? There is great promise in recommending exercise as a non-invasive intervention to encourage consolidation, which, in addition to the memory benefit, could bring broader benefits in cognition and mood [58, 59]. Hitherto, most studies examining the effect of acute exercise on consolidation have done so within laboratory-based settings using student populations [30]. Despite obvious advantages of laboratory studies, including robust experimental control, such investigations lack the rich contextual features of naturalistic settings. Therefore, for exercise to be recommended as a memory-based intervention, there is a need for empirical studies in naturalistic settings [26]. If successful, there could be scope for exercise to be a recommended consolidation intervention, which may be especially beneficial for those with compromised memory [e.g., older adults; 40] and clinical populations [e.g., those with depression; 60] who may be unable to engage sufficiently with existing invasive (e.g., therapeutic) and non-invasive (e.g., quiet rest) interventions designed to improve memory retention. Thus, there is a need for further evidence demonstrating that an acute bout of exercise can enhance consolidation within naturalistic settings. To achieve this, further insights regarding the intensity of exercise required to achieve a positive effect in memory are required. Given that current evidence [36] indicates that vigorous-intensity exercise benefits consolidation, while light-intensity does not, it is logical to compare the effects of these two exercise intensities on consolidation in a natural setting as a first step towards establishing exercise as a non-invasive memory-promoting intervention.
To this end, the current study aimed to establish whether higher-intensity (vigorous) exercise encourages declarative memory consolidation in naturalistic settings, relative to lower-intensity (light) exercise. This was investigated through a laboratory-based consolidation paradigm [10, 15, 16] that was modified for delivery in the field, where, to promote ecological validity, it was moulded around the normal exercise activities of adult endurance runners. In keeping with contemporary theories and evidence [e.g., 10, 28], it was predicted that participants should demonstrate superior memory retention when encoding is followed by higher intensity exercise than lower-intensity exercise.
Materials and methods
Ethics statement
This research was approved by the Faculty of Health and Life Sciences’ Research Ethics Committee at Northumbria University (Ref: 49278). Informed written consent was acquired from participants following an initial study briefing and procedures adhered to the appropriate ethical principles for research in humans.
Participants
An a priori sample size calculation conducted using G*Power 3.1 [61] indicated that a minimum sample of 34 participants was required to detect a significant within-subject difference in a paired samples t-test (two tailed) when considering 80% power, an alpha level of .05, and a medium effect size (d = 0.5). The chosen effect size resonated with existing laboratory-based work that used similar paradigms, where large effect sizes are often observed [e.g., 10]. A more conservative medium effect was used in the current study given that it was conducted in the field, where extraneous variables likely influenced data to a degree. The minimum sample required (n = 34) was exceeded through the recruitment of 35 adults aged 18 years and older (females: n = 16, males: n = 19; mean age = 48.14 years, SD = 8.78, age range: 29–70 years). All individuals had normal or corrected-to-normal visual acuity, no known premorbid psychiatric or neurological disorders, and were amateur endurance runners recruited from an athletics club in the North East of England, UK. Endurance runners were recruited for the current study to ensure the study could be delivered in a naturalistic setting and that the physical demands of our post-encoding delay conditions (lower vs. higher intensity exercise) could be delivered safely. For example, these individuals were already familiar with the Rate of Perceived Exertion scale [RPE; 62], which was used to guide participants in achieving the desired physical activity exertion level in the two delay conditions–see Procedure. Study recruitment occurred from October to December 2022 and collected data were accessed for analyses following completion of data collection, i.e., from December 2022.
Design
A repeated measures design was employed to experimentally examine retention of aurally presented lists of words. Participants completed two study sessions spaced two weeks apart. In each session, they were presented a different list of 15 words adopted from the Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test [RAVLT; 63] and asked to recall them freely on three occasions: (i) immediately following their presentation, (ii) after a 30-minute retention interval, and (iii) after 24 hours. Crucially, the 30-minute retention interval between the immediate and 30-minute recall tests comprised our experimental manipulation. Specifically, in Session 1, participants experienced 25 minutes of higher intensity exercise (interval running). In Session 2, they experienced 25 minutes of lower intensity exercise (walking). For practical reasons (to fit our study procedure around participants’ regular exercise training programmes), delay conditions were not counterbalanced: all participants experienced the higher intensity condition first and lower intensity condition second. The study was delivered in a naturalistic setting in North East England, UK, except for the 24-hour delayed recall tests, which were delivered remotely over the phone. Data were collected between October-December 2022. See Fig 1 for an overview of the study procedure.
Materials
Two wordlists adopted from the RAVLT [63] were used to probe memory retention. Each wordlist contained 15 short, concrete, unrelated English nouns. Because the RAVLT has three standardised lists that are comparable in how memorable they are [e.g., 64], a different wordlist was presented in each of our two study sessions. Specifically, List A was presented in the first session and List B was presented in the second session. Verbal instructions were presented by the experimenter throughout the procedure using a set script. Responses during encoding and testing phases were collected from participants aurally and the experimenter wrote down participants’ responses. Subjective ratings of physical activity exertion were collected during the physical activity delay conditions through the Rate of Perceived Exertion scale [RPE; 62]. Objective data surrounding physical activity exertion were collected via heart rate data using participants’ own fitness watch devices (subset of participants, n = 18), and they reported these data to the researchers after the completion of the study. In addition to memory performance and physical exertion, participants’ self-reported fitness level, as well as pre- and post-study alertness, mood, and arousal were recorded. Methodological details and data analyses for these measures are reported in full in the Supplementary Information.
Procedure
Prior to the experimental procedure, participants provided their informed consent in writing after reading a study information sheet. They were then asked to complete a short online survey that requested details surrounding their age, gender, education level, perceived fitness level, and regularity of exercise. The subsequent experimental procedure was delivered in the field. Specifically, it was completed on a seafront promenade in the North East of England, UK, where participants would typically complete exercise activities as part of their regular training programme. This was done to promote ecological validity and ensure the delay condition activities (higher intensity vs. lower intensity exercise) were in keeping with participants’ natural sporting activities and routines.
In keeping with previous work [e.g., 65], a washout period separated study sessions, which took place two weeks apart. The same experimental procedure was used across both sessions except for (i) the list of words that were encoded and recalled by participants and (ii) the activities of the 30-minute retention interval, were our experimental manipulation occurred. List A was always presented during the first study session and List B during the second study session. A higher intensity (vigorous) exercise condition was always presented during the first study session and a lower intensity (light) exercise condition was always presented during the second session.
On arrival at a study session, the researcher welcomed the participant and explained the RPE scale measure [62]. While participants (amateur endurance runners) were familiar with this measure through their regular exercise training programmes, this explanation was provided at the start of a session to minimise possible errors in applying and reporting RPE scores during the study procedure (see later). Following this, the research presented the participant with a list of 15 words from the RAVLT [63]. Words were presented aurally by the researcher at a pace of one word per second. Immediately following this, participants were asked to freely recall as many of the words as possible. There was no time limit on the time they had to recall the words. Following the immediate recall test, participants completed one of two 30-minute exercise conditions. Both conditions were completed in the same naturalistic environment to minimise the likelihood of between-session extraneous variables influencing memory performance. To ensure integrity of both conditions, the exercise activities that participants completed were designed by a qualified Triathlon Coach.
In the higher intensity exercise condition, participants were required to complete a 25-minute interval run. Specifically, they ran continuously for 12 minutes, took a 1-minute walked recovery, then ran 12 minutes back to the start point. The pace during the two 12-minute runs was alternated using equidistant lampposts placed along the route to signal interval changes: two lampposts at a fast pace followed by two lampposts at a moderate pace, repeated for the duration of the activity. Using the RPE scale as a guide, participants were asked to ensure their fast pace mirrored an RPE of 6–7 (vigorous activity) and their moderate pace mirrored an RPE of 4–5 (moderate activity). This meant that lampposts served a dual purpose: (i) to alternate physical activity intensity in keeping with typical training programmes, and (ii) to provide a cognitive task to deter the likelihood of active mnemonic strategies. The latter is especially relevant to studies examining awake consolidation, where, for example, internal rehearsal of encoded materials may reduce or negate any effect of our experimental manipulation. For the 1-minute recovery walk in the middle of this higher intensity exercise condition, participants were requested to maintain a comfortable walking pace that corresponded to an RPE of 2–3 (light activity).
In the lower intensity exercise condition, participants walked along the same route as the higher intensity condition. Specifically, they walked in one direction for 12.5 minutes before retracing their steps to provide a total duration of 25 minutes. Throughout this exercise condition, they were requested to maintain a comfortable walking pace that corresponded to an RPE of 2–3 (light activity). While physical activity intensity was experimentally manipulated between conditions, to encourage comparable mental activity in both conditions, participants were asked to count lampposts as they walked.
Following completion of the exercise condition, participants completed a 30-minute delayed free recall test for the words encoded prior to the delay. They were then asked about their experiences during the exercise condition. Specifically, they were asked to reflect on their mental activities during the delay condition to consider if any thoughts may have influenced retention of the words. Participants were asked to rate on a 5-point Likert scale how much they had (i) thought about the learned material, (ii) imagined the learned material, and (iii) tried to remember the learned material. For each measure, 1 = not at all and 5 = constant [66].
Participants were then asked to report their level of physical exertion from three times points during the delay condition: 2, 12.5 and 25 minutes from commencing the physical activity. These time points broadly reflected the start, middle, and end of the physical activity delay condition. To do this, they rated their exertion using the 10-point RPE Scale [62], where 1 corresponds to no exertion at all and 10 refers to maximal exertion. They provided a rating for each of the three noted timepoints. Heart rate data were also collected during the physical activity delay condition from a subset of participants (n = 18) using their own devices, e.g., fitness watch. Following completion of the study session, participants were asked to report the mean absolute heart rate achieved during the delay condition to the researcher. These data were used as absolute values and also converted into relative heart rate maximum (%HR max) values to control for age [67]. In addition to heart rate values, participants reported their RPE scores.
Twenty-four hours after a study session, participants completed a 24-hour delayed recall test over the phone, where they were again asked to freely recall the items from the wordlist encoded one day earlier. To encourage the ecological validity of our methods and outcomes, we did not control participants activities (e.g., sleep, diet, or physical activity) during the interval between the 30-minute and 24-hour recall tests. Participants were free to go about their normal activities between testing sessions, which is not uncommon in paradigms investigating the effect of post-encoding activities on early consolidation [16, 68]. Like for the 30-minute test, participants were not informed in advance that they would complete a further free recall test for the wordlist, but they were aware that the researchers would follow up with them at this time. This was done to reduce the likelihood of mnemonic strategies between the 30-minute and 24-hour delayed recall tests though participants will have naturally expected a 30-minute and 24-hour recall test for the words encoded in study session two due to having already experienced the same protocol during the first study session.
Scoring
To examine the retention of wordlist materials, the total number of words recalled correctly was extracted for the (i) immediate, (ii) 30-minute, and (iii) 24-hour recall tests. To control for individual differences in immediate recall and between-condition variation, percentage retention scores were also computed for the 30-minute and 24-hour delayed recall tests to establish how many words recalled at the immediate stage were retained over the duration of these retention intervals [16, 25, 69]. These percentage retention scores were calculated by dividing the total number of words recalled correctly in the 30-minute and 24-hour tests by the total number of words recalled correctly in the immediate test. These values were then multiplied by 100 to provide a percentage score. In keeping with previous consolidation research [e.g., 22], scores were capped at 100%. If the participant recalled a word in the 30-minute or 24-hour test that was not recalled in a previous recall test, it was still recorded as correct. Mean heart rate data were collected from a subset of participants (n = 18) and converted to %HRmax scores [67] using the formula: %HRmax = (Heart Rate / Maximum Heart Rate) * 100, where Maximum Heart Rate was computed as 220 minus a participant’s age, e.g., 220–40 = 180 Maximum Heart Rate. Post-experimental ratings regarding thoughts about encoded materials were scored in keeping with published work [12], where raw scores were extracted for analyses, and this was also the case for the RPE Scale [62].
Statistical analyses
Analyses were performed using SPSS Statistics 28 (copyright IBM Corp., NY, USA), with the alpha level set to .05. Raw memory scores (i.e., number of words recalled) were analysed using a 3x2 repeated measures ANOVA that used within-subject factors time of test (3 levels: immediate vs. 30-minute vs. 24-hour) and exercise intensity condition (2 levels: higher vs. lower). Further to this, to control for individual differences in encoding and immediate recall, percentage retention scores for the 30-minute and 24-hour tests were analysed using a 2x2 ANOVA that used within subject factors time of test (2 levels: 30-minutes vs. 24-hour) and exercise intensity condition (2 levels: higher vs. lower). Paired t-tests (two tailed) were used to examine possible differences in raw and percentage retention scores between conditions or time points, for example, comparison of raw scores between conditions in the immediate, 30-minute, and 24-hour recall tests. Data from the post-experimental questionnaire (e.g., ratings surrounding the degree to which participants thought about the wordlists during the exercise delay conditions) were compared between conditions using a paired t-test (two tailed). Mean heart rate and %HR max scores were also compared between conditions using paired t-tests (two tailed). Finally, RPE scores were examined using a 3x2 repeated measures ANOVA that used within-subject factors time of test (3 levels: 2 minutes vs. 12.5 minutes vs. 25 minutes) and exercise intensity condition (2 levels: higher vs. lower), as well as paired t-tests to examine possible differences in RPE scores between conditions at the three time points. In all cases where multiple paired t-tests were conducted, Bonferroni-corrected alpha levels (p = .05 / number of within-family comparisons) were used to reduce the likelihood of Type I errors. All study data are available on the project OSF site at osf.io/xds49.
Results
Memory performance
Table 1 reports the mean number of words recalled in the immediate, 30-minute, and 24-hour recall tests for the higher intensity and lower intensity conditions. A 3x2 repeated measures ANOVA revealed significant main effects of exercise condition (F(1,34) = 8.977, p = .005, ηp2 = .209) and time of test (F(2,68) = 118.454, p < .001, ηp2 = .777), as well as a significant interaction between exercise condition and time of test (F(2,68) = 6.826, p = .002, ηp2 = .167). Paired t-tests (two-tailed) demonstrated that the number of words recalled in the immediate test did not differ significantly between the higher intensity and lower intensity exercise conditions (t(34) = 0.848, p = .402, d = 0.143), but the number of words recalled in the higher intensity condition was significantly greater when memory was probed in the 30-minute recall test (t(34) = 3.686, p < .001, d = 0.623) and 24-hour recall test (t(34) = 3.954, p < .001, d = 0.668). These significant findings survived Bonferroni-corrected alpha levels of p = .017 (p = .050 / 3 comparisons) that controlled for multiple within-family comparisons.
Table 1. Recall performance.
Test time | Lower intensity | Higher intensity | t-test (two tailed) |
---|---|---|---|
Immediate test | 8.06 (2.28) | 8.37 (1.65) | t(34) = 0.848, p = .402, d = 0.143 |
30-minute test | 5.66 (2.31) | 6.71 (1.82) | t(34) = 3.686, p < .001, d = 0.623 |
24-hour test | 4.97 (2.15) | 6.17 (1.89) | t(34) = 3.954, p < .001, d = 0.668 |
The mean number of words recalled in the immediate, 30-minute, and 24-hour recall tests are shown for the higher intensity and lower intensity conditions. Standard deviation values are reported in parentheses. Outcomes from paired t-tests (two-tailed) are also reported. The number of words recalled in the immediate recall test was comparable between conditions, but participants recalled significantly more words in the 30-minute and 24-hour recall tests in the higher intensity exercise condition.
Meanwhile, comparison of data between testing timepoints revealed that participants recalled significantly fewer words in the 30-minute recall test than immediate recall test (lower intensity: (t(34) = 8.806, p < .001, d = 1.488; higher intensity: t(34) = 8.261, p < .001, d = 1.396), and significantly fewer words in the 24-hour recall test than 30-minute recall test (lower intensity: t(34) = 4.680, p < .001, d = 0.791; higher intensity: t(34) = 4.117, p < .001, d = 0.696), i.e., forgetting of wordlist materials occurred between each of the testing points. These significant findings survived a Bonferroni-corrected alpha level of p = .013 (p = .050 / 4 comparisons) that controlled for multiple within-family comparisons.
Taken together, these outcomes demonstrate that participants experienced significant forgetting of wordlist material between testing time points in both conditions, but especially in the lower intensity exercise condition. This is reflected in Table 1, where a mean of 2.20 words were forgotten between the immediate recall test and 24-hour recall test in the higher intensity condition, whereas a mean of 3.09 words were forgotten between the immediate and 24-hour recall tests in the lower intensity condition.
Further to examining raw recall scores, percentage retention scores, which account for individual differences in the encoding of presented materials, were analysed. Percentage retention scores for the 30-minute and 24-hour recall tests are provided in Fig 2. A 2x2 repeated measures ANOVA revealed that wordlist retention was significantly greater in the higher intensity condition than in the lower intensity condition (F(1,34) = 14.763, p < .001, ηp2 = .303). A significant main effect of time was also observed because retention of words was poorer when memory was probed after 24 hours than 30 minutes (F(1,34) = 34.861, p < .001, ηp2 = .506), but no significant interaction was observed between delay condition and time of test (F(1,34) = 0.445, p = .509, ηp2 = .013). This indicates that, when controlling for individual differences in encoding, the rate of forgetting between the 30-minute and 24-hour recall tests was comparable in both conditions. Paired t-tests confirmed that percentage retention scores were significantly greater in the higher intensity than lower intensity condition in the 30-minute delayed recall test (t(34) = 3.641, p < .001, d = .615) and 24-hour delayed recall test (t(34) = 3.635, p < .001, d = .614). Similarly, they confirmed that there was a significant decline in percentage retention scores between the 30-minute and 24-hour recall tests in both the higher intensity (t(34) = 4.193, p < .001, d = .709) and lower intensity (t(34) = 4.842, p < .001, d = .818) exercise conditions. These significant findings survived Bonferroni-corrected alpha levels of p = .025 (p = .050 / 2 comparisons) to correct for multiple within-family comparisons.
Post-experimental questionnaire
Following completion of the 30-minute delayed recall test, participants were asked about their experiences during the exercise condition. Data from one participant was lost for this measure and thus the following analyses are reported from a sample of n = 34. Participants responses for this measure comprised rating, on a 5-point Likert scale, how often they (i) thought about (higher intensity: M = 1.41, SD = 0.74; lower intensity: M = 1.74, SD = 1.02; t(33) = -1.683, p = .102, d = -.289), (ii) imagined (higher intensity: M = 1.35, SD = 0.54; lower intensity: M = 1.62, SD = 0.99; t(33) = -1.358, p = .184, d = -.233), or (iii) tried to remember (higher intensity: M = 1.47, SD = 0.83; lower intensity: M = 1.56, SD = 0.99; t(33) = -0.415, p = .681, d = -.071) the wordlist materials, where higher scores reflect greater activity [12]. Scores across all three dimensions were comparable and relatively low, indicating that (i) participants did not actively engage in mnemonic strategies during the study and (ii) the level of mental activity surrounding the wordlists was comparable in the lower intensity and higher intensity exercise conditions.
Physical exertion
Heart rate
To quantify the level of physiological exertion experienced by participants, heart rate data were recorded from a subset of individuals (n = 18) throughout the higher intensity and lower intensity exercise delay conditions. A paired t-test confirmed that mean absolute heart rate values were significantly greater (t(17) = 18.118, p < .001, d = 4.270) in the higher intensity condition (M = 154.44, SD = 11.63) than in the lower intensity condition (M = 87.00; SD = 12.70). Similarly, when relative heart rate maximum (%HRmax) values were computed, participants demonstrated significantly greater %HRmax scores (t(17) = -19.766, p < .001, d = -4.659) in the higher intensity condition (M = 89.27, SD = 5.08) than the lower intensity condition (M = 50.39, SD = 7.75). Mean %HRmax values in the lower and higher intensity conditions aligned with published thresholds [70] for light intensity exercise (%HRmax score of 40 < 55) and vigorous intensity exercise (%HRmax score of 70 < 90), respectively. Both findings survived a Bonferroni-corrected alpha level of .025 (p = .050 / 2 comparisons) to correct for multiple within-family comparisons.
Pearson correlations provided no evidence for a relationship between mean absolute heart rate values and memory performance (both conditions p > .122). This was also true for %HRmax values and memory performance (both conditions p > .326).
Rate of Perceived Exertion (RPE)
To complement objective heart rate data, participants were asked to provide subjective reports of their RPE [62]. Table 2 shows mean scores for both conditions for measurements taken 2, 12.5, and 25 minutes after commencing the two exercise conditions. Higher intensity condition responses were indicative of moderate to vigorous physical exertion, whereas lower intensity condition responses were indicative of light physical exertion. These data, combined with the heart rate data reported earlier, indicate that participants exercised at the desired intensity in our two delay conditions.
Table 2. Rate of perceived exertion (RPE).
Measurement time | Condition | RPE score | Range | Descriptor |
---|---|---|---|---|
2 minutes | Higher intensity | 5.31 (1.71) | 2–8 | Moderate activity |
Lower intensity | 2.29 (1.49) | 1–8 | Light activity | |
12.5 minutes | Higher intensity | 6.54 (1.04) | 4–8 | Vigorous activity |
Lower intensity | 2.46 (1.38) | 1–7 | Light activity | |
25 minutes | Higher intensity | 7.77 (1.35) | 4–10 | Vigorous activity |
Lower intensity | 2.51 (1.34) | 1–7 | Light activity |
Mean RPE scores, standard deviations, and the range of scores for the higher intensity and lower intensity exercise conditions are shown for measurements collected 2, 12.5, and 25 minutes after commencing physical activity. Category descriptors for the mean scores are also shown.
A 3x2 repeated measures ANOVA revealed a significant main effect of exercise condition, where, expectedly, higher exertion scores were reported in the higher intensity condition (F(1,34) = 318.317, p < .001, ηp2 = .903). A significant main effect of time was also observed, where participants reported greater exertion as the physical activity progressed (F(2,68) = 26.341, p < .001, ηp2 = .437). The interaction between exercise condition and time was found to be significant (F(2,68) = 22.462, p < .001, ηp2 = .398) because a greater increase in exertion levels over time was observed in the higher intensity than lower intensity condition, where scores for the latter remained relative stable across the delay (see Table 2). Paired t-tests (two tailed) confirmed that the difference between conditions was significant at each time point (all p < .001). The increase in exertion levels over time in the higher intensity condition was significant between measurements taken at 2 minutes and 12.5 minutes (t(35) = -3.605, p < .001, d = -.609) and 12.5 minutes and 25 minutes (t(34) = -5.375, p < .001, d = -.909). In the lower intensity condition, no significant change in scores was observed between measurements taken at 2 minute and 12.5 minutes (t(34) = -1.974, p = .057, d = -.334) or those taken at 12.5 and 25 minutes (t(34) = -1.435, p = .160, d = -.243). Significant findings survived a Bonferroni-corrected alpha level of p = 0.125 (p = 0.05 / 4 comparisons) that controlled for multiple within-family comparisons.
Discussion
The purpose of the current study was to examine whether lower and higher intensity exercise completed in a naturalistic setting differentially affect the early consolidation of new declarative memories. To achieve this, we adapted an established consolidation paradigm used in laboratory research [10, 15, 16] to accommodate the normal exercise activities of adult endurance runners. Wordlist retention probed 30 minutes and 24 hours after encoding was significantly better when participants experienced higher intensity (interval running) exercise than lower intensity (walking) exercise. Our finding of superior memory following higher intensity exercise reinforces existing laboratory work [28, 32, 33, 35, 71] and, crucially, demonstrates that such effects translate to naturalistic settings. We consider possible explanations for our findings and the further work that is required for applied consequences to be achieved, for example, in educational environments and clinical populations.
Why did higher intensity exercise benefit the retention of new memories? Our repeated measures design allows us to rule out the contribution of random noise between participants that could affect an independent samples design. The reported effect is also unlikely to be explained by within-subject differences in participants’ mental state and mood between sessions: comparable alertness, arousal, and mood levels were reported prior to completing study sessions (see SI). Similarly, the effect in retention is unlikely to be explained by variations in the initial encoding of wordlist materials: participants received the same treatment during the encoding of both wordlists and immediate memory performance was matched across the two conditions. This is pertinent given that different wordlists from the RAVLT [63] were presented in our two study sessions, though current evidence highlights that the employed lists (A and B) are comparably memorable [e.g., 64]. Given that a difference in memory retention only emerged following the 30-minute retention interval, it is most likely that activities that occurred during the retention interval–where our experimental manipulation occurred–were responsible for the observed differences in delayed memory performance.
It is possible that the reported effect may simply be a direct result of mnemonic strategies (e.g., intentional rehearsal) during the 30-minute retention interval between the immediate and 30-minute recall tests. However, this appears unlikely. In keeping with existing consolidation research [10, 16, 69], we attempted to minimise the likelihood of participants engaging in mnemonic strategies by (i) embedding a cognitive task (lamppost counting) during both conditions and (ii) not informing participants that they would complete subsequent (30-minute and 24-hour) recall tests for the encoded materials. Of course, given that our sessions were not counterbalanced for practical purposes (to fit our study procedure around participants’ existing exercise training programmes), participants will have been aware that their memory for the wordlist will be tested at multiple time points in the second study session. If this knowledge affected our study outcomes, for example, because participants actively rehearsed materials in the second session, superior retention would be expected in the second (walk) than first (run) session. Conversely, participants demonstrated superior delayed memory for materials in the first session. This is reflected in our data, which demonstrate that participants reported thinking about, imagining, and remembering the wordlists to comparable level in the two conditions. Thus, differences in (conscious) mental activities during the delay conditions are unlikely to account for the reported effect in memory. Furthermore, by ensuring the duration of the conditions was equivalent, the most plausible explanation for the differences observed in memory retention is the experimental manipulation of exercise intensity.
We propose that the most likely explanation for our findings is that higher intensity exercise benefited memory retention, relative to lower intensity exercise, because it provided a state that was more conducive to the early consolidation of new wordlist memories. This consolidation hypothesis is in keeping with existing laboratory findings from work in humans and rodents [28, 32, 33, 35, 71]. At a neurobiological level, it is plausible that the higher intensity condition resulted in greater blood flow, including to brain regions implicated in memory functioning, namely the hippocampus and associated cortical networks [1, 3, 18]. Indeed, heart rate and RPE data demonstrated that participants (i) experienced a significantly greater level of exertion in the higher intensity condition, and (ii) the rate of exertion in the higher intensity condition and lower intensity condition corresponded to vigorous and light exertion, respectively, according to published thresholds [70].
It is therefore possible that participants experienced greater brain blood flow and oxygen availability in the higher intensity condition, which encouraged neurobiological mechanisms of consolidation. Indeed, even a brief period of moderate exercise can increase hippocampal blood flow [46, 47]. This may have also been coupled with increased concentrations of neurotransmitters including BNDF [48, 49] and the induction of LTP and synaptic plasticity [50], which are implicated in the success of consolidation [51–53]. It also remains possible that a physiological stress response, through activation of the Hypothalamic-Pituitary-Adrenal (HPA) axis and release of cortisol, facilitated consolidation in the higher intensity exercise condition and not the lower intensity exercise condition. This possibility resonates with (i) the well-documented U-shaped dose-response relationship between stress and cortisol in the hippocampus [42], where cortisol concentrations predicts consolidation functioning [72], and (ii) data from the current study showing that RPE and %HRmax scores in the higher intensity group corresponded to a state of vigorous exercise, which may have induced a stress response that was sufficient to encourage consolidation but not excessive to the point of inducing detrimental effects on memory [73]. It is, however, worth noting that our assessment of exercise intensity comes with some limitations. Heart rate data, which were our primary physiological indicator of exercise intensity, were only collected from a subset of participants, and not the full sample. Furthermore, these data were collected from participants’ own fitness devices, which could have influenced the accuracy of data and contributed to the between and within-subject variability. Still, when considered in its entirety and alongside RPE scores, our data do suggest that the two exercise conditions differed in their intensity level. Therefore, it is possible that a neurobiological explanation could, at least partly, explain the observed difference in memory retention between our higher intensity and lower intensity exercise conditions.
In addition to a neurophysiological explanation, we cannot rule out the possibility that–above and beyond providing a conducive neurophysiological state for consolidation–higher intensity exercise may have evoked a more efficient cognitive state of arousal and improved participants’ mood, which facilitated the recall of information. While less likely for the 24-hour recall test that was completed after a washout period, this possibility is pertinent to the 30-minute recall test, which occurred in close proximity to the exercise condition. Our data do resonate with this possibility: while significant improvements in alertness, mood, and arousal were observed following both exercise conditions, there improvements were greater when the exercise condition was higher in intensity (see S1 Appendix). It is well established that human cognitive processes, including learning and memory, can be influenced by affective states [74, 75]. Indeed, it is proposed that exercise has a modulating effect on memory and information is better remembered when paired with an emotional stimulus, such as high intensity exercise [76]. It is evident from our data that higher levels of alertness, arousal and a more positive mood state followed completion of the high-intensity physical activity condition. Thus, further work is required to understand the possible contribution of affective states to our findings and to exclude that the reported effect in memory retention can be explained by enhanced retrieval without enhancement of consolidation. Still, the latter appears less likely given that (i) the benefit of higher intensity exercise remained after 24 hours when improvements in alertness, mood, and arousal would have been expected to have dissipated by this point, and (ii) existing work demonstrates that acute, vigorous exercise is conducive to consolidation and memory retention when controlling for possible effects in retrieval, e.g., through probing memory after an extended delay [36].
We also cannot rule out the possibility that other phenomenon, for example, behavioural tagging [77], where the consolidation of malleable memory traces can be enhanced through post-encoding novelty [78–80] may have contributed to our findings. Such novelty is proposed to support consolidation through stimulating the release of neurotransmitters including dopamine and the encouragement of protein synthesis in the hippocampus [81]. Indeed, navigation of virtual environments has been shown to be sufficient to induce such novelty [78, 79] though findings are mixed [82]. In the current study, it is possible that higher intensity physical activity in a natural setting provided such novelty in the first condition, whereas walking in the same environment for the lower intensity condition two weeks later did not induce such novelty. While this remains possible, it is less likely given that participants were already highly familiar with the natural study environment because they train in this environment on a regular basis. Indeed, to encourage the ecological validity of our study, our measures were built around their typical training activities, including the interval run in the higher intensity condition. Still, further work is required to clarify the possible cognitive, affective, and neurophysiological bases of the reported effect in memory retention.
Irrespective of the mechanisms that underpin the observed effect, it is noteworthy that it was not transient but long lasting and detectable after at least 24 hours. This demonstrates that the effect of acute exercise in naturalistic settings is durable and can be observed even after an overnight period of sleep, which is in keeping with laboratory-based work [34, 35, 40]. Crucial to the aims of the current study, we observed this durable effect in memory retention even when our procedure was delivered in a naturalistic setting and when not controlling for participants’ activities (e.g., sleep, physical activity) in the interval between the 30-minute and 24-hour test. To promote ecological validity, our study procedure, which was based on published laboratory work [10, 15, 16], was moulded around participants normal exercise activities. It was for this reason that we recruited endurance runners for this initial investigation. We acknowledge that this does however limit the generalisability of our findings and further work is required to establish whether the characteristics of recruited individuals influenced outcomes. In particular, it would be valuable to identify whether the longer-term (24-hour) effect in the current study is observable in those who do not practice exercise regularly given that chronic exercise is proposed to encourage the molecular framework for consolidation to succeed [26]. Thus, it is possible that our sample of chronic exercisers already existed in a neurophysiological state that was conducive to consolidation.
Rigorous standardisation procedures were applied in the current study to reduce the likelihood of any potential contextual and extraneous differences that may have influenced the outcome of the study and/or introduced bias. The importance of mitigating bias when comparing exercise modes in terms setting and load is deemed important in exercise research [83]. Despite our efforts to address both of those elements, we set out to examine the effects of exercise on memory in a natural setting, to reflect a real-life scenario. Consequently, there was a higher risk of compromise from ecological factors, for example, differences in temperature and weather conditions, that are easier to address in a controlled laboratory-based setting. This includes likely inter-individual differences in heart rate data due to participants using their own devices [84, 85]. However, the specific aim of this study was to observe an effect of exercise in a natural setting because many people choose to carry out exercise in parks, fields, streets, towns, countryside, and coastlines and in all weather conditions. For these reasons, we chose to conduct the experiment in the field so we could be confident that the results would be more generalisable to a ‘real life’ scenario. Despite the scientific limitations that this approach brings, we achieved this aim.
Can our findings have applied consequences? While it is generally accepted that exercise has significant benefits for physical and mental well-being throughout the lifespan [58], to the best of our knowledge, our data provide the first field-based evidence that an acute bout of higher intensity exercise positively affects the retention of new declarative memories, relative to lower intensity exercise. As a result, it is possible that our findings may provide tentative evidence for interventions or lifestyle changes that abate forgetting. However, for this to be achievable, several questions must be addressed. For example, what are the precise physiological (and cognitive) mechanisms that underly the observed effect and what level of physical activity is sufficient–and optimal—to achieve the effect across individuals of varying abilities consistently? Exploring related questions in children and older adults with intact and comprised memory systems may also provide valuable insights. Children who practice regular aerobic activity have been found to perform better in verbal, perceptual and arithmetic tests [86, 87]. Similarly, contemporary evidence indicates that acute and chronic physical activity in older age is associated positively with episodic memory [88, 89]. If the reported effect is translatable across different settings and populations and the underlying neurophysiological mechanisms can be established, this could lead to novel targets for interventions and memory-enhancing recommendations that can be applied, for example, in education and care settings. Furthermore, given that findings–including our own–demonstrate that exercise not only benefits cognition but can have broader effects, for example, in arousal and mood, there may be scope for interventions in clinical populations, including those with depression who often report reduced mood and memory functioning [60, 90, 91]. If future work can characterise the criteria required to achieve a memory benefit of acute exercise in naturalistic settings, this can provide a significant stepping stone towards healthcare professionals’ ability to recommend (at least) short bouts of higher intensity exercise as a non-invasive memory-enhancing intervention.
Conclusions
Our data indicate that higher intensity exercise in the minutes immediately following new learning benefited the long-term retention of new verbal memories relative to a period of lower intensity exercise. Crucially, this finding was observed in the field and provides evidence that laboratory-based findings demonstrating that acute exercise benefits memory retention translates to naturalistic settings. We attribute this finding to a consolidation account, where higher intensity exercise is likely to have provided a physiological (and possibly cognitive) state that was more conducive to the early stabilisation and strengthening of new declarative memories, relative to lower intensity exercise. While further work is required to qualify the scope of this finding and its neurocognitive basis, it provides tentative evidence that exercise interventions can support the retention of new memories in naturalistic settings.
Supporting information
Acknowledgments
Thanks go to all participants who gave their time to take part in our research.
Data Availability
All relevant data for this study are publicly available from the OSF repository (https://osf.io/xds49/).
Funding Statement
The author(s) received no specific funding for this work.
References
- 1.Dudai Y. The neurobiology of consolidations, or, how stable is the engram? Annu Rev Psychol. 2004;55:51–86. Epub 2004/01/28. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.142050 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Wixted JT. The psychology and neuroscience of forgetting. Annu Rev Psychol. 2004;55:235–69. Epub 2004/01/28. doi: 10.1146/annurev.psych.55.090902.141555 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Squire LR, Genzel L, Wixted JT, Morris RG. Memory consolidation. Cold Spring Harb Perspect Biol. 2015;7(8):a021766. Epub 2015/08/05. doi: 10.1101/cshperspect.a021766 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4526749. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Dudai Y, Karni A, Born J. The Consolidation and Transformation of Memory. Neuron. 2015;88(1):20–32. Epub 2015/10/09. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2015.09.004 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Kandel ER, Dudai Y, Mayford MR. The molecular and systems biology of memory. Cell. 2014;157(1):163–86. Epub 2014/04/01. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2014.03.001 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Frankland PW, Bontempi B. The organization of recent and remote memories. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2005;6(2):119–30. doi: 10.1038/nrn1607 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Mednick SC, Cai DJ, Shuman T, Anagnostaras S, Wixted JT. An opportunistic theory of cellular and systems consolidation. Trends Neurosci. 2011;34(10):504–14. Epub 2011/07/12. doi: 10.1016/j.tins.2011.06.003 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3183157. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Hasselmo ME. Neuromodulation: acetylcholine and memory consolidation. Trends Cogn Sci. 1999;3(9):351–9. Epub 1999/08/26. doi: 10.1016/s1364-6613(99)01365-0 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Wamsley EJ, Summer T. Spontaneous Entry into an "Offline" State during Wakefulness: A Mechanism of Memory Consolidation? J Cogn Neurosci. 2020;32(9):1714–34. Epub 2020/06/13. doi: 10.1162/jocn_a_01587 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Craig M, Dewar M. Rest-related consolidation protects the fine detail of new memories. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):6857. Epub 2018/05/03. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-25313-y ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5931514. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Wamsley EJ, Tucker MA, Payne JD, Stickgold R. A brief nap is beneficial for human route-learning: The role of navigation experience and EEG spectral power. Learn Mem. 2010;17(7):332–6. Epub 2010/06/29. doi: 10.1101/lm.1828310 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2904102. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Brokaw K, Tishler W, Manceor S, Hamilton K, Gaulden A, Parr E, et al. Resting state EEG correlates of memory consolidation. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2016;130:17–25. Epub 2016/01/24. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2016.01.008 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Stickgold R. Sleep-dependent memory consolidation. Nature. 2005;437(7063):1272–8. Epub 2005/10/28. doi: 10.1038/nature04286 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Lahl O, Wispel C, Willigens B, Pietrowsky R. An ultra short episode of sleep is sufficient to promote declarative memory performance. J Sleep Res. 2008;17(1):3–10. Epub 2008/02/16. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2869.2008.00622.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Craig M, Dewar M, Harris MA, Della Sala S, Wolbers T. Wakeful rest promotes the integration of spatial memories into accurate cognitive maps. Hippocampus. 2016;26(2):185–93. Epub 2015/08/04. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22502 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16.Dewar M, Alber J, Butler C, Cowan N, Della Sala S. Brief wakeful resting boosts new memories over the long term. Psychol Sci. 2012;23(9):955–60. Epub 2012/07/26. doi: 10.1177/0956797612441220 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Tambini A, Ketz N, Davachi L. Enhanced brain correlations during rest are related to memory for recent experiences. Neuron. 2010;65(2):280–90. Epub 2010/02/16. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2010.01.001 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3287976. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Carr MF, Jadhav SP, Frank LM. Hippocampal replay in the awake state: a potential substrate for memory consolidation and retrieval. Nat Neurosci. 2011;14(2):147–53. Epub 2011/01/29. doi: 10.1038/nn.2732 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3215304. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Murphy M, Stickgold R, Parr ME, Callahan C, Wamsley EJ. Recurrence of task-related electroencephalographic activity during post-training quiet rest and sleep. Sci Rep. 2018;8(1):5398. Epub 2018/03/31. doi: 10.1038/s41598-018-23590-1 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5876367. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Poskanzer C, Denis D, Herrick A, Stickgold R. Using EEG Microstates to Examine Post-Encoding Quiet Rest and Subsequent Word-Pair Memory. bioRxiv. 2021:2020.05.08.085027. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2021.107424 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Ego-Stengel V, Wilson MA. Disruption of ripple-associated hippocampal activity during rest impairs spatial learning in the rat. Hippocampus. 2010;20(1):1–10. Epub 2009/10/10. doi: 10.1002/hipo.20707 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2801761. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Dewar M, Alber J, Cowan N, Della Sala S. Boosting long-term memory via wakeful rest: intentional rehearsal is not necessary, consolidation is sufficient. PLoS One. 2014;9(10):e109542. Epub 2014/10/22. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0109542 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4198139. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23.Alber J, Della Sala S, Dewar M. Minimizing interference with early consolidation boosts 7-day retention in amnesic patients. Neuropsychology. 2014;28(5):667–75. Epub 2014/05/14. doi: 10.1037/neu0000091 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Dewar M, Garcia YF, Cowan N, Della Sala S. Delaying interference enhances memory consolidation in amnesic patients. Neuropsychology. 2009;23(5):627–34. Epub 2009/08/26. doi: 10.1037/a0015568 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC2808210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Dewar M, Pesallaccia M, Cowan N, Provinciali L, Della Sala S. Insights into spared memory capacity in amnestic MCI and Alzheimer’s Disease via minimal interference. Brain Cogn. 2012;78(3):189–99. Epub 2012/01/21. doi: 10.1016/j.bandc.2011.12.005 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Roig M, Nordbrandt S, Geertsen SS, Nielsen JB. The effects of cardiovascular exercise on human memory: a review with meta-analysis. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2013;37(8):1645–66. Epub 2013/06/29. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2013.06.012 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Dal Maso F, Desormeau B, Boudrias MH, Roig M. Acute cardiovascular exercise promotes functional changes in cortico-motor networks during the early stages of motor memory consolidation. Neuroimage. 2018;174:380–92. Epub 2018/03/21. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroimage.2018.03.029 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Delancey D, Frith E, Sng E, Loprinzi PD. Randomized Controlled Trial Examining the Long-Term Memory Effects of Acute Exercise During the Memory Consolidation Stage of Memory Formation. Journal of Cognitive Enhancement. 2019;3(3):245–50. doi: 10.1007/s41465-018-0106-z [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Loprinzi PD, Edwards MK, Frith E. Potential avenues for exercise to activate episodic memory-related pathways: a narrative review. Eur J Neurosci. 2017;46(5):2067–77. Epub 2017/07/13. doi: 10.1111/ejn.13644 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Loprinzi PD, Roig M, Tomporowski PD, Javadi AH, Kelemen WL. Effects of acute exercise on memory: Considerations of exercise intensity, post-exercise recovery period and aerobic endurance. Mem Cognit. 2023;51(4):1011–26. Epub 2022/11/19. doi: 10.3758/s13421-022-01373-4 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC9676734. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.van Dongen EV, Kersten IHP, Wagner IC, Morris RGM, Fernández G. Physical Exercise Performed Four Hours after Learning Improves Memory Retention and Increases Hippocampal Pattern Similarity during Retrieval. Curr Biol. 2016;26(13):1722–7. Epub 2016/06/21. doi: 10.1016/j.cub.2016.04.071 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Most SB, Kennedy BL, Petras EA. Evidence for improved memory from 5 minutes of immediate, post-encoding exercise among women. Cogn Res Princ Implic. 2017;2(1):33. Epub 2017/09/12. doi: 10.1186/s41235-017-0068-1 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5569643. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Jentsch VL, Wolf OT. Acute physical exercise promotes the consolidation of emotional material. Neurobiol Learn Mem. 2020;173:107252. Epub 2020/05/23. doi: 10.1016/j.nlm.2020.107252 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34.Loprinzi PD, Day S, Hendry R, Hoffman S, Love A, Marable S, et al. The Effects of Acute Exercise on Short- and Long-Term Memory: Considerations for the Timing of Exercise and Phases of Memory. Eur J Psychol. 2021;17(1):85–103. Epub 2021/03/20. doi: 10.5964/ejop.2955 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7957845. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35.Labban JD, Etnier JL. The Effect of Acute Exercise on Encoding and Consolidation of Long-Term Memory. J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2018;40(6):336–42. Epub 2018/12/14. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2018-0072 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36.Loprinzi PD, Blough J, Crawford L, Ryu S, Zou L, Li H. The Temporal Effects of Acute Exercise on Episodic Memory Function: Systematic Review with Meta-Analysis. Brain Sci. 2019;9(4). Epub 2019/04/21. doi: 10.3390/brainsci9040087 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6523402. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37.Loprinzi PD, Chism M, Marable S. Does Engaging in Acute Exercise Prior to Memory Encoding and During Memory Consolidation have an Additive Effect on Long-Term Memory Function? Journal of Science in Sport and Exercise. 2020;2(1):77–81. doi: 10.1007/s42978-019-00040-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 38.Jaffery A, Edwards MK, Loprinzi PD. The Effects of Acute Exercise on Cognitive Function: Solomon Experimental Design. J Prim Prev. 2018;39(1):37–46. Epub 2018/01/07. doi: 10.1007/s10935-017-0498-z . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39.Crush EA, Loprinzi PD. Dose-Response Effects of Exercise Duration and Recovery on Cognitive Functioning. Percept Mot Skills. 2017;124(6):1164–93. Epub 2017/08/23. doi: 10.1177/0031512517726920 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 40.Etnier JL, Vance JC, Ueno A. Effects of acute exercise on memory performance in middle-aged and older adults. Journal of Aging and Physical Activity. 2021;29(5):753–60. [Google Scholar]
- 41.Loprinzi PD. Intensity-specific effects of acute exercise on human memory function: considerations for the timing of exercise and the type of memory. Health Promot Perspect. 2018;8(4):255–62. Epub 2018/11/28. doi: 10.15171/hpp.2018.36 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 42.Diamond DM, Bennett MC, Fleshner M, Rose GM. Inverted‐U relationship between the level of peripheral corticosterone and the magnitude of hippocampal primed burst potentiation. Hippocampus. 1992;2(4):421–30. doi: 10.1002/hipo.450020409 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 43.Craig M, Ottaway G, Dewar M. Rest on it: Awake quiescence facilitates insight. Cortex. 2018;109:205–14. Epub 2018/11/06. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2018.09.009 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 44.Moore D, Loprinzi PD. Exercise influences episodic memory via changes in hippocampal neurocircuitry and long-term potentiation. Eur J Neurosci. 2021;54(8):6960–71. Epub 2020/04/03. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14728 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 45.Mulser L, Moreau D. Effect of acute cardiovascular exercise on cerebral blood flow: A systematic review. Brain Research. 2023;1809:148355. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2023.148355 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46.Palmer JA, Morris JK, Billinger SA, Lepping RJ, Martin L, Green Z, et al. Hippocampal blood flow rapidly and preferentially increases after a bout of moderate-intensity exercise in older adults with poor cerebrovascular health. Cerebral Cortex. 2023;33(9):5297–306. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhac418 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 47.Steventon JJ, Foster C, Furby H, Helme D, Wise RG, Murphy K. Hippocampal Blood Flow Is Increased After 20 min of Moderate-Intensity Exercise. Cerebral Cortex. 2019;30(2):525–33. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhz104 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 48.Hillman CH, Erickson KI, Kramer AF. Be smart, exercise your heart: exercise effects on brain and cognition. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2008;9(1):58–65. Epub 2007/12/21. doi: 10.1038/nrn2298 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 49.Szuhany KL, Bugatti M, Otto MW. A meta-analytic review of the effects of exercise on brain-derived neurotrophic factor. J Psychiatr Res. 2015;60:56–64. Epub 2014/12/03. doi: 10.1016/j.jpsychires.2014.10.003 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4314337. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50.Kovalchuk Y, Hanse E, Kafitz KW, Konnerth A. Postsynaptic Induction of BDNF-Mediated Long-Term Potentiation. Science. 2002;295(5560):1729–34. Epub 2002/03/02. doi: 10.1126/science.1067766 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 51.Vedovelli K, Giacobbo BL, Corrêa MS, Wieck A, Argimon IIL, Bromberg E. Multimodal physical activity increases brain-derived neurotrophic factor levels and improves cognition in institutionalized older women. Geroscience. 2017;39(4):407–17. Epub 2017/07/15. doi: 10.1007/s11357-017-9987-5 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5636777. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52.Wheeler MJ, Green DJ, Ellis KA, Cerin E, Heinonen I, Naylor LH, et al. Distinct effects of acute exercise and breaks in sitting on working memory and executive function in older adults: a three-arm, randomised cross-over trial to evaluate the effects of exercise with and without breaks in sitting on cognition. Br J Sports Med. 2020;54(13):776–81. Epub 2019/05/01. doi: 10.1136/bjsports-2018-100168 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53.Bathina S, Das UN. Brain-derived neurotrophic factor and its clinical implications. Arch Med Sci. 2015;11(6):1164–78. Epub 2016/01/21. doi: 10.5114/aoms.2015.56342 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4697050. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54.Marin Bosch B, Bringard A, Logrieco MG, Lauer E, Imobersteg N, Thomas A, et al. A single session of moderate intensity exercise influences memory, endocannabinoids and brain derived neurotrophic factor levels in men. Scientific Reports. 2021;11(1):14371. doi: 10.1038/s41598-021-93813-5 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 55.Tsai C-L, Pan C-Y, Tseng C-T, Chen F-C, Chang Y-C, Wang T-C. Acute effects of high-intensity interval training and moderate-intensity continuous exercise on BDNF and irisin levels and neurocognitive performance in late middle-aged and older adults. Behavioural Brain Research. 2021;413:113472. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2021.113472 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 56.Griffin ÉW, Mullally S, Foley C, Warmington SA, O’Mara SM, Kelly ÁM. Aerobic exercise improves hippocampal function and increases BDNF in the serum of young adult males. Physiology & Behavior. 2011;104(5):934–41. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2011.06.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 57.Cefis M, Prigent-Tessier A, Quirié A, Pernet N, Marie C, Garnier P. The effect of exercise on memory and BDNF signaling is dependent on intensity. Brain Structure and Function. 2019;224(6):1975–85. doi: 10.1007/s00429-019-01889-7 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58.Callaghan P. Exercise: a neglected intervention in mental health care? J Psychiatr Ment Health Nurs. 2004;11(4):476–83. Epub 2004/07/17. doi: 10.1111/j.1365-2850.2004.00751.x . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 59.Basso JC, Suzuki WA. The Effects of Acute Exercise on Mood, Cognition, Neurophysiology, and Neurochemical Pathways: A Review. Brain Plast. 2017;2(2):127–52. Epub 2017/03/28. doi: 10.3233/bpl-160040 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5928534. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 60.Loprinzi PD, Frith E, Edwards MK, Sng E, Ashpole N. The Effects of Exercise on Memory Function Among Young to Middle-Aged Adults: Systematic Review and Recommendations for Future Research. Am J Health Promot. 2018;32(3):691–704. Epub 2017/11/08. doi: 10.1177/0890117117737409 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61.Faul F, Erdfelder E., Lang A.-G., & Buchner A. G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods. 2007;39:175–91. doi: 10.3758/bf03193146 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 62.Borg GA. Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 1982; 14:377–81. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 63.Schmidt M. Rey auditory verbal learning test: A handbook: Los Angeles, CA: Western Psychological Services.; 1964. [Google Scholar]
- 64.Rahimi-Golkhandan S, Maruff P, Darby D, Wilson P. Barriers to Repeated Assessment of Verbal Learning and Memory: A Comparison of International Shopping List Task and Rey Auditory Verbal Learning Test on Build-Up of Proactive Interference. Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology. 2012;27(7):790–5. doi: 10.1093/arclin/acs074 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65.Marchant D, Hampson S, Finnigan L, Marrin K, Thorley C. The Effects of Acute Moderate and High Intensity Exercise on Memory. Front Psychol. 2020;11:1716. Epub 2020/08/09. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2020.01716 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7381212. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66.Humiston GB, Tucker MA, Summer T, Wamsley EJ. Resting States and Memory Consolidation: A Preregistered Replication and Meta-Analysis. Sci Rep. 2019;9(1):19345. Epub 2019/12/20. doi: 10.1038/s41598-019-56033-6 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6920145. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67.Publication manual of the American Psychological Association (7th ed.) 2020. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68.Craig M, Dewar M, Della Sala S, Wolbers T. Rest boosts the long-term retention of spatial associative and temporal order information. Hippocampus. 2015;25(9):1017–27. Epub 2015/01/27. doi: 10.1002/hipo.22424 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69.Craig M, Della Sala S, Dewar M. Autobiographical thinking interferes with episodic memory consolidation. PLoS One. 2014;9(4):e93915. Epub 2014/04/17. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0093915 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3988030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 70.Norton K, Norton L, Sadgrove D. Position statement on physical activity and exercise intensity terminology. Journal of science and medicine in sport. 2010;13(5):496–502. doi: 10.1016/j.jsams.2009.09.008 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71.Etnier JL, Wideman L, Labban JD, Piepmeier AT, Pendleton DM, Dvorak KK, et al. The Effects of Acute Exercise on Memory and Brain-Derived Neurotrophic Factor (BDNF). J Sport Exerc Psychol. 2016;38(4):331–40. Epub 2016/07/08. doi: 10.1123/jsep.2015-0335 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72.Sandi C, Loscertales M, Guaza C. Experience‐dependent facilitating effect of corticosterone on spatial memory formation in the water maze. European journal of neuroscience. 1997;9(4):637–42. doi: 10.1111/j.1460-9568.1997.tb01412.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73.Kim JJ, Diamond DM. The stressed hippocampus, synaptic plasticity and lost memories. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2002;3(6):453–62. doi: 10.1038/nrn849 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74.Tyng CM, Amin HU, Saad MNM, Malik AS. The Influences of Emotion on Learning and Memory. Front Psychol. 2017;8:1454. Epub 2017/09/09. doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01454 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC5573739. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 75.Vuilleumier P. How brains beware: neural mechanisms of emotional attention. Trends Cogn Sci. 2005;9(12):585–94. Epub 2005/11/18. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2005.10.011 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76.McGaugh JL. Make mild moments memorable: add a little arousal. Trends Cogn Sci. 2006;10(8):345–7. Epub 2006/06/24. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2006.06.001 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77.Moncada D, Ballarini F, Viola H. Behavioral Tagging: A Translation of the Synaptic Tagging and Capture Hypothesis. Neural Plast. 2015;2015:650780. Epub 2015/09/18. doi: 10.1155/2015/650780 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4562088. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78.Baumann V, Birnbaum T, Breitling-Ziegler C, Tegelbeckers J, Dambacher J, Edelmann E, et al. Exploration of a novel virtual environment improves memory consolidation in ADHD. Scientific reports. 2020;10(1):21453. doi: 10.1038/s41598-020-78222-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79.Clemenson GD, Stark CE. Virtual Environmental Enrichment through Video Games Improves Hippocampal-Associated Memory. J Neurosci. 2015;35(49):16116–25. Epub 2015/12/15. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.2580-15.2015 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4682779. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 80.Clemenson GD, Deng W, Gage FH. Environmental enrichment and neurogenesis: from mice to humans. Current Opinion in Behavioral Sciences. 2015;4:56–62. [Google Scholar]
- 81.Okuda K, Højgaard K, Privitera L, Bayraktar G, Takeuchi T. Initial memory consolidation and the synaptic tagging and capture hypothesis. Eur J Neurosci. 2021;54(8):6826–49. Epub 2020/07/11. doi: 10.1111/ejn.14902 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 82.Quent JA, Henson RN. Novel immersive virtual reality experiences do not produce retroactive memory benefits for unrelated material. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology. 2022;75(12):2197–210. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 83.Hecksteden A, Faude O, Meyer T, Donath L. How to Construct, Conduct and Analyze an Exercise Training Study? Front Physiol. 2018;9:1007. Epub 2018/08/25. doi: 10.3389/fphys.2018.01007 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC6094975. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84.Fuller D, Colwell E, Low J, Orychock K, Tobin MA, Simango B, et al. Reliability and Validity of Commercially Available Wearable Devices for Measuring Steps, Energy Expenditure, and Heart Rate: Systematic Review. JMIR Mhealth Uhealth. 2020;8(9):e18694. Epub 2020/09/09. doi: 10.2196/18694 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC7509623. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85.Gillinov S, Etiwy M, Wang R, Blackburn G, Phelan D, Gillinov AM, et al. Variable Accuracy of Wearable Heart Rate Monitors during Aerobic Exercise. Med Sci Sports Exerc. 2017;49(8):1697–703. Epub 2017/07/15. doi: 10.1249/MSS.0000000000001284 . [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86.Voss MW, Chaddock L, Kim JS, Vanpatter M, Pontifex MB, Raine LB, et al. Aerobic fitness is associated with greater efficiency of the network underlying cognitive control in preadolescent children. Neuroscience. 2011;199:166–76. Epub 2011/10/27. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2011.10.009 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC3237764. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87.Sibney B, Etnier J. The Relationship between Physical Activity and Cognition in Children: A Meta-Analysis. Pediatric Exercise Science. 2003;15(3):243–56. doi: 10.1515/ijsl.2000.143.183 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 88.Hayes SM, Alosco ML, Hayes JP, Cadden M, Peterson KM, Allsup K, et al. Physical Activity Is Positively Associated with Episodic Memory in Aging. J Int Neuropsychol Soc. 2015;21(10):780–90. Epub 2015/11/20. doi: 10.1017/S1355617715000910 ; PubMed Central PMCID: PMC4711930. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 89.Tomoto T, Verma A, Kostroske K, Tarumi T, Patel NR, Pasha EP, et al. One-year aerobic exercise increases cerebral blood flow in cognitively normal older adults. Journal of Cerebral Blood Flow & Metabolism. 2023;43(3):404–18. doi: 10.1177/0271678x221133861 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90.James TA, Weiss-Cowie S, Hopton Z, Verhaeghen P, Dotson VM, Duarte A. Depression and episodic memory across the adult lifespan: A meta-analytic review. Psychol Bull. 2021;147(11):1184–214. Epub 2022/03/04. doi: 10.1037/bul0000344 . [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91.Rottenberg J. Mood and emotion in major depression. Current Directions in Psychological Science. 2005;14(3):167–70. [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
All relevant data for this study are publicly available from the OSF repository (https://osf.io/xds49/).