Skip to main content
Nature Portfolio logoLink to Nature Portfolio
. 2024 Jul 10;8(9):1771–1783. doi: 10.1038/s41562-024-01919-3

Investigating the impact of poverty on mental illness in the UK Biobank using Mendelian randomization

Mattia Marchi 1,2,3, Anne Alkema 3, Charley Xia 4,5, Chris H L Thio 6,10, Li-Yu Chen 3, Winni Schalkwijk 3, Gian M Galeazzi 1,11,, Silvia Ferrari 1,2, Luca Pingani 1,2, Hyeokmoon Kweon 7, Sara Evans-Lacko 8, W David Hill 4,5, Marco P Boks 3,9,12,
PMCID: PMC11420075  PMID: 38987359

Abstract

It is unclear whether poverty and mental illness are causally related. Using UK Biobank and Psychiatric Genomic Consortium data, we examined evidence of causal links between poverty and nine mental illnesses (attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), anorexia nervosa, anxiety disorder, autism spectrum disorder, bipolar disorder, major depressive disorder, obsessive-compulsive disorder, post-traumatic stress disorder and schizophrenia). We applied genomic structural equation modelling to derive a poverty common factor from household income, occupational income and social deprivation. Then, using Mendelian randomization, we found evidence that schizophrenia and ADHD causally contribute to poverty, while poverty contributes to major depressive disorder and schizophrenia but decreases the risk of anorexia nervosa. Poverty may also contribute to ADHD, albeit with uncertainty due to unbalanced pleiotropy. The effects of poverty were reduced by approximately 30% when we adjusted for cognitive ability. Further investigations of the bidirectional relationships between poverty and mental illness are warranted, as they may inform efforts to improve mental health for all.

Subject terms: Risk factors, Economics, Human behaviour, Psychiatric disorders


Although poverty and mental illness are strongly correlated, it is unclear whether they are causally linked. Using UK Biobank and Psychiatric Genomic Consortium data, Marchi et al. provide evidence of a causal relationship between poverty and some mental illnesses.

Main

The association between mental illness and social class was first demonstrated in a 1958 study by Hollingshead and Redlich, who found that individuals from lower socio-economic backgrounds had a higher incidence of severe and persistent mental illness and received less adequate treatment1. More than 50 years later, the same social conditions persist and affect mental health worldwide. Epidemiological studies throughout the world have demonstrated an association between mental health and socio-economic status (SES)24, with mental illness being more common among people from lower social classes5,6. Also, studies on income fluctuations have found consistent changes in mental health7,8.

Although the association between poverty and mental illness is strong across these studies, there is limited evidence that supports a causal relationship. Several factors, such as reverse causation and residual confounding, make it difficult to determine whether poverty causally contributes to mental illness or whether it is the other way around and mental illness leads to poverty. However, understanding the causality of the relationship between poverty and mental illness may be crucial for public health policies, as they may target essential aspects of poverty and improve public mental health9.

To date, the direction of the association between poverty and mental illness remains uncertain, and two explanatory hypotheses compete: social causation and social selection10. According to the social causation theory, the socio-economic adversity faced by lower socio-economic groups precipitates mental illness in vulnerable individuals, possibly mediated by factors such as housing insecurity, substance use and stress. Conversely, the social selection theory suggests that the overrepresentation of lower SES among people with mental illness is mainly attributable to downward social mobility as a consequence of the impairment associated with poor mental health11.

Conducting randomized controlled trials to determine the causality of the role of poverty is neither feasible nor ethical. An alternative method to investigate is Mendelian randomization (MR), which uses genetic data from genome-wide association studies (GWAS) to examine whether a risk factor fits in a causal model for an outcome12. The MR method takes advantage of the fact that genetic variants are fixed at conception and are less susceptible to the confounding effects that make results from observational studies difficult to interpret13.

Studies on poverty come with the challenge of defining poverty. Most of them use a single measure, usually based on income recorded at the household level, individual income or operationalized employment status of an individual, by occupational income7,11. However, no single indicator can capture the multiple dimensions of poverty, such as lack of material goods, limited access to education and health-care services, inadequate living standards, disempowerment, poor quality of work, the threat of violence, and living in areas that are environmentally hazardous, among others1417. For that reason, the UK health authorities commonly use a composite measure known as the Townsend Deprivation Index to assess material deprivation within a population18.

In this study, we applied MR to examine the causal effects of poverty on nine mental illnesses. First, to maximize the power required to examine the common effects of poverty across its multidimensional aspects, we derived a poverty factor using household income (HI), occupational income (OI) and social deprivation (SD). These three measures capture poverty at the levels of the individual, the household and the area in which one lives and so facilitate a more informed understanding of which aspects of poverty present the greatest risk to individual mental health. Second, we investigated the causal relationship of the common factor of poverty and each of the three indicators of poverty with nine mental illnesses: attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder (ADHD), anorexia nervosa (AN), anxiety disorder (ANX), autism spectrum disorder (ASD), bipolar disorder (BD), major depressive disorder (MDD), obsessive-compulsive disorder (OCD), post-traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and schizophrenia (SZ). To account for potential confounding effects, we also extended the MR analyses to include cognitive ability (CA).

Results

The schematic overview of the study is represented in Fig. 1.

Fig. 1. Research design.

Fig. 1

β1 is the association between the genetic variants and the exposure of interest (MR relevance assumption), i is the potential violation of the MR independence assumption, p is the potential pleiotropic effect of the genetic variants on the outcome (that is, violation of the MR exclusion restriction assumption) and β2 is the causal association of interest. Created with BioRender.com.

Latent-poverty-factor estimation

To analyse the joint genetic architecture of poverty, we ran a multivariable GWAS for which a common factor defined by genetic indicators is regressed on a single nucleotide polymorphism (SNP). The multivariable GWAS of the latent poverty factor (PF) was modelled from three indicators: HI, SD and OI (see Methods and Supplementary Information for details and Supplementary Table 1 showing the factor loading of each indicator). The heritability of the common factor GWAS was estimated with h2 and was around 8.4%. The mean χ2, multivariable linkage disequilibrium score regression (LDSC) intercept and h2 for the common factor GWAS and the GWAS of HI, SD and OI are presented in Supplementary Table 2.

The GWAS of the latent poverty factor identified 90 significantly associated independent loci at the GWAS threshold (P ≤ 5 × 10−8), as displayed in Fig. 2.

Fig. 2. Manhattan plot of the latent poverty factor.

Fig. 2

The x axis indicates the chromosomal position, and the y axis indicates the significance of the association (−log10(P)). The red line represents the genome-wide significance level (5 × 10−8). SNP effects are estimated with a linear regression model. The P values are two-sided and not adjusted for multiple testing.

The genetic correlation between PF and CA was strong (rg = 0.74, s.e. = 0.029; Supplementary Table 3). When we ran bidirectional MR of CA against PF, we found stronger evidence supporting the causal effect of CA on PF (inverse variance weighted (IVW) βCA→PF = −0.390 (95% confidence interval (CI): −0.408, −0.372)) rather than vice versa (IVW βPF→CA = −0.274 (95% CI: −0.288, −0.261)), as suggested by the results of Steger’s test (P < 0.001 and P = 0.423, respectively). The full results are presented in Supplementary Table 4 and Supplementary Figs. 14.

Bidirectional univariable MR of common factor poverty against mental illness

For the univariable analyses, the mean F statistic ranged from 31.5 to 45.6, indicating that the estimates were probably not subject to weak instrument bias. Forward IVW analysis of PF against the considered mental illnesses showed significant causal effects of PF on ADHD (β = 0.330 (95% CI: 0.287, 0.373)), ANX (β = 0.229 (95% CI: 0.101, 0.357)), MDD (β = 0.115 (95% CI: 0.074, 0.156)), PTSD (β = 0.140 (95% CI: 0.073, 0.207)) and SZ (β = 0.110 (95% CI: 0.074, 0.145)), and with the opposite direction on AN (β = −0.192 (95% CI: −0.254, −0.129)) and OCD (β = −0.202 (95% CI: −0.355, −0.049)). The estimate of the causal effect using the weighted median (WM) method was consistent in magnitude for ADHD, AN, MDD, PTSD and SZ, but not for ANX and OCD. We did not identify any significant causal effect of PF on BD or ASD.

Backward IVW analysis of mental illness against PF showed significant causal effects of ADHD (β = 0.402 (95% CI: 0.348, 0.455)), BD (β = −0.091 (95% CI: −0.133, −0.049)) and SZ (β = 0.082 (95% CI: 0.061, 0.102)) on PF. The estimate of the causal effect using the WM method was confirmed for both ADHD and SZ, but not for BD.

The results are displayed in Fig. 3 (see also the scatterplots in Supplementary Figs. 513 and 2326) and in Supplementary Table 5 (see also Supplementary Table 6 for conversion to odds ratios).

Fig. 3. Results of univariable bidirectional MR analysis of poverty against mental illness.

Fig. 3

a, Forward analysis. b, Backward analysis. Poverty is a latent variable built using HI as the unit identification, so that an increase in the indicator’s load stands for increased income; the regression coefficients have therefore been reversed to facilitate interpretation of the effect of poverty. The effect estimates on the x axis are log-odds for binary traits (that is, for mental illnesses) and unstandardized linear regression coefficients for continuous traits (that is, for poverty); the error bars represent 95% CIs. Poverty factor, N = 453,688; ADHD, N = 225,534; AN, N = 72,517; ANX, N = 21,761; ASD, N = 46,350; BD, N = 413,466; MDD, N = 138,884; OCD, N = 9,725; PTSD, N = 206,655; SZ, N = 320,404.

Steiger tests indicated that the causal direction between the exposure and outcome was correct in all the analyses. Cochran’s Q heterogeneity statistics were significant in all the analyses (except for the effects of PF on ANX and P on OCD), which is suggestive of pleiotropy. Furthermore, we looked at the MR Egger regression (MR-Egger) intercept, which significantly deviated from 0 in the analysis of PF against ADHD (P = 0.001) and of PF against ASD (P = 0.035), suggesting that pleiotropy was unbalanced in these relationships. The MR-Egger causal effect, which provides a better estimate in cases of unbalanced pleiotropy than IVW MR, yielded uncertain results for the effect of PF on ADHD (β = −0.232 (95% CI: −0.568, 0.104)) and evidence of an inverse relationship between PF and ASD (β = −0.477 (95% CI: −0.899, −0.0.54), Egger’s intercept P = 0.035). MR pleiotropy residual sum and outlier (MR-PRESSO) did not detect bias in the estimates due to horizontal pleiotropy in all the estimates. Causal Analysis Using Summary Effect Estimates (CAUSE) confirmed the causal effect of PF on ADHD bidirectionally, and unidirectionally on AN, MDD and PTSD. No significant difference between the sharing and the causal models was found for the effect of PF on SZ bidirectionally. The results of CAUSE are presented in Supplementary Table 7.

Leave-one-out analyses, in both the forward and backward directions, showed that the direction of the effect of each SNP is consistent with the direction of the overall causal estimate. In addition, for most of the analyses, a change in the estimations of less than 10% was obtained by leaving out each SNP, suggesting that none of the genetic variants were overly influential. The results of the leave-one-out analyses are provided in Supplementary Figs. 1422 and 2729. Finally, to test whether our results were robust to bias due to reverse causality, we repeated the MR analyses on a subset of SNPs selected through Steiger filtering. This sensitivity analysis confirmed the results of the main analysis, except for the effect of PF on OCD, which was no longer significant after Steiger filtering (β = −0.013 (95% CI: −0.199, 0.173)). The results are presented in Supplementary Table 8.

To further explore whether the effect of poverty on mental illness is driven by specific indicators, we performed bidirectional univariable MR using each of the poverty indicators (that is, HI, OI and SD) as the exposure. The results are presented in Fig. 4; Supplementary Tables 9 and 10 (HI), 13 and 14 (OI), and 17 and 18 (SD); and Supplementary Figs. 30107. The HI GWAS had a higher level of power than the OI and SD GWAS, as indicated by the number of SNPs selected for the analyses (~50, ~30 and ~10, respectively).

Fig. 4. Results of univariable bidirectional MR analysis for each poverty measure against mental illness.

Fig. 4

a, Forward analysis. b, Backward analysis. To have a consistent direction of the effect, the SD effect has been reversed. The effect estimates on the x axis are log-odds for binary traits (that is, for mental illnesses) and unstandardized linear regression coefficients for continuous traits (that is, for the poverty indicators); the error bars represent 95% CIs. Missing results are due to an insufficient number of SNPs selected for the MR analysis. HI, N = 379,598; OI, N = 282,963; SD, N = 440,350; ADHD, N = 225,534; AN, N = 72,517; ANX, N = 21,761; ASD, N = 46,350; BD, N = 413,466; MDD, N = 138,884; OCD, N = 9,725; PTSD, N = 206,655; SZ, N = 320,404.

Overall, the direction of the associations obtained using the poverty common factor was consistent with those obtained for each indicator, where bidirectional causal effects of poverty on some mental illnesses were observed. Specifically, using HI as a poverty measure, we found evidence of bidirectional effects with ADHD (β = −0.830 (95% CI: −1.01, −0.647); β = −0.103 (95% CI: −0.120, −0.086); IVW forward and backward, respectively) and SZ (β = −0.415 (95% CI: −0.565, −0.265); β = −0.031 (95% CI: −0.037, −0.024); IVW forward and backward, respectively) and unidirectional causal effects on MDD (β = −0.422 (95% CI: −0.589, −0.255)) and AN (β = 0.448 (95% CI: 0.191, 0.704)). However, the effect of HI on ADHD was biased by high heterogeneity and unbalanced pleiotropy (Egger’s intercept P < 0.001), with the MR-Egger estimate yielding inconsistent results (β = 0.606 (95% CI: −0.684, 1.90)). The bidirectional effect of OI on ADHD (β = −0.859 (95% CI: −1.05, −0.669); β = −0.102 (95% CI: −0.120, −0.083); IVW forward and backward, respectively) and unidirectional effect on AN (β = −0.187 (95% CI: −0.344, −0.030)) and MDD (β = −0.322 (95% CI: −0.502, −0.143)) were also confirmed using OI as a measure of poverty. The effect of OI on SZ was also confirmed bidirectionally (β = −0.187 (95% CI: −0.344, −0.030); β = −0.010 (95% CI: −0.017, −0.003)), but sensitivity analyses using CAUSE and Steiger filtering selection detected potential bias due to pleiotropy in the forward analysis. Notably, the effect of OI on ADHD changed after Steiger filtering, with evidence of pleiotropy (Egger’s intercept, P = 0.033) and a non-significant MR-Egger estimate (β = 0.094 (95% CI: −1.36, 1.55)). Backward analyses using OI as the outcome confirmed the effect of BD on poverty using the IVW method but not the WM method (β = 0.042 (95% CI: 0.028, 0.057); β = 0.020 (95% CI: −0.005, 0.046); IVW and WM, respectively) as found using the common factor poverty; but new evidence was found for the causal effect of AN on OI (β = 0.045 (95% CI: 0.011, 0.080)), which was not replicated using the poverty common factor or other measures of poverty. Finally, using SD as a measure of poverty, the bidirectional effect on SZ (β = 0.213 (95% CI: 0.046, 0.380); β = 0.042 (95% CI: 0.026, 0.058); IVW forward and backward, respectively) and the unidirectional effect on AN (β = −0.352 (95% CI: −0.618, −0.087)) were confirmed. The bidirectional effect of SD on ADHD was also replicated (β = 0.713 (95% CI: 0.504, 0.922); β = 0.222 (95% CI: 0.181, 0.262); IVW forward and backward, respectively), with stronger evidence for the reverse effect as indicated by Steiger tests (forward, P = 0.079; backward, P < 0.001). However, when we applied Steiger filtering selection, the effect of SD on ADHD held strong (β = 0.557 (95% CI: 0.331, 0.784)). None of the other effects of SD remained significant after Steiger filtering, which is consistent with potential bias due to reverse causation in the relationship between SD and mental illness (for the CAUSE and Steiger filtering MR results, see Supplementary Tables 11 and 12 (HI), 15 and 16 (OI), and 19 and 20 (SD)).

Bidirectional univariable MR of HI categories against mental illness

To explore the shape of the relationship between HI and mental health, and specifically to answer the question whether there is a particular HI threshold at which the effect of HI on mental health kicks in, we used GWAS data for the HI categories and investigated the effect of each category on the considered mental illnesses. That led to the creation of four dichotomous dummy variables: (1) low HI (LHI), for which cases were those <£18,000 and controls were those ≥£18,000; (2) low-mid HI (LMHI), for which cases were those <£29,999 and controls were those ≥£29,999; (3) mid-high HI (MHHI), for which cases were those >£52,000 and controls were those ≤£52,000; and (4) high HI (HHI), for which cases were those >£100,000 and controls were those ≤£100,000. For each of these categories, bidirectional univariable MR was performed.

As shown in Fig. 5 and Supplementary Tables 2124 (see also Supplementary Table 25 for conversion to odds ratios), we found a significant causal effect of being in the lowest HI class on ANX (β = 0.628 (95% CI: 0.095, 1.16)), BD (β = 0.306 (95% CI: 0.125, 0.487)), MDD (β = 0.351 (95% CI: 0.189, 0.513)) and PTSD (β = 0.506 (95% CI: 0.253, 0.759)), and bidirectional effects on ADHD (forward, β = 0.610; (95% CI: 0.415, 0.805); backward, β = 0.210 (95% CI: 0.170, 0.250)) and SZ (forward, β = 0.648 (95% CI: 0.488, 0.808); backward, β = 0.082 (95% CI: 0.066, 0.098)). In the LMHI class, the direction of the associations observed in the LHI class was maintained, and the effect size decreased, consistent with our leading hypothesis of more deleterious effects of poverty on mental health at very low levels of income. The exceptions were AN, ASD and OCD, for which being in the LMHI class was protective (β = −0.309 (95% CI: −0.526, −0.091); β = −0.268 (95% CI: −0.488, −0.047); β = −0.774 (95% CI: −1.34, −0.212), respectively). The analyses in the higher classes (that is, MHHI and HHI) showed that with each incremental increase or decrease of income, there is a corresponding effect on mental illness. For instance, AN and ASD (but not OCD) were causally associated with higher incomes, with stronger evidence for AN (βMHHI→AN = 0.371 (95% CI: 0.193, 0.550); βMHHI→ASD = 0.480 (95% CI: 0.303, 0.658); βHHI→AN = 0.333 (95% CI: 0.065, 0.600); βHHI→ASD = 0.094 (95% CI: −0.121, 0.308)), whereas the associations with ADHD, MDD, PTSD and SZ had the opposite sign, confirming the causal relationship with lower income. Notably, ADHD and SZ showed a consistent bidirectional pattern across income levels, where a lower income was causally related to these illnesses and the illnesses were also causal factors for lower incomes. This evidence supports a vicious cycle between poverty and severe mental illness.

Fig. 5. Results of univariable bidirectional MR analysis of HI levels against mental illnesses.

Fig. 5

a, Forward analysis. b, Backward analysis. For LHI, cases <£18,000 and controls ≥£18,000; for LMHI, cases <£29,999 and controls ≥£29,999; for MHHI, cases >£52,000 and controls ≤£52,000; and for HHI, cases >£100,000 and controls ≤£100,000. The effect estimates on the x axis are log-odds for both the forward and backward analyses given that all traits are binary; the error bars represent 95% CIs. Missing results are due to an insufficient number of SNPs selected for the MR analysis. HI, N = 379,598; ADHD, N = 225,534; AN, N = 72,517; ANX, N = 21,761; ASD, N = 46,350; BD, N = 413,466; MDD, N = 138,884; OCD, N = 9,725; PTSD, N = 206,655; SZ, N = 320,404.

The effect of the HI levels on BD showed a U-shaped distribution, in which BD was causally associated with both low and high HI (βLHI→BD = 0.306 (95% CI: 0.125, 0.487); βMHHI→BD = 0.179 (95% CI: 0.053, 0.304); βHHI→BD = 0.608 (95% CI: 0.423, 0.792)). However, these associations were not replicated using the WM method, and MR-PRESSO detected significant distortion in the estimate due to pleiotropy in the relationship between BD and MHHI, with non-significant outlier-adjusted causal estimates (β = 0.002 (95% CI: −0.039, 0.042)).

The estimate of the causal effect using the WM method was consistent in magnitude for the bidirectional effects of LHI on ADHD and SZ and for the unidirectional effects of LHI on ANX, MDD and PTSD and of MHHI and HHI on ASD.

Q statistics were suggestive of high levels of heterogeneity in most of the analyses; however, MR-Egger showed a significant intercept and causal effect only for the relationship between MHHI and BD (β = 1.69 (95% CI: 0.376; 3.00); Egger’s intercept P = 0.033). Steiger tests indicated that the causal direction between the exposure and outcome was correct in all the analyses except those on ASD (P = 0.994 and P = 0.057, respectively, for the associations with LMHI and MHHI).

The MR analyses on a subset of SNPs selected through Steiger filtering confirmed a stronger causal effect of the lowest HI level on ADHD, MDD, PTSD and SZ. The protective effect of MHHI on ASD was also confirmed. Interestingly, the effects observed in the main analysis were not confirmed for BD and AN, yielding evidence of reverse causation rather than direct causality for these associations. These results can be found in Supplementary Table 26.

Multivariable MR of poverty factor and CA against mental illness

Like other measures of SES, genetic effects are unlikely to influence poverty directly. Rather, genetic effects are likely to influence poverty through intermediary traits (such as health, personality, intelligence and other characteristics) that are themselves heritable19. Previous studies have shown that CA (referred to as general cognitive function, performance or intelligence) is one likely causal factor in income differences19 as well as being genetically associated with mental illness20. We used multivariable MR (MVMR) to estimate the effect of poverty on each mental illness, while controlling for CA. First, we performed bidirectional univariable MR of CA on mental illness, finding evidence supporting a bidirectional inverse causal relationship between CA and ADHD (β = −0.638 (95% CI: −0.720, −0.556); β = −0.151 (95% CI: −0.172, −0.131); forward and backward direction, respectively) and SZ (β = −0.297 (95% CI: −0.365, −0.228); β = −0.055 (95% CI: −0.063, −0.047); forward and backward direction, respectively), a unidirectional negative causal effect of CA on MDD and PTSD (β = −0.140 (95% CI: −0.215, −0.065) and β = −0.139 (95% CI:−0.264, −0.013), respectively) and a unidirectional causal effect of CA on AN and ASD (β = 0.306 (95% CI: 0.190, 0.422) and β = 0.310 (95% CI: 0.193, 0.427), respectively). These results are displayed in Supplementary Tables 2730 and Supplementary Figs. 108133. The MVMR analysis yielded results supporting a causal effect of PF on ADHD, AN, ANX, MDD, OCD, PTSD and SZ, beyond CA. Still, the effect of PF on mental illness decreased when including CA in the model. These results suggest that the univariable MR results for PF on mental illness are slightly biased by CA, but a direct effect of PF on ADHD, AN and ANX remains, nonetheless. The results of MVMR are shown in Table 1.

Table 1.

MVMR results for poverty and CA on mental illness

MVMR No. of SNPs IVW β (95% CI) IVW P
Outcome: ADHD 203
Exposure 1: PF 0.232 (0.169, 0.295) 7.28×10−13
Exposure 2: CA −0.477 (−0.612, −0.342) 4.31×10−12
Outcome: AN 205
Exposure 1: PF −0.109 (−0.183, −0.035) 0.004
Exposure 2: CA 0.149 (−0.008, 0.306) 0.062
Outcome: ANX 207
Exposure 1: PF 0.114 (0.007, 0.221) 0.036
Exposure 2: CA −0.344 (−0.571, −0.117) 0.003
Outcome: ASD 208
Exposure 1: PF 0.022 (−0.054, 0.099) 0.564
Exposure 2: CA 0.217 (0.054, 0.379) 0.009
Outcome: BD 208
Exposure 1: PF 0.020 (−0.044, 0.085) 0.538
Exposure 2: CA 0.006 (−0.132, 0.144) 0.931
Outcome: MDD 208
Exposure 1: PF 0.076 (0.030, 0.123) 0.001
Exposure 2: CA −0.091 (−0.190, 0.008) 0.071
Outcome: OCD 208
Exposure 1: PF −0.202 (−0.340, −0.0.064) 0.004
Exposure 2: CA 0.320 (0.024, 0.615) 0.034
Outcome: PTSD 208
Exposure 1: PF 0.111 (0.048, 0.173) 4.90×10−4
Exposure 2: CA −0.160 (−0.294, −0.027) 0.019
Outcome: SZ 208
Exposure 1: PF 0.092 (0.018, 0.167) 0.015
Exposure 2: CA −0.297 (−0.457, −0.136) 2.94×10−4

‘IVW’ indicates multivariable MR via the IVW method (random effects); the β effect estimates are log-odds.

Poverty is a latent variable built using HI as a unit identification, so that an increase in the indicator’s load stands for increased income; the regression coefficients have therefore been reversed to facilitate interpretation of the effect of poverty. All statistical tests are two-sided. The P values were not adjusted for multiple testing; therefore, P < 0.05 was considered significant (and is reported in bold).

Importantly, and unlike the univariable models, the inclusion of CA in the MVMR model produced highly dissimilar results comparing between the poverty factor and each indicator of poverty. First, the inclusion of CA in the MVMR model removed the effect of HI on mental illness. Second, for both OI and SD, some significant effects remained, where OI demonstrated a direct effect on ASD and SD showed a direct effect on AN. The results of these additional MVMR models are shown in Supplementary Tables 3033. Taken together, these results support the idea that the majority of the genetic effects that link poverty to mental illness do act on CA. However, by using a general factor of poverty, the resulting increase in statistical power facilitated the discovery of genetic effects linking poverty to mental health independent of the effects of CA. Therefore, while CA is correlated with poverty, it does not fully account for the observed genetic effects on mental health. These findings, when considered alongside the results of the MR of PF and CA, point to CA fitting as an upstream component in the complex causal pathway linking poverty to mental illness.

Discussion

Building on data from 18 GWAS, this study provides support for a causal relationship between poverty and some mental illnesses. Jointly modelling different indicators of poverty using genomic structural equation modelling and subsequent MR provided converging evidence supporting bidirectional causal effects of poverty and ADHD/SZ, a unidirectional causal effect of poverty on MDD and an inverse causal relationship between poverty and AN. Notably, the evidence for a causal relationship between poverty and ADHD was influenced by unbalanced pleiotropy. Our findings, in two Western-ancestry samples, complement the available evidence on social inequalities in mental health collected across different countries, samples and study designs2,7,11,21,22, including previous MR studies focusing on MDD, HI and unemployment2325. However, our research adds evidence that the relationship between poverty and mental illness is valid across different measures of poverty and extends the investigation to a wider range of mental illnesses.

This study primarily builds on genetic evidence and therefore warrants caution with respect to the conceptualization of poverty and mental illness. Moreover, while poverty and other measures of SES are heritable traits, it is very unlikely that there are direct genetic effects. Genetic associations with socio-economic traits are probably the result of vertical pleiotropy. Vertical pleiotropy describes instances where one trait is causally associated with a second trait. In these cases, the genetic effects that act on the first trait will also be detected as being associated with the second26. In that context, MR can best be viewed as a way to approximately randomly assign heritable traits that give rise to income differences. Two key candidates for such underlying traits are intelligence and personality. For example, intelligence may lead to both educational advantage and socio-economic success as well as healthier behaviours and thus lead to good mental health2731. In this study, we adjusted for CA and made two additional observations. First, when we controlled for CA, the causal effect of the general factor of poverty decreased by around 30% compared with univariate estimates, indicating that CA was a contributor to the causal association between poverty and mental illness. Second, when we examined each of the indicators of poverty separately, the effect of HI on mental illness was no longer statistically significant after controlling for CA. For both OI and SD, most of the evidence of a direct causal effect of poverty was absent after adjusting for CA; however, lower levels of OI remained causally associated with increases in ADHD, and higher SD remained in an inverse association with AN. Nevertheless, the higher power obtained by using the common factor of poverty rather than the individual indicators probably facilitated the detection of the influence of heritable poverty traits above and beyond the role of CA and enabled the detection of the causal role of poverty in mental health.

Of note is the vast literature showing that health characteristics (for example, obesity), physical appearance and mental state can influence economic outcomes, including employment opportunities and wages3234. Our results showing a causal effect of poverty on mental illness therefore should not be taken as proof supporting genetic determinism, but rather as epidemiological evidence of the detrimental effects of poverty and socio-economic inequalities on mental health, regardless of the mechanisms (see also the Frequently Asked Questions in the Supplementary Information).

The causal relationship between poverty and mental illness is likely to involve material, psychological, behavioural and biological pathways. For example, those with heritable traits linked to lower levels of poverty may be more able to move away from stressful environments/jobs/neighbourhoods and so place themselves at a lower risk of disorders35. Furthermore, the level of development of the welfare system may be a material mediator of the health-damaging effects of income losses36,37. Psychosocial mechanisms are a result of the interaction between people’s social environment and their feelings: living on a low income is stressful, and at the same time people in disadvantaged situations may have fewer resources to cope with difficult circumstances. Increasingly, biological research is providing evidence showing how experiences such as social defeat can ‘get under the skin’, causing biochemical changes in the body and brain and increasing the risk of developing mental health problems38,39. Other contributing factors to the relation between poverty and mental illness are the negative health behaviours that are more prevalent in socially disadvantaged groups40, probably as a consequence of the higher financial cost of healthy behaviours. For example, a healthy diet is more expensive than processed foods, and joining a gym or sporting clubs can be costly. Moreover, unhealthy behaviours such as smoking and drinking alcohol may be used as coping strategies for stressful situations41.

The findings of this study reiterate the need to further unravel the roles of poverty and mental illness and to use this insight to advance mental health for all. The choice of which action to take to address the problem is a political matter, but attention is warranted considering increasing income inequalities worldwide42, as well as the increasing incidence of mental illness43,44.

Previous studies examining antipoverty programmes reported positive and sustained effects on mental health45, including reduction of depression and suicide rates11,46,47. For example, a recent randomized controlled trial found that cash incentives provided to low-income individuals led to meaningful improvements in depression48. Additionally, specific economic interventions such as family assets, employment support and rental assistance may be effective in enhancing the mental health of programme beneficiaries49,50. However, it is important to note that previous quasi-experimental research using natural experiments (such as on the lottery winner population) have provided supportive evidence for a small association between SES and mental health51, and qualitative studies have shown that receiving money could be perceived as a debt or burdensome responsibility52,53, which may contribute to increased stress54,55. Further research is therefore necessary to identify the factors within economic support interventions (for example, microcredit, loans and personal health budgets) that influence mental health outcomes. Early studies have found that that combining antipoverty interventions, such as financial incentives and financial mentoring programmes, appears more effective in improving mental health than providing financial incentives alone47. Given these complexities, policymakers could consider policies that can be beneficial for both individuals living in poverty and those living with severe mental illness. Policymakers should also consider a key finding of this study about the effect of CA in the causal pathway linking poverty to mental illness. CA is closely associated with educational attainment and occupational status, which are often regarded as SES variables relevant to health27,29,56. Although the genetic relationship between CA and mental illness differs from the genetic relationship between education and mental illness20,57, educational attainment is a likely causal factor in CA differences5860 and is a more straightforward target for interventions than CA. Future research should thus explore strategies aimed at facilitating individuals’ participation in education, which may lead to better mental health8,61.

This study has limitations that should be considered when interpreting the results. First, for ANX, ASD, MDD, OCD and PTSD, the GWAS sample sizes were relatively small, precluding their use as exposure variables for investigating the impact of these disorders on poverty. This is due to the insufficient number of SNPs retained following the selection, clumping and harmonization steps. For SNP selection, we used the threshold of P < 5 × 10−8, which is recommended by MR guidelines to ensure the validity of the MR relevance assumption62. We could have relaxed this threshold to include additional SNPs, but this could introduce bias. We therefore refrained from performing those MR analyses and trust that future, more powerful GWAS on these conditions will yield more suitable instruments for MR, bridging this gap. Second, most genetic studies were conducted in populations of European ancestry from high-income countries. Generalizability to populations from other ancestries and from low- and middle-income countries is therefore uncertain63. In particular, this raises questions about the specificity and universality of the effects of poverty on mental health in different cultures and contexts with different political and social systems. Third, it is well acknowledged that psychiatric phenotypes are complex and heterogeneous, which generally translates to low power. This is reflected by the relatively modest effects of each poverty indicator on mental illness. Fourth, it is essential to acknowledge the potential influence of the dynastic effect, wherein characteristics transmitted across generations (such as the association between a parent’s genotype and the offspring’s phenotype) are a known source of bias in MR studies as they violate the second assumption (that is, independence)64. Detecting the exact magnitude of bias resulting from this effect is challenging, as current sample sizes preclude such analyses today. Future studies examining the causal link between poverty and mental health outcomes in an MR framework will be better placed to assess any influence of dynastic effects on the causal estimates identified. We also advise triangulation of our findings using complementary research methods in future studies. Fifth, it should be noted that previous research pointed out that UK Biobank participants are on average healthier and live in less socio-economically deprived areas than the UK average65. This could introduce potential selection bias in MR studies using UK Biobank data, inflating the risk of type I error66,67. However, the impact of selection bias is particularly strong when the selection effect is large—for instance, when studying disease progression, secondary diseases or specific subpopulations, such as elderly people66. In the context of our research, the selection effect is likely to be low. Furthermore, there is evidence that the UK Biobank is sufficiently large and heterogeneous to provide valid scientific inferences of associations between exposures and health conditions, which is in line with the aim of our research65. Sixth, the selection of SNPs associated with the latent poverty factor was performed in the UK Biobank, leading to the potential risk of overestimation of the SNP–phenotype association due to winner’s curse bias. However, the impact of this bias is generally not substantial in large samples, such as the UK Biobank68. Other limitations of MR concern temporality and linearity: MR is thought to provide estimates of lifetime risk and assumes linear effects. Although the use of MR on HI levels and the inclusion of mental illness with onsets across the whole person’s lifespan (for example, ranging from ADHD and ASD to MDD and ANX) may have mitigated these limitations, future studies should particularly focus on assessing whether there are critical windows or acute reactions to poverty exposure. Finally, the genetic variants captured by our measure of poverty are likely to have pleiotropic effects69. However, to break the assumptions of MR, it is not sufficient for the genetic variants in the instrumental variable to have pleiotropic effects70. The genetic variants used as instrumental variables must have horizontally pleiotropic effects mediated via mechanisms other than those captured by poverty. Should genetic variants have vertically pleiotropic effects (for example, SNP → neuron → intelligence → education → poverty → health → psychiatric disorder), then our MR-derived causal estimates will not be biased. Furthermore, if the SNPs affect other phenotypes, but these phenotypes do not affect outcome, then these effects will not bias our MR estimates. It is possible that the genetic variants identified in our poverty GWAS do have horizontally pleiotropic effects, but it is unclear what mechanisms would mediate such effects (for example, personality). In the current study, we investigated potentially pleiotropic effects using MVMR to examine the role of CA71. Future research should use MVMR to investigate the roles of other traits that link poverty to mental health outcomes.

Methods

Study design and data sources

We conducted a two-sample MR study using summary-level GWAS data that for the most part were publicly available. Specifically, for this study, new GWAS on HI and SD were performed using UK Biobank data under application number 10279 (see ‘Data availability’ for the links to these data), the OI data are publicly available and can be downloaded from https://osf.io/rg8sh/, the CA data were obtained from the author of the relevant publication20, and the summary statistics files for the mental illness phenotypes are publicly available for download from the Psychiatric Genomic Consortium (PGC) website at https://pgc.unc.edu/for-researchers/download-results/. Further information on the sources of the summary data is reported in Table 2. Ethical approval was obtained in all original studies.

Table 2.

GWAS information for each phenotype

Trait N No. of cases Consortium Reference
ADHD 225,534 38,691 PGC Demontis et al.79
AN 72,517 16,992 PGC Watson et al.80
ANX 21,761 7,016 PGC Otowa et al.81
ASD 46,350 18,381 PGC Grove et al.82
BD 413,466 41,917 PGC Mullins et al.83
CA 248,482 NA UKB Hill et al.20
HI 379,598 NA UKB NA
MDD 138,884 43,204 PGC Howard et al.84
OCD 9,725 2,688 PGC Arnold et al.85
OI 282,963 NA NA Kweon et al.76
PTSD 206,655 32,428 PGC Nievergelt et al.86
SD 440,350 NA UKB NA
SZ 320,404 76,755 PGC Trubetskoy et al.87

The traits are presented in alphabetical order.

NA, not applicable; PGC, Psychiatric Genomic Consortium; UKB, UK Biobank.

MR relies on three main assumptions on the validity of the genetic instrument variable: relevance, independence and exclusion restriction72. These require that the genetic variant (1) is related to the exposure (that is, only SNPs that reach genome-wide significance in the association with the exposure, or P < 5 × 10−8, are used), (2) is not correlated with confounders in the exposure–outcome relationship and (3) affects the outcome only through the risk factor (that is, the ‘no horizontal pleiotropy’ rule).

Poverty and CA instruments

For this research, seven new GWAS were performed: five on HI, one on SD and one on the common factor of poverty. The GWAS on CA and OI were publicly available. A total of 379,598 participants of European ancestry provided genotype data and data on their level of yearly HI before tax. HI was primarily analysed as a continuous variable. To investigate whether the relation between HI and mental illness is particularly strong at any specific level of income, we further categorized HI as a dichotomous variable, defining the cases in the following way: (1) LHI: less than £18,000; (2) LMHI: less than £29,999; (3) MHHI: more than £52,000; (4) HHI: more than £100,000. One GWAS for each HI category versus all the other categories was performed. A total of 440,350 individuals of European ancestry had genotype data and data on their level of SD, measured with the Townsend Deprivation Index, which was analysed as a continuous variable. GWAS of HI and SD were performed in REGENIE (version 3.1.3)73; for detailed methods, see the Supplementary Information. Previously published GWAS summary data on CA of 248,482 individuals were included20. The CA measure was derived from an already existing dataset of multiple cohorts in which each participant had been administered a battery of cognitive tests20. Correlations between tests of CA are high, with estimates between r = 0.8 and r = 1.0 being reported74,75. Further information on the CA data can be found in the Supplementary Information. In this study, CA was analysed as a continuous variable.

The results from a meta-analysis of two OI GWAS76, including the UK Biobank and the Health and Retirement Study (the latter involving participants from white American ancestry), were used to generate the instruments for OI, which were analysed as continuous variables.

We estimated a latent factor GWAS of poverty (PF) by jointly modelling cross-trait liability of continuous HI, OI and SD, using a common factor model in genomic structural equation modelling77. Before running the multivariable GWAS, we aligned the alleles across HI, SD and OI traits using the HAPMAP3 reference panel, and summary statistics passed quality control by selecting SNPs with minor allele frequency >0.01 and INFO >0.9. We then estimated the LDSC78 across HI, OI and SD using the 1000G European reference panel. Finally, we combined the LDSC output with the HI, OI and SD summary statistics to run the multivariable common factor GWAS. This common factor poverty GWAS was built using HI as the unit of identification, and the effective sample size was 453,689. To allow the estimation of poverty instead of income in the MR analyses on mental illness, we reversed the regression coefficients. The full details on the genomic structural equation modelling methods are available in the Supplementary Information.

Mental illness instruments

Results from the most updated GWAS of ADHD79, AN80, ANX81, ASD82, BD83, MDD84, OCD85, PTSD86 and SZ87 were obtained. The two-sample MR method requires minimal sample overlap between the exposure and outcome GWAS88; therefore, mental illness summary-level data were obtained mainly from the PGC, excluding UK Biobank participants. The GWAS of the mental illness traits considered were provided and analysed as case/control. The effective sample size across the cohorts contributing to the GWAS meta-analysis was calculated for each trait and ranged from 320,000 (for SZ) to 10,000 (for OCD). Table 2 summarizes the GWAS information.

MR

The full set of GWAS summary statistics for each exposure was first restricted to the variants reaching the genome-wide significance threshold (that is, P < 5 × 10−8). Then, to ensure independence between instruments, we applied a strict clumping procedure (LD r2 < 0.001 within 10 Mb, using 1000G EUR as the reference panel). Following that, SNP alleles were harmonized between exposure GWAS and outcome GWAS, using action = 2 (which tries to infer positive strand alleles, using allele frequencies for palindromes), before running MR.

We conducted bidirectional univariable MR between each poverty measure and mental illness. The IVW method was used to estimate effects in the primary analyses89, and the results are presented using forest plots. The WM90, MR-Egger91 and MR-PRESSO92 methods were used as sensitivity analyses, because each method makes different assumptions regarding instrument validity. Specifically, MR-Egger and MR-PRESSO are better than IVW in cases of horizontal pleiotropy (that is, violation of the exclusion restriction assumption). In addition, we used CAUSE93 to account for the presence of correlated or uncorrelated pleiotropy (that is, violation of the independence and exclusion restriction assumptions). Further information on MR-PRESSO and CAUSE is available in the Supplementary Information.

We present the results of the MR analyses as β values corresponding to the log-odds for binary traits (for example, mental illness) or to the unstandardized linear regression coefficients for continuous traits (for example, poverty measures) with their respective 95% CIs. When the outcome of the analysis was binary (that is, for mental illness and HI levels), we also provide a conversion to odds ratios with corresponding 95% CIs.

We also conducted leave-one-out analyses to investigate whether the effects are driven by one or a subset of the SNPs and investigated whether the instruments represent the correct causal direction using Steiger analyses, including Steiger tests and Steiger filtering94. Steiger filtering is particularly useful to avoid false positive findings due to reverse causation (that is, violation of the exclusion restriction assumption). The mr_wrapper() function of the TwosampleMR package performs Steiger tests on each SNP, evaluating whether the R2 of the exposure is greater than the R2 of the outcome and indicating the correct direction of the association. Subsequent Steiger filtering excluded SNPs with false results, allowing MR analyses to be performed on the subset of SNPs with verified associations.

Finally, we hypothesized that CA is probably involved in the poverty and mental illness causal pathway, and our instruments for poverty are possibly related to CA. We therefore used MVMR72 to estimate the direct effect of poverty on mental illness, independent of CA. For this purpose, we clumped the full list of SNPs from each poverty indicator GWAS (to ensure that only independent SNPs are included) and restricted it to those SNPs found in the outcome GWAS, and then ran the analyses on each instrument–mental illness set.

Instrument strength was quantified using the mean F statistic within the univariable IVW analyses, considering a value of F < 20 as indicative of weak instruments95.

All the analyses were conducted in R (version 4.4.0)96, using the packages TwosampleMR (version 0.6.2)97, CAUSE (version 1.2.0)93, and GenomicSEM (version 0.0.5)77. The statistical tests were two-tailed. The significance threshold was P < 0.05. Since only one relationship was tested—that between poverty and mental illness—we did not apply a multiple-testing P-value correction.

Reporting summary

Further information on research design is available in the Nature Portfolio Reporting Summary linked to this article.

Supplementary information

Supplementary Information (3.5MB, pdf)

Supplementary Figs. 1–133, Tables 1–33, Methods and Frequently Asked Questions (FAQ).

Reporting Summary (1.9MB, pdf)
Peer Review File (483KB, pdf)

Acknowledgements

The authors received no specific funding for this work. We thank M. C. Murari from the University of Modena and Reggio Emilia for help with High Performance Computing Cluster. W.D.H. and C.X. are supported by a Career Development Award from the Medical Research Council (no. MR/T030852/1) for the project titled ‘From genetic sequence to phenotypic consequence: genetic and environmental links between cognitive ability, socioeconomic position, and health’.

Author contributions

Conceptualization: M.M., M.P.M.B. and W.D.H. Methodology: M.M., M.P.M.B., W.D.H., C.X. and C.H.L.T. Software: M.M., C.X., W.D.H. and C.H.L.T. Formal Analysis: M.M., W.D.H. and C.X. Resources: M.M., W.D.H., C.X., C.H.L.T., H.K. and M.P.M.B. Data Curation: M.M., M.P.M.B., W.D.H., C.X. and H.K. Writing - Original Draft: M.M., A.A., W.S. and L.-Y.C. Writing - Review & Editing: M.P.M.B., G.M.G., S.F., L.P., S.E.-L., C.H.L.T., C.X., W.D.H., M.M., A.A. and W.S. Visualization: M.M. and W.D.H. Supervision: M.P.M.B. and W.D.H. All the authors approved the final version of the manuscript.

Peer review

Peer review information

Nature Human Behaviour thanks the anonymous reviewers for their contribution to the peer review of this work. Peer reviewer reports are available.

Data availability

The UK Biobank data used in this study are available via the UK Biobank data access process (http://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/register-apply/). The PGC data can be publicly accessed via https://pgc.unc.edu/for-researchers/download-results/. The OI data are publicly available and can be downloaded from https://osf.io/rg8sh/. The CA data were obtained from the author of the relevant publication, accessible at https://www.nature.com/articles/s41380-017-0001-5. The full GWAS summary statistics on HI, SD and poverty have been uploaded to the GWAS catalogue (https://www.ebi.ac.uk/gwas/deposition/submission/6572ff9a53c4ef000109d322; study accession IDs: GCST90302879, GCST90302880, GCST90302881, GCST90302882, GCST90302883, GCST90302884 and GCST90302885).

Code availability

The code for replicating the analyses reported in this Article can be accessed at https://github.com/MattiaMarchi/Common-factor-GWAS–MR.

Competing interests

The authors declare no competing interests.

Footnotes

Publisher’s note Springer Nature remains neutral with regard to jurisdictional claims in published maps and institutional affiliations.

Contributor Information

Gian M. Galeazzi, Email: GianMaria.Galeazzi@ausl.re.it

Marco P. Boks, Email: m.p.m.boks@umcutrecht.nl

Supplementary information

The online version contains supplementary material available at 10.1038/s41562-024-01919-3.

References

  • 1.Hollingshead, A. B. & Redlich, F. C. Social Class and Mental Illness: Community Study (Wiley, 1958); 10.1037/10645-000 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 2.Chung, R. Y. N. et al. Deprivation is associated with worse physical and mental health beyond income poverty: a population-based household survey among Chinese adults. Qual. Life Res.27, 2127–2135 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Torquati, L., Mielke, G. I., Brown, W. J., Burton, N. W. & Kolbe-Alexander, T. L. Shift work and poor mental health: a meta-analysis of longitudinal studies. Am. J. Public Health109, E13–E20 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Rapp, M. A. et al. When local poverty is more important than your income: mental health in minorities in inner cities. World Psychiatry14, 249–250 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Price, J. H., Khubchandani, J. & Webb, F. J. Poverty and health disparities: what can public health professionals do? Health Promot. Pract.19, 170–174 (2018). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Vaalavuo, M. Deterioration in health: what is the role of unemployment and poverty? Scand. J. Public Health44, 347–353 (2016). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Thomson, R. M. et al. How do income changes impact on mental health and wellbeing for working-age adults? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Public Health7, e515–e528 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Ribeiro, W. S. et al. Income inequality and mental illness-related morbidity and resilience: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Lancet Psychiatry4, 554–562 (2017). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Park, A.-L. et al. Economic analyses of supported employment programmes for people with mental health conditions: a systematic review. Eur. Psychiatry65, e51 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Murali, V. & Oyebode, F. Poverty, social inequality and mental health. Adv. Psychiatr. Treat.10, 216–224 (2004). [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Ridley, M., Rao, G., Schilbach, F. & Patel, V. Poverty, depression, and anxiety: causal evidence and mechanisms. Science370, eaay0214 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Wootton, R. E., Jones, H. J. & Sallis, H. M. Mendelian randomisation for psychiatry: how does it work, and what can it tell us? Mol. Psychiatry27, 53–57 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Burgess, S., Foley, C. N., Allara, E., Staley, J. R. & Howson, J. M. M. A robust and efficient method for Mendelian randomization with hundreds of genetic variants. Nat. Commun.11, 376 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Alkire, S. et al. Multidimensional Poverty Measurement and Analysis (Oxford Academic, 2015).
  • 15.Galobardes, B., Shaw, M., Lawlor, D. A., Lynch, J. W. & Smith, G. D. Indicators of socioeconomic position (part 2). J. Epidemiol. Community Health60, 95–101 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Galobardes, B., Shaw, M., Lawlor, D. A., Lynch, J. W. & Smith, G. D. Indicators of socioeconomic position (part 1). J. Epidemiol. Community Health60, 7–12 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Johnson, S. et al. Acute psychiatric care: approaches to increasing the range of services and improving access and quality of care. World Psychiatry21, 220–236 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Townsend, P., Phillimore, P. & Beattie, A. Health and Deprivation: Inequality and the North (Croom Helm Ltd, 1988).
  • 19.Hill, W. D. et al. Genome-wide analysis identifies molecular systems and 149 genetic loci associated with income. Nat. Commun.10, 5741 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Hill, W. D. et al. A combined analysis of genetically correlated traits identifies 187 loci and a role for neurogenesis and myelination in intelligence. Mol. Psychiatry24, 169–181 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Lee, J. Maternal economic well-being and mental health among young adult children: race/ethnicity. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health18, 5691 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Lin, B. & Okyere, M. A. Multidimensional energy poverty and mental health: micro-level evidence from Ghana. Int. J. Environ. Res. Public Health17, 6726 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Campbell, D. et al. Effects of depression on employment and social outcomes: a Mendelian randomisation study. J. Epidemiol. Community Health76, 563–571 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Zheng, X. et al. Modifiable risk factors of major depressive disorder: a Mendelian randomization study. PLoS ONE18, e0289419 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Liu, G., Liu, W., Zheng, X. & Li, J. The higher the household income, the lower the possibility of depression and anxiety disorder: evidence from a bidirectional Mendelian randomization study. Front. Psychiatry14, 1264174 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.van Rheenen, W., Peyrot, W. J., Schork, A. J., Lee, S. H. & Wray, N. R. Genetic correlations of polygenic disease traits: from theory to practice. Nat. Rev. Genet.20, 567–581 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Singh-Manoux, A., Ferrie, J. E., Lynch, J. W. & Marmot, M. The role of cognitive ability (intelligence) in explaining the association between socioeconomic position and health: evidence from the Whitehall II prospective cohort study. Am. J. Epidemiol.161, 831–839 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Batty, G. D., Deary, I. J. & Gottfredson, L. S. Premorbid (early life) IQ and later mortality risk: systematic review. Ann. Epidemiol.17, 278–288 (2007). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Batty, G. D. & Deary, I. J. Early life intelligence and adult health: associations, plausible mechanisms, and public health importance are emerging. Br. Med. J.329, 585–586 (2004). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Deary, I. J., Hill, W. D. & Gale, C. R. Intelligence, health and death. Nat. Hum. Behav.5, 416–430 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Pettersson, E., Larsson, H., D’Onofrio, B. M., Bölte, S. & Lichtenstein, P. The general factor of psychopathology: a comparison with the general factor of intelligence with respect to magnitude and predictive validity. World Psychiatry19, 206–213 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Han, E., Norton, E. C. & Stearns, S. C. Weight and wages: fat versus lean paychecks. Health Econ.18, 535–548 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Judge, T. A., Hurst, C. & Simon, L. S. Does it pay to be smart, attractive, or confident (or all three)? Relationships among general mental ability, physical attractiveness, core self-evaluations, and income. J. Appl. Psychol.94, 742–755 (2009). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Brouwers, E. P. M. Social stigma is an underestimated contributing factor to unemployment in people with mental illness or mental health issues: position paper and future directions. BMC Psychol.8, 36 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Abdellaoui, A. et al. Genetic correlates of social stratification in Great Britain. Nat. Hum. Behav.3, 1332–1342 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Morris, J. N., Donkin, A. J. M., Wonderling, D., Wilkinson, P. & Dowler, E. A. A minimum income for healthy living. J. Epidemiol. Community Health54, 885–889 (2000). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Evans, G. W. & Kantrowitz, E. Socioeconomic status and health: the potential role of environmental risk exposure. Annu. Rev. Public Health23, 303–331 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Evans, G. W. & English, K. The environment of poverty: multiple stressor exposure, psychophysiological stress, and socioemotional adjustment. Child Dev.73, 1238–1248 (2002). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 39.Montagud-Romero, S., Miñarro, J. & Rodríguez-Arias, M. Unravelling the neuroinflammatory mechanisms underlying the effects of social defeat stress on use of drugs of abuse. Curr. Top. Behav. Neurosci.54, 153–180 (2022). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 40.Mackenbach, J. P. et al. Socioeconomic inequalities in health in 22 European countries. N. Engl. J. Med.358, 2468–2481 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Raphael, D. et al. Researching income and income distribution as determinants of health in Canada: gaps between theoretical knowledge, research practice, and policy implementation. Health Policy72, 217–232 (2005). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Schorrocks, A., Davies, J. B. & Lluberas, R. Global Wealth Report 2020 (Credit Suisse Research Institute, 2020); https://www.credit-suisse.com/media/assets/corporate/docs/about-us/research/publications/global-wealth-report-2020-en.pdf
  • 43.The WHO Special Initiative for Mental Health (2019–2023): Universal Health Coverage for Mental Health (WHO). https://apps.who.int/iris/handle/10665/310981 Accessed 27 January 2023.
  • 44.Ferrari, A. J. et al. Global, regional, and national burden of 12 mental disorders in 204 countries and territories, 1990–2019: a systematic analysis for the Global Burden of Disease Study 2019. Lancet Psychiatry9, 137–150 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Zimmerman, A. et al. The impact of cash transfers on mental health in children and young people in low-income and middle-income countries: a systematic review and meta-analysis. BMJ Glob. Health6, e004661 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Bauer, A. et al. Integrating youth mental health into cash transfer programmes in response to the COVID-19 crisis in low-income and middle-income countries. Lancet Psychiatry8, 340–346 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Ohrnberger, J., Fichera, E., Sutton, M. & Anselmi, L. The effect of cash transfers on mental health—new evidence from South Africa. BMC Public Health20, 436 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Bradley, C. J. & Saunders, H. G. Impact of cash incentives for low-income individuals to seek a primary care visit on mental health outcomes: evidence from a randomized controlled trial. Soc. Sci. Med.265, 113515 (2020). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Denary, W. et al. Does rental assistance improve mental health? Insights from a longitudinal cohort study. Soc. Sci. Med.282, 114100 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Cavazos-Rehg, P. et al. Evaluating potential mediators for the impact of a family-based economic intervention (Suubi+Adherence) on the mental health of adolescents living with HIV in Uganda. Soc. Sci. Med.280, 113946 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.Cesarini, D., Lindqvist, E., Östling, R. & Wallace, B. Wealth, health, and child development: evidence from administrative data on Swedish lottery players. Q. J. Econ.131, 687–738 (2016). [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Micai, M. et al. Benefits and challenges of a personal budget for people with mental health conditions or intellectual disability: a systematic review. Front. Psychiatry13, 1733 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Ibrahim, F. et al. Microcredit as a public health initiative? Exploring mechanisms and pathways to health and wellbeing. Soc. Sci. Med.270, 113633 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.Fernald, L. C. H., Hamad, R., Karlan, D., Ozer, E. J. & Zinman, J. Small individual loans and mental health: a randomized controlled trial among South African adults. BMC Public Health8, 409 (2008). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Plagerson, S., Patel, V., Harpham, T., Kielmann, K. & Mathee, A. Does money matter for mental health? Evidence from the Child Support Grants in Johannesburg, South Africa. Glob. Public Health10.1080/17441692.2010.516267 (2010). [DOI] [PubMed]
  • 56.Batty, G. D., Der, G., Macintyre, S. & Deary, I. J. Does IQ explain socioeconomic inequalities in health? Evidence from a population based cohort study in the west of Scotland. Br. Med. J.332, 580–583 (2006). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Hill, W. D., Harris, S. E. & Deary, I. J. What genome-wide association studies reveal about the association between intelligence and mental health. Curr. Opin. Psychol.27, 25–30 (2019). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Anderson, E. L. et al. Education, intelligence and Alzheimer’s disease: evidence from a multivariable two-sample Mendelian randomization study. Int. J. Epidemiol.49, 1163–1172 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.Ritchie, S. J. & Tucker-Drob, E. M. How much does education improve intelligence? A meta-analysis. Psychol. Sci.10.1177/0956797618774253 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed]
  • 60.Davies, N. M. et al. Multivariable two-sample Mendelian randomization estimates of the effects of intelligence and education on health. eLife8, e43990 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Transforming Our World: The 2030 Agenda for Sustainable Development Preamble (United Nations, 2030).
  • 62.Davies, N. M., Holmes, M. V. & Davey Smith, G. Reading Mendelian randomisation studies: a guide, glossary, and checklist for clinicians. Br. Med. J.362, k601 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 63.Mills, M. C. & Rahal, C. A scientometric review of genome-wide association studies. Commun. Biol.2, 9 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 64.Brumpton, B. et al. Avoiding dynastic, assortative mating, and population stratification biases in Mendelian randomization through within-family analyses. Nat. Commun.11, 3519 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 65.Fry, A. et al. Comparison of sociodemographic and health-related characteristics of UK Biobank participants with those of the general population. Am. J. Epidemiol.186, 1026–1034 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 66.Gkatzionis, A. & Burgess, S. Contextualizing selection bias in Mendelian randomization: how bad is it likely to be? Int. J. Epidemiol.48, 691–701 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 67.Schoeler, T. et al. Participation bias in the UK Biobank distorts genetic associations and downstream analyses. Nat. Hum. Behav.7, 1216–1227 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 68.Jiang, T., Gill, D., Butterworth, A. S. & Burgess, S. An empirical investigation into the impact of winner’s curse on estimates from Mendelian randomization. Int. J. Epidemiol.52, 1209–1219 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 69.Pickrell, J. K. et al. Detection and interpretation of shared genetic influences on 42 human traits. Nat. Genet.48, 709–717 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 70.Hemani, G., Bowden, J. & Davey Smith, G. Evaluating the potential role of pleiotropy in Mendelian randomization studies. Hum. Mol. Genet.27, R195–R208 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 71.Sanderson, E., Davey Smith, G., Windmeijer, F. & Bowden, J. An examination of multivariable Mendelian randomization in the single-sample and two-sample summary data settings. Int. J. Epidemiol.48, 713–727 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 72.Burgess, S. & Thompson, S. G. Multivariable Mendelian randomization: the use of pleiotropic genetic variants to estimate causal effects. Am. J. Epidemiol.181, 251–260 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 73.Mbatchou, J. et al. Computationally efficient whole-genome regression for quantitative and binary traits. Nat. Genet.53, 1097–1103 (2021). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 74.Johnson, W., Nijenhuis, Jte & Bouchard, T. J. Still just 1 g: consistent results from five test batteries. Intelligence36, 81–95 (2008). [Google Scholar]
  • 75.Johnson, W., Bouchard, T. J., Krueger, R. F., McGue, M. & Gottesman, I. I. Just one g: consistent results from three test batteries. Intelligence32, 95–107 (2004). [Google Scholar]
  • 76.Kweon, H. et al. Genetic Fortune: Winning or Losing Education, Income, and Health (Tinbergen Institute, 2020).
  • 77.Grotzinger, A. D. et al. Genomic structural equation modelling provides insights into the multivariate genetic architecture of complex traits. Nat. Hum. Behav.3, 513–525 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 78.Bulik-Sullivan, B. et al. LD score regression distinguishes confounding from polygenicity in genome-wide association studies. Nat. Genet.47, 291–295 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 79.Demontis, D. et al. Genome-wide analyses of ADHD identify 27 risk loci, refine the genetic architecture and implicate several cognitive domains. Nat. Genet.55, 198–208 (2023). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 80.Watson, H. J. et al. Genome-wide association study identifies eight risk loci and implicates metabo-psychiatric origins for anorexia nervosa. Nat. Genet.51, 1207–1214 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 81.Otowa, T. et al. Meta-analysis of genome-wide association studies of anxiety disorders. Mol. Psychiatry21, 1391–1399 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 82.Grove, J. et al. Identification of common genetic risk variants for autism spectrum disorder. Nat. Genet.51, 431–444 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 83.Mullins, N. et al. Genome-wide association study of more than 40,000 bipolar disorder cases provides new insights into the underlying biology. Nat. Genet.53, 817–829 (2021). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 84.Howard, D. M. et al. Genome-wide meta-analysis of depression identifies 102 independent variants and highlights the importance of the prefrontal brain regions. Nat. Neurosci.22, 343–352 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 85.Arnold, P. D. et al. Revealing the complex genetic architecture of obsessive-compulsive disorder using meta-analysis. Mol. Psychiatry23, 1181–1188 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 86.Nievergelt, C. M. et al. International meta-analysis of PTSD genome-wide association studies identifies sex- and ancestry-specific genetic risk loci. Nat. Commun.10, 4558 (2019). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 87.Trubetskoy, V. et al. Mapping genomic loci implicates genes and synaptic biology in schizophrenia. Nature604, 502–508 (2022). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 88.Burgess, S., Davies, N. M. & Thompson, S. G. Bias due to participant overlap in two-sample Mendelian randomization. Genet. Epidemiol.40, 597–608 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 89.Lawlor, D. A., Harbord, R. M., Sterne, J. A. C., Timpson, N. & Smith, G. D. Mendelian randomization: using genes as instruments for making causal inferences in epidemiology. Stat. Med.27, 1133–1163 (2008). [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 90.Bowden, J., Davey Smith, G., Haycock, P. C. & Burgess, S. Consistent estimation in Mendelian randomization with some invalid instruments using a weighted median estimator. Genet. Epidemiol.40, 304–314 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 91.Bowden, J., Smith, G. D. & Burgess, S. Mendelian randomization with invalid instruments: effect estimation and bias detection through Egger regression. Int. J. Epidemiol.44, 512–525 (2015). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 92.Verbanck, M., Chen, C. Y., Neale, B. & Do, R. Detection of widespread horizontal pleiotropy in causal relationships inferred from Mendelian randomization between complex traits and diseases. Nat. Genet.50, 693–698 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 93.Morrison, J., Knoblauch, N., Marcus, J. H., Stephens, M. & He, X. Mendelian randomization accounting for correlated and uncorrelated pleiotropic effects using genome-wide summary statistics. Nat. Genet.52, 740–747 (2020). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 94.Hemani, G., Tilling, K. & Davey Smith, G. Orienting the causal relationship between imprecisely measured traits using GWAS summary data. PLoS Genet.13, e1007081 (2017). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 95.Bowden, J. et al. Assessing the suitability of summary data for two-sample Mendelian randomization analyses using MR-Egger regression: the role of the I2 statistic. Int. J. Epidemiol.45, 1961–1974 (2016). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 96.R Core Team. R: A Language and Environment for Statistical Computing, v.4.4.0 (R Foundation for Statistical Computing, 2024). https://www.R-project.org/
  • 97.Hemani, G. et al. The MR-Base platform supports systematic causal inference across the human phenome. eLife7, e34408 (2018). [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Associated Data

This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

Supplementary Materials

Supplementary Information (3.5MB, pdf)

Supplementary Figs. 1–133, Tables 1–33, Methods and Frequently Asked Questions (FAQ).

Reporting Summary (1.9MB, pdf)
Peer Review File (483KB, pdf)

Data Availability Statement

The UK Biobank data used in this study are available via the UK Biobank data access process (http://www.ukbiobank.ac.uk/register-apply/). The PGC data can be publicly accessed via https://pgc.unc.edu/for-researchers/download-results/. The OI data are publicly available and can be downloaded from https://osf.io/rg8sh/. The CA data were obtained from the author of the relevant publication, accessible at https://www.nature.com/articles/s41380-017-0001-5. The full GWAS summary statistics on HI, SD and poverty have been uploaded to the GWAS catalogue (https://www.ebi.ac.uk/gwas/deposition/submission/6572ff9a53c4ef000109d322; study accession IDs: GCST90302879, GCST90302880, GCST90302881, GCST90302882, GCST90302883, GCST90302884 and GCST90302885).

The code for replicating the analyses reported in this Article can be accessed at https://github.com/MattiaMarchi/Common-factor-GWAS–MR.


Articles from Nature Human Behaviour are provided here courtesy of Nature Publishing Group

RESOURCES