Abstract
Play is an essential part of childhood, and growing attention has focused on the potential health benefits of ‘risky’ or ‘thrill-seeking’ play. Such play behavior is readily observed on any playground, where it can sometimes lead to injuries––most often from fall impacts––that require medical attention. Monkey bars account for ~7% of childhood arm fractures in the USA, an alarming statistic that raises difficult questions over its costs and benefits. Many authors view monkey bars as a public health hazard, but it is plausible that our childhood impulse toward thrill-seeking play is a result of selective pressures throughout our primate evolutionary history. Indeed, emerging evidence suggests that the developmental benefits of thrill-seeking play extend into adulthood, outweighing the occasional costs of injury. Disparate and consequential, these dueling perspectives have fueled debate among health professionals and policymakers, but with little attention to the work of biological anthropologists. Here we call attention to the hominin fossil record and play behaviors of non-human primates, providing a novel perspective that bolsters arguments for the adaptive significance of thrill-seeking play. The moment for such a review is timely, for it commemorates the centennial anniversaries of two playground icons: the jungle gym and monkey bars.
Keywords: risky play, playground equipment, pediatric bone fracture, anxiety
INTRODUCTION
Hazards and risks are similar but distinct concepts. Hazards are potential sources of harm, whereas risks reflect the probability of harm [1]. It follows that risk-taking requires agency––a conscious decision to engage in activities with uncertain outcomes [2, 3]. Because its calculated nature demands physical and emotional management, two essential life skills [3], many developmental psychologists view risky play as integral to child growth and well-being, as children typically develop risk-management skills through such experiences [3]. However, the subtle distinction between hazard and risk is often blurred on modern playgrounds. Childhood injuries from ‘risky’, ‘adventurous’, or ‘thrill-seeking’ play in these settings has fueled parental anxiety and governmental regulation [4, 5]. It is plausible that risk mitigation efforts, although well-intentioned, have inadvertently harmed children’s psychological and physical development [6–8]. This debate over the costs and benefits of thrill-seeking play has far-reaching importance for child well-being and public health policy, but it is seldom addressed through the lens of human evolutionary biology, a discipline that is well-positioned to contextualize thrill-seeking play within a broader understanding of primate motor development.
Public perceptions of playground hazards are rooted in some alarming statistics. Monkey bars [Box 1], for instance, are responsible for more hospital visits and fractures during childhood than any other playground structure [9–12]. Most fractures are the result of fall impacts [13, 14], with heights of 2 m having an odds of fracture eleven times greater than those <1 m [15]. In Canada, monkey bars accounted for 5% of emergency department (ED) visits by children, 64% of which entailed bone fracture [10]. Another study found that monkey bars accounted for 50% of playground-related extremity fractures admitted to EDs in the USA, and 55% of severe extremity fractures [12]. This same study found strong cohort differences, with the greatest incidence of fracture occurring among 5–9 year-olds. Fractures of the wrist or forearm are most common (Fig. 1) [16], accounting for 6–21% of all bone fractures during childhood [17–19]. Rates of bone fracture are comparable between boys and girls [9], but a statistical difference emerges with monkey bars––girls are moderately more likely to suffer a fracture [9] despite similar levels of thrill-seeking play at ‘great heights’ [20]. Such sobering statistics prompted calls to reduce the height of monkey bars to <2 m, while also requiring deeper (~20–30 cm) and/or more compliant surface materials such as sand, bark chips or rubber [21–24].
Box 1: Evolution and the monkey bars.
Original jungle gym at the Winnetka Historical Society, Winnetka, Illinois. Photograph by E. Jason Wambsgans. © Chicago Tribune/Tribune News Service, reproduced with permission.
The jungle gym was invented in 1920 by Sebastian (‘Ted’) Hinton, an attorney [116]. In a series of four patent applications––the first of which was awarded October 23, 1923––Hinton [117] described a metal climbing frame of layered cubes, eight feet tall, arranged like a ‘forest top through which a troop of children may play in a manner somewhat similar to that of a troop of monkeys through the treetops in a jungle’ (p. 1). His final application, approved in 1924, included an accessory monkey runway, which he described as an elevated horizontal ladder attached to the side of the jungle gym and suspended at the corners by vertical poles [118]. He argued that such a runway would ‘permit suspensory exercise, such as swinging by the arms and hanging travel, hand over hand’ (p. 2). Thus, Hinton’s climbing frame (today’s jungle gym) and monkey runway (today’s monkey bars) were envisioned as conjoined structures. Here, we view them interchangeable forms of playground equipment because both invite climbing, putting them into the same category of risky play––danger or injury from falling [3]. Most studies consider them together when reporting injuries [14].
Hinton lived in Winnetka, Illinois, a Chicago suburb and epicenter for the rise and spread of progressive education [116]. This factor was a major contributor to the success of his inventions. Winnetka school administrators, chief among them superintendent Carleton Washburn, were disciples of John Dewey, a prominent philosopher and proponent of ‘whole pupil’ education [119]. Dewey advocated for curricula that balanced formal academics with practical activities meant to promote the mental, physical, and spiritual development of children. Physical education was a cornerstone of his pedagogy [120], as ‘the proper development of the mind depends on the proper use of the muscles and senses’ (p. 7). He viewed risky play as essential to learning: “the activities of a child are not so aimless as they seem to adults, but are the means by which [they become] acquainted with [their] world and by which [they learn] the use and limits of [their] own powers (p. 8). Thus, innovative playgrounds appealed to Washburn, who approved the first installation of a jungle gym at the North Shore Country Day School in 1920 [116]. Hinton continued to innovate his design until his death in 1923. A decade later, his wife, Carmelita Chase Hinton, another Dewey acolyte and progressive educator, founded The Putney School in Putney, Vermont.
Hinton’s use of evolutionary reasoning––an apparent first in US patent history––is an underappreciated aspect of his legacy. In 1923 [117], he argued that ‘climbing is the natural mode of locomotion which the evolutionary predecessors of the human race were designed to practice’, before concluding that it is ‘ideally suited for children’ (p. 1). These words predate the announcement, in 1924, of the Taung skull, the first-known specimen of Australopithecus, a hominin genus with unequivocal adaptations for climbing [121, 122]. They also came at a fraught time in American cultural history, preceding the infamous Scopes ‘Monkey’ Trial of 1925. Thus, Hinton wrote with considerable conviction and courage, but he never lived to see the prescience of his words. A century on, his views on the importance of climbing during human evolution are essentially unassailable, bolstered by decades of field research in paleoanthropology and primatology.
In light of these injuries, the US Consumer Product Safety Commission published a Public Playground Safety Handbook [25, 26] with height guidelines for monkey bars (described as horizontal overhead ladders). Devoid of anthropometric or kinematic data, the handbook leans heavily on the word ‘hazard’ to recommend maximum inter-rung distances of 12 inches (30.5 cm) and 15 inches (38.1 cm), with maximum heights of 60 inches (152 cm) and 84 inches (213 cm), for preschool- and school-aged children, respectively. These guidelines are not requirements, but seven states have codified them into law [4]. Compliance with these parameters has proven difficult, leading many municipalities and schools to remove monkey bars from their playgrounds. For example, the New York City Department of Parks and Recreation removed monkey bars from a majority of its 862 playgrounds during the 1980s and 90s [27] (Fig. 2). Still, a recent survey of 49 playgrounds in New Jersey, USA found that 100% of monkey bars exceeded 1.5 m and 37% exceeded 2 m in height [28]. At one primary school, the vertical height exceeded 2.5 m, which begs the question: at what height do monkey bars become a hazard?
Many public health professionals view monkey bars as unacceptably risky [29], a perspective with scant evidence. Death on playgrounds is essentially nil, with risk calculated at 0.15 per 100 000 children [16] or 1 in 30 million [30]. In other words, driving toward a playground carries a greater risk of childhood death than falls from monkey bars [30, 31]. Parents cite injury concerns for limiting thrill-seeking play on playgrounds, but such risk is low––calculated between 0.26 and 0.59 injuries per 100 000 uses [32]––and dwarfed by organized sports or gym class as causes of hospitalizations [33, 34]. Though painful, injuries resulting from falls from monkey bars rarely cause permanent harm [35]. Most playground injuries are low severity; nearly 95% of ED visitors are treated and released without further hospitalization [36]. Buckle and greenstick fractures of the distal radius or ulna can be immobilized with removable splints, wraps, or soft casts for as little as three weeks (Fig. 1a) [37]. For children under 10 years of age, some displacement (50%) in complete fractures is acceptable without referral to an orthopedist [37]. Yet, even with severe injuries, children are remarkably resilient. A recent study of pediatric admissions found that 88% and 92% of children hospitalized for severe injuries returned to baseline quality of life at 4 and 12 months post-injury, respectively [38].
Such evidence instead points to monkey bars as posing a modest risk. Yet, societal and parental tolerance of risk generally––and monkey bars more specifically––is in rapid decline across Western cultures [39]. Wyver et al. [30] pointed to a shift in views toward child safety––from the community (How can we keep our kids safe?) to the individual (How can I keep my kids safe?)––to explain how the subject transformed into a modern moral imperative [7, 40]. This shift in the focus of responsibility coincides with the emergence of intensive parenting––defined as over-extended, child-centered devotion of time, money and energy [41]––and the rise of ‘surplus safety’ environments [30], as exemplified by the proliferation of ‘ultra-safe’ playgrounds during the 1990s and the ensuing ‘bubble wrap’ generation [42]. Parents who ignore efforts to maximize child safety may be viewed as negligent, amplifying pressure to adhere to surplus safety, even if their own lived experiences speak to the benefits of thrill-seeking play [43]. With the weight of public consciousness so focused on the physical and reputational costs of playground injuries, we rarely consider the potential benefits, particularly from the evolutionary perspectives of biological anthropologists.
PSYCHOLOGICAL BENEFITS OF THRILL-SEEKING PLAY
On shorter timescales, thrill-seeking play may actually mitigate childhood injuries by honing risk-perception skills [44–47]. Children as young as four have an awareness of their physical abilities and will express caution on playground equipment beyond their capabilities [2]. This argument raises the possibility that ultra-safe playgrounds are detrimental to the development of accurate risk assessment, but it is a challenging concept to test. Related to this idea is the paradoxical notion that ultra-safe playgrounds might even promote excessive risk-taking, or ‘risk compensation’. Examples include the inappropriate use of safety equipment [48, 49] and the increased allure of higher-risk settings for play behaviors (e.g. train tracks, roads, etc.) [47]. Complementing this idea is the argument that intensive parenting diminishes risk perception via negative feedback during routine supervision (e.g. ‘slow down’, ‘not so high’, and ‘be careful’), words that could instill doubt in a child’s innate judgment [46].
On longer timescales, thrill-seeking play may promote children’s mental health by enhancing self-confidence, improving coping skills and promoting resilience [50]. In their seminal article, Sandseter and Kennair [3] described the evolutionary paradox inherent in thrill-seeking play––navigating monkey bars may cause injury, but low-risk thrills also inure children to maladaptive fear levels. In other words, thrill-seeking play strengthens psychological coping mechanisms––such as diminishing an innate fear of heights––that is essential for child development. Dodd and Lester [51] extended this model by folding in a discussion of anxiety, arguing for a developmental mismatch between a child’s innate proclivity for risk-taking and the rise of ultra-safe and intensively-parented play spaces, factors that may have contributed to parallel increases in youth anxiety.
Supporting this inferred causation, Dodd et al. [52] used data from two online surveys of Canadian parents to demonstrate an association between thrill-seeking play and positive behavioral outcomes during the coronavirus disease 2019 pandemic. Specifically, they found that greater time spent in thrill-seeking play was associated with a reduction in internalizing problems (e.g. fears, worries, nervousness, poor relationships with peers), and greater positive affect, suggesting that thrill-seeking play mitigated some of the uncertainty caused by pandemic-era lockdowns. Another study found that children of self-reported ‘challenge parents’––parents who encourage thrill-seeking play by their children––experienced fewer anxiety symptoms [53]. This finding may explain why overprotective ‘helicopter’ parenting is linked with diminished coping skills, perfectionism and narcissism in young adults [54, 55], as well as reductions in internal locus of control [56]. The latter result has far-reaching importance; individuals with a strong internal locus of control, who believe that their own efforts matter more in the direction of their lives than external events [57], have better mental health outcomes [56].
PHYSICAL BENEFITS OF THRILL-SEEKING PLAY
Thrill-seeking play is a frequent source of joy [45], and it is positively correlated with higher physical activity levels [58, 59]. It is practiced by children regardless of gender, although some nuance in the type of thrill-seeking play may exist [20]. In a randomized control study of 5–7 year-olds, Engelen et al. [60] found that riskier playground environments led to a 12% increase in moderate-to-vigorous physical activity relative to controls. Thrill-seeking play also promotes motor training [50], which is thought to stimulate cognitive and muscle development while fine-tuning the motor skills needed during adulthood [51]. Five-year-old children with ready access to independent thrill-seeking play had significantly better motor skills than those with fewer opportunities [61]. In another group of 5–7 year-olds, daily access to risky outdoor play (1–2 hr per day) was linked with significantly higher balance and coordination scores on a standardized motor fitness test [62]. Greater motor competency could stem from ‘self-handicapping’ [63], defined as the deliberate creation of physically challenging, moderately frightening, and unpredictable scenarios requiring locomotor versatility to overcome. The hypothesized advantage of such play is that it trains children to cope with comparable scenarios during adulthood [7].
On longer timescales, vigorous thrill-seeking play could enhance skeletal mass and strength [64]. For instance, adding 30 min of moderately intense physical activity per day can increase the bone strength of 5–11 year-olds by 3–5% [65]. Exercise-induced bone deposition occurs mainly on periosteal surfaces, providing crucial fracture resistance during aging; indeed, these material changes can be maintained throughout adolescence and even into adulthood [64]. Five-year-old children who engaged in frequent moderate-to-vigorous physical activity had higher bone mineral contents (a proxy for later gains in bone mass) at 8 (6–14% higher) and 11 years of age (4–7% higher) compared to lower physical activity groups, even after controlling for physical characteristics and current activity levels [66]. Another longitudinal study of former gymnasts found that frequent mechanical loading of the forelimbs during adolescence was linked to greater bone mass and size than non-gymnasts, with such changes detectable 4–9 years after activity cessation [67]. Likewise men who engaged in more rigorous forelimb loading during youth maintained proportionately larger and stronger humeri than control groups, even after 50 years of de-training [68].
SELECTIVE PRESSURES ON THRILL-SEEKING PLAY
Evolutionary reasoning guided Hinton’s patent application in 1924 [Box 1], but this insight is all but forgotten in the current debate over playground safety, monkey bars and child growth and development. Our goal here is to revisit Hinton’s premise by drawing on a century of discovery in our disciplines of biological anthropology and primatology. This perspective has the potential to move debate forward in productive ways.
Climbing is essential to primate life, facilitating the acquisition of food, escape from predation, and sleep. It was practiced by our earliest ancestors, the hominins [Box 2], and it continues to hold importance for many foraging peoples [69–71]. Death from injuries related to climbing is a risk factor for all primates, but it is attributed primarily to fall impacts from extreme heights. For example, food-harvesting in some hunter-gatherers entails tree-climbing to heights up to 50 m, with fall impacts accounting for ~7% of male deaths in the Baka of the Central African Republic and ~2% in the Agta of the Philippines [71]. Chimpanzees and orangutans commonly climb to heights exceeding 20 m [72, 73], and 4–10% of chimpanzee mortality is attributed to injuries sustained from falling [74, 75]. Teleki [76] and Goodall [74] described 51 falls by Gombe chimpanzees; of these, 41% were >5 m and 25% were >10 m, with one juvenile and one adult dying subsequent to drops of 14 and 25 m, respectively. Shimada and Yano [77] described a fall of 7 m for a juvenile chimpanzee; the individual recovered, but was temporarily immobilized with head trauma. Among wild orangutans, 76% of individuals treated by veterinarians for serious injuries fell from heights of 20–50 m, with two dying of skull trauma [78]. There is also evidence of fall-induced deaths in the hominin fossil record [Box 2].
Box 2—Paleoanthropology of climbing.
Perimortem fractures (outlined in red) of the upper extremity of A.L. 288-1 (‘Lucy’), an adult female Australopithecus afarensis. Photographs by J. Kappelman, reproduced with permission.
Ample evidence of arboreal activity exists in the human fossil record, including a 3.3 million-year-old (Ma) infant of Australopithecus afarensis, the Dikika child [122]. Climbing adaptations include a gorilla-like shoulder, chimpanzee-like fingers, and a flexible foot [123, 124]. The infant died at ~3 years of age, and it is tempting to imagine its propensity for thrill-seeking play in the treetops of Pliocene Africa; indeed, its age falls squarely in the age-range of maximum arborealism and suspension among chimpanzees (Fig. 3a) [86, 89].
Still, some hominin fossils hint at mortality from falls. For instance, Kappelman et al. [125] described bone fractures in the skeleton of ‘Lucy’––an adult female A. afarensis dated to 3.2 Ma––and attributed them to a fall of ~13 m. The authors identified perimortem fractures consistent with vertical deceleration impacts along both forelimbs, including a four-part fracture of the proximal head of the right humerus, a greenstick (or hinge) and spiral fracture on the lateral surface of the right humeral midshaft, a compressive impact fracture on the proximal head of left humerus, and a transverse break of the right distal radius. Such fractures suggest a high-energy impact scenario with outstretched arms. It is interesting––and perhaps not coincidental––that the greenstick fracture is comparable to those commonly found in pediatric patients that fall from playground equipment (Fig. 1a) [37]. In another example, L’Abbé et al. [126] attributed a distal ulnar bending fracture in the skeleton of MH2––an adult female A. sediba dated to ~2.0 Ma––to a fall. Like those of Lucy, the fracture indicates an axially-placed load on the forelimb, suggesting an attempt to break its fall with outstretched arms.
These heights and the corresponding mortality risks do not apply to most playgrounds today. A better point of comparison revolves around the risk of skeletal fracture. Healed fractures are found in the long bones of all great ape species, suggesting that nonfatal falls are somewhat frequent [79]. For example, the frequency of healed bone fractures (typically forelimbs) ranges from 21% to 36% of individuals in populations of wild chimpanzees [79, 80]. Comparable rates are evident among wild gorillas (20%), gibbons (36%), and orangutans (61%) [81, 82], as well as those human populations that climb trees often [71]. In one example from Papua New Guinea, tree falls accounted for 27% of hospital admissions, most of which involved fractures of the distal radius [83]. Falls from mango trees accounted for 16% of admissions for skeletal fractures at a hospital in Fiji, with 69% being pediatric inpatients and 56% of fractures occurring in the forelimb [84]. Likewise, falls from coconut trees resulted in a large number of admissions at a rural hospital in the Solomon Islands, with 57% of falls resulting in fracture; and it is telling that 40% of admittees were 10–14 years old [85]. Thus, skeletal fracture and recovery is a relatively common experience in the life history of most apes, including humans.
It follows that natural selection should operate strongly on juvenile primates to master the art of climbing; indeed, climbing and swinging are more common among younger age classes than any other ontogenetic stage (Fig. 3) [87, 88]. Among chimpanzees, infants (0–5 yrs) and juveniles (5–10 yrs) spend 15% and 27% more time climbing and arm-swinging than adults (+ 20 yrs), respectively [86]. This difference exists because infants and juveniles allocate more time (~40%) to arboreal activities [89]. Not only are juveniles more apt to climb than adults but they also exhibit a far greater diversity of movements and postures [87]. Exemplifying this point, juvenile langurs have 65% more distinct positional behaviors (both postures and movements) and 92% more distinct suspensory positional behaviors than adults [90]. Such locomotor experimentation is a crucial part of juvenile development, as it enables the fine-tuning of motor skills needed during adulthood [88]. Tellingly, infant chimpanzees that engage in more social play (a category that included locomotor play) tend to reach social (e.g. first non-maternal groom) and motor (e.g. first independent travel) milestones at earlier ages [91]. Thus, play-climbing represents a form of practice: one that promotes an awareness of branch material properties––an important consideration when navigating arboreal environments [92, 93]––and the mechanical loading caused by one’s own mass. It is best to practice these perceptual skills at a size that minimizes the force of impact and risk of injury. Yet, mounting morphological and physiological evidence points to juvenile skeletons being resistant to the mechanical challenges and impacts that occur during thrill-seeking play.
Juvenile primates have underdeveloped bodies and weaker muscles than adults, yet they move in riskier ways; so it stands to reason that natural selection has acted on their skeletons to improve locomotor efficiency and safety [94]. In terms of efficiency, juvenile primates possess higher effective mechanical advantage of their limb muscles than adults, meaning juveniles apply relatively less muscular force to produce the same output forces for a given posture [94]. For example, juvenile capuchins enjoy a greater anatomical mechanical advantage (AMA) of the biceps brachii and triceps brachii muscles, increasing the efficiency of suspensory behaviors [95]. Likewise, juveniles have better grasping abilities than adults because they have disproportionately large digits [94, 96]. For example, the relatively wide phalanges of juvenile baboons enable disproportionate pull strengths, exceeding adult values by 200% [97].
Juvenile bones are also relatively fracture-resistant. For example, the juvenile humerus and femur have disproportionately greater bending strengths than those of adults [98], in part because mineralization is incomplete. In short, juvenile bones are structurally tougher and more elastic, factors that absorb energy and minimize fracture severity [94]. Capuchin monkeys have the highest bone safety factors (i.e. the strength of their bones relative to predicted loads) at the onset of juvenilization, when the risk of falling is greatest [98]. Similarly, the humeri of chimpanzee infants (0–5 yrs) are relatively more resistant to bending and torsion than those of older individuals [89], a pattern shared with human children of similar ages [99]. These traits could be linked to the greater head-to-body mass ratio of juveniles, which moves their center of mass cranially and favors use of the hands to minimize the impacts of falls [100]. Using outstretched hands to mitigate head trauma during falls could explain the evolution of disproportionate forelimb strengths at the juvenile stage of development [98]; so, it is telling that many of the buckle and greenstick fractures to the radii and ulnae of children are the result of hands-first impacts (Fig. 1a) [37].
OUR VIEWS AS BIOLOGICAL ANTHROPOLOGISTS
Like Hinton [Box 1], we view play-climbing as a legacy of primate evolution that promotes child health and well-being. But this perspective is often overshadowed by the arguments of public health professionals and policymakers, who tend to problematize forelimb fractures as a costly and preventable playground injury. The tension between these perspectives is evident in the diverging play-climbing standards of several countries. In Australia, the maximum acceptable ‘free height of fall’ for climbing equipment was increased from 2.5 to 3.0 m in 2014 (standard AS 4684-2014), putting it into alignment with an earlier European standard (EN 1176-2008) (Fig. 3c) [101]. Conversely, many clinicians have argued for free-fall heights <2.0 m, with some favoring reductions to ≤1.5 m [21–24]. Both perspectives are rooted in statistical data, giving the impression of evidenced-based policymaking, but there is a problem: there is scant empirical data on how children interact with climbing equipment. To fill this void, it is useful to consider the tools of primatologists, researchers adept at quantifying limb movements during climbing [102, 103] or applying deep learning methods to track postures [104]. Such data would inform our understanding of how playground equipment shapes the childhood propensity for thrill-seeking play, moving us closer to human-centered design principles.
When faced with forearm shaft fractures, pediatric orthopedists have turned increasingly toward nonoperative treatments, even in severe cases [105–107]. This trend speaks to the outstanding resilience of juvenile forelimbs (Fig. 4), and it suggests that a wide safety margin is baked into the juvenile life stage, a premise that may inform enduring questions related to human life history evolution. Juvenile growth among primates, especially humans, is exceedingly slow [108], but the selective advantages of prolonging this life history stage are debated. It could mitigate against periods of low food availability [108, 109] or enable the development of advanced foraging and social skills [110–112]. Another possibility is that it is essential for developing motor competence, an idea linked to the evolution of play behaviors [63, 113], but not extended juvenility. It follows that arborealism is a strong predictor of juvenile duration across primates; and that the evolution of the hominin foot and ankle––traits that may have increased the risk of falling during arboreal activities [114]––was a major selective force on human life history evolution. In short, the selective advantages of motor competency in an arboreal milieu may have contributed to the evolution of prolonged juvenility across primates, and particularly hominins, a hypothesis that invites future research.
SWINGING FORWARD
Ensuring the safety of children is a justifiable concern, and we understand societal apprehension over the relatively high rates of fracture associated with monkey bars. What is less understood are the long-term physical and mental health costs of reducing or eliminating monkey bars or other catalysts of thrill-seeking play. In debates over the costs and benefits of such play, it is practical to consider the regularity of forelimb fractures across the arc of primate evolution, a perspective that begins to contextualize the injury with the adaptive importance of thrill-seeking play during the juvenile life stage of primates, especially humans. We believe the enduring appeal of monkey bars today is a testament to this evolutionary legacy, and that we must find ways to avoid extreme injuries while also letting children face developmentally appropriate risks.
A century ago, Hinton invented monkey bars to encourage primate-inspired play. Over the next 100 years, we may consider taking the next step of encouraging thrill-seeking play with natural materials in less-developed settings [115]. For instance, outdoor ‘adventure’ playgrounds have emerged in recent years across North America [7]. Already common in Europe, adventure playgrounds offer standard challenges (e.g. great heights; Figure 3c), as well as loose elements that encourage interactive and constructive play; e.g. recycled junk, nature-based materials, and access to fire and water [7]. Adventure playgrounds exemplify the spirit and intent of Hinton’s original desire for innovative playground equipment, and we hope that future discussions of thrill-seeking play will acknowledge the potential benefits while continuing to draw inspiration from our evolutionary and arboreal legacies.
ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
We dedicate this review to the memory of Michelle Bezanson, a cherished colleague and friend who inspired many of the ideas described here. We thank E.E. Butler, E.G. Dominy, E.J. Dominy, T. Dudek, S.W. Fannin, J.G. Gamble, C. Hobaiter, M. McChesney, M. T. O’Connor, and M. Ozug for practical assistance, and J. Kappelman for thoughtful comments.
Contributor Information
Luke D Fannin, Department of Anthropology, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA; Graduate Program in Ecology, Evolution, Environment, and Society, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA.
Zaneta M Thayer, Department of Anthropology, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA.
Nathaniel J Dominy, Department of Anthropology, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA; Department of Biological Sciences, Dartmouth College, Hanover, NH, USA.
AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS
Luke Fannin (Conceptualization [Equal], Writing—original draft [Equal], Writing—review & editing [Equal]), Zaneta Thayer (Conceptualization [Equal], Writing—original draft [Equal], Writing—review & editing [Equal]), and Nathaniel Dominy (Conceptualization [Equal], Writing—original draft [Equal], Writing—review & editing [Equal])
CONFLICT OF INTEREST
The authors declare no confict of interest.
FUNDING
LDF acknowledges support from the National Science Foundation (Graduate Research Fellowship no. 1840344).
REFERENCES
- 1. Scheer D, Benighaus C, Benighaus L. et al. The distinction between risk and hazard: understanding and use in stakeholder communication. Risk Anal 2014;34:1270–85. DOI: 10.1111/risa.12169 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2. Little H, Wyver S.. Individual differences in children’s risk perception and appraisals in outdoor play environments. Int J Early Years Educ 2010;18:297–313. DOI: 10.1080/09669760.2010.531600 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 3. Sandseter EBH, Kennair LEO.. Children’s risky play from an evolutionary perspective: the anti-phobic effects of thrilling experiences. Evol Psychol 2011;9:257–84. DOI: 10.1177/147470491100900212 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4. Solomon SG. How to revitalize American playgrounds. In: Waller T, Ärlemalm-Hagsér E, Sandseter EBH, et al. (eds). The SAGE Handbook of Outdoor Play and Learning. London: Sage, 2017, 195–210. DOI: 10.4135/9781526402028 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 5. Oliver BE, Nesbit RJ, McCloy R. et al. Parent perceived barriers and facilitators of children’s adventurous play in Britain: a framework analysis. BMC Public Health 2022;22:636. DOI: 10.1186/s12889-022-13019-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6. Gray P. The decline of play and the rise of psychopathology in children and adolescents. Am J Play 2011;3:443–63. [Google Scholar]
- 7. Gray P. Risky play: why children love and need it. In: Loebach J, Little S, Cox A, et al. (eds). The Routledge Handbook of Designing Public Spaces for Young People: Processes, Practices, and Policies for Youth Inclusion. London: Routledge, 2020, 39–49. [Google Scholar]
- 8. Haidt J. The Anxious Generation: How the Great Rewiring of Childhood is Causing an Epidemic of Mental Illness. NY: Penguin Press, 2024. [Google Scholar]
- 9. Loder RT. The demographics of playground equipment injuries in children. J Pediatr Surg 2008;43:691–9. DOI: 10.1016/j.jpedsurg.2007.12.061 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10. Migneault D, Chang A, Choi E. et al. Pediatric falls: are monkey bars bad news? Cureus 2018;10:e3548. DOI: 10.7759/cureus.3548 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11. Tuckel P, Milczarski W, Silverman DG.. Injuries caused by falls from playground equipment in the United States. Clin Pediatr (Phila) 2018;57:563–73. DOI: 10.1177/0009922817732618 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12. Blanchard A, Hamilton A, Li G. et al. Playground equipment-related extremity fractures in children presenting to US emergency departments, 2006–2016. Inj Epidemiol 2020;7:56. DOI: 10.1186/s40621-020-00275-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13. Mott A, Rolfe K, James R. et al. Safety of surfaces and equipment for children in playgrounds. Lancet 1997;349:1874–6. DOI: 10.1016/S0140-6736(96)10343-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14. Waltzman ML, Shannon M, Bowen AP. et al. Monkeybar injuries: complications of play. Pediatrics 1999;103:e58–e58. DOI: 10.1542/peds.103.5.e58 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15. Chalmers DJ, Marshall SW, Langley JD. et al. Height and surfacing as risk factors for injury in falls from playground equipment: a case-control study. Inj Prev 1996;2:98–104. DOI: 10.1136/ip.2.2.98 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 16. Norton C, Nixon J, Sibert JR.. Playground injuries to children. Arch Dis Child 2004;89:103–8. DOI: 10.1136/adc.2002.013045 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17. Sinikumpu J-J, Pokka T, Serlo W.. The changing pattern of pediatric both-bone forearm shaft fractures among 86,000 children from 1997 to 2009. Eur J Pediatr Surg 2013;23:289–96. DOI: 10.1055/s-0032-1333116 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18. Nakaniida A, Sakuraba K, Hurwitz EL.. Pediatric orthopaedic injuries requiring hospitalization: epidemiology and economics. J Orthop Trauma 2014;28:167–72. DOI: 10.1097/BOT.0b013e318299cd20 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19. Shah NS, Buzas D, Zinberg EM.. Epidemiologic dynamics contributing to pediatric wrist fractures in the United States. Hand 2014;10:266–71. DOI: 10.1007/s11552-014-9710-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20. Jerebine A, Fitton-Davies K, Lander N. et al. “All the fun stuff, the teachers say, ‘that’s dangerous!’’’ Hearing from children on safety and risk in active play in schools: a systematic review. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act 2022;19:72. DOI: 10.1186/s12966-022-01305-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21. Sherker S, Ozanne-Smith J, Rechnitzer G. et al. Out on a limb: risk factors for arm fracture in playground equipment falls. Inj Prev 2005;11:120–4. DOI: 10.1136/ip.2004.007310 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22. Adelson SL, Chounthirath T, Hodges NL. et al. Pediatric playground-related injuries treated in hospital emergency departments in the United States. Clin Pediatr (Phila) 2018;57:584–92. DOI: 10.1177/0009922817732144 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 23. Richmond SA, Clemens T, Pike I. et al. A systematic review of the risk factors and interventions for the prevention of playground injuries. Can J Public Health 2018;109:134–49. DOI: 10.17269/s41997-018-0035-8 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24. Curnow H, Millar R.. Too far to fall: exploring the relationship between playground equipment and paediatric upper limb fractures. J Paediatr Child Health 2021;57:1651–7. DOI: 10.1111/jpc.15583 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25. US Consumer Product Safety Commission. Public Playground Safety Handbook. Bethesda: US Consumer Product Safety Commission, 1997. [Google Scholar]
- 26. US Consumer Product Safety Commission. Public Playground Safety Handbook. Bethesda: US Consumer Product Safety Commission, 2015. [Google Scholar]
- 27. Martin D. That upside-down high will be only a memory: monkey bars fall to safety pressures. NY Times 1996;27:30. [Google Scholar]
- 28. Teitelbaum MP, Stankovits L, Curatolo E.. Monkey bar dimensions associated with pediatric upper extremity fractures show deviations from United States product safety commission recommendations. Cureus 2020;12:e6534. DOI: 10.7759/cureus.6534 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29. Hinchliffe J. No more monkeying around: push to remove dangerous play equipment. The Sydney Morning Herald 2018. https://www.smh.com.au/lifestyle/health-and-wellness/no-more-monkeying-around-push-to-remove-dangerous-play-equipment-20181121-p50hea.html. [Google Scholar]
- 30. Wyver S, Tranter P, Naughton G. et al. Ten ways to restrict children’s freedom to play: the problem of surplus safety. Contemp Issues Early Child 2010;11:263–77. DOI: 10.2304/ciec.2010.11.3.263 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 31. Borse NN, Gilchrist J, Dellinger AM, et al. CDC Childhood Injury Report: Patterns of Unintentional Injuries among 0-19 Year Olds in the United States, 2000-2006. Atlanta: Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, National Center for Injury Prevention and Control, 2008. [Google Scholar]
- 32. Nixon JW, Acton CHC, Wallis B. et al. Injury and frequency of use of playground equipment in public schools and parks in Brisbane, Australia. Inj Prev 2003;9:210–3. DOI: 10.1136/ip.9.3.210 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33. Schwebel DC, Brezausek CM.. Child development and pediatric sport and recreational injuries by age. J Athl Train 2014;49:780–5. DOI: 10.4085/1062-6050-49.3.41 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 34. Nauta J, Martin-Diener E, Martin BW. et al. Injury risk during different physical activity behaviours in children: a systematic review with bias assessment. Sports Med 2015;45:327–36. DOI: 10.1007/s40279-014-0289-0 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 35. Bergeron N, Bergeron C, Lapointe L. et al. Don’t take down the monkey bars: rapid systematic review of playground-related injuries. Can Fam Physician 2019;65:e121–8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 36. Cheng TA, Bell JM, Haileyesus T. et al. Nonfatal playground-related traumatic brain injuries among children, 2001–2013. Pediatrics 2016;137:e20152721. DOI: 10.1542/peds.2015-2721 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 37. Patel DS, Statuta SM, Ahmed N.. Common fractures of the radius and ulna. Am Fam Physician 2021;103:345–54. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 38. Schneeberg A, Ishikawa T, Kruse S. et al. A longitudinal study on quality of life after injury in children. Health Qual Life Outcomes 2016;14:120. DOI: 10.1186/s12955-016-0523-6 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 39. Clements R. An investigation of the status of outdoor play. Contemp Issues Early Child 2004;5:68–80. DOI: 10.2304/ciec.2004.5.1.10 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 40. Thomas AJ, Stanford PK, Sarnecka BW.. No child left alone: moral judgments about parents affect estimates of risk to children. Collabra 2016;2:10. DOI: 10.1525/collabra.33 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 41. Yerkes MA, Hopman M, Stok FM. et al. In the best interests of children? The paradox of intensive parenting and children’s health. Crit Public Health 2021;31:349–60. DOI: 10.1080/09581596.2019.1690632 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 42. Malone K. The bubble-wrap generation: children growing up in walled gardens. Environ Educ Res 2007;13:513–27. DOI: 10.1080/13504620701581612 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 43. Watchman T, Spencer-Cavaliere N.. Times have changed: parent perspectives on children’s free play and sport. Psychol Sport Exerc 2017;32:102–12. DOI: 10.1016/j.psychsport.2017.06.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 44. Little H, Wyver S.. Outdoor play: does avoiding the risks reduce the benefits? Aust J Early Child 2008;33:33–40. DOI: 10.1177/183693910803300206 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 45. Wakes S, Beukes A.. Height, fun and safety in the design of children’s playground equipment. Int J Inj Contr Saf Promot 2012;19:101–8. DOI: 10.1080/17457300.2011.603148 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 46. Waddington KC, Pearson ES.. Parental perspectives on the barriers and facilitators to risky-play in preschool children. J Multidiscip Res Trent 2021;3:104–23. [Google Scholar]
- 47. Kvalnes Ø, Sandseter EBH.. Risky play, then and now. In: Kvalnes Ø, Sandseter EBH (eds). Risky Play: An Ethical Challenge. Cham: Springer, 2023, 13–29. [Google Scholar]
- 48. Morrongiello BA, Lasenby J, Walpole B.. Risk compensation in children: why do children show it in reaction to wearing safety gear? J Appl Dev Psychol 2007b;28:56–63. DOI: 10.1016/j.appdev.2006.10.005 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 49. Morrongiello BA, Walpole B, Lasenby J.. Understanding children’s injury-risk behavior: wearing safety gear can lead to increased risk taking. Accid Anal Prev 2007;39:618–23. DOI: 10.1016/j.aap.2006.10.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 50. Sandseter EBH, Kleppe R, Ottesen Kennair LE.. Risky play in children’s emotion regulation, social functioning, and physical health: an evolutionary approach. Int J Play 2023;12:127–39. DOI: 10.1080/21594937.2022.2152531 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 51. Dodd HF, Lester KJ.. Adventurous play as a mechanism for reducing risk for childhood anxiety: a conceptual model. Clin Child Fam Psychol Rev 2021;24:164–81. DOI: 10.1007/s10567-020-00338-w [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 52. Dodd HF, Nesbit RJ, FitzGibbon L.. Child’s play: examining the association between time spent playing and child mental health. Child Psychiatry Hum Dev 2023;54:1678–86. DOI: 10.1007/s10578-022-01363-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 53. Lazarus RS, Dodd HF, Majdandžić M. et al. The relationship between challenging parenting behaviour and childhood anxiety disorders. J Affect Disord 2016;190:784–91. DOI: 10.1016/j.jad.2015.11.032 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 54. Segrin C, Woszidlo A, Givertz M. et al. Parent and child traits associated with overparenting. J Soc Clin Psychol 2013;32:569–95. DOI: 10.1521/jscp.2013.32.6.569 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 55. Fletcher KL, Pierson EE, Speirs Neumeister KL. et al. Overparenting and perfectionistic concerns predict academic entitlement in young adults. J Child Fam Stud 2020;29:348–57. DOI: 10.1007/s10826-019-01663-7 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 56. Kwon K-A, Yoo G, Bingham GE.. Helicopter parenting in emerging adulthood: support or barrier for Korean college students’ psychological adjustment? J Child Fam Stud 2016;25:136–45. DOI: 10.1007/s10826-015-0195-6 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 57. Twenge JM, Zhang L, Im C.. It’s beyond my control: a cross-temporal meta-analysis of increasing externality in locus of control, 1960-2002. Pers Soc Psychol Rev 2004;8:308–19. DOI: 10.1207/s15327957pspr0803_5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 58. Bundy AC, Luckett T, Tranter PJ. et al. The risk is that there is ‘no risk’: a simple, innovative intervention to increase children’s activity levels. Int J Early Years Educ 2009;17:33–45. DOI: 10.1080/09669760802699878 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 59. Sando OJ, Kleppe R, Sandseter EBH.. Risky play and children’s well-being, involvement and physical activity. Child Indic Res 2021;14:1435–51. DOI: 10.1007/s12187-021-09804-5 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 60. Engelen L, Bundy AC, Naughton G. et al. Increasing physical activity in young primary school children — it’s child’s play: a cluster randomised controlled trial. Prev Med 2013;56:319–25. DOI: 10.1016/j.ypmed.2013.02.007 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 61. Hüttenmoser M. Children and their living surroundings: empirical investigations into the significance of living surroundings for the everyday life and development of children. Child Environ 1995;12:403–13. [Google Scholar]
- 62. Fjørtoft I. The natural environment as a playground for children: the impact of outdoor play activities in pre-primary school children. Early Child Educ J 2001;29:111–7. [Google Scholar]
- 63. Spinka M, Newberry RC, Bekoff M.. Mammalian play: training for the unexpected. Q Rev Biol 2001;76:141–68. DOI: 10.1086/393866 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 64. Gunter KB, Almstedt HC, Janz KF.. Physical activity in childhood may be the key to optimizing lifespan skeletal health. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 2012;40:13–21. DOI: 10.1097/JES.0b013e318236e5ee [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 65. Janz KF, Gilmore JME, Levy SM. et al. Physical activity and femoral neck bone strength during childhood: the Iowa Bone Development Study. Bone 2007;41:216–22. DOI: 10.1016/j.bone.2007.05.001 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 66. Janz KF, Letuchy EM, Eichenberger Gilmore JM. et al. Early physical activity provides sustained bone health benefits later in childhood. Med Sci Sports Exerc 2010;42:1072–8. DOI: 10.1249/MSS.0b013e3181c619b2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 67. Scerpella TA, Dowthwaite JN, Rosenbaum PF.. Sustained skeletal benefit from childhood mechanical loading. Osteoporos Int 2011;22:2205–10. DOI: 10.1007/s00198-010-1373-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 68. Warden SJ, Mantila Roosa SM, Kersh ME. et al. Physical activity when young provides lifelong benefits to cortical bone size and strength in men. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2014;111:5337–42. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1321605111 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 69. Demps K, Zorondo-Rodríguez F, García C. et al. Social learning across the life cycle: cultural knowledge acquisition for honey collection among the Jenu Kuruba, India. Evol Hum Behav 2012;33:460–70. DOI: 10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2011.12.008 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 70. Venkataraman VV, Kraft TS, Dominy NJ.. Tree climbing and human evolution. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2013;110:1237–42. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1208717110 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 71. Kraft TS, Venkataraman VV, Dominy NJ.. A natural history of human tree climbing. J Hum Evol 2014;71:105–18. DOI: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2014.02.002 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 72. Thorpe SKS, Crompton RH.. Locomotor ecology of wild orangutans (Pongo pygmaeus abelii) in the Gunung Leuser Ecosystem, Sumatra, Indonesia: a multivariate analysis using log-linear modeling. Am J Phys Anthropol 2005;127:58–78. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.20151 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 73. Hernandez-Aguilar RA, Moore J, Stanford CB.. Chimpanzee nesting patterns in savanna habitat: environmental influences and preferences. Am J Primatol 2013;75:979–94. DOI: 10.1002/ajp.22163 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 74. Goodall J. The Chimpanzees of Gombe: Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge: Harvard University Press, 1986. [Google Scholar]
- 75. Anderson JR. Chimpanzees and death. Philos Trans R Soc London Ser B 2018;373:20170257. DOI: 10.1098/rstb.2017.0257 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 76. Teleki G. Group response to the accidental death of a chimpanzee in Gombe National Park, Tanzania. Folia Primatol 1973;20:81–94. DOI: 10.1159/000155569 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 77. Shimada M, Yano W.. Behavioral responses of wild chimpanzees toward a juvenile that suddenly lost its animacy due to a fall accident. Sci Rep 2023;13:16661. DOI: 10.1038/s41598-023-43229-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 78. Kehoe MM, Chan LC.. Fractures, dislocations and contusions in the Bornean orangutan (Pongo pygmaeus pygmaeus)––a review of 21 cases. Vet Rec 1986;118:633–6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 79. Jurmain R. Skeletal evidence of trauma in African apes, with special reference to the Gombe chimpanzees. Primates 1997;38:1–14. DOI: 10.1007/BF02385918 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 80. Carter ML, Pontzer H, Wrangham RW. et al. Skeletal pathology in Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii in Kibale National Park, Uganda. Am J Phys Anthropol 2008;135:389–403. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.20758 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 81. Schultz AH. Notes on diseases and healed fractures of wild apes. Bull Hist Med 1939;7:571–82. [Google Scholar]
- 82. Lovell NC. Patterns of Injury and Illness in Great Apes. Washington DC: Smithsonian Institution Press, 1990. [Google Scholar]
- 83. Barss P, Dakulala P, Doolan M.. Falls from trees and tree associated injuries in rural Melanesians. Br Med J 1984;289:1717–20. DOI: 10.1136/bmj.289.6460.1717 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 84. Gupta A, Reeves B.. Fijian seasonal scourge of mango tree falls. ANZ J Surg 2009;79:898–900. DOI: 10.1111/j.1445-2197.2009.05141.x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 85. Rehan R, Jones PD, Abdeen H. et al. The dangers to children from coconut tree trauma, in KiraKira, Solomon Islands: a retrospective clinical audit. Arch Public Health 2016;74:14. DOI: 10.1186/s13690-016-0125-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 86. Sarringhaus LA, MacLatchy LM, Mitani JC.. Locomotor and postural development of wild chimpanzees. J Hum Evol 2014;66:29–38. DOI: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2013.09.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 87. Bezanson M, Morbeck ME.. Future adults or old children? Integrating life history frameworks for understanding primate positional patterns. In: Clancy KBH, Hinde K, Rutherford JN (eds.). Building Babies: Primate Development in Proximate and Ultimate Perspective. NY: Springer, 2013, 435–58. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4614-4060-4_19 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 88. Bezanson M. Primate positional behavior development and evolution. Annu Rev Anthropol 2017;46:279–98. DOI: 10.1146/annurev-anthro-102116-041503 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 89. Sarringhaus LA, MacLatchy LM, Mitani JC.. Long bone cross-sectional properties reflect changes in locomotor behavior in developing chimpanzees. Am J Phys Anthropol 2016;160:16–29. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.22930 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 90. Workman C, Covert HH.. Learning the ropes: the ontogeny of locomotion in red-shanked douc (Pygathrix nemaeus), Delacour’s (Trachypithecus delacouri), and Hatinh langurs (Trachypithecus hatinhensis) I. positional behavior. Am J Phys Anthropol 2005;128:371–80. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.20205 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 91. Heintz MR, Murray CM, Markham AC. et al. The relationship between social play and developmental milestones in wild chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes schweinfurthii). Am J Primatol 2017;79:e22716. DOI: 10.1002/ajp.22716 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 92. Thorpe SKS, Holder R, Crompton RH.. Orangutans employ unique strategies to control branch flexibility. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2009;106:12646–51. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0811537106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 93. Janisch J, Myers LC, Schapker N. et al. Pump and sway: wild primates use compliant supports as a tool to augment leaping in the canopy. Am J Biol Anthropol 2024;184:e24914. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.24914 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 94. Young JW, Shapiro LJ.. Developments in development: what have we learned from primate locomotor ontogeny? Am J Phys Anthropol 2018;165:37–71. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.23388 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 95. Young JW. Ontogeny of muscle mechanical advantage in capuchin monkeys (Cebus albifrons and Cebus apella). J Zool 2005;267:351–62. DOI: 10.1017/S0952836905007521 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 96. Young JW, Heard-Booth AN.. Grasping primate development: ontogeny of intrinsic hand and foot proportions in capuchin monkeys (Cebus albifrons and Sapajus apella). Am J Phys Anthropol 2016;161:104–15. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.23013 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 97. Boulinguez-Ambroise G, Herrel A, Berillon G. et al. Increased performance in juvenile baboons is consistent with ontogenetic changes in morphology. Am J Phys Anthropol 2021;175:546–58. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.24235 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 98. Young JW, Fernández D, Fleagle JG.. Ontogeny of long bone geometry in capuchin monkeys (Cebus albifrons and Cebus apella): implications for locomotor development and life history. Biol Lett 2009;6:197–200. DOI: 10.1098/rsbl.2009.0773 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 99. Ruff C. Ontogenetic adaptation to bipedalism: age changes in femoral to humeral length and strength proportions in humans, with a comparison to baboons. J Hum Evol 2003;45:317–49. DOI: 10.1016/j.jhevol.2003.08.006 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 100. Sawyer JR, Flynn JM, Dormans JP. et al. Fracture patterns in children and young adults who fall from significant heights. J Pediatr Orthop 2000;20:197–202. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 101. Cassell E, Clapperton A.. Preventing serious fall injury in children (1): overview and playground equipment. Hazard 2014;77:1–20. [Google Scholar]
- 102. Dunham NT, McNamara A, Shapiro L. et al. A user’s guide for the quantitative analysis of substrate characteristics and locomotor kinematics in free-ranging primates. Am J Phys Anthropol 2018;167:569–84. DOI: 10.1002/ajpa.23686 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 103. Fannin LD, Joy MS, Dominy NJ. et al. Downclimbing and the evolution of ape forelimb morphologies. R Soc Open Sci 2023;10:230145. DOI: 10.1098/rsos.230145 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 104. Wiltshire C, Lewis-Cheetham J, Komedová V. et al. DeepWild: application of the pose estimation tool DeepLabCut for behaviour tracking in wild chimpanzees and bonobos. J Anim Ecol 2023;92:1560–74. DOI: 10.1111/1365-2656.13932 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 105. Bazzi AA, Brooks JT, Jain A. et al. Is nonoperative treatment of pediatric type I open fractures safe and effective? J Child Orthop 2014;8:467–71. DOI: 10.1007/s11832-014-0616-x [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 106. Sinikumpu J-J, Victorzon S, Antila E. et al. Nonoperatively treated forearm shaft fractures in children show good long-term recovery. Acta Orthop 2014;85:620–5. DOI: 10.3109/17453674.2014.961867 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 107. Sinikumpu J-J, Serlo W, Serlo W.. The shaft fractures of the radius and ulna in children: current concepts. J Pediatr Orthop B 2015;24:200–6. DOI: 10.1097/BPB.0000000000000162 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 108. Jones JH. Primates and the evolution of long, slow life histories. Curr Biol 2011;21:R708–17. DOI: 10.1016/j.cub.2011.08.025 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 109. Janson CH, van Schaik CP.. Ecological risk aversion in juvenile primates: slow and steady wins the race. In: Pereira ME, Fairbanks LAS (eds). Juvenile Primates: Life History, Development, and Behavior. NY: Oxford University Press, 1993, 57–74. [Google Scholar]
- 110. Joffe TH. Social pressures have selected for an extended juvenile period in primates. J Hum Evol 1997;32:593–605. DOI: 10.1006/jhev.1997.0140 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 111. Kaplan H, Hill K, Lancaster J. et al. A theory of human life history evolution: diet, intelligence, and longevity. Evol Anthropol 2000;9:156–85. DOI: [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 112. Street SE, Navarrete AF, Reader SM. et al. Coevolution of cultural intelligence, extended life history, sociality, and brain size in primates. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2017;114:7908–14. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.1620734114 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 113. Byers JA, Walker C.. Refining the motor training hypothesis for the evolution of play. Am Nat 1995;146:25–40. DOI: 10.1086/285785 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 114. DeSilva JM. Functional morphology of the ankle and the likelihood of climbing in early hominins. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2009;106:6567–72. DOI: 10.1073/pnas.0900270106 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 115. Brussoni M, Ishikawa T, Brunelle S. et al. Landscapes for play: effects of an intervention to promote nature-based risky play in early childhood centres. J Environ Psychol 2017;54:139–50. DOI: 10.1016/j.jenvp.2017.11.001 [DOI] [Google Scholar]
- 116. Washburne CW, Marland SP.. Winnetka: The History and Significance of an Educational Experiment. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, 1963. [Google Scholar]
- 117. Hinton, S. 1923. Climbing structure. U.S. Patent No. 1,471,465. Washington DC: U.S. Patent and Trademark Office. [Google Scholar]
- 118. Hinton, S. 1924. Climbing structure. U.S. Patent No. 1,488,246. Washington DC: U.S. Patent and Trademark Office. [Google Scholar]
- 119. Dewey J. Democracy and Education. NY: Macmillan, 1916. [Google Scholar]
- 120. Dewey J, Dewey E.. Schools of To-Morrow. NY: New Dutton, 1915. [Google Scholar]
- 121. Madison P, Wood B.. Birth of Australopithecus. Evol Anthropol 2021;30:298–306. DOI: 10.1002/evan.21917 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 122. Alemseged Z. Reappraising the palaeobiology of Australopithecus. Nature 2023;617:45–54. DOI: 10.1038/s41586-023-05957-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 123. Alemseged Z, Spoor F, Kimbel WH. et al. A juvenile early hominin skeleton from Dikika, Ethiopia. Nature 2006;443:296–301. DOI: 10.1038/nature05047 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 124. DeSilva JM, Gill CM, Prang TC. et al. A nearly complete foot from Dikika, Ethiopia and its implications for the ontogeny and function of Australopithecus afarensis. Sci Adv 2018;4:eaar7723. DOI: 10.1126/sciadv.aar7723 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 125. Kappelman J, Ketcham RA, Pearce S. et al. Perimortem fractures in Lucy suggest mortality from fall out of tall tree. Nature 2016;537:503–7. DOI: 10.1038/nature19332 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 126. L’Abbé EN, Symes SA, Pokines JT. et al. Evidence of fatal skeletal injuries on Malapa Hominins 1 and 2. Sci Rep 2015;5:15120. DOI: 10.1038/srep15120 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]