Abstract
Background
Alcohol consumption and withdrawal alter social behaviour in humans in a sex-dependent manner. The three-chamber test is a widely used paradigm to assess rodents' social behaviour, including sociability and social novelty. Automatic tracking systems are commonly used to score time spent with conspecifics, despite failing to score direct interaction time with conspecifics rather than time in the nearby zone. Thereby, the automatically scored results are usually inaccurate and need manual corrections.
New method
New advances in artificial intelligence (AI) have been used recently to analyze complex behaviours. DeepLabCat is a pose-estimation toolkit that allows the tracking of animal body parts. Thus, we used DeepLabCut, to introduce a scoring model of the three-chamber test to investigate alcohol withdrawal effects on social behaviour in mice considering sex and withdrawal periods. We have compared the results of two automatic pose estimation methods: automatic tracking (AnyMaze) and DeepLabCut considering the manual scoring method, the current gold standard.
Results
We have found that the automatic tracking method (AnyMaze) has failed to detect the significance of social deficits in female mice during acute withdrawal. However, tracking the animal's nose using DeepLabCut showed a significant social deficit in agreement with manual scoring. Interestingly, this social deficit was shown only in females during acute and recovered by the protracted withdrawal. DLC and manually scored results showed a higher Spearman correlation coefficient and a lower bias in the Bland-Altman analysis.
Conclusion
our approach helps improve the accuracy of scoring the three-chamber test while outperforming commercial automatic tracking systems.
Keywords: Sociability, Deeplabcut, Novelty, Artificial intelligence, 3-Chambers, Memory
Highlights
-
•
A deep learning procedure to analyze the 3-chamber test is introduced to extract data on sociability and social novelty.
-
•
The method has been compared with manual scoring and commercial automatic tracking.
-
•
Methods based on deep learning provide unbiased and reliable analysis of behaviour in the 3-chambers test.
-
•
Analysis with DeepLabCut/SimBA correlates better to manual scores than commercial automatic tracking.
1. Introduction
The study of the effects of chemical compounds on behaviour depends to a great extent on having robust methods providing objective non-biased analysis of specific moments in behavioural performance of controlled environments and mazes. The three-chamber sociability and social novelty test is one of the most used paradigms to assess sociability preference in mice models [1]. In this paradigm, sociability preference is when the subject mouse spends more time interacting directly with the social target (mouse) than the inanimate novel object whereas social novelty preference is when the subject mouse spends more time interacting directly with the novel target (never met before) than the familiar target (met before). Acute alcohol administration promotes social preference in male and female mice [2,3] whereas withdrawal from alcohol reduces sociability preference in DBA male mice, but not in C57 male mice [4]. In the three-chambers studies, time scoring could be done manually by counting the duration of direct interactions (facing/sniffing) of the subject mouse with the stimulus target (empty, social, familiar, or novel target). Nevertheless, this method is very time-consuming and requires experienced annotators who can accidently introduce variability or bias [[5], [6], [7], [8], [9]]. Also, automated behaviour tracking software (e.g., Ethovision© by Noldus and Any-Maze© by Stoelting) could be used for time scoring, this kind of automatic tracking estimates the overall position of the whole animal body as a center of mass depending on the differences between the animal color and the background color [10] but not able to track single body parts (e.g. nose) [11] so that those automated tracking softwares are commonly used by most labs to score the spent time by the subject animal close to the stimulus target rather than the time of direct interaction [12]. In this context, automated scoring may give inaccurate results, for example, in case an animal stays or self-grooming close to the stimulus target without direct interaction, thereby, manual correction is still recommended to correct and confirm the automatically scored results [12]. New advances in artificial intelligence (AI) have made tracking an animal's different body parts possible. In this context, DeepLabCut (DLC) (https://github.com/DeepLabCut) [13,14], an open-source pose-estimation toolkit, allows tracking of multiple body parts of an animal. It trains a deep neural network to learn to anticipate poses using user-provided samples (extracted and annotated frames from provided videos) and generate tracking data (CSV/H5 files) of multiple body parts' positions, orientations, and movements. Thus, it would be possible to track a single body part like an animal's nose in the very close proximity to the stimulus target (1 cm around the cup) as a proxy for direct interaction (facing/sniffing) with the stimulus target. In this approach, Simple Behavioural Analysis (SimBA) [15] was used to analyze the tracking data generated by DeepLabCut.
Thus, this work aimed to introduce an analysis model of behaviour in the three-chambers test by using DeepLabCut/SimBA. We have checked this procedure to investigate alcohol withdrawal effects on sociability and social novelty preferences in mice considering sex and withdrawal periods (acute and protracted withdrawal). Additionally, we have compared the results of two automatic pose estimation methods: commercial automatic tracking (AnyMaze) and DeepLabCut considering the manual scoring method, the current gold standard.
2. Methods
2.1. Animals
Male and female mice (C57BL/6) at the age of 3–4 months and weight of 28–31 g were socially housed by sex after weaning (2–3 animals per cage) and then randomly distributed into four groups, Saline treated males (CTR-M), Ethanol treated males (ETH-M), Saline treated females (CTR-F), and ethanol-treated females (ETH-F). The stages of the estrus cycle were determined for the female mice using vaginal cytology and delegated to an experimental group to balance the saline and ethanol groups. Estrus cycle stages were identified by different cell types (nucleated epithelial, cornified epithelial, and leukocytes) that are linked to the different stages of the estrus cycle (proestrus, estrus, metestrus, and diestrus) [16]. The animals were kept under standard conditions (12:12 h light/dark cycle and ±21 °C; food and water). All animal procedures were approved by the Committee of Ethics and Animal Welfare of the Universitat Jaume I, Conselleria de Agricultura de la Generalitat Valenciana (procedure 2023-VSC-PEA-0227), and agreed with directive 86/609/EEC of the European Community on the protection of experimental animals.
2.2. Treatment
Ethanol, purchased from Panreac (Barcelona, Spain), was diluted to 20 % (v/v) with 0.9 % normal saline. Mice were injected intraperitoneally with either the diluted ethanol solution (2.4 g/kg) or the same volume of 0.9 % normal saline every other day for 21 days for a total of 10 injections. All injections were administered during daylight between 9 a.m. and 10 a.m. The weights of the animals were measured before treatment (ethanol or saline), 48 h after the last injection, and 7 days after the last injection (Fig. 1A).
2.3. Three-chamber sociability and social novelty test
The mice performed a three-chamber test to assess sociability and social novelty preferences. The test was performed before starting the treatment to represent the basal behaviour, 48 h after the last injections to represent the acute withdrawal behaviour, and 7 days after the last injection to represent the protracted withdrawal behaviour (Fig. 1A). All tests were performed between 10 a.m. and 1:00 p.m. and video recorded with a Logitech C615 HD webcam and OBS Studio v27.01 at 30 fps. The apparatus was an open clear acrylic box (19.5 cm × 40.2 cm x 60 cm) divided by two walls into three chambers (one central and two lateral). Each wall had a central door (7 × 7.2 cm) to form a channel between the three chambers. Each lateral chamber contained a wire cage on one side so that it was divided virtually into two zones of the same size in the video tracking: the empty zone (EZ) and the Holding Zone (HZ) where the wire cages were placed. The social, familiar, or novel animals were placed inside the perforated wire cages to allow sniffing but limit aggressive or sexual interaction between animals. The area around each wire cage was considered to analyze the time spent exploring the animal inside (WZ). The test occurred in three sessions of 10 min: (I) The first session was habituation in which the subject mouse adapted to the empty three chambers. (II) The second session was “sociability” in which the subject mouse encountered a social target (never-before-met) in a wired cage over one lateral chamber and an empty wired cage over the opposite lateral chamber. (III) The third session was “social novelty” in which the wire cage enclosing the familiar mouse (the social target in the sociability session) was moved to the opposite lateral chamber that had been empty during the sociability session or was kept in his chamber, and a second novel mouse (never-before-met) was placed in the other lateral chamber in a wired cage. The subject mouse was free to explore either the social target or the empty cage in the sociability session, and the familiar mouse or the novel mouse (unfamiliar) in the social novelty session (Fig. 1B–C). After, each session the subject mouse was removed from the apparatus and gently returned to its home cage inside the room of the experiment for 5-min break and during that time the apparatus and the cups were wiped down with 70 % ethanol to remove any residual odours that may affect subsequent session/test. The time the subject mouse spent in each chamber and the time spent sniffing each wire cage were measured in each session. The sociability preference index was calculated from the following formula [(time exploring the social target-time exploring the empty target)/(time exploring the social target + time exploring the empty target)] whereas the social novelty preference index was [(time exploring the novel target-time exploring the familiar target)/(time exploring the novel target + time exploring the familiar target)].
2.4. Video analysis
Video analysis was performed manually by an experimentally blind researcher, and the time spent by the subject animal sniffing each cup was measured. Additionally, videos were analyzed using both AnyMaze (Stoelting, Wood Dale, IL, USA) and DeepLabCut/SimBA deep learning algorithms.
2.4.1. Video analysis with anymaze
Anymaze behaviour tracking software (version 7.4) (Stoelting, Wood Dale, IL) was used. As the software tracks the animal as a center of mass, a wide ROI was drawn around each wired cup (WZ, about 5 cm around the cup) (Fig. 3E) to be suitable for tracking the subject animal's body in close proximity to each wired cup. The amount of time spent by the subject animal inside each ROI (close to the cup rather than direct interaction)was measured. Additionally, Anymaze software enables tracking of the animal's head, it uses the center of masses as the key reference point to plot the head or the tail positions, so that we tracked the animal's head and scored the spent time in close proximity to each wired cup (about 1 cm around the cup) as a proxy for interaction time.
2.4.2. Video analysis using DeepLabCut/SimBA
DeepLabCut (version 2.3.8) [13,14] was used for body parts tracking. A step-by-step protocol for installation and work procedures is available on their website (https://github.com/DeepLabCut). For DeepLabCut/SimBA analysis we have used the following protocol: (1) Labeling of animal body parts; a total number of 200 frames from 10 videos were extracted using k-means algorithm and labeled with eight key points of the body (left ear, nose, right ear, center, left lateral, right lateral, tail base & tail end) (Fig. 1D). (2) Network training and validation; the network was trained and evaluated using Google Colaboratory Notebook; the training fraction was set to 0.95, and the batch size was set to 8. The network was trained for 140 K iterations using ResNet_50 pre-trained network and validated with 1 number of shuffles, the train and test errors were 2.44 and 3.31 pixels, respectively, and 2.44 and 3.28 pixels respectively with P cutoff of 0.6 (3) Video analysis and defining region of interest (ROI); the validated network was used to analyze all videos of the three-chamber test. Labeled videos were created, and visual inspection confirmed accurate tracking of all body parts. Simple Behaviour Analysis (SimBA) toolkit version 1.87.2 (https://github.com/sgoldenlab/simba) [15] was used to analyze DeepLabCut tracking data; a very narrow ROI was drawn around each wired cup (about 1 cm around the cup) to be suitable for tracking the subject animal's nose but not the whole body. (4) Extracting data; The amount of time spent by the subject animal's nose inside the ROI (facing/sniffing each wired cup) was measured. The trained model with instruction to reuse is available at https://osf.io/b8g7w/.
2.4.3. Statistics
Statistical analysis was performed using the GraphPad Prism 9 software (GraphPad Software Inc., La Jolla, CA, USA). Normality of the data was assessed using the Shapiro-Wilk test. Two-way analysis of variance (ANOVA) followed by post-hoc Bonferroni's multiple comparison test was used to compare the different experimental groups. Spearman correlation was performed by using a confidence interval of 95 % and measuring linear regression to correlate automatically (AnyMaze) scored and DLC-scored results with manually scored results. A Bland-Altman Plot analysis was performed to evaluate the agreement levels between the two scoring methods and manual scoring. The α value was set at 0.05 for all analyses. Statistical significance was established at p < 0.05 (*p < 0.05, **p < 0.01, ***p < 0.001, ****p < 0.0001). Supplementary tables contain details of the statistical analysis.
3. Results
We have made the analysis on 6 animals per group for each experimental condition. The Shapiro-Wilk analysis of normality showed that all groups passed the normality test. Thus, a comparative ANOVA can be applied.
3.1. All animals preferred the social target and novel target in the basal condition
Mice are social species that have a natural tendency to approach social stimulus and investigate novel (unfamiliar) conspecifics [17]. Therefore, all animal groups (CTR-F, ETH-F, CTR-M, & ETH-M) were tested for sociability and social novelty preferences using the three-chamber test before starting the alcohol intoxication protocol to confirm normal behaviour among the experimental groups. The results are referred to as basal behaviour. Both male and female mice showed a significant preference for the social target during the sociability session, and for the novel target during the social novelty session. Two-way ANOVA revealed a significant target effect (females sociability: P = 0,0001, F (1, 20) = 22,85; females social novelty: P = 0,0009, F (1, 20) = 15,32; males sociability: P < 0,0001, F (1, 20) = 37,22; males social novelty: P = 0,0044, F (1, 20) = 10,28) and no group effect (CTR vs ETH) (Supplementary Table 1). Bonferroni's post hoc analysis showed a significant preference for the social target during the sociability session (Fig. 1E,G) and a preference for the novel target over the familiar target during the social novelty session in the basal condition (Fig. 1F and H). There were no differences in sociability or social novelty levels between the control and ethanol groups under basal conditions in either male or female mice (Supplementary Table 1) (Fig. 1E:1H, 2A: 2D).
3.2. Sex-specific effect of alcohol withdrawal on sociability and social novelty preferences
Sociability and social novelty were assessed during acute and protracted withdrawal periods. Two-way-ANOVA has revealed significant period and group effects on female mice sociability (P = 0,0005, F (2, 30) = 9807; P = 0,0202, F (1, 30) = 6015; respectively) and social novelty session (P = 0,0317, F (2, 30) = 3883; P = 0,0112, F (1, 30) = 7,298, respectively). Using Bonferroni's post hoc analysis, alcohol group showed a significant deficit in sociability (P = 0.006) and social novelty (P = 0.015) vs control group during the acute withdrawal (Fig. 2A and B), and by the protracted withdrawal, this sociability deficit was significantly restored to the basal level (P = 0.0003) (Fig. 2A) whereas the social novelty deficit was insignificantly restored (P = 0.069) (Fig. 2B) (Supplementary Table 1). Interestingly, no such alterations in sociability and social novelty behaviours were observed in male mice during the different withdrawal periods (Fig. 2C and D).
3.3. The DeepLabCut results outweigh the commercial automatic tracking results (AnyMaze)
The tracking precision of DeepLabCut has already been demonstrated by its developers [13] so we used DLC tracking of a single body part (nose) along with a commercial automatic tracking system of the animal's whole body (AnyMaze) as a proxy for animals' interactions in the three-chamber test. The results of the two different scoring methods, DLC and automatic tracking, were cross-referenced and compared to the manually scored results. Both scoring methods showed preferences for the social target and the novel target at the basal behaviour of all animals, in agreement with the manually scored results; however, the DLC results showed more similarity to manually scored results. (Supplementary Table 3) (Fig. 1I:1L, 1M:1P). The DLC scoring method showed significant deficits in sociability and social novelty, as indicated by the manually scored results in female mice during acute withdrawal (Fig. 2I and J) whereas the automatic tracking scoring method showed insignificant deficits (Fig. 2E and F). Spearman correlation analysis revealed that the results from both scoring methods, DLC and automatic tracking, showed significantly high correlations with the manually scored results (R(278) = 0.982, P < 0,0001; R(278) = 0.873, P < 0,0001; respectively), with a higher correlation coefficient of the DLC results (Supplementary Table 5) (Fig. 3A 3B). Additionally, the descriptive analysis of Bland-Altman revealed that the results from both scoring methods were in good agreement with the manually scored results. However, DLC and manually scored results had lower bias values (bias = 2.23, Sd = 11.02, 95 % agreement limits: 19,36:23,83) compared to automatic tracking and manually scored results (bias = 61.31 S d = 53.40, 95 % agreement limits: 43,35:166,0) (Supplementary Table 5) (Fig. 3C and D).
4. Discussion
Several studies have shown that social deficits, which are related to an inability to maintain amicable relationships are the main features of many psychiatric disorders such as autism spectrum disorder [18], social anxiety [19,20] schizophrenia [21] and addiction [22]. Therefore, in mice models of those disorders, measuring social deficit is crucial for assessing pathological phenotypes and developing treatment strategies [23]. In this approach, the three-chamber test is widely used to evaluate social behaviours in mice including sociability and social novelty preferences [12], especially in ASD research [17,[24], [25], [26], [27], [28], [29]]. Since the three-chamber test was developed in 2004 [1,30,31], many automatic scoring methods have been introduced. One method involves the use of photocells embedded in each doorway to track the time spent in each chamber [1,17]. Another possibility is the use of automated tracking software (for example AnyMaze and Ethovision), which estimates the overall position and tracks the time spent by the whole animal body in close proximity to the wired cup, rather than the entire chamber [12]. However, neither method was able to detect direct interaction (sniffing/facing) of the subject mouse with the wired cup Luxem et al., 2023; [10], chamber time/animal in zone time, and direct interaction time usually correlates positively with each other. Nevertheless, there are many occasions when the chamber time/animal in zone time is insignificant, but the direct interaction time is significant. Therefore, manual scoring is recommended to correct false positive and false negative scoring, and verify the automatically scored results, such as when the subject animal remains in the vicinity of the wired cup without direct interaction (Fig. 3E) or software tracking errors (Fig. 3G) [12,17]. Alternaively, tracking animal's head inside the ROI could be a proxy for the interaction time, AnyMaze enables tracking of the animal's head by using the center of mass as the key reference point to plot the head or the tail positions [10], this kind of body parts tracking usually results in frequent mismatching and head/tail swapping (Supplementary Figs. 1K–L) which resulted in less accurate results and lower Spearman correlation coefficient with the manually scored results compared to chamber time/animal in zone time method (Supplementary Tables 4–5) (Supplementary Fig. 1I). In this approach, DeepLabCut, a pose-estimation toolkit that allows the tracking of an animal's multiple body parts [13], enabled the tracking of the subject animal's nose in very close proximity to the wired cup as a proxy for the time spent in interaction rather than chamber time/animal in zone time tracking (Fig. 3F and H, see also supplementary video 1 and 2). While it is likely that an interaction (facing/sniffing) is occurring if the subject animal's nose is in the tight ROI around the cup, this method cannot exclude moments when the animal's nose is in the ROI, but no interaction is taking place at that time. This limitation applies to both automated behaviour tracking software and DeepLabCut.
Supplementary video related to this article can be found at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2024.e36352
The following is/are the supplementary data related to this article:
In this study, we have found that commercial automatic tracking systems for an animal's whole body/head (eg. AnyMaze) has failed to detect the significance of the social deficits shown by manual scoring in female mice during acute withdrawal from alcohol. However, tracking the animal's nose using DeepLabCut showed a significant social deficit, in agreement with manual scoring. Interestingly, this social deficit was shown exclusively in female mice during acute withdrawal from alcohol and was recovered by the protracted withdrawal suggesting a starting point for investigating the underlying neural mechanism of this sex-dependent impairment and introducing treatment targets. Additionally, DLC and manually scored results have shown a higher Spearman correlation coefficient and a lower bias in the Bland-Altman analysis which confirms that using DLC in tracking the animal's nose, outweighs the commercial automatic method of animal whole-body/head tracking in scoring the three-chamber sociability and social novelty test. Moreover, DLC has a fast-growing community that enables data sharing and reuse among laboratories to train and improve networks. In addition, the tracking data on the position and movement of the animal's multiple parts could be further analyzed using a supervised machine-learning approach to quantify complex behaviours such as self-grooming, rearing, or jumping. One of the most widely used toolkits for this purpose is SimBA [15]. Another approach to further analysis of DLC tracking data could be an unsupervised machine-learning approach to discover and cluster new animal behaviour patterns, such as B-SOiD [[32], [33]].
Data availability statement
Raw data that support the findings of this study are deposited at https://zenodo.org/records/11453185 DOI 10.5281/zenodo.11453185″ title = "doi:DOI 10.5281/zenodo.11453185">DOI 10.5281/zenodo.11453185.
CRediT authorship contribution statement
Mohamed Aly Zahran: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Software, Methodology, Investigation, Formal analysis, Data curation, Conceptualization. Aroa Manas-Ojeda: Writing – review & editing, Software, Methodology, Formal analysis. Mónica Navarro-Sánchez: Writing – review & editing, Software, Methodology, Conceptualization. Esther Castillo-Gómez: Writing – review & editing, Supervision, Project administration, Funding acquisition, Conceptualization. Francisco E. Olucha-Bordonau: Writing – review & editing, Writing – original draft, Supervision, Resources, Project administration, Methodology, Funding acquisition, Formal analysis.
Declaration of competing interest
The authors declare no competing interests in this paper.
Acknowledgments
This research was funded by the Grisolia program of the Generalitat Valenciana GRISOLIA/2021/131 (MAEZ); Universitat Jaume I (PREDOC/2021/19) (MNS), AICO Generalitat Valenciana, grant number AICO/2021/246 (EC-G, FE O-B), the Plan Nacional sobre Drogas of the Spanish Ministerio de Sanidad y Consumo grant number 2020I012 (FE O-B and E C-G); UJI-B2019-54 (FEO- B) and HORIZON-MSCA-2021-SE-01-01 (PsyCoMed Project 101086247).Ministerio de Ciencia, Innovación y Universidades, (MCIU PID2023-153074OB-I00. Supported by Red Española de Investigación en Estrés/Spanish Network for Stress Research RED2022-134191-T financed by MCIU/AEI/10.13039/501100011033. We are indebted to the animal facility (Servicio de Experimentación Animal, SEA, UJI).
Footnotes
Supplementary data to this article can be found online at https://doi.org/10.1016/j.heliyon.2024.e36352.
Contributor Information
Esther Castillo-Gómez, Email: escastil@med.uji.es.
Francisco E. Olucha-Bordonau, Email: folucha@med.uji.es.
Appendix A. Supplementary data
The following is/are the supplementary data to this article:
References
- 1.Nadler J.J., Moy S.S., Dold G., Trang D., Simmons N., Perez A., Young N.B., Barbaro R.P., Piven J., Magnuson T.R. Automated apparatus for quantitation of social approach behaviours in mice. Gene Brain Behav. 2004;3:303–314. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-183X.2004.00071.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 2.Kent K., Butler K., Wood R.I. Ethanol induces conditioned social preference in male mice. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 2014;38:1184–1192. doi: 10.1111/acer.12342. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 3.Wood R.I., Knoll A.T., Levitt P. Social housing conditions and oxytocin and vasopressin receptors contribute to ethanol conditioned social preference in female mice. Physiol. Behav. 2015;151:469–477. doi: 10.1016/j.physbeh.2015.08.018. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 4.Sidhu H., Kreifeldt M., Contet C. Affective disturbances during withdrawal from chronic intermittent ethanol inhalation in C57BL/6J and DBA/2J male mice. Alcohol Clin. Exp. Res. 2018;42:1281–1290. doi: 10.1111/acer.13760. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 5.Anderson D.J., Perona P. Toward a science of computational ethology. Neuron. 2014;84:18–31. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2014.09.005. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 6.Bühler D., Power Guerra N., Müller L., Wolkenhauer O., Kuhla A., Wolfien M. Leptin deficiency-caused behavioural change–A comparative analysis using EthoVision and DeepLabCut. Front. Neurosci. 2023;17:1052079. doi: 10.3389/fnins.2023.1052079. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 7.Datta S.R., Anderson D.J., Branson K., Perona P., Leifer A. Computational neuroethology: a call to action. Neuron. 2019;104:11–24. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2019.09.038. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 8.Niesink R.J.M., Van Ree J.M. Short-term isolation increases social interactions of male rats: a parametric analysis. Physiol. Behav. 1982;29:819–825. doi: 10.1016/0031-9384(82)90331-6. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 9.Segalin C., Williams J., Karigo T., Hui M., Zelikowsky M., Sun J.J., Perona P., Anderson D.J., Kennedy A. The Mouse Action Recognition System (MARS) software pipeline for automated analysis of social behaviours in mice. Elife. 2021;10 doi: 10.7554/eLife.63720. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 10.Sturman O., von Ziegler L., Schläppi C., Akyol F., Privitera M., Slominski D., Grimm C., Thieren L., Zerbi V., Grewe B. Deep learning-based behavioural analysis reaches human accuracy and is capable of outperforming commercial solutions. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2020;45:1942–1952. doi: 10.1038/s41386-020-0776-y. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 11.Redish A.D. Vicarious trial and error. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2016;17:147–159. doi: 10.1038/nrn.2015.30. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 12.Rein B., Ma K., Yan Z. A standardized social preference protocol for measuring social deficits in mouse models of autism. Nat. Protoc. 2020;15:3464–3477. doi: 10.1038/s41596-020-0382-9. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 13.Mathis A., Mamidanna P., Cury K.M., Abe T., Murthy V.N., Mathis M.W., Bethge M. DeepLabCut: markerless pose estimation of user-defined body parts with deep learning. Nat. Neurosci. 2018;21:1281–1289. doi: 10.1038/s41593-018-0209-y. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 14.Nath T., Mathis A., Chen A.C., Patel A., Bethge M., Mathis M.W. Using DeepLabCut for 3D markerless pose estimation across species and behaviours. Nat. Protoc. 2019;14:2152–2176. doi: 10.1038/s41596-019-0176-0. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 15.Nilsson S.R.O., Goodwin N.L., Choong J.J., Hwang S., Wright H.R., Norville Z.C., Tong X., Lin D., Bentzley B.S., Eshel N. Simple Behavioural Analysis (SimBA)–an open source toolkit for computer classification of complex social behaviours in experimental animals. bioRxiv. 2020:2020–2024. [Google Scholar]
- 16.McLean A.C., Valenzuela N., Fai S., Bennett S.A.L. Performing vaginal lavage, crystal violet staining, and vaginal cytological evaluation for mouse estrous cycle staging identification. JoVE. 2012 doi: 10.3791/4389. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 17.Yang M., Silverman J.L., Crawley J.N. Automated three-chambered social approach task for mice. Curr Protoc Neurosci. 2011;56:8–26. doi: 10.1002/0471142301.ns0826s56. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 18.Kennedy D.P., Adolphs R. The social brain in psychiatric and neurological disorders. Trends Cognit. Sci. 2012;16:559–572. doi: 10.1016/j.tics.2012.09.006. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 19.Marcin M.S., Nemeroff C.B. The neurobiology of social anxiety disorder: the relevance of fear and anxiety. Acta Psychiatr. Scand. 2003;108:51–64. doi: 10.1034/j.1600-0447.108.s417.4.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 20.Stein M.B., Deutsch R. In search of social phobia subtypes: similarity of feared social situations. Depress. Anxiety. 2003;17:94–97. doi: 10.1002/da.10093. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 21.Pinkham A.E., Penn D.L., Perkins D.O., Lieberman J. Implications for the neural basis of social cognition for the study of schizophrenia. Am. J. Psychiatr. 2003;160:815–824. doi: 10.1176/appi.ajp.160.5.815. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 22.Quednow B.B. Cognition and Addiction. Elsevier; 2020. Social cognition in addiction; pp. 63–78. [Google Scholar]
- 23.DeLorey T.M., Sahbaie P., Hashemi E., Homanics G.E., Clark J.D. Gabrb3 gene deficient mice exhibit impaired social and exploratory behaviours, deficits in non-selective attention and hypoplasia of cerebellar vermal lobules: a potential model of autism spectrum disorder. Behav. Brain Res. 2008;187:207–220. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2007.09.009. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 24.Hamilton S.M., Spencer C.M., Harrison W.R., Yuva-Paylor L.A., Graham D.F., Daza R.A.M., Hevner R.F., Overbeek P.A., Paylor R. Multiple autism-like behaviours in a novel transgenic mouse model. Behav. Brain Res. 2011;218:29–41. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2010.11.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 25.Jamain S., Radyushkin K., Hammerschmidt K., Granon S., Boretius S., Varoqueaux F., Ramanantsoa N., Gallego J., Ronnenberg A., Winter D. Reduced social interaction and ultrasonic communication in a mouse model of monogenic heritable autism. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA. 2008;105:1710–1715. doi: 10.1073/pnas.0711555105. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 26.Moy S.S., Nadler J.J., Young N.B., Perez A., Holloway L.P., Barbaro R.P., Barbaro J.R., Wilson L.M., Threadgill D.W., Lauder J.M. Mouse behavioural tasks relevant to autism: phenotypes of 10 inbred strains. Behav. Brain Res. 2007;176:4–20. doi: 10.1016/j.bbr.2006.07.030. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 27.Nakatani J., Tamada K., Hatanaka F., Ise S., Ohta H., Inoue K., Tomonaga S., Watanabe Y., Chung Y.J., Banerjee R. Abnormal behaviour in a chromosome-engineered mouse model for human 15q11-13 duplication seen in autism. Cell. 2009;137:1235–1246. doi: 10.1016/j.cell.2009.04.024. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 28.Silverman J.L., Turner S.M., Barkan C.L., Tolu S.S., Saxena R., Hung A.Y., Sheng M., Crawley J.N. Sociability and motor functions in Shank1 mutant mice. Brain Res. 2011;1380:120–137. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2010.09.026. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 29.Silverman J.L., Yang M., Lord C., Crawley J.N. Behavioural phenotyping assays for mouse models of autism. Nat. Rev. Neurosci. 2010;11:490–502. doi: 10.1038/nrn2851. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 30.Crawley J.N. Designing mouse behavioural tasks relevant to autistic-like behaviours. Ment. Retard. Dev. Disabil. Res. Rev. 2004;10:248–258. doi: 10.1002/mrdd.20039. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 31.Moy S.S., Nadler J.J., Perez A., Barbaro R.P., Johns J.M., Magnuson T.R., Piven J., Crawley J.N. Sociability and preference for social novelty in five inbred strains: an approach to assess autistic-like behaviour in mice. Gene Brain Behav. 2004;3:287–302. doi: 10.1111/j.1601-1848.2004.00076.x. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 32.Luxem K., Sun J.J., Bradley S.P., Krishnan K., Yttri E., Zimmermann J., Pereira T.D., Laubach M. Open-source tools for behavioural video analysis: setup, methods, and best practices. Elife. 2023;12 doi: 10.7554/eLife.79305. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
- 33.Hsu A.I., Yttri E.A. B-SOiD, an open-source unsupervised algorithm for identification and fast prediction of behaviors. Nat Commun. 2021;12(1):5188. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-25420-x. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Associated Data
This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.
Supplementary Materials
Data Availability Statement
Raw data that support the findings of this study are deposited at https://zenodo.org/records/11453185 DOI 10.5281/zenodo.11453185″ title = "doi:DOI 10.5281/zenodo.11453185">DOI 10.5281/zenodo.11453185.