Skip to main content
PLOS Biology logoLink to PLOS Biology
. 2024 Sep 24;22(9):e3002772. doi: 10.1371/journal.pbio.3002772

Dopamine has no direct causal role in the formation of treatment expectations and placebo analgesia in humans

Angelika Kunkel 1,, Livia Asan 1,, Isabel Krüger 1, Clara Erfurt 1, Laura Ruhnau 1, Elif Buse Caliskan 1, Jana Hackert 1, Katja Wiech 1,2, Katharina Schmidt 1, Ulrike Bingel 1,*
Editor: Ben Seymour3
PMCID: PMC11421806  PMID: 39316644

Abstract

Dopamine-based reward and learning mechanisms have been suggested to contribute to placebo effects. However, the exact role of dopaminergic neurotransmission in their generation and maintenance is still unclear. This study aimed to shed light on the causal role of dopamine in establishing positive treatment expectations, as well as on the magnitude and duration of their effect on pain. To this end, we used an established placebo analgesia paradigm in combination with 2 opposing pharmacological modulations of dopaminergic tone, i.e., the dopamine antagonist sulpiride and the dopamine precursor L-dopa which were both applied in an experimental, double-blind, randomized, placebo-controlled trial with a between-subject design in N = 168 healthy volunteers. The study medication successfully altered dopaminergic tone during the conditioning procedure. Contrary to our hypotheses, the medication did not modulate the formation of positive treatment expectation and placebo analgesia tested 1 day later. Placebo analgesia was no longer detectable on day 8 after conditioning. Using a combined frequentist and Bayesian approach, our data provide strong evidence against a direct dopaminergic influence on the generation and maintenance of placebo effects. Further exploration of the neurochemical mechanisms underlying placebo analgesia remains paramount in the quest to exploit these effects for optimal treatment outcomes.

Trial registration: ClinicalTrials.gov German Clinical Trials Register, ID: DRKS00029366, https://drks.de/search/en/trial/DRKS00029366.


Dopamine signalling has been linked to pain analgesia and placebo effects but whether there is causal relationship remains unclear. This pre-registered study shows that enhancing dopaminergic tone does not alter the generation of positive treatment expectations or placebo analgesia.

Introduction

Placebo effects are an individual’s psychophysiological response to contextual information and associated expectations to treatments that are physically and pharmacologically inert. The strength of placebo effects varies considerably among and within individuals depending on contextual factors, prior experiences of treatment benefit, and expectations regarding the treatment [1,2]. The effects and mechanisms of expectation have been best characterized in the field of experimental pain and placebo analgesia (PA), i.e., the pain relief following the administration of an inert treatment and/or the expectation that a potent analgesic substance is being administered [3,4]. Experimentally, this expectation is typically generated by combining verbal suggestions and learning processes by surreptitiously lowering pain stimulus intensities during a conditioning phase.

The brain systems and neurochemical pathways underlying PA have been studied extensively over the past 2 decades. Converging evidence from functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), electroencephalography (EEG), and positron emission tomography (PET) studies indicates that PA is associated with changes in nociceptive processing, including alterations at the level of the spinal cord [5], thalamus, and cortical brain areas related to nociception and pain [68]. However, the effect sizes of this influence on brain areas implicated in nociception are considerably smaller than their underlying behavioral effects [9]. This points towards processes other than inhibition of bottom-up nociceptive signaling and most likely involves changes in affective and evaluative mechanisms [10,11]. In line with this view, PA has been linked to both the top-down activation of descending pain modulatory pathways but also intracortical mechanisms, driven by limbic and paralimbic regions.

At the neurochemical level, the endogenous opioid system has been shown to play a critical role in PA, as indicated by behavioral and fMRI studies using the opioid antagonist naloxone and PET studies using in vivo receptor binding approaches with opioidergic ligands (for review, see [7]). However, neither the engagement of descending pain modulatory pathways nor the involvement of opioids can so far fully explain PA.

Instead, there is growing evidence for a role of non-opioidergic neurotransmitter systems in PA [1214]. In particular, dopamine-based reward mechanisms appear to contribute to placebo responses [13,1518]. A pioneering study using PET imaging found that dopamine (DA) signaling in the nucleus accumbens (NAc) increased during the anticipation of analgesia following the administration of a placebo treatment. The signal strength was proportional to the individuals’ PA response. Interestingly, PA also correlated positively with NAc activation during anticipation of monetary reward in the same individuals. Further, interindividual differences in PA have been linked to dopamine-related personality traits and gray matter density in the ventral striatum [19]. These and related findings suggest a key role for the mesolimbic reward system in PA.

First efforts to elucidate the significance of dopaminergic signaling to analgesia have targeted dopaminergic neurotransmission pharmacologically during the test phase of PA paradigms. We demonstrated that blocking D2/D3 receptor activity with the antagonist haloperidol specifically reduced placebo-related activity in the striatum but did not affect the magnitude of PA at the behavioral level [14]. Similarly, in a study with patients suffering from neuropathic pain, PA was influenced neither by haloperidol nor the DA precursor L-dopa [20]. While the findings of both studies render a direct analgesic role of dopamine unlikely, they suggest an involvement of the dopaminergic system in other processes inherently linked to PA, such as the acquisition of positive treatment expectation induced by positive prior treatment experiences, or the inherent reward associated with experiencing pain relief. In this view, DA may affect PA through a modulatory influence during the learning phase of PA. We previously tested if increasing dopaminergic tone using L-dopa during the conditioning of PA could boost PA in the later test phase [10]. Although the study hints at an enhancing effect of L-dopa on the acquisition of conditioned PA, particularly in women, this finding was difficult to interpret as our experimental manipulation failed to induce robust PA independent of the dopaminergic manipulation. Strikingly, recent evidence from clinical populations suggests that the intake of L-dopa together with the non-steroidal anti-inflammatory drug naproxen can serve to prevent the transition from acute to chronic back pain [21]. The mechanistic basis of this effect could be rooted in the same principle of enhancing the reward system during the experience of analgesia, which we propose for conditioned PA.

Taken together, despite clear evidence for the involvement of mesolimbic DA signaling in PA, its exact functional role and contribution to the development of positive treatment expectation and PA is still poorly understood. Deciphering the neuropharmacological foundations of PA could be of direct clinical relevance, as it enables the development of active DA-enhancing interventions to boost placebo components in pain treatments or to reduce placebo effects via DA inhibition in clinical randomized controlled trials, where they hamper the assay sensitivity to detect novel therapeutic targets.

To investigate the general contribution of DA in the acquisition of positive treatment expectation and its subsequent effect on pain in a proof-of-concept manner, we employed an established experimental paradigm of conditioned PA in combination with 2 pharmacological manipulations of dopaminergic signaling in the brain using a randomized, placebo-controlled, double-blind design. We allocated N = 168 healthy participants to either receive a single dose of the D2 receptor antagonist sulpiride, the DA precursor L-dopa, or an inactive control prior to the conditioning phase of the PA paradigm. Expectation of analgesia and its effect on experimental heat pain was assessed in the conditioning session and in 2 test sessions on days 2 and 8.

For statistical analyses, we applied classic frequentist statistics as well as Bayesian analyses. This hybrid approach allowed us to present well-known frequentist, p-value–based metrics to compare the results with those of previous studies and meta-analyses, and to additionally quantify the evidence using the framework of Bayesian inference, which is increasingly embraced as a method of reporting evidence in the field of neuroscience. Particularly, we implemented Bayesian parameter effect analyses to complement frequentist results for parameter significance and effect size. Further, Bayesian hypothesis testing enabled us to rate the strength of evidence in favor of or against our proposed hypotheses on a continuous scale by using the Bayes factor (BF), which allowed us to discriminate evidence of absence from absence of evidence. Combining both frequentist and Bayesian approaches is recommended as a pragmatic and powerful way to report and communicate scientific evidence [22].

In this study, we focused on 3 hypotheses (1 main and 2 exploratory hypotheses):

  • Main hypothesis (1): Pharmacologic manipulation of DA signaling during the experience of pain relief associated with the placebo treatment in the conditioning session modulates the magnitude of PA. To address this main hypothesis, we checked for interaction effects between medication (L-dopa versus sulpiride versus inactive pill) and our experimental condition (placebo cream versus control cream) in test session 1 (day 2).

  • Exploratory hypothesis (2): (a) PA persists up to day 8, as indicated by a significant main effect of our experimental condition (placebo cream versus control cream) on day 8. (b) Pharmacological manipulation of DA signaling during conditioning affects the persistence of PA on day 8. In this analysis, we tested the longevity of the PA in our experiment and probe putative long-term effects of the pharmacological dopaminergic modulation applied during conditioning.

  • Exploratory hypothesis (3): DA-manipulation during conditioning alters the establishment of treatment expectation. We propose that treatment expectation is formed in a DA-dependent way during the experience of a positive treatment effect (conditioning session). We thus hypothesize that the (anti)dopaminergic medication influences the acquisition of treatment expectation during the conditioning session as measured by the change in expectation ratings before conditioning and before the test session of PA (see Fig 1B).

Fig 1.

Fig 1

(A) Upper panel. The experiment takes place on 3 days with the conditioning on day 1, test session 1 on day 2, and test session 2 on day 8. Participants are treated with 2 identical creams with the PLC introduced as “analgesic cream” and the CTR as inactive sensory control cream. The location of the control (gray area) and placebo (blue area) site on the left volar forearm is pseudo-randomized. Painful heat stimulus intensity levels are individualized to correspond to target ratings of 40, 60, and 80 on a 101-point VAS with endpoints marked not painful and unbearably painful. Lower panel. Trial timing. Each trial consists of 5 phases: ITI with the flanker task, anticipation phase, pain stimulus, short pause, and pain intensity rating. The ITI has a random duration of 15–25 s. The anticipation phase begins when the white crosshair turns red, indicating that a painful stimulation is about to follow. After a variable delay time, the painful heat stimulus (duration 20 s) is administered, and 3–7 s after the end of the heat stimulation participants provide pain intensity ratings using a VAS. (B) Experimental schedule. Double-blind and random allocation of participants to one of the 3 medication groups: sulpiride (SUL), L-dopa (DOPA), or inactive control (INA). Differential pharmacokinetic profiles require a staggered pill intake at 2 different time points where the SUL group takes an active pill (sulpiride 400 mg) at time point one, and the DOPA group takes an active pill (levodopa/carbidopa 100/25 mg) at time point 2. For the INA group, pills at both time points are inactive. Treatment expectation at 4 different time points (EXPECT), and efficiency ratings of the placebo manipulation (EFFECT) are measured at three time points via the GEEE. CTR, control; ITI, inter-trial interval; PLC, placebo; VAS, visual analogue scale.

Methods

Ethics and participants

The study protocol was approved by the ethics committee of the University Hospital Essen (19-8858-BO), and the study was preregistered at the German Clinical Trials Register (https://www.drks.de, ID: DRKS00029366) before study start. All experiments were conducted in accordance with the Declaration of Helsinki. Healthy participants aged 18 to 40 years were recruited and were told that the study investigates the influence of a DA-targeted pharmacological intervention on pain processing and modulation.

At an initial, standardized structured telephone interview, participants were screened for exclusion criteria. Exclusion criteria were the presence of any acute or chronic somatic or mental diseases, including several chronic pain conditions, abuse of illegal substances or alcohol, regular use of medication (except thyroid medication, allergy medication, occasional use of over-the-counter analgesics, and contraception), hypersensitivity, contraindications, allergy, or intolerance to any ingredients of the study medication; pregnancy or breastfeeding; and/or insufficient German language proficiency. Participants must not have taken part in studies using investigational drugs within the past 3 months and shall refrain from eating 2 h prior to the experimental sessions. Written voluntary informed consent was obtained before any experimental procedures began. Participants were tested for recent drug use (including THC, opiates, and amphetamines) using a commercially available urine test (Diagnostik Nord, Schwerin, Germany). Women were further tested for pregnancy using a urine pregnancy test. Participants received 220 € as compensation for study participation.

Sampling plan

The power calculation of this study was aimed to best address our main hypothesis 1 for frequentist statistics. We determined the sample size to detect even small effects of our pharmacological modulation on PA. The calculated sample size was simultaneously utilized for a fixed-N-approach for Bayesian effect analysis and hypothesis tests [23].

Previous studies have reported medium to large effects of pharmacological interventions that acted directly on PA. For example, pharmacologically blocking opioidergic transmission with naloxone exhibited a large effect in decreasing PA (d = 0.69 [6]). In regard to the DA system, the work from Scott and colleagues [13] suggests a medium to large contribution of dopaminergic transmission to the formation of PA, since DA activity in the NAc explained PA well (r = 0.5, R2 = 25%). Yet, it remains to be investigated whether an active pharmacologic manipulation during the conditioning phase can exert similarly large effects on PA. The sample size for our study is, therefore, powered to even detect a small effect (Cohen’s d of 0.3) of medication on PA. Importantly, our study design is especially suited to detect any dopamine-dependent modulatory effects, since L-dopa and sulpiride act on dopaminergic transmission in opposing directions, expanding the range of possible observable effects (boosting versus blocking) on PA.

The total sample size needed to detect a significant effect in a repeated-measures analyses of variance (rmANOVA) with f = 0.15 (corresponding to Cohen’s d = 0.3), an alpha-error probability of 5% (p = 0.05), and a power of 1-ß = 0.9 is N = 144 (calculated with G*Power [24]). Assuming a reasonable drop-out-rate of about approximately 10%, we aimed for a total of N = 165 participants (55 per group).

The power of the remaining exploratory hypothesis tests is dependent on this predetermined sample size.

Design

The study employed a mixed, randomized, controlled, double-blind factorial group design with the between-subject factor medication (L-dopa versus sulpiride versus inactive pill) and the within-subject factors experimental condition (placebo versus control). The study included a prior intervention (conditioning procedure) and 2 assessment time points of PA (day 2 and day 8). Participants were randomly assigned to one of the 3 medication groups according to a randomized list created with R version 4.3.2 (R Core Team; 2023).

Experimental procedure

For a comprehensive overview about the experimental procedure, see Fig 1.

We used a well-established placebo heat pain paradigm with a prior conditioning session [6,8,2527]. Experiments took place on 3 separate days, with the conditioning session on day 1, the first test session on day 2, and the second test session on day 8. Fig 1B gives a detailed overview of the experimental schedule. The placebo treatment consisted of a skin cream that was introduced to participants as a local analgesic containing the pharmacologically active agent lidocaine, while it was de facto inert (hidden placebo). A chemically identical cream was described to the participants as a “control cream” that served as inactive sensory control. Heat pain stimuli were delivered using an ATS-thermode (30 × 30 mm, Pathway System, Medoc, Israel).

Day 1. At the first visit, participants were told that (a) the study investigates the influence of established medications that intervene with the brain’s dopaminergic system on the efficiency of an established local analgesic cream; (b) the perceived pain intensity to painful heat stimuli is measured on skin that has been treated with the analgesic cream (= actual deceptive placebo condition) or a control cream (= control condition); and (c) double-blind allocation to one of the 3 medication groups, L-dopa (levodopa/carbidopa 100/25 mg), sulpiride (sulpiride 400 mg), or inactive pill (99.5% mannitol, 0.5% siliciumdioxide) is performed randomly at equal chances for each group (33.3%) (for literal translation of the verbal placebo manipulation, see supplementary information S1 Text). L-dopa is a naturally occurring dopamine precursor that can pass the blood–brain barrier to get metabolized into active DA in the brain. It is well known as a treatment for Parkinson’s disease. In contrast, the D2 receptor antagonist sulpiride, an antipsychotic agent used for treating psychosis and schizophrenia, was applied to block dopaminergic neurotransmission. The drug dosages used in this study have proven in the past to effectively alter dopamine-related behavior by increasing dopaminergic tone after administration of 100 mg of L-dopa [2830] or decreasing dopaminergic signaling with 400 mg sulpiride [12,3133].

The medication intake was timed for plasma drug levels to peak right at the beginning of the conditioning phase. Due to the differential pharmacokinetic profiles, this requires a staggered pill intake at 2 different time points as depicted in Fig 1B (sulpiride: maximum plasma concentration after 180 min; L-dopa after 60 min [3436]). Pills were prepared by an unblinded third person who was not involved in the study according to the randomization list and were handed out in non-transparent cups with lids in order to maintain double-blinding for the experimenter and participant. Ratings for treatment expectation (EXPECT) concerning pain perception via the Generic Rating for Treatment pre-Experiences, Treatment Expectations, and Treatment Effects Scale (GEEE, a screening tool that allowed for the general assessment and quantification of patients’ treatment expectations and their effects on clinical outcomes [37]), state variables of anxiety and depression (State-Trait-Anxiety-Depression-Inventory State (STADI State) [38]), and symptoms of drug side effects (Generic Assessment of Side Effects in Clinical Trials (GASE) [39]) were assessed once before and once after medication intake, prior to conditioning. Since this study was part of a large collaborative research effort investigating treatment expectation in different experimental settings (https://treatment-expectation.de/en/), participants were asked to complete a battery of various additional psychological questionnaires at the beginning of day 1 after the briefing procedure. These data were not included for primary analyses of this study but were used for exploratory investigations and/or as control variables. These questionnaires consisted of state and trait psychological questionnaires and pain-related cognitions as well as pretreatment experiences (see supplementary information S1 Text for details and references). All questionnaires were presented and assessed using the open-source survey tool LimeSurvey (LimeSurvey GmbH, Hamburg, Germany).

After the second pill intake, when both drugs were already expected to rise to their peak and effective plasma concentration, participants underwent a calibration procedure on one of 5 pseudo-randomized skin locations on the left volar forearm, each outlined by a standardized marker (Fig 1A) and used for later conditioning and testing. First, the individual heat pain threshold was determined using the method of limits [14,40]. Subsequently, one practice trial plus a pseudo-random sequence of 20 heat pain stimuli with varying temperatures (−1.5 °C–+3.5 °C around the participants’ individual heat pain threshold) were applied as tonic heat pain stimuli for 20 s each. These stimuli were each rated for pain intensity on a 0 to 100 visual analogue scale (VAS) with anchors (0 = not painful to 100 = unbearably painful). Subsequently, individual target temperatures were calculated using linear regression: VAS 40 for light pain, VAS 60 for moderate pain, and VAS 80 for strong pain. VAS scales and crosshairs during pain stimulation were presented on a monitor and behavioral data were recorded using Presentation software (Version 22.0, Neurobehavioral Systems, Berkeley, California, USA). Participants were instructed not to talk during the experimental session and to maintain fixation at the crosshair at the center of the screen. Although previous studies showed no effect of the augmentation or blockade of dopaminergic signaling on heat [14,20,41], and the time point of the calibration procedure sufficiently accounts and controls for any acute effect of the pharmacological modulations of thermal pain sensitivity, we assessed thermal pain thresholds using the method of limits [14,40] prior to the pharmacological modulation and immediately before the calibration session to allow for a direct assessment of drug effects on pain sensitivity.

Next, the placebo and control creams were applied on 2 skin locations that were distinct from the site that had been used for the calibration. The site allocation was pseudo-randomized across participants. A subsequent waiting period of 20 min was proclaimed to serve as the time for the analgesic cream to take effect. For conditioning, blocks of 15 heat pain stimuli were applied each to the control (Fig 1A, outlined in gray) and the placebo (outlined in blue) site. To induce the experience of a treatment effect, a lower temperature, corresponding to an individual pain intensity of VAS 40, was applied to the placebo site, mimicking a potent analgesic effect of the cream, while an intensity of VAS 80 was used at the control site. Participants were unaware of this manipulation as they were told that the same temperature is used for each stimulus and location. Each stimulus was rated on the VAS right after stimulus presentation at the end of every trial (see Fig 1A for details on the trial timing). Prior to each block, participants rated their current state of arousal and anxiety. Block order was pseudo-randomized.

At the end of day 1, efficiency rating of the placebo cream (EFFECT) and the Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) [42] was obtained and a blood sample for determining serum concentration of prolactin (Central Laboratory, Department of Research and Education, University Hospital Essen) and L-dopa (MVZ Dr. Eberhard & Partner Dortmund GbR) were taken to confirm the efficiency of the pharmacological manipulation. We decided to validate effective modulation of dopaminergic tone by assessing common physiological measures of modulation. To monitor an effective blockade of D2 receptors by sulpiride, we exploited the D2 receptor-dependent release of the protein prolactin in the pituitary [43]. Inhibition of D2 receptors in the pituitary leads to a quick surge in prolactin serum levels. This has been shown for a single-dose intake of sulpiride as well as other antipsychotic drugs that target the D2 receptor. For the L-dopa group, we determined L-dopa in the blood, as we and others did before to check effective serum levels for pharmacologic manipulation [10,30].

Days 2 and 8. In the test sessions, PA effects were tested. The placebo cream and the control cream were applied on the 2 skin locations that were distinct from the site that has been used for the calibration and conditioning and remained on the skin for 20 min. STADI State and EXPECT for the analgesic effect were obtained. The first heat stimulus presented in a session is often rated as disproportionately painful due to the novelty of the stimulus, which may elicit a startle response that aggravates pain perception. To avoid systematic errors and to prevent a compromised credibility of placebo treatment caused by the heightened sensibility to the first stimulus, we mimicked a repeated heat pain threshold determination on both sites, with a temperature rise time of 1.0 °C/s on the control site and 0.7 °C/s on the placebo site in order to keep the illusion of weaker perceived pain at the treated site [14]. Then, the test session was performed in blocks of 14 trials per condition in pseudo-randomized order with heat stimuli of 20 s duration. While participants were told that the same temperatures as during conditioning were applied, we in fact stimulated at a fixed temperature corresponding to VAS 60 in both conditions. Again, stimuli were rated for pain intensity after each trial. At the end of the test sessions, an EFFECT rating was obtained.

Analysis plan

All proposed hypotheses were tested using frequentist statistics (F-statistics of 3 × 2 rmANOVA, stating p-values). The interpretation of our results was determined by frequentist statistics. To complement frequentist analyses and expand interpretability of results, Bayesian metrics were calculated and reported (BF for parameter inclusion, estimated marginal means of posterior distributions for parameters and contrasts, credible intervals, BF for hypothesis testing). All analyses were conducted using R. Successful induction of PA is indicated by a main effect of experimental condition, with lower pain ratings for the placebo than the control condition. Details can be inspected in the Design Table (Table 1).

Table 1. Design table.

Question Hypothesis Sampling plan (e.g., power analysis) Analysis plan Interpretation given to different outcomes Observed outcome
(1) Does DA signaling during the experience of positive treatment effects modulate PA? H1. Pharmacologic manipulation of DA signaling during the experience of pain relief associated with the placebo treatment in the conditioning session modulates PA. Sample size estimation was calculated to meet requirements for sufficient power in frequentist analysis. At an expected effect size of pharmacological manipulation (Cohen’s d of 0.3), the total sample size recommended for rmANOVA in G*Power with an alpha error probability of 0.05 and a power of 1-ß = 0.9 is n = 144. Considering a dropout rate of approximately 10%, we aimed to include a total of N = 165 subjects (55 per group).
Bayesian Inference was conducted using the fixed-n-approach of N = 165 as determined by frequentist sample size estimation. This number is deemed sufficiently large to generate a meaningful Bayes Factor to inform about the strength of evidence for and against our hypothesis.
Frequentist analysis:
 Two-way rmANOVA with medication group (sulpiride vs. L-dopa vs. inactive pill) and experimental condition (placebo vs. control) to compare pain ratings on day 2.
Bayesian Analysis:
Bayesian repeated-measures ANOVA comparing models with medication group (sulpiride vs. L-dopa vs. inactive pill) and experimental condition (placebo vs. control) to define the evidence for or against the inclusion of model terms to model pain ratings on day 2 (BFincl). JASP’s default priors were used (Cauchy distribution with r scale for fixed effects = 0.5 and r scale for random effects = 1).
Frequentist analysis:
An F-test determined significance of variance explained by the specified factors in the ANOVA. For the interaction effect:
  - p > 0.05: reject H1
  - p < 0.05: reject H0.
 Post hoc Bonferroni–Holm corrections were applied for multiplicity-adjusted pairwise comparisons if F-statistic yields significant results.
Effect sizes were quantified using partial eta squared ηp2:
  - ηp2 = 0.01: small effect
  - ηp2 = 0.06: medium effect
  - ηp2 = 0.14: large effect
Bayesian Analysis:
BFincl
  - >100: Extreme evidence for inclusion/H1.
  - 30–100: Very strong evidence for inclusion/H1
  - 10–30: Strong evidence for inclusion/H1
  - 3–10: Moderate evidence for inclusion/H1
  - 1–3: Anecdotal evidence for inclusion/H1
  - 1/3–1: Anecdotal evidence against inclusion/for H0
  - 1/10–1/3: Moderate evidence against inclusion/for H0
  - 1/30–1/10: Strong evidence against inclusion/for H0
  - 1/100–1/30: Very strong evidence against inclusion/for H0
  - <1/100: Extreme evidence against inclusion/for H0
Frequentist analysis:
 Significant main effect of experimental condition (F(1,151) = 8.29, p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.05)
No main effect of medication group (F(2,151) = 0.64, p = 0.53, ηp2 = 0.01)
No interaction between medication and experimental condition (F(2,151) = 0.35, p = 0.71, ηp2 < 0.01).
  - p > 0.05: H1 is disconfirmed
Bayesian analysis:
The BFincl of experimental condition is 4.32.
The BFincl of medication group is 0.17.
The BFincl of the interaction between experimental condition and medication group is 0.06.
  - BF = 1/30–1/10: Strong evidence against inclusion/for H0
(2) Does DA signaling during the experience of pain relief influence the persistence of PA? H2. Pharmacologic manipulation of DA signaling during conditioning affects the persistence of PA up to 6 days after test session 1 (day 8). The sample size for this analysis was determined by the analysis of H1. Frequentist analysis:
 Two-way rmANOVA with medication group (sulpiride vs. L-dopa vs. inactive pill) and experimental condition (placebo vs. control) to compare pain ratings on day 8.
Bayesian analysis:
Bayesian repeated-measures ANOVA comparing models with medication group (sulpiride vs. L-dopa vs. inactive pill) and experimental condition (placebo vs. control) to define the evidence for or against the inclusion of model terms to model pain ratings on day 8 (BFincl). JASP’s default priors were used (Cauchy distribution with r scale for fixed effects = 0.5 and r scale for random effects = 1).
The interpretation of different outcomes relies on the same specifications as shown for H1. Trend for effect of experimental condition (F(1,150) = 3.19, p = 0.08, ηp2 = 0.02)
No main effect of medication group (F(2,150) = 0.33, p = 0.71, ηp2< 0.01).
No interaction between medication and experimental condition (F(2,151) = 1.05, p = 0.35, ηp2 = 0.01).
  - p > 0.05: H2 is disconfirmed
Bayesian analysis:
The BFincl of experimental condition is 0.38.
The BFincl of medication group is 0.11.
The BFincl of the interaction between experimental condition and medication group is 0.04
  - BF = 1/30–1/10: Strong evidence against inclusion/for H0
(3) Does DA signaling during the experience of pain relief alter treatment expectation? H3. DA-manipulation during conditioning differentially alters the establishment of treatment expectation. The sample size for this analysis was determined by the analysis of H1. Frequentist analysis:
 A two-way rmANOVA with medication group (sulpiride vs. L-dopa vs. inactive pill) and rating time point (pre conditioning vs. post conditioning) to compare the EXPECT scores between groups.
Bayesian analysis:
Bayesian repeated-measures ANOVA comparing models with medication group (sulpiride vs. L-dopa vs. inactive pill) and rating time point (pre conditioning vs. post conditioning) to define the evidence for or against the inclusion of model terms to model EXPECT scores (BFincl). JASP’s default priors were used (Cauchy distribution with r scale for fixed effects = 0.5 and r scale for random effects = 1).
The interpretation of different outcomes relies on the same specifications as shown for H1. Significant main effect of experimental condition (F(1,149) = 20.11, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.12).
No main effect of medication group (F(2,149) = 1.44, p = 0.24, ηp2 = 0.02).
No interaction between medication and experimental condition (F(2,149) = 0.38, p = 0.68, ηp2 < 0.01).
  - p > 0.05: H3 is disconfirmed
Bayesian analysis:
The BFincl of rating time point is 512.31.
The BFincl of medication group is 0.19.
The BFincl of the interaction between rating time point and medication group is 0.07
  - BF = 1/30–1/10: Strong evidence against inclusion/for H0

Frequentist analysis

Hypothesis 1. To examine the main research question of whether DA signaling during the experience of pain relief influences the establishment of PA, a mixed rmANOVA with the factors medication (L-dopa versus sulpiride versus inactive pill) and experimental condition (placebo versus control) was fitted to compare the dependent variable (pain ratings during test session 1 on day 2) across the medication groups. To address this main hypothesis, we checked for interaction effects between the medication and the experimental condition (see Fig 2). A significant interaction effect was followed by post hoc tests.

Fig 2. Hypothesis 1.

Fig 2

Group design (A) and potential effects on pain ratings of day 2 for the main hypothesis 1 (B).

Hypothesis 2. (a) To test the hypothesis that PA persists up to day 8, we examined the main effect of experimental condition on the dependent variable, i.e., pain ratings during test session 2 on day 8. (b) The influence of dopamine signaling during the conditioning phase on the persistence of PA up to day 8 is expressed by the interaction of the factors medication (L-dopa versus sulpiride versus inactive pill) and experimental condition (placebo versus control), parallel to the model of hypothesis 1 (see Fig 3).

Fig 3. Hypothesis 2a and 2b.

Fig 3

Group design (A) and hypothesized effects on pain ratings of day 8 for hypothesis 2a and 2b (B).

Hypothesis 3. To examine the hypothesis that treatment expectation is influenced by medication, we fitted a mixed 3 × 2 rmANOVA with the factors medication (L-dopa versus sulpiride versus inactive pill) and rating time point in respect to the pharmacological intervention (pre conditioning versus post conditioning). The dependent variable treatment expectation corresponds to the EXPECT score and was measured just before the conditioning session on day 1 and just before the test session on day 2 (see Fig 4).

Fig 4. Hypothesis 3.

Fig 4

Group design (A) and hypothesized effects on EXPECT ratings before and after the conditioning session (B).

These 3 hypotheses were followed up by further exploratory analyses, e.g., regarding the temporal dynamics of effects, the potential modulatory influences exerted by the medication over time, as well as changes in expectation.

For all frequentist analyses, within- and between-group differences were tested at a two-tailed alpha level of 0.05 with Bonferroni–Holm corrections for post hoc multiple comparisons. Effect sizes were calculated using partial eta squared p2).

Bayesian inference

The results of the rmANOVAs were complemented with Bayesian statistics by Bayesian hierarchical regression modeling and calculating the inclusion Bayes factor (BFincl), which states the evidence for or against the inclusion of the different predictors (inclusion BF, BFincl) to model pain ratings. Effect sizes of parameters and contrast posteriors are given as estimated marginal means and credible intervals (based on equal-tailed interval). In addition, Bayesian hypothesis testing is conducted to quantify the evidence in favor of or against the proposed hypotheses (BF of the alternative hypothesis over the point null hypothesis, BF10).

Hypothesis 1. To address Hypothesis 1, 5 candidate models that include the analysis of varying main effects or interactions will be compared to explain measured pain ratings in test session 1 on day 2:

  1. medication

  2. experimental condition

  3. medication + experimental condition

  4. medication + experimental condition + medication: experimental condition

  5. null model.

BFincl for each main effect and interaction effect were calculated. Since pharmacologic agents that enhance or block dopaminergic signaling have not shown any direct effects on perceived intensity of acute pain in previous studies [14,20], we deem the model with medication as the only fixed effect (model 1) as less likely than the others. Hence, prior probabilities for best model performance were defined to be (1) 0.1; (2) 0.225; (3) 0.225; (4) 0.225; and (5) 0.225.

Hypothesis 2. Five candidate models were compared in their ability to explain pain ratings in test session 2 on day 8. BFincl for each main effect and interaction effect will be calculated. Prior probabilities of the different models were equally distributed (0.2 each).

  1. medication

  2. experimental condition

  3. medication + experimental condition

  4. medication + experimental condition + medication: experimental condition

  5. null model.

Hypothesis 3. We model EXPECT scores and again construct 5 competing models:

  1. medication

  2. rating time point

  3. rating time point + medication

  4. rating time point + medication + rating time point: medication

  5. null model.

Prior probabilities of the different models were equally distributed (0.2 each).

In all Bayesian models, subject number was included as a random effect term with varying intercept to account for intraindividual correlation of within-subject measurements and to reduce variation. Parameter priors were broad normal distributions with μ = 0 and σ = 100, expressing little prior knowledge about direction and magnitude of effects. The models that explain the data best for each hypothesis underwent diagnostic tests including analysis of chains convergence (Ȓ), chains autocorrelation, and effective sample size. Pairwise comparisons were conducted by computing contrasts on parameter posteriors. All parameter and contrast posterior statistics will be stated with estimated marginal mean and 95% credible intervals (equal-tailed interval). Lastly, Bayesian hypothesis testing quantified the evidence for our hypotheses against the point null hypotheses, expressed as the BF of the alternative hypothesis, H1, over the null hypothesis, H0 (BF10). For semantic valuation of strength of evidence given by BF10, we use the commonly applied adjusted Jeffrey’s Scale of Evidence [44] for the interpretation of BF: BF10 > 100: Extreme evidence for H1. BF10 = 30–100: Very strong evidence for H1. BF10 = 10–30: Strong evidence for H1. BF10 = 3–10: Moderate evidence for H1. BF10 = 1–3: Anecdotal evidence for H1. BF10 = 1/3–1: Anecdotal evidence for H0. BF10 = 1/10–1/3: Moderate evidence for H0. BF10 = 1/30–1/10: Strong evidence for H0. BF10 = 1/100–1/30: very strong evidence for H0. BF10 < 1/100: Extreme evidence for H0.

Results

Results are reported as mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM), unless stated otherwise. Statistics related to the hypotheses are explained in detail in the text and visualized in figures, while the descriptive presentation of the data collected on each study day is presented in tables and supplemented with ANOVAs testing the explanation of variance by the factor of medication to illustrate group differences. Note that as these ANOVA statistics are used to describe our data set and do not represent confirmatory tests, uncorrected p-values are reported. They were not preregistered in the Stage 1 protocol of this registered report.

Since the preparation of our Stage 1 manuscript, the JASP software (JASP Team, 2024, version 0.18.3) for Bayesian analysis has gained popularity in psychology and neuroscience due to its user-friendly interface and accurate Bayesian techniques. In choosing JASP, we aimed to improve the transparency, comprehensibility, and reproducibility of our Bayesian analyses. JASP’s ability to perform a Bayesian ANOVA and its efficient computation of model-averaged results, including inclusion Bayes factors and marginal posterior distributions, matched our analytical objectives. To accommodate JASP, we slightly modified our original analysis plan. As preregistered, we evaluated the predictive performance of 5 models, from simple to complex, to calculate the posterior probability of the inclusion of model terms (BFincl) to quantify the evidence for or against each of our 3 hypotheses (see “Bayesian inference” of Stage 1). We followed JASP’s default priors (Cauchy distribution with r scale for fixed effects = 0.5 and r scale for random effects = 1) for parameter estimation, reflecting minimal prior assumptions. As originally intended, we report inclusion Bayes factors (BFincl) and estimated marginal means and 95% credible intervals of model-averaged posteriors.

Sample description

We enrolled N = 168 participants in our study (see Table 2 for participant characteristics), and 14 participants (8.3%) had to be excluded due to protocol violations or disruption during testing: abnormal ECG: 2, positive for SARS-CoV2: 2, positive drug test: 3, prior medication intake: 1, non-compliance to fasting rule: 1, distraction by nearby construction noise: 3, technical issues: 1, recent participation in another study on pain and placebo: 1. The final sample size was n = 154. Except for 1 participant (group SUL, male) who missed day 8, all participants completed the entire experimental schedule.

Table 2. Participant characteristics.

Group: all INA DOPA SUL Group difference
Enrolled 168 55 56 58 -
Final sample 154 51 53 50 (49 completed day 8) -
Sex (% female) 57.8% 51.0% 58.5% 64.0% (65.3% on day 8) -
Age (in years) 24.8 ± 0.3 25.4 ± 0.5 24.8 ± 0.6 24.1 ± 0.4 F = 1.60, p = 0.21, ηp2 = 0.02
BAS (Behavioral Activation System)-Reward 16.3 ± 0.2 16.4 ± 0.3 16.0 ± 0.3 16.4 ± 0.3 F = 0.73, p = 0.47, ηp2 = 0.01
BAS-Drive 12.5 ± 0.2 12.5 ± 0.3 12.5 ± 0.2 12.5 ± 0.3 F = 0.02, p = 0.97, ηp2 < 0.01
BAS-Fun 11.9 ± 0.1 12.0 ± 0.2 12.1 ± 0.3 11.7 ± 0.3 F = 0.64, p = 0.53, ηp2 = 0.01
BIS (Behavioral Inhibition System)-Score 19.8 ± 0.3 20.5 ± 0.5 19.5 ± 0.5 19.5 ± 0.5 F = 1.41, p = 0.25, ηp2 = 0.02
STADI Trait Anxiety 18.4 ± 0.4 19.6 ± 0.8 17.7 ± 0.7 17.9 ± 0.7 F = 2.03, p = 0.14, ηp2 = 0.03
STADI Trait Depression 17.4 ± 0.4 17.8 ± 0.7 17.4 ± 0.7 17.0 ± 0.7 F = 0.31, p = 0.73, ηp2 < 0.01
PCS (Pain Catastrophizing) 18.6 ± 0.7 19.0 ± 1.3 18.3 ± 1.3 18.7 ± 1.3 F = 1.00, p = 0.91, ηp2 < 0.01

Metrics are given as mean ± SEM. Group effects are calculated using ANOVAs.

Conditioning session

Before reporting the hypotheses tests, we describe the conditioning procedure and baseline measurements used in all groups, providing evidence for the validity of our paradigm to test our hypotheses. These descriptions were not preregistered in Stage 1.

Pain sensitivity and treatment expectations at baseline

At baseline, the 3 medication groups did not differ in terms of pain sensitivity (heat pain threshold, HPT) or positive expectations towards the placebo treatment (Table 3). Participants received either L-dopa (Levodopa/Carbidopa-neuraxpharm 100/25 mg, neuraxpharm), sulpiride (Dogmatil 400 mg, Sanofi), or inactive pills only according to their allocated group. To minimize potential unblinding by pill appearance, inactive pills matching the appearance of L-dopa and sulpiride were given at the respective time points of the staggered pill intake (matching L-dopa: P-Tablets white 7 mm Lichtenstein, Winthrop; matching sulpiride: Placebo Tablets oval white 17 × 8 mm, Caesar & Loretz GmbH). HPT after drug intake, calibrated temperatures, and average pain ratings on the control site during conditioning were comparable between groups, indicating that medication alone did not induce any immediate analgesic or hyperalgesic effects (see Table 3).

Table 3. Baseline measurements and results of the conditioning session.
Group: all INA DOPA SUL Group effect
HPT baseline (°C) 44.9 ± 0.2 44.7 ± 0.5 45.0 ± 0.4 45.1 ± 0.3 F = 0.28, p = 0.76, ηp2 < 0.01,
HPT after medication (°C) 43.9 ± 0.2 43.8 ± 0.4 43.8 ± 0.4 44.2 ± 0.3 F = 0.49, p = 0.61, ηp2 = 0.01
Temp. VAS 40 (°C) 44.7 ± 0.1 44.6 ± 0.2 44.6 ± 0.2 44.9 ± 0.1 F = 1.29, p = 0.28, ηp2 = 0.02
Temp. VAS 60 (°C) 45.4 ± 0.1 45.3 ± 0.2 45.3 ± 0.2 45.6 ± 0.1 F = 0.88, p = 0.42, ηp2 = 0.01,
Temp. VAS 80 (°C) 46.1 ± 0.1 46.0 ± 0.2 46.0 ± 0.2 46.2 ± 0.1 F = 0.51, p = 0.61, ηp2 = 0.01
EXPECT score at BL (0–10) 5.7 ± 0.1 5.8 ±0.2 5.7 ± 0.3 5.6 ± 0.3 F = 0.29, p = 0.70, ηp2 < 0.01
EXPECT score at preCOND (0–10) 5.8 ± 0.1 6.0 ± 0.2 5.8 ± 0.2 5.6 ± 0.3 F = 0.68, p = 0.51, ηp2 = 0.01
EFFECT score (0–10) 6.7 ± 0.2 7.1 ± 0.3 6.9 ± 0.2 6.1 ± 0.3 F = 3.63, p = 0.029*, ηp2 = 0.05
Pain rating at control (VAS) 72.2 ± 1.0 71.9 ± 2.1 72.9 ± 1.3 71.8 ± 1.9 F = 0.13, p = 0.88, ηp2 < 0.01
Pain rating at placebo (VAS) 30.9 ± 1.2 29.3 ± 2.0 30.3 ± 2.2 33.0 ± 2.0 F = 0.84, p = 0.44, ηp2 = 0.01
Pain relief experience (VAS control–VAS placebo) 41.3 ± 1.3 42.6 ± 2.5 42.6 ± 2.1 38.8 ± 2.4 F = 0.88, p = 0.42, ηp2 = 0.01
GASE side effect symptom count 2.2 ± 0.3 2.5 ± 0.7 2.3 ±0.4 1.9 ± 0.3 F = 0.69, p = 0.69, ηp2 < 0.01
GASE side effect attribution to medication 0.9 ± 0.3 1.2 ± 0.7 1.3 ± 0.7 0.3 ± 0.1 F = 0.96, p = 0.39, ηp2 = 0.01
Serum medication indicator levels - - L-dopa: 0.57 ± 0.04 μg/ml Prolactin: 63.3 ± 6.0 ng/ml -
Placebo first 51.3% 54.7% 45.1% 54.0% -

Metrics are given as mean ± SEM. Group differences are calculated using ANOVAs.

* = adjusted p-value, correcting for type I error rate in the family of 13 tests in these results from study day 1: p = 0.38.

Conditioning procedure

Across groups, participants reported a mean pain reduction of 41.3 ± 1.3 VAS points on the placebo site as compared to the control site. No medication-specific differences were detected in pain ratings on the placebo or control site during conditioning or in reported pain relief (Table 3). Groups only differed in EFFECT score (perceived effectiveness of the placebo cream, rated on a scale from 0–10 as part of the GEEE) after conditioning (Table 3), with SUL participants reporting smaller treatment benefits. This observation did not survive correction for multiple testing across the family of 13 tests on day 1 (adjusted p-value = 0.38).

Serum indicators confirm pharmacological manipulation

Conditioning began 57.5 ± 1.7 min after DOPA intake and 179.3 ± 0.6 min after SUL intake, consistent with the known kinetics for the respective peak drug levels. L-dopa serum levels were measured in the DOPA group immediately after completion of the conditioning session, on average 35.3 ± 1.6 min after the start of conditioning. Fig 5A shows serum levels of the L-dopa medication in the DOPA group with a mean of 0.57 ± 0.04 μg/ml. This lies within the range considered to be therapeutic for Parkinson’s disease medication (0.2 to 4 μg/ml) [45], confirming the pharmacokinetic efficacy of our manipulation. We measured prolactin levels as a proxy of D2 receptor antagonisms in the SUL group, on average 33.0 ± 1.3 min after the start of conditioning. Prolactin levels were robustly elevated, with 63.3 ± 6.2 ng/ml, throughout the SUL group (Fig 5A). In both sexes, levels were well above the reference values for men (<17.7 ng/ml) and non-pregnant, non-lactating women (<29.2 ng/ml; see also Fig 6D for display of sex differences in prolactin levels).

Fig 5. Dopaminergic medication did not modulate treatment expectations or placebo analgesia in test sessions 1 (day 2) and 2 (day 8).

Fig 5

Single participant data points in gray. Black line and box depict mean ± SEM. Violin shapes illustrate data distribution. Asterisks indicate significance level (* = p < 0.05; ** = p < 0.01; *** = p < 0.001). Underlying data can be found in the Supporting information (S1 Data). (A) L-dopa serum levels were determined in the DOPA group immediately after completion of the conditioning session. (B) Prolactin levels were measured as a proxy for sulpiride D2-antagonism in the SUL group. (C) Positive treatment expectations towards the placebo treatment were successfully enhanced through the conditioning procedure in contrast to all other time points. The medication did not differentially affect the development of positive treatment expectations. (D) Placebo analgesia could be induced at test session 1 (day 2). However, there was no modulatory effect of medication on PA. (E) Across groups, PA was no longer apparent at test session 2 (day 8). Again, there was no modulatory effect of medication on PA. DOPA, group L-dopa; INA, group inactive pill; PA, placebo analgesia; SUL, group sulpiride.

Fig 6. Details of medication serum indicator levels and correlations with treatment expectations and placebo analgesia.

Fig 6

Individual data points represent results from individual participants. Orange color indicates data from the DOPA group (top row of figures), green color shows data from SUL group (bottom row of figures). (A, D): Black line and box show mean ± SEM. Violin shapes illustrate data distribution. Asterisks indicate significance level (* = p < 0.05; ** = p < 0.01; *** = p < 0.001). (B, C, E, F): empty circles = data from females, filled circles = data from males. Regression lines are shown for illustration. See main text for details on exploratory statistics. Underlying data can be found in the Supporting information (S2 Data). (A) L-dopa serum levels measured in the DOPA group, separated by sex. (B) No significant relationship could be detected between L-dopa levels in the DOPA group and PA, irrespective of sex. (C) No significant relationship could be detected between L-dopa levels and the formation of positive treatment expectations through conditioning, measured as change in EXPECT scores from before conditioning (preCOND) to before test session 1 (preT1), irrespective of sex. (D) Prolactin serum levels measured in the SUL group, separated by sex. (E) No significant relationship could be detected between prolactin serum levels and PA, irrespective of sex. (F) No significant relationship could be detected between prolactin serum levels and the formation of positive treatment expectations through conditioning, measured as change in EXPECT scores from before conditioning (preCOND) to before test session 1 (preT1), irrespective of sex. DOPA, group L-dopa; PA, placebo analgesia; SUL, group sulpiride.

Test sessions 1 and 2—Hypothesis testing

The following section presents the test results of our 3 hypotheses as preregistered in the Stage 1 protocol. Table 4 gives an overview on the data that was collected during study day 2 and day 8.

Table 4. Results of test sessions 1 and 2.

Group: all INA DOPA SUL Group effect
Test session 1 (day 2)
EXPECT score preT1 (0–10) 6.5 ± 0.2 6.8 ± 0.3 6.6 ± 0.2 6.1 ± 0.3 F = 1.69, p = 0.19, ηp2 = 0.02
EXPECT score change from preCOND to preT1 0.7 ± 0.1 0.7 ± 0.2 0.8 ± 0.3 0.5 ± 0.3 F = 0.38, p = 0.68, ηp2 = 0.01
EFFECT score (0–10) 4.4 ± 0.2 4.9 ± 0.3 4.4 ± 0.3 3.8 ± 0.4 F = 2.35, p = 0.10, ηp2 = 0.03
Pain rating at control (VAS) 52.3 ± 1.3 50.7 ± 2.3 54.0 ± 2.2 52 ± 2.5 F = 0.50, p = 0.60, ηp2 = 0.01
Pain rating at placebo (VAS) 49.1 ± 1.4 46.8 ± 2.6 50.3 ± 2.1 50.1 ± 2.4 F = 0.69, p = 0.51, ηp2 = 0.01
Placebo analgesia (VAS control–VAS placebo) 3.2 ± 1.1 3.9 ± 1.8 3.7 ± 2.1 1.9 ± 1.7 F = 0.35, p = 0.71, ηp2 < 0.01
Placebo first 50.0% 51.0% 43.4% 56.0% -
Test session 2 (day 8)
EXPECT score preT2 (0–10) 6.0 ± 0.2 6.2 ± 0.3 6.0 ± 0.3 5.7 ± 0.3 F = 0.94, p = 0.39, ηp2 = 0.01
EXPECT score change from preCOND to preT2 0.1 ± 0.2 0.2 ± 0.2 0.3 ± 0.3 −0.04 ± 0.3 F = 0.32, p = 0.72, ηp2 < 0.01
EFFECT score (0–10) 4.1 ± 0.2 4.6 ± 0.3 3.9 ± 0.4 3.9 ± 0.4 F = 1.41, p = 0.25, ηp2 = 0.02
Pain rating at control (VAS) 45.9 ± 1.4 48.0 ± 2.3 46.0 ± 2.3 43.7 ± 2.7 F = 0.74, p = 0.48, ηp2 = 0.01
Pain rating at placebo (VAS) 43.9 ± 1.4 43.5 ± 2.5 45.0 ± 2.1 43.0 ± 2.8 F = 0.18, p = 0.84, ηp2 < 0.01
Placebo analgesia (VAS control–VAS placebo) 2.1 ± 1.2 4.4 ± 1.9 1.0 ± 2.1 0.7 ± 2.0 F = 1.05, p = 0.35, ηp2 = 0.01
Placebo first 49.0% 43.1% 49.1% 55.1% -

Metrics are given as mean ± SEM. Group differences are calculated with ANOVAs.

Hypothesis 1: Placebo analgesia at test session 1 (day 2)

Participants overall showed significant PA on day 2 (Fig 5D), as indicated by a significant main effect of experimental condition (F(1,151) = 8.29, p = 0.004, ηp2 = 0.05), with a mean pain relief of 3.2 ± 1.1 VAS points across all 3 groups. The BFincl of experimental condition is 4.32, indicating that our data support that models including experimental condition as a factor are 4.32 times more likely to explain pain ratings than models without this factor. This is considered moderate evidence in favor of including experimental condition as a factor. There were no differences in pain ratings between medication groups (F(2,151) = 0.64, p = 0.53, ηp2 = 0.01), and, importantly, PA was not modulated by medication as indicated by a nonsignificant interaction between medication and experimental condition (F(2,151) = 0.35, p = 0.71, ηp2 < 0.01). The BFincl of this interaction effect in the prediction of pain ratings was 0.06, indicating the absence of any effect of medication on PA. Bayesian model comparisons, Bayes factors, and model-averaged posteriors with estimated marginal means and 95% credible intervals can be viewed in the supporting information (S1 Table).

Hypothesis 2: Placebo analgesia at test session 2 (day 8)

At test session 2, seven days after conditioning, a trend for PA remained with a small mean pain relief of 2.1 ± 1.2 VAS points across all groups (experimental condition, F(1,150) = 3.19, p = 0.08, ηp2 = 0.02, BFincl = 0.38). Again, there was no significant main effect of medication on pain ratings (F(2,150) = 0.33, p = 0.72, ηp2 < 0.01). Again, individual PA was not modulated by medication, as shown in the nonsignificant interaction between medication and experimental condition (F(2,151) = 1.05, p = 0.35, ηp2 = 0.01).

The BFincl of the interaction effect for predicting pain ratings was 0.04, indicating the absence of any effect of medication on PA. Details of the Bayesian analysis can be found in the Supporting information (S2 Table).

Hypothesis 3: Generation of positive treatment expectations

Positive treatment expectations towards the placebo cream were measured through EXPECT scores on different time points relating to conditioning and testing (see also Fig 1B) on a scale ranging from 0 (no pain relief expected) to 10 (highest possible pain relief expected). We successfully enhanced positive treatment expectations through our conditioning paradigm across all 3 groups (F(1,149) = 20.11, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.12), with EXPECT scores increasing by an average of 0.7 ± 0.1 points from preCOND to preT1 (see Fig 5C). EXPECT scores did not significantly differ between medication groups (F(2,149) = 1.44, p = 0.24, ηp2 = 0.02). Importantly, medication did not significantly modulate the change in EXPECT scores from preCOND to preT1 (interaction between medication and rating time point, F(2,149) = 0.38, p = 0.68, ηp2 < 0.01). The BFincl of the interaction effect for predicting EXPECT scores was 0.07, indicating the absence of an interaction effect. Details of Bayes analyses are depicted in the Supporting information (S3 Table). In an explorative, not preregistered ANOVA including all 4 EXPECT score rating time points as factor levels (BL, preCOND, preT1, preT2), as well as medication and the interaction between medication and rating time points, we again observed no main effect of medication (F(2,148) = 1.10, p = 0.34, ηp2 = 0.01), as well as no significant interaction effect (F(6,444) = 0.24, p = 0.97, ηp2 < 0.01), but still a highly significant main effect of rating time point (F(6,444) = 10.55, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.07). Post hoc contrasts with correction for multiple comparisons for 6 tests confirmed that preT1 (i.e., before test session 1) was in fact the only rating time point that was significantly different from all other time points (p < 0.001 in comparison to every other time point), while the other time points were statistically indifferent, indicating no lasting effect of the conditioning procedure on EXPECT scores on day 8.

Exploratory analyses

We performed additional analyses to (1) gain further insight into potential dopamine-related effects in our data; and (2) explore other factors present in our data set that may modulate how individual treatment expectations and placebo analgesia are generated. We restricted all these analyses on PA to test session 1, as PA was no longer significant in test session 2. Since we focused directly on PA as opposed to condition-wise pain ratings, and we explored more complex models, PA is operationalized in all explorative analyses as the difference between mean pain ratings in the control condition and mean pain ratings in the placebo condition, and not expressed as an effect of experimental condition on pain ratings, as done in the main hypotheses above. We used ANOVAs to examine the influence of categorical independent variables on the dependent variable. Linear modeling was used to assess the predictive power of continuous variables on specified outcomes; overall linear model significance is reported to assess the collective effect of all predictors included in the model, while the significance of each term in the model was tested using ANOVA on the model predictors. Significant linear model predictors are reported with coefficient estimates (β) and standard errors to illustrate the strength and direction of their linear influence. Linear mixed effects modeling was used to include the random effect of subject intercept and slope to account for the inter-individual variability in the response trajectories over repeated measurements, which becomes important when examining differences in response dynamics between groups across several stimulus repetitions. The reference level for the factor medication was the inactive control group (INA). The following exploratory analyses were not predefined in the Stage 1 manuscript of the registered report.

Influence of sex on dopaminergic modulation of PA

We investigated the effects of medication and sex using an ANOVA to examine sex-specific effects of the dopaminergic perturbation on PA. There was no main effect of medication (F(2,148) = 0.34, p = 0.71, η2 < 0.01) or sex (F(1,148) = 0.54, p = 0.46, η2 < 0.01). Similarly, sex did not modulate the effect of medication on PA (sex × medication: F(2,148) = 0.24, p = 0.78, η2 < 0.01). We conclude from this analysis that dopaminergic modulation of PA is not linked to sex-specific differences.

Influence of medication serum indicators on PA and the formation of positive treatment expectations

We tested whether the individual serum levels of L-dopa or prolactin, measured immediately after conditioning, are associated with PA or the change in EXPECT scores from preCOND to preT1 within the DOPA or the SUL group, respectively. This was done to explore potential dose dependencies of dopaminergic effects. We found sex differences in mean L-dopa levels (females: 0.63 ± 0.06 μg/ml; males: 0.49 ± 0.07 μg/ml, Wilcoxon rank sum test: p = 0.022; Fig 6A) and mean prolactin levels (females: 83.18 ± 7.99 ng/ml, males: 31.33 ± 2.41 ng/m, Wilcoxon rank sum test: p < 0.001; Fig 6D). We therefore included the factor sex in our analysis. Accordingly, the linear models fitted to predict PA or the change in EXPECT scores included sex, serum L-dopa levels, and their interaction in the DOPA group; and sex, prolactin levels, and their interaction in the SUL group.

The model for PA in the DOPA group (Fig 6B) was overall not statistically significant (F(3,49) = 0.60, p = 0.62, R2 = −0.02). The ANOVA table of the model also showed that neither sex (F(1,49) = 0.31, p = 0.58, ηp2 < 0.01), L-dopa levels (F(1,49) = 0.20, p = 0.65, η2 < 0.01), nor their interaction (F(1,49) = 1,29, p = 0.26, ηp2 = 0.03) significantly predicted PA. Similarly, the model for the change in EXPECT scores (Fig 6C) was not significant (F(3,48) = 1.00, p = 0.40, R2 < 0.01), and again, neither sex (F(1,49) = 1.05, p = 0.31, ηp2 = 0.03), L-dopa levels (F(1,48) = 1.52, p = 0.223, ηp2 = 0.03), nor their interaction (F(1,48) = 0.442, p = 0.509, ηp2 < 0.01) could significantly predict the change in EXPECT score.

In the SUL group, the overall model for PA (Fig 6E) was also not significant (F(3,43) = 0.83, p = 0.49, R2 = −0.01) and none of the variables predicted PA (sex: F(1,43) = 0.97, p = 0.33, ηp2 = 0.01; prolactin levels: F(1,43) = 0.13, p = 0.72, ηp2 < 0.01; sex × prolactin levels: F(1,43) = 1.39, p = 0.25, ηp2 = 0.03). The overall model for EXPECT scores (Fig 6F) was also not significant (F(3,43) = 1.80, p = 0.16, R2 = 0.05). Neither sex (F(1,43) < 0.001, p = 0.97, ηp2 = 0.03) nor the interaction of sex and prolactin levels (F(1,43) = 1.63, p = 0.21, ηp2 = 0.04) predicted the change in EXPECT score.

Relationship between positive treatment expectations and PA

Positive treatment expectations are thought to drive PA [2,3]. In our study, we examined whether positive treatment expectations, as assessed by EXPECT scores at preT1, could predict PA at T1, and whether this relationship would be modified by medication. The linear regression model including medication and EXPECT scores at preT1 did not significantly predict PA (F(5,148) = 0.41, p = 0.84, R2 = −0.02). The ANOVA on the model predictors revealed that no term significantly explained PA (medication: F(2,148) = 0.34, p = 0.71, ηp2 < 0.01; EXPECT scores: F(1,148) = 0.58, p = 0.45, ηp2 < 0.01; medication × EXPECT scores: F(2,148) = 0.40, p = 0.67, ηp2 < 0.01).

Similarly, also the model for predicting PA with the factors of individual change in EXPECT scores from preCOND to preT1, medication and their interaction did not predict PA (F(5,146) = 0.85, p = 0.52, R2 = −0.01), and no predictor was significant (medication: F(2,146) = 0.34, p = 0.72, ηp2 < 0.01; EXPECT score change: F(1,146) = 1.36, p = 0.25, ηp2 < 0.01; medication × EXPECT score change F(2,146) = 1.11, p = 0.33, ηp2 = 0.02).

Influence of experienced pain relief during conditioning on PA and treatment expectations

We next examined whether the amount of pain relief experienced during conditioning from the placebo treatment was related to PA or the formation of treatment expectations. Experienced pain relief was defined as the difference between the mean pain ratings for the placebo condition and the control condition during the conditioning procedure.

The linear model with the parameters medication, experienced pain relief and their interaction did not predict PA (F(5,158) = 0.96, p = 0.41, R2 < 0.01). None of the individual parameters were significant (medication: F(2,148) = 0.35, p = 0.71, ηp2 < 0.01; experienced pain relief: F(1,148) = 2.32, p = 0.13, ηp2 = 0.15; medication × experienced pain relief: F(2,148) = 0.90, p = 0.40, ηp2 = 0.01). However, the change in EXPECT scores from preCOND to preT1 was indeed significantly modeled by medication, experienced pain relief and their interaction (F(5,146) = 2.77, p = 0.02, R2 = 0.05). In detail, greater pain relief experienced during conditioning was significantly positively associated with the formation of increased positive treatment expectations (experienced pain relief: F(1,146) = 12.68, p = <0.001, ηp2 = 0.08) (Fig 7D). Again, medication was no significant predictor (F(2,146) = 0.41, p = 0.67, ηp2 < 0.01) and did not modulate the influence of experienced pain relief (medication × experienced pain relief: F(2,146) = 0.17, p = 0.85, ηp2 < 0.01).

Fig 7. Trial-wise pain ratings and relationships between conditioning experience, expectation, and pain intensities.

Fig 7

Underlying data can be found in the Supporting information (S3 Data). (A) Display of average pain ratings across the15 stimulus repetitions during the conditioning procedure in placebo and control condition separated by group. (B) Average pain ratings across the 14 stimulus repetitions during the test session 1 on day 2 in placebo and control condition, separated by group. (C) The individual extent of pain relief experienced during conditioning was not a predictor for PA on day 2, irrespective of group. Single data points represent averaged differences between VAS pain ratings on control and the placebo condition. (D) The individual extent of pain relief experienced during conditioning was positively associated with the development of more positive treatment expectation, as measured in the change in positive treatment expectations (EXPECT score) from preCOND to preT1. (E) The absolute pain intensity rated in the control condition predicted the magnitude of PA during test session 1 on day 2. preCOND = time point for expectation measurement just before conditioning on day 1; preT1 = time point for expectation measurement just before test session 1 on day 2. DOPA, group L-dopa; GEEE, Generic rating scale for previous treatment experiences; INA, group inactive pill; PA, placebo analgesia; SUL, group ulpiride; VAS, visual analogue scale.

We also tested the 3 hypotheses only including data from participants who reported a substantial mean pain relief of at least 20 VAS points on average with the placebo during conditioning (remaining sample size: N = 139 (90.3% of all); with INA: n = 46 (90.2% of all); DOPA: n = 49 (92.5% of all), and SUL: n = 44 (88.0% of all)). However, statistical analyses conducted with this smaller sample did not lead to different conclusions from the original tests with the whole sample (Hypothesis 1: experimental condition: F(1,136) = 10.08, p = 0.002, ηp2 = 0.07; medication: F(2,136) = 1.12, p = 0.33, ηp2 = 0.01; medication × experimental condition: F(2,136) = 0.48, p = 0.62, ηp2 < 0.01; Hypothesis 2: experimental condition: F(2,136) = 2.97, p = 0.09, ηp2 < 0.01, medication: F(2,136) = 0.32, p = 0.73, ηp2 < 0.01; medication × experimental condition: F(2,136) = 0.76, p = 0.47, ηp2 = 0.01; Hypothesis 3: rating time point: F(1,134) = 24.00, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.15, medication: F(2,134) = 1.38, p = 0.26, ηp2 = 0.02; medication × rating time point: F(2,134) = 0.48, p = 0.62, ηp2 < 0.01).

Considering absolute pain intensity

It has been shown that pain relief by PA is greater when the pain intensity level of the experimental stimulus is higher [46]. Higher pain levels could lead to a stronger desire for a rewarding pain relief through the placebo treatment, and dopamine has been shown to be associated with the “reward wanting” aspect formed during reward-related learning [47,48]. Therefore, we wanted to explore whether the effects of dopamine perturbation on PA depended on the average pain intensity felt during heat pain stimulation in the control condition. We performed a linear model to predict PA with mean pain ratings of the control condition in the test session 1, medication, and their interaction. Indeed, the overall model significantly predicted PA (F(5,148) = 6.72, p < 0.001, R2 = 0.16). The results of the ANOVA on the model predictors showed that the variation in PA was significantly explained by the term mean pain ratings in the control condition (F(1,148) = 27.18, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.16; Fig 7E), while the medication term showed no explanatory power (F(2,148) = 0.41, p = 0.66, ηp2 < 0.01), and the interaction between medication and mean pain ratings did not reach significance (F(2,148) = 2.79, p = 0.06, ηp2 = 0.04).

Temporal dynamics of PA throughout stimulus repetitions

Given the known role of dopamine in reinforcement learning, we decided to explore whether the temporal dynamics of PA over the 14 trials per condition in the test sessions would reveal any group-specific differences, e.g., through a putative dopamine-mediated influence on the extinction rate of PA within the block of pain trials. The lines in Fig 7B illustrate the mean pain ratings throughout the stimulus repetitions for each group in each condition separately. The distance between the mean pain rating in the control and the placebo condition graphically represent PA. Similar to Zunhammer and colleagues [10], we tested whether groups differed in the dynamics of PA throughout the set of the 14 stimulus repetitions per block by performing a full factorial mixed model analysis (random intercepts and slopes for subject) with the factors medication and stimulus repetitions. The fixed effects analysis showed no significant medication effect (F(2, 151) = 0.16, p = 0.85, ηp2 < 0.01), but a significant positive linear influence of stimulus repetitions on PA (F(1, 151) = 6.56, p = 0.011, ηp2 = 0.04), with an increase of 0.49 ± 0.26 VAS points per stimulus repetition in the reference group INA (β = 0.50 ± 0.26, t(151) = 1.93, p = 0.06). However, the interaction between medication and stimulus repetition was not significant (F(2,151) = 0.17, p = 0.84, ηp2 < 0.01). These results indicate that PA increases incrementally with stimulus repetitions, irrespective of the medication group. As can be readily observed in Fig 7B, participants appeared to sensitize to the pain stimulus, showing higher mean pain ratings with increasing stimulus repetitions. This was confirmed with linear mixed model analysis (again with random intercepts and slopes for subject) to explain pain ratings in the control condition with the predictors medication and stimulus repetitions. Stimulus repetitions significantly explained pain ratings (F(1,150) = 103.35, p < 0.001, ηp2 = 0.41), with an increase of 1.58 ± 0.30 VAS points per stimulus repetition in the reference group INA (β = 1.58 ± 0.30, t(151) = 5.34, p < 0.001). There was no main effect or modulation by medication (medication: F(2,150) = 0.090, p = 0.914, ηp2 < 0.01; medication × stimulus repetition: F (2,150) = 0.20, p = 0.82, ηp2 < 0.01). In conclusion, absolute pain ratings and PA increased throughout the block of stimuli in the test session. However, these temporal dynamics did not differ between medication groups.

Discussion

This study experimentally investigated the effect of a pharmacological manipulation targeting dopaminergic signaling on the formation of positive treatment expectations during the experience of pain relief in response to a placebo treatment. With a final sample of n = 154 participants, our study provides strong evidence against an influence of our dopamine manipulation on treatment expectations (induced by instructed and conditioned pain relief in combination with a sham treatment) and PA. Here, we critically evaluate the validity of our experimental approach, summarize the conclusions drawn from our results, and discuss implications for our understanding of the role of dopamine in placebo analgesia. Although we did not confirm the expected effects of dopamine in our experiment, and the a priori hypotheses were rejected, our data contribute to a more nuanced understanding of the neurobiology underpinning placebo analgesia which aids the characterization of the intricate interplay between cognition, neurochemistry, and treatment outcome.

Critically, a series of manipulation checks and control analyses confirmed the validity of our experimental approach to test the predefined hypotheses. We found no direct analgesic effects of the study medication on heat pain sensitivity or pain ratings during conditioning, confirming previous experimental studies using pro- or antidopaminergic drugs [41,49]. A decrease in pain ratings in the placebo compared to the control condition was confirmed across all 3 medication groups, with no significant between-group differences. Notably, we successfully induced placebo analgesia during the first test session, even though testing took place 1 day after conditioning. Most previous studies have performed conditioning for PA and subsequent testing on the same day, which explains the relatively modest placebo effect observed in our paradigm compared to the literature (medium effect size of ηp2 = 0.05 in our experiment corresponding to a Cohen’s d of 0.46), whereas a Cohen’s d of 0.95 is described in the literature for conditioned PA tested on the same day [50]. Furthermore, we can assume that dopaminergic transmission was successfully altered in the SUL and DOPA group, as previous research has demonstrated reward-related behavioral and neuroimaging effects in dopamine-dependent behavior using the same agent and dosages [12,51]. Importantly, our predefined surrogate parameters for successful manipulation of dopaminergic transmission exhibited high values for serum L-dopa in the DOPA group and elevated prolactin in the SUL group, respectively. Our data could therefore not support a link between dopaminergic neurotransmission and placebo analgesia, contrary to what has been proposed previously based on a smaller correlational study [13]. Bayesian analyses confirmed that our data provide strong evidence for the absence of an effect of the pharmacological dopamine manipulation. Hence, considering the present evidence, the conceptual framework for the role of dopamine in placebo analgesia and positive treatment expectations needs to be re-evaluated and critically discussed.

Dopaminergic neural activity is known to be associated with encoding prediction errors in reward learning [52]. When an unexpected reward is received, dopamine neurons increase their firing rate. Importantly, this increase in dopaminergic firing occurs not only when a reward is received, but also during the perception of a conditioned stimulus that has been associated with the impending reward through learning [53]. Applying this principle to the context of pain, the experience of pain relief can be considered a rewarding outcome. The administration of a treatment, such as an analgesic cream, could act as an external stimulus that becomes associated with pain relief through conditioning. This notion is supported by evidence demonstrating the effectiveness of conditioning in eliciting PA in experimental settings [52]. Moreover, the integration of conditioning with verbal suggestion and/or observational learning appears to enhance PA, suggesting that conditioning serves, at least in part, as an independent pathway for the establishment of PA [54].

Despite the conceptual similarities between reward learning and conditioned PA, our experiment did not reveal any effects of the dopaminergic manipulation during the learning experience on positive treatment expectations and PA. There are several potential reasons why dopamine may not have influenced PA in this case. The clear association between the placebo cream and pain relief, communicated through verbal suggestions, already creates an expectation of pain relief in the participants. In such a scenario, where participants expect pain relief with the placebo, and only experience confirmations of their expectations given the 100% contingency between the stimulus (placebo) and the outcome (reduced pain) during conditioning, it is conceivable that no relevant prediction error can form, and dopaminergic firing, which typically modulates learning through the detection of prediction errors, may not play a significant role. In addition, the 15 trials per block during conditioning may have created a ceiling effect, resulting in a high certainty of the learnt association between placebo and pain relief in all groups, leaving limited room for dopamine to exert its influence. Furthermore, previous research has shown that dopamine receptor blockade with sulpiride does not affect the learning rate in a reinforcement learning task compared to a control group. Both groups learned to choose a rewarded option at the same rate. Instead, the drug was found to lower the incentive value of the reward, with sulpiride recipients being slower to choose the reward than the controls [12]. Similarly, the dopaminergic mechanisms for PA in our study may be more pertinent to the attribution of incentive value of reward, rather than for forming predictive associations between a stimulus and a rewarding outcome.

Another dopamine-dependent psychological component of reward is the “wanting” aspect, which refers to the motivation to obtain the reward [47]. Dopamine mediates the incentive salience of a reward, thereby promoting choices and behaviors that lead to obtaining the reward. Although we ensured that the applied heat stimuli were perceived as moderately painful in both test sessions, it is plausible that the participants did not develop sufficient motivation, or desire for pain relief. Additionally, the block design of the treatment conditions may have reduced the ability to desire pain relief during the placebo block, as participants were aware that successive stimuli would only occur on the “treated” site. In situations where there is high certainty of effective treatment, the level of desire may be minimal to begin with. In contrast, the experimental design used by Scott and colleagues [13,18] involved a more invasive paradigm in which hypertonic saline was infused into the masseter muscle, producing a persistent, tonic pain, and participants are instructed to expect a decrease in this tonic pain when the placebo was administered. This difference in design may have led to a greater overall desire for pain relief compared to the classic experimental heat pain placebo paradigm used in the current study.

Furthermore, the paradigm used in this study did not involve an active role on the part of the participant. Both control and placebo creams were passively administered by the experimenter, and the participants were passively guided through the experiment with no active engagement with the pain stimulation or the treatment. A relationship between active behavior and the extent of pain relief as a reward has been demonstrated in previous studies [55,56]. Individuals reported more pain relief when they “earned” it as a result of their choices in a wheel-of-fortune game than when they just passively watched the game before being informed of the subsequent pain decrease [55]. It was the participants’ agency that led to the outcome of pain relief. A more recent study showed that L-dopa supplementation markedly increased this modulatory effect of agency, with pain relief being more pronounced in the “active” than the “passive” condition [57]. This finding suggests a dopaminergic modulation of the influence of agency on pain perception.

We know from experimental data that choice of preferred treatment can enhance placebo effects [58]. Similarly, the beneficial effects of an analgesic might be modulated by the degree of agency during its administration. Indeed, clinical trials have confirmed that postoperative pain can be alleviated more efficiently when patients use patient-controlled devices than when they receive the same dose passively [59]. However, the dopaminergic modulation of the effect of agency has not yet been tested directly in clinical pain conditions. A general modulatory effect of dopaminergic drugs on the efficacy of pain treatment has yielded conflicting results in patients. One study reported no effect of L-dopa or haloperidol on pain intensity in neuropathic pain conditions during open and hidden local application of lidocaine, which was administered by the study personnel [20]. Another study suggested that the intake of L-dopa together with the analgesic naltrexone for several weeks could decrease pain levels in female patients with subacute low back pain and may have even prevented the development of chronic back pain [21]. It could be speculated that the involvement of the “agency” aspect explains these discrepant results, with the active, prolonged use of naltrexone over weeks potentially accentuating the dopaminergic influence, and no significant dopaminergic modulation under the single passive application of lidocaine, similar to the findings of our study. However, the findings by Reckziegel and colleagues may also reflect a more general change in dopaminergic transmission in chronic pain patients which may lead to very different results with L-dopa augmentation [60,61].

Conclusions

The evidence presented here argues against a direct causal role for dopamine during the experience of a treatment effect in the establishment of positive treatment expectations and placebo analgesia in healthy volunteers. Rather, in line with previous literature, we suggest a more nuanced role of dopamine. Certain dopamine-dependent dimensions of reward processing, including active agency and motivational aspects, may interact with pain experience and contribute to placebo analgesia. Future efforts to advance the understanding of dopaminergic mechanisms for modulating treatment response in pain must consider the undoubtedly complex involvement of dopaminergic neurotransmission in pain and its modulation.

Supporting information

S1 Text. Overview of questionnaires with references and verbal placebo instructions.

(DOCX)

pbio.3002772.s001.docx (21KB, docx)
S1 Table. Detailed results of Bayesian quantification of evidence for Hypothesis 1 from JASP output.

(A) Model comparison. Note that models include subject and random slopes for all repeated measures factors. (B) Analysis of effects showing the inclusion Bayes factors (BFincl) of the model terms. (C) Summary of model averaged posteriors showing estimated marginal means, standard deviations, and 95% credible intervals for all factor levels.

(DOCX)

pbio.3002772.s002.docx (23.5KB, docx)
S2 Table. Detailed results of Bayesian quantification of evidence for Hypothesis 2 from JASP output.

(A) Model comparison. Note that models include subject and random slopes for all repeated measures factors. (B) Analysis of effects showing the inclusion Bayes factors (BFincl) of the model terms. (C) Summary of model averaged posteriors showing estimated marginal means, standard deviations, and 95% credible intervals for all factor levels.

(DOCX)

pbio.3002772.s003.docx (19.8KB, docx)
S3 Table. Detailed results of Bayesian quantification of evidence for Hypothesis 3 from JASP output.

(A) Model comparison. Note that models include subject and random slopes for all repeated measures factors. (B) Analysis of effects showing the inclusion Bayes factors (BFincl) of the model terms. (C) Summary of model averaged posteriors showing estimated marginal means, standard deviations, and 95% credible intervals for all factor levels.

(DOCX)

pbio.3002772.s004.docx (19.9KB, docx)
S1 Data. Individual quantitative observations that underlie the data summarized in Fig 5.

Excel file with single sheets for each figure panel (Fig 5A–5E). Abbreviations: “participant”: participant ID. “blood_Dopa”: serum L-dopa levels in μg/ml. “blood_Prolactin”: prolactin serum levels in ng/ml. “group”: medication group. “expectation”: EXPECT score from 0–10. “expectation_timepoint”: rating time point for EXPECT score. “Expectation_BL”: EXPECT score at baseline at day 1 (BL). “Expectation_day1”: EXPECT score before conditioning at day 1 (preCOND). “Expectation_day2”: EXPECT score at day 2 (preT1); “Expectation_day8”: EXPECT score at day 8 (preT2). “INA”: inactive control group, “DOPA”: L-dopa group, “SUL”: sulpiride group. “Delta_VAS_Day2”: VAS mean difference pain rating between control condition minus placebo condition on day 2 (test session 1). “Delta_VAS_Day8”: VAS mean difference pain rating between control condition minus placebo condition on day 8 (test session 2).

(XLSX)

pbio.3002772.s005.xlsx (31.8KB, xlsx)
S2 Data. Individual quantitative observations that underlie the data summarized in Fig 6.

Excel file with single sheets for each figure panel (Fig 6A–6F). Abbreviations: “participant”: participant ID. “blood_Dopa”: serum L-dopa levels in μg/ml. “gender”: participant sex. “blood_Prolactin”: prolactin serum levels in ng/ml. “group”: medication group. “Delta_Expect_day2minusday1”: mean difference between EXPECT scores at preT1 (before test session 1 on day 2) vs. preCOND (before conditioning at day 1). “INA”: inactive control group, “DOPA”: L-dopa group, “SUL”: sulpiride group. “Delta_VAS_Day2”: VAS mean difference in pain rating between control condition vs. placebo condition on day 2 (test session 1).

(XLSX)

pbio.3002772.s006.xlsx (20.5KB, xlsx)
S3 Data. Individual quantitative observations that underlie the data summarized in Fig 7.

Excel file with single sheets for each figure panel (Fig 7A–7E). “Trial”: individual trial number. “group”: medication group. “INA”: inactive control group, “DOPA”: L-dopa group, “SUL”: sulpiride group. “n” = n-number. “sd” = standard deviation. “ControlCream_Conditioning_VAS”: VAS pain rating score in control condition during conditioning (day 1). “PlaceboCream_Conditioning_VAS”: VAS pain rating score in placebo condition during conditioning (day 1). “ControlCream_Day2_VAS”: VAS pain rating score in control condition during test session 1 (day 2). “PlaceboCream_Day2_VAS”: VAS pain rating score in placebo condition during test session 1 (day 2). “Delta_VAS_Cond”: mean VAS pain rating difference between control and placebo condition during conditioning (day 1). “Delta_VAS_Day2”: mean VAS pain rating difference between control condition vs. placebo condition on day 2 (test session 1). “Delta_Expect_day2minusday1”: mean difference between EXPECT scores at preT1 (before test session 1 on day 2) vs. preCOND (before conditioning on day 1). “Day2_VAS_ControlCream”: mean VAS pain rating of the control condition during test session 1 (day 2).

(XLSX)

pbio.3002772.s007.xlsx (26.8KB, xlsx)

Acknowledgments

We gratefully acknowledge Sarah Hoppen and Detlef Pucher for their assistance in medication delivery. We thank Julian Kleine-Borgmann for designing Fig 1, Katarina Forkmann for technical support, and Matthias Zunhammer for his support during study planning.

Abbreviations

(rm)ANOVA

(repeated measures) analysis of variance

BAS

Behavioral Activation System

BF

Bayes factor

BFI-10

10-item-Big-Five-Inventory

BFincl

inclusion Bayes factor

BIDS

Brain Imaging Data Structure

BIS

Behavioral Inhibition System

BL

time point baseline

CTR

control

DA

dopamine

DOPA

group L-dopa

ECG

electrocardiogram

EEG

electroencephalography

EFFECT score

subscore of the GEEE for rating positive treatment effects

EXPECT score

subscore of the GEEE for rating positive treatment expectation

fMRI

functional magnetic resonance imaging

FPQ-III

Fear of Pain Questionnaire

GASE

Generic Assessment of Side Effects in Clinical Trials

GEEE

Generic rating scale for previous treatment experiences, treatment expectations, and treatment effects

HPT

heat pain threshold

INA

group inactive pill

ITI

inter-trial interval

mm

millimeter

NAc

nucleus accumbens

PA

placebo analgesia

PANAS

Positive and Negative Affect Schedule

PCS

Pain Catastrophizing Scale

PET

positron emission tomography

PLC

placebo

preCOND

time point just before conditioning

preT1

time point just before test session 1 (day 2)

preT2

time point just before test session 2 (day 8)

PSS-10

Perceived Stress Scale

SARS-CoV-2

Severe Acute Respiratory Syndrome Coronavirus 2

SEM

standard error of the mean

SSAS

Somatosensory Amplification Scale

STADI State

State-Trait-Anxiety-Depression-Inventory State

SUL

group sulpiride

VAS

visual analogue scale

μm

micrometer

Data Availability

Data and code to reproduce all results are publicly shared on the Open Science Framework (OSF) (https://osf.io/f49dt/). R code and input data to perform frequentist statistics are shared, as well as the JASP file for Bayesian analyses. The complete dataset is additionally provided in Brain Imaging Data Structure (BIDS) format. The in-principle-accepted Stage 1 manuscript of this Preregistered Research Article was deposited as public registration in OSF (https://osf.io/py8b3).

Funding Statement

This work was funded by the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft (DFG, German Research Foundation, https://www.dfg.de/; Project-ID 422744262 - TRR 289, to UB; and Project ID-FU 356/12 - UMEA, to LA) and the Medical Faculty Essen and Stiftung Universitätsmedizin Essen (Project ELAN, https://www.uni-due.de/med/elan/, to IK). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

References

  • 1.Petrie KJ, Rief W. Psychobiological Mechanisms of Placebo and Nocebo Effects: Pathways to Improve Treatments and Reduce Side Effects. Annu Rev Psychol. 2019;70:599–625. doi: 10.1146/annurev-psych-010418-102907 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 2.Schedlowski M, Enck P, Rief W, Bingel U. Neuro-Bio-Behavioral Mechanisms of Placebo and Nocebo Responses: Implications for Clinical Trials and Clinical Practice. Pharmacol Rev. 2015;67(3):697–730. doi: 10.1124/pr.114.009423 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 3.Benedetti F. Mechanisms of placebo and placebo-related effects across diseases and treatments. Annu Rev Pharmacol Toxicol. 2008;48:33–60. doi: 10.1146/annurev.pharmtox.48.113006.094711 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 4.Bingel U. Placebo 2.0: the impact of expectations on analgesic treatment outcome. Pain. 2020;161(Suppl 1):S48–S56. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001981 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 5.Eippert F, Finsterbusch J, Bingel U, Buchel C. Direct evidence for spinal cord involvement in placebo analgesia. Science. 2009;326(5951):404. doi: 10.1126/science.1180142 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 6.Eippert F, Bingel U, Schoell ED, Yacubian J, Klinger R, Lorenz J, et al. Activation of the opioidergic descending pain control system underlies placebo analgesia. Neuron. 2009;63(4):533–543. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2009.07.014 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 7.Wager TD, Atlas LY. The neuroscience of placebo effects: connecting context, learning and health. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16(7):403–418. doi: 10.1038/nrn3976 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 8.Wager TD, Rilling JK, Smith EE, Sokolik A, Casey KL, Davidson RJ, et al. Placebo-induced changes in FMRI in the anticipation and experience of pain. Science. 2004;303(5661):1162–1167. doi: 10.1126/science.1093065 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 9.Wager TD, Atlas LY, Lindquist MA, Roy M, Woo CW, Kross E. An fMRI-based neurologic signature of physical pain. N Engl J Med. 2013;368(15):1388–1397. doi: 10.1056/NEJMoa1204471 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 10.Zunhammer M, Gerardi M, Bingel U. The effect of dopamine on conditioned placebo analgesia in healthy individuals: a double-blind randomized trial. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2018;235(9):2587–2595. doi: 10.1007/s00213-018-4951-3 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 11.Zunhammer M, Spisak T, Wager TD, Bingel U, Placebo IC. Meta-analysis of neural systems underlying placebo analgesia from individual participant fMRI data. Nat Commun. 2021;12(1):1391. doi: 10.1038/s41467-021-21179-3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 12.Eisenegger C, Naef M, Linssen A, Clark L, Gandamaneni PK, Muller U, et al. Role of dopamine D2 receptors in human reinforcement learning. Neuropsychopharmacology. 2014;39(10):2366–2375. doi: 10.1038/npp.2014.84 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 13.Scott DJ, Stohler CS, Egnatuk CM, Wang H, Koeppe RA, Zubieta JK. Placebo and nocebo effects are defined by opposite opioid and dopaminergic responses. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2008;65(2):220–231. doi: 10.1001/archgenpsychiatry.2007.34 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 14.Wrobel N, Wiech K, Forkmann K, Ritter C, Bingel U. Haloperidol blocks dorsal striatum activity but not analgesia in a placebo paradigm. Cortex. 2014;57:60–73. doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2014.02.023 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 15.de la Fuente-Fernandez R, Ruth TJ, Sossi V, Schulzer M, Calne DB, Stoessl AJ. Expectation and dopamine release: mechanism of the placebo effect in Parkinson’s disease. Science. 2001;293(5532):1164–1166. doi: 10.1126/science.1060937 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 16.Leknes S, Lee M, Berna C, Andersson J, Tracey I. Relief as a reward: hedonic and neural responses to safety from pain. PLoS ONE. 2011;6(4):e17870. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0017870 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 17.Peciña M, Zubieta JK. Molecular mechanisms of placebo responses in humans. Placebo and Pain. 2013. p. 25–36. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 18.Scott DJ, Stohler CS, Egnatuk CM, Wang H, Koeppe RA, Zubieta JK. Individual differences in reward responding explain placebo-induced expectations and effects. Neuron. 2007;55(2):325–336. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2007.06.028 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 19.Schweinhardt P, Seminowicz DA, Jaeger E, Duncan GH, Bushnell MC. The anatomy of the mesolimbic reward system: a link between personality and the placebo analgesic response. J Neurosci. 2009;29(15):4882–4887. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.5634-08.2009 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 20.Skyt I, Moslemi K, Baastrup C, Grosen K, Benedetti F, Petersen GL, et al. Dopaminergic tone does not influence pain levels during placebo intervent ions in patients with chronic neuropathic pain. Pain. 2018;159(2):261–272. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000001089 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 21.Reckziegel D, Tetreault P, Ghantous M, Wakaizumi K, Petre B, Huang L, et al. Sex-Specific Pharmacotherapy for Back Pain: A Proof-of-Concept Randomized Trial. Pain Ther. 2021;10(2):1375–1400. doi: 10.1007/s40122-021-00297-2 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 22.Keysers C, Gazzola V, Wagenmakers EJ. Using Bayes factor hypothesis testing in neuroscience to establish evidence of absence. Nat Neurosci. 2020;23(7):788–799. doi: 10.1038/s41593-020-0660-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 23.Schonbrodt FD, Wagenmakers EJ. Bayes factor design analysis: Planning for compelling evidence. Psychon Bull Rev. 2018;25(1):128–142. doi: 10.3758/s13423-017-1230-y [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 24.Faul F, Erdfelder E, Buchner A, Lang AG. Statistical power analyses using G*Power 3.1: tests for correlation and regression analyses. Behav Res Methods. 2009;41(4):1149–1160. doi: 10.3758/BRM.41.4.1149 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 25.Colloca L, Benedetti F. How prior experience shapes placebo analgesia. Pain. 2006;124(1–2):126–133. doi: 10.1016/j.pain.2006.04.005 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 26.Montgomery GH, Kirsch I. Classical conditioning and the placebo effect. Pain. 1997;72(1–2):107–113. doi: 10.1016/s0304-3959(97)00016-x [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 27.Voudouris NJ, Peck CL, Coleman G. The role of conditioning and verbal expectancy in the placebo response. Pain. 1990;43(1):121–128. doi: 10.1016/0304-3959(90)90057-K [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 28.Delaveau P, Salgado-Pineda P, Wicker B, Micallef-Roll J, Blin O. Effect of levodopa on healthy volunteers’ facial emotion perception: an FMRI study. Clin Neuropharmacol. 2005;28(6):255–261. doi: 10.1097/01.wnf.0000186651.96351.2e [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 29.Pessiglione M, Seymour B, Flandin G, Dolan RJ, Frith CD. Dopamine-dependent prediction errors underpin reward-seeking behaviour in humans. Nature. 2006;442(7106):1042–1045. doi: 10.1038/nature05051 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 30.Weis T, Puschmann S, Brechmann A, Thiel CM. Effects of L-dopa during auditory instrumental learning in humans. PLoS ONE. 2012;7(12):e52504. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0052504 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 31.Dodds CM, Clark L, Dove A, Regenthal R, Baumann F, Bullmore E, et al. The dopamine D2 receptor antagonist sulpiride modulates striatal BOLD signal during the manipulation of information in working memory. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2009;207(1):35–45. doi: 10.1007/s00213-009-1634-0 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 32.Ojala KE, Janssen LK, Hashemi MM, Timmer MHM, Geurts DEM, Ter Huurne NP, et al. Dopaminergic Drug Effects on Probability Weighting during Risky Decision Making. eNeuro. 2018;5(2). doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0330-18.2018 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 33.Weber SC, Beck-Schimmer B, Kajdi ME, Muller D, Tobler PN, Quednow BB. Dopamine D2/3- and mu-opioid receptor antagonists reduce cue-induced responding and reward impulsivity in humans. Transl Psychiatry. 2016;6(7):e850. [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 34.Helmy SA. Therapeutic drug monitoring and pharmacokinetic compartmental analysis of sulpiride double-peak absorption profile after oral administration to human volunteers. Biopharm Drug Dispos. 2013;34(5):288–301. doi: 10.1002/bdd.1843 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 35.Huang MC, Ho HO, Yeh GC, Ke WT, Lin LC, Hsu TM, et al. Development of a high-performance liquid chromatographic method for bioanalytical applications with sulpiride. J Chromatogr B Biomed Sci Appl. 2001;763(1–2):157–163. doi: 10.1016/s0378-4347(01)00387-5 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 36.Nyholm D, Lewander T, Gomes-Trolin C, Backstrom T, Panagiotidis G, Ehrnebo M, et al. Pharmacokinetics of levodopa/carbidopa microtablets versus levodopa/benserazide and levodopa/carbidopa in healthy volunteers. Clin Neuropharmacol. 2012;35(3):111–117. doi: 10.1097/WNF.0b013e31825645d1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 37.Rief W, Nestoriuc Y, Mueller E, Hermann C, Schmidt K, Bingel U. Generic rating scale for previous treatment experiences, treatment expectations, and treatment effects (GEEE). PsychArchives. 2021. [Google Scholar]
  • 38.Laux L, Hock M, Bergner-Köther R, Hodapp V, Renner K-H. Das State-Trait-Angst-Depressions-Inventar: STADI; Manual 2013.
  • 39.Rief W, Glombiewski JA, Barsky A. Generic assessment of side effects: GASE: Verlag Hans Huber, Bern. 2009.
  • 40.Fruhstorfer H, Lindblom U, Schmidt WC. Method for quantitative estimation of thermal thresholds in patients. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry. 1976;39(11):1071–1075. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.39.11.1071 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 41.Becker S, Ceko M, Louis-Foster M, Elfassy NM, Leyton M, Shir Y, et al. Dopamine and pain sensitivity: neither sulpiride nor acute phenylalanine and tyrosine depletion have effects on thermal pain sensations in healthy volunteers. PLoS ONE. 2013;8(11):e80766. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0080766 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 42.Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect: the PANAS scales. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1988; 54(6):1063–70. [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 43.Ben-Jonathan N, Hnasko R. Dopamine as a prolactin (PRL) inhibitor. Endocr Rev. 2001;22(6):724–763. doi: 10.1210/edrv.22.6.0451 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 44.Jeffreys H. The Theory of Probability. OUP; Oxford; 1998. [Google Scholar]
  • 45.Regenthal R, Krueger M, Koeppel C, Preiss R. Drug levels: therapeutic and toxic serum/plasma concentrations of common drugs. J Clin Monit Comput. 1999;15(7–8):529–544. doi: 10.1023/a:1009935116877 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 46.Levine JD, Gordon NC, Bornstein JC, Fields HL. Role of pain in placebo analgesia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 1979;76(7):3528–3531. doi: 10.1073/pnas.76.7.3528 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 47.Berridge KC, Robinson TE, Aldridge JW. Dissecting components of reward: ‘liking’, ‘wanting’, and learning. Curr Opin Pharmacol. 2009;9(1):65–73. doi: 10.1016/j.coph.2008.12.014 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 48.Arias-Carrion O, Poppel E. Dopamine, learning, and reward-seeking behavior. Acta Neurobiol Exp (Wars). 2007;67(4):481–488. doi: 10.55782/ane-2007-1664 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 49.Tiemann L, Heitmann H, Schulz E, Baumkotter J, Ploner M. Dopamine precursor depletion influences pain affect rather than pain sensation. PLoS ONE. 2014;9(4):e96167. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0096167 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 50.Vase L, Riley JL 3rd, Price DD. A comparison of placebo effects in clinical analgesic trials versus studies of placebo analgesia. Pain. 2002;99(3):443–452. doi: 10.1016/S0304-3959(02)00205-1 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 51.McCabe C, Huber A, Harmer CJ, Cowen PJ. The D2 antagonist sulpiride modulates the neural processing of both rewarding and aversive stimuli in healthy volunteers. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2011;217(2):271–278. doi: 10.1007/s00213-011-2278-4 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 52.Schultz W, Dayan P, Montague PR. A neural substrate of prediction and reward. Science. 1997;275(5306):1593–1599. doi: 10.1126/science.275.5306.1593 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 53.Schultz W. Dopamine reward prediction error coding. Dialogues Clin Neurosci. 2016;18(1):23–32. doi: 10.31887/DCNS.2016.18.1/wschultz [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 54.van Lennep J, van Middendorp H, Veldhuijzen DS, Peerdeman KJ, Blythe JS, Thomaidou MA, et al. The Optimal Learning Cocktail for Placebo Analgesia: A Randomized Controlled Trial Comparing Individual and Combined Techniques. J Pain. 2023;24(12):2240–2256. doi: 10.1016/j.jpain.2023.07.009 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 55.Becker S, Gandhi W, Kwan S, Ahmed AK, Schweinhardt P. Doubling Your Payoff: Winning Pain Relief Engages Endogenous Pain Inhibition. eNeuro. 2015;2(4). doi: 10.1523/ENEURO.0029-15.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 56.Strube A, Horing B, Rose M, Buchel C. Agency affects pain inference through prior shift as opposed to likelihood precision modulation in a Bayesian pain model. Neuron. 2023;111(7):1136–51 e7. doi: 10.1016/j.neuron.2023.01.002 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 57.Desch S, Schweinhardt P, Seymour B, Flor H, Becker S. Evidence for dopaminergic involvement in endogenous modulation of pain relief. Elife. 2023:12. doi: 10.7554/eLife.81436 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 58.Tang B, Barnes K, Geers A, Livesey E, Colagiuri B. Choice and the Placebo Effect: A Meta-analysis. Ann Behav Med. 2022;56(10):977–988. doi: 10.1093/abm/kaab111 [DOI] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 59.McNicol ED, Ferguson MC, Hudcova J. Patient controlled opioid analgesia versus non-patient controlled opioid analgesia for postoperative pain. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2015;2015(6):CD003348. doi: 10.1002/14651858.CD003348.pub3 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 60.Martikainen IK, Nuechterlein EB, Pecina M, Love TM, Cummiford CM, Green CR, et al. Chronic Back Pain Is Associated with Alterations in Dopamine Neurotransmission in the Ventral Striatum. J Neurosci. 2015;35(27):9957–9965. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4605-14.2015 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • 61.Taylor AMW, Becker S, Schweinhardt P, Cahill C. Mesolimbic dopamine signaling in acute and chronic pain: implications for motivation, analgesia, and addiction. Pain. 2016;157(6):1194–1198. doi: 10.1097/j.pain.0000000000000494 [DOI] [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

Lucas Smith

16 Nov 2021

Dear Dr Kunkel,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript entitled "The role of dopamine for positive treatment expectations and placebo analgesia" for consideration as a Preregistered Research Article by PLOS Biology. Please accept my apologies for the delay in sending the decision below.

Your manuscript has now been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editorial staff. We have also discussed your proposal with two academic editors, one with expertise in the biological questions you are addressing and another one with expertise in Pre-registered Reports. I am writing to let you know that we are interested in peer-reviewing your proposal, but before we can do that, we would like you to address some concerns raised by the Academic Editor with expertise in Pre-registered Reports. These issues are very likely to come up with the reviewers and so, we think, addressing them now will save you time in the end. You can find the comments from the Academic Editors below my signature. When you re-submit, please provide a point-by-point response to her/his concerns.

In addition, and BEFORE you start your revision, we need you to complete your submission by providing the metadata that is required for full assessment. To this end, please login to Editorial Manager where you will find the paper in the 'Submissions Needing Revisions' folder on your homepage. Please click 'Revise Submission' from the Action Links and complete all additional questions in the submission questionnaire. (YOU DONT NEED TO SUBMIT YOUR REVISION YET, JUST COMPLETE THE METADATA).

Please re-submit your manuscript within two working days, i.e. by Nov 18 2021 11:59PM.

Login to Editorial Manager here: https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology

During resubmission, you will be invited to opt-in to posting your pre-review manuscript as a bioRxiv preprint. Visit http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/preprints for full details. If you consent to posting your current manuscript as a preprint, please upload a single Preprint PDF when you re-submit.

Once your full submission is complete, your paper will undergo a series of checks. Once they are complete, I will stamp a Major Revision decision to give you time to address the concerns below.

Feel free to email us at plosbiology@plos.org if you have any queries relating to your submission.

Kind regards,

Lucas

Lucas Smith

Associate Editor

PLOS Biology

lsmith@plos.org

COMMENTS FROM THE ACADEMIC EDITOR

My main concern is that the analysis plans don't align sufficiently clearly with the hypotheses or power analysis, and I suspect may be overcomplicated, with the consequence that the study may be underpowered to detect differential effects between cells of d=0.3. For example, if I understand correctly, Hypothesis 1 is two independent t-tests (sulpride vs control; L-dopa vs control) but the analysis plan is for a 2 x 3 mixed ANOVA, and I didn't understand how the within-subjects conditioning variable (placebo vs control) is taken into account in the hypothesis (e.g. through a differential subtraction?). I had similar concerns about the other hypotheses. The authors should make clear, for each hypothesis, the predicted pattern of results in graphical form, and then ensure that the statistical tests and sampling plan map directly on to that predicted pattern specifically. For instance, the power analysis for a 2 x 3 ANOVA will return the power to detect *any* interaction, when only one of several possible interaction patterns would support the hypothesis. Therefore power is overestimated. Unless I'm mistaken, I think that all of the authors' hypotheses can be reduced to combinations of pairwise comparisons (e.g. between difference scores), which might be best addressed using Bayesian hypothesis tests (dispensing with p values entirely). But the design isn't sufficiently clear at this stage to say for certain either way.

If the authors do continue to use frequentist and Bayesian tests, they also need to make clear which outcomes (Bayesian or frequentist) will determine the interpretation. Otherwise there are too many interpretative degrees of freedom and consequent risk of bias.

Decision Letter 1

Lucas Smith

19 Nov 2021

Dear Dr Kunkel,

Thank you for providing the metadata for your Preregistered Research Article, entitled "The role of dopamine for positive treatment expectations and placebo analgesia". Now that your manuscript has passed our initial technical checks, I am writing to formally invite a revision of your manuscript. As noted in my previous email, one of our Academic Editors has raised several concerns regarding the design and analysis proposed in your manuscript which we think should be addressed before we send your manuscript to reviewers. The Academic Editor's comments were included in my last email, and are appended again here, below my signature.

We are pleased to offer you the opportunity to address the comments from the Academic Editor in a revision that we anticipate should not take you very long. We will then assess your revised manuscript and, if satisfied, will send your study to the reviewers for their assessment.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within 1 month.

Please email us (plosbiology@plos.org) if you have any questions or concerns, or would like to request an extension. At this stage, your manuscript remains formally under active consideration at our journal; please notify us by email if you do not intend to submit a revision so that we may end consideration of the manuscript at PLOS Biology.

**IMPORTANT - SUBMITTING YOUR REVISION**

Your revisions should address the specific points made by each reviewer. Please submit the following files along with your revised manuscript:

1. A 'Response to Editor' file - this should detail your responses to the editorial requests, with a point-by-point response.

*NOTE: In your point by point response, please provide the full context of each comment. Do not selectively quote paragraphs or sentences to reply to. The entire set of reviewer comments should be present in full and each specific point should be responded to individually.

You should also cite any additional relevant literature that has been published since the original submission and mention any additional citations in your response.

2. In addition to a clean copy of the manuscript, please also upload a 'track-changes' version of your manuscript that specifies the edits made. This should be uploaded as a "Related" file type.

*Resubmission Checklist*

When you are ready to resubmit your revised manuscript, please refer to this resubmission checklist: https://plos.io/Biology_Checklist

To submit a revised version of your manuscript, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' where you will find your submission record.

Please make sure to read the following important policies and guidelines while preparing your revision:

*Published Peer Review*

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*PLOS Data Policy*

Please note that as a condition of publication PLOS' data policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability) requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions arrived at in your manuscript. If you have not already done so, you must include any data used in your manuscript either in appropriate repositories, within the body of the manuscript, or as supporting information (N.B. this includes any numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.). For an example see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Thank you again for your submission to our journal. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive thus far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Lucas Smith

Associate Editor

PLOS Biology

lsmith@plos.org

*****************************************************

COMMENTS FROM THE ACADEMIC EDITOR

My main concern is that the analysis plans don't align sufficiently clearly with the hypotheses or power analysis, and I suspect may be overcomplicated, with the consequence that the study may be underpowered to detect differential effects between cells of d=0.3. For example, if I understand correctly, Hypothesis 1 is two independent t-tests (sulpride vs control; L-dopa vs control) but the analysis plan is for a 2 x 3 mixed ANOVA, and I didn't understand how the within-subjects conditioning variable (placebo vs control) is taken into account in the hypothesis (e.g. through a differential subtraction?). I had similar concerns about the other hypotheses. The authors should make clear, for each hypothesis, the predicted pattern of results in graphical form, and then ensure that the statistical tests and sampling plan map directly on to that predicted pattern specifically. For instance, the power analysis for a 2 x 3 ANOVA will return the power to detect *any* interaction, when only one of several possible interaction patterns would support the hypothesis. Therefore power is overestimated. Unless I'm mistaken, I think that all of the authors' hypotheses can be reduced to combinations of pairwise comparisons (e.g. between difference scores), which might be best addressed using Bayesian hypothesis tests (dispensing with p values entirely). But the design isn't sufficiently clear at this stage to say for certain either way.

If the authors do continue to use frequentist and Bayesian tests, they also need to make clear which outcomes (Bayesian or frequentist) will determine the interpretation. Otherwise there are too many interpretative degrees of freedom and consequent risk of bias.

Decision Letter 2

Lucas Smith

15 Dec 2021

Dear Dr Kunkel,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript "The role of dopamine for positive treatment expectations and placebo analgesia" for consideration as a Preregistered Research Article by PLOS Biology. I have now obtained advice from the Academic Editor, who is largely satisfied by your thorough response to his previous comments.

The comments from the Academic Editor are appended below my signature. As you will see, s/he has two last minor requests which we think should be addressed before we review the study. We therefore invite you to address the remaining comments in another revision. Once these comments are satisfactorily addressed, we will send your manuscript to reviewers.

While we expect that this revision will not take very long, given the upcoming holidays, we will set a deadline of 3 weeks for your revision.

To submit your revision, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' to find your submission record. Your revised submission must include the following:

-  a cover letter that should detail your responses to any editorial requests, if applicable, and whether changes have been made to the reference list

-  a track-changes file indicating any changes that you have made to the manuscript. 

NOTE: If Supporting Information files are included with your article, note that these are not copyedited and will be published as they are submitted. Please ensure that these files are legible and of high quality (at least 300 dpi) in an easily accessible file format. For this reason, please be aware that any references listed in an SI file will not be indexed. For more information, see our Supporting Information guidelines:

https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/supporting-information  

*Published Peer Review History*

Please note that you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*Early Version*

Please note that an uncorrected proof of your manuscript will be published online ahead of the final version, unless you opted out when submitting your manuscript. If, for any reason, you do not want an earlier version of your manuscript published online, uncheck the box. Should you, your institution's press office or the journal office choose to press release your paper, you will automatically be opted out of early publication. We ask that you notify us as soon as possible if you or your institution is planning to press release the article.

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Please do not hesitate to contact me should you have any questions.

Sincerely,

Lucas Smith, Ph.D.,

Associate Editor,

lsmith@plos.org,

PLOS Biology

COMMENTS FROM THE ACADEMIC EDITOR

The authors have responded quite thoroughly, and I think it's nearly ready for review. Two final points before then:

1. I would recommend that the authors include figures R1, R2, and R3 in the manuscript because they are extremely helpful for clarity and are likely to help avoid confusion from reviewers.

2. The authors note in their response that "We would like to emphasize that the interpretation of our results will be determined by frequentist statistics to allow for direct comparisons with results of previous studies and meta-analyses." I may have missed this, but is this stated anywhere in the Stage 1 manuscript? If not, it should be added as it is an important prespecification of how results will be interpreted.

Decision Letter 3

Lucas Smith

8 Mar 2022

Dear Dr Kunkel,

I am writing on behalf of my colleague Dr Lucas Smith, who is currently on paternity leave. Thank you for submitting your manuscript "The role of dopamine for positive treatment expectations and placebo analgesia" for consideration as a Preregistered Research Article at PLOS Biology. Your manuscript has been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editors, by an Academic Editor with relevant expertise, and by three independent reviewers. Please accept my sincere apologies for the long delay in communicating the decision below to you.

In light of the reviews (below), we will not be able to accept the current version of the manuscript, but we would welcome re-submission of a much-revised version that takes into account the reviewers' comments. We cannot make any decision about publication until we have seen the revised manuscript and your response to the reviewers' comments. Your revised manuscript is also likely to be sent for further evaluation by the reviewers.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within 3 months.

Please email us (plosbiology@plos.org) if you have any questions or concerns, or would like to request an extension. At this stage, your manuscript remains formally under active consideration at our journal; please notify us by email if you do not intend to submit a revision so that we may end consideration of the manuscript at PLOS Biology.

**IMPORTANT - SUBMITTING YOUR REVISION**

Your revisions should address the specific points made by each reviewer. We think reviewer 3 makes a series of very good points, which if not properly addressed would potentially confound the whole study. Thus, we expect their comments to be addressed in a full way.

Please submit the following files along with your revised manuscript:

1. A 'Response to Reviewers' file - this should detail your responses to the editorial requests, present a point-by-point response to all of the reviewers' comments, and indicate the changes made to the manuscript.

*NOTE: In your point by point response to the reviewers, please provide the full context of each review. Do not selectively quote paragraphs or sentences to reply to. The entire set of reviewer comments should be present in full and each specific point should be responded to individually, point by point.

You should also cite any additional relevant literature that has been published since the original submission and mention any additional citations in your response.

2. In addition to a clean copy of the manuscript, please also upload a 'track-changes' version of your manuscript that specifies the edits made. This should be uploaded as a "Related" file type.

*Re-submission Checklist*

When you are ready to resubmit your revised manuscript, please refer to this re-submission checklist: https://plos.io/Biology_Checklist

To submit a revised version of your manuscript, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' where you will find your submission record.

Please make sure to read the following important policies and guidelines while preparing your revision:

*Published Peer Review*

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*PLOS Data Policy*

Please note that as a condition of publication PLOS' data policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability) requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions arrived at in your manuscript. If you have not already done so, you must include any data used in your manuscript either in appropriate repositories, within the body of the manuscript, or as supporting information (N.B. this includes any numerical values that were used to generate graphs, histograms etc.). For an example see here: http://www.plosbiology.org/article/info%3Adoi%2F10.1371%2Fjournal.pbio.1001908#s5

*Blot and Gel Data Policy*

We require the original, uncropped and minimally adjusted images supporting all blot and gel results reported in an article's figures or Supporting Information files. We will require these files before a manuscript can be accepted so please prepare them now, if you have not already uploaded them. Please carefully read our guidelines for how to prepare and upload this data: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/figures#loc-blot-and-gel-reporting-requirements

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Thank you again for your submission to our journal. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive thus far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Gabriel Gasque, on behalf of

Lucas Smith

Associate Editor

PLOS Biology

lsmith@plos.org

*****************************************************

REVIEWS:

Reviewer #1: This Stage 1 manuscript describes a comprehensive set of novel analytical approaches to examine the influence of dopaminergic mechanisms on the development of expectations, conditioning and placebo analgesia. It provides a novel approach not previously examined to determine pharmacological influences on the processes related to placebo analgesia

Reviewer #2: * The importance of the research question(s).

Very important as it can shed light on the interactions between expectations and pharmacological treatment in potentially all drug trials. The idea that dopamine may be a key transmitter for the formation of expectancies (rather than the inhibition of e.g. nociceptive signals) has not been tested in the rigorous, double-blind, systematic way proposed here.

* The logic, rationale, and plausibility of the proposed hypotheses (does the manuscript provide a valid rationale for the proposed study, with clearly identified and justified research questions?)

Yes. The logic and rationale are clearly outlined. There is no reason to believe that the hypotheses are not valid, even if previous evidence is scarce and there is not much evidence to build on. Rather than a shortcoming I believe that the scarcity of previous data (there are some studies) points to the need for this type of study. The hypotheses are in line with previous data.

* The soundness and feasibility of the methodology and analysis pipeline (including statistical power analysis where appropriate). Is the protocol technically sound and planned in a manner that will lead to a meaningful outcome and allow testing of the stated hypotheses?

The power had to be calculated on datasets that were not using the same design, and this may be a risk for the feasibility of getting results here. In fact, the effect size used to calculate power in this study was coming from a study with a different design (Scott et al. 2008), that was using raclopride PET ligands to measure DA binding in the brain as opposed to giving treatments with dopaminergic effects as suggested here. This means that the effect sizes used to calculate the power here are drawn from a very different context (that potentially has stronger effects). However, the authors have used a lower effect size in their power calculations to account for the potential lower effect sizes in this trial. This gives me reassurance that the analyses are likely to have enough power.

* Whether the clarity and degree of methodological detail is sufficient to exactly replicate the proposed experimental procedures and analysis pipeline.

Yes. There is always risk that individual experimenters (and their behavior during application of cream) may affect the participant in a placebo analgesia experiment, especially since previous data suggest that non-verbal behavior will affect placebo responses in spite of clear instructions to experimenters (Kaptchuk et al. 2008). This is difficult to control for and the authors have at least provided the verbal script for these interactions and clear instructions for the interactions.

* Whether the authors have pre-specified sufficient outcome-neutral tests for ensuring that the results obtained are able to test the stated hypotheses, including positive controls and quality checks.

Yes.

Reviewer #3: The authors propose a study designed to ask whether modulating dopamine systems through a pharmacological agonist (levodopa) or antagonist (sulpiride) modulates placebo analgesia by affecting treatment expectations. More specifically, they plan to administer levodopa, sulpiride, or vehicle prior to a placebo conditioning visit, in which participants will complete questionnaires, a pain calibration, and experience painful stimuli on a control site and a placebo site. Stimulus intensity will be lowered on the placebo site (VAS = 40, rather than VAS = 80), to induce expectations about pain reduction. The hypothesis is that the learning component that gives rise to expectations is dopamine-mediated. Although the hypotheses are not stated in directional terms, the figures indicate that the main hypothesis is that administration of levodopa will enhance expectations about pain relief, and thus that these participants will expect less pain and report larger reductions in pain (i.e. larger placebo effects) relative to a placebo group. In contrast, it is expected that a group that receives sulpiride will expect more pain than a placebo group, and have lower levels of placebo analgesia than either of the other groups. Placebo effects will be tested on days 2 and 8 following the initial visit. This study builds on previous work from this team, which indicated that administration of haloperidol reduced placebo-related activity in the striatum but not placebo analgesia itself. Thus they propose that rather than dopamine affecting pain processing itself, it is more related to learning, consistent with research outside the field of pain and placbeo. This is also consistent with findings of links between placebo-induced dopamine signalling in the nucleus accumbens being related to striatal reward responses, dopamine-related personality traits, and gray matter density in the striatum.

In principle, I think that continued investigation of the role of dopamine in placebo and pain modulation is important for the field. However, given that this is a registered report, I do not think that this study design will allow for incontrovertible evidence as to the precise contribution of dopamine to placebo, even if the results are in the hypothesized directions. First, although the authors previously found no impact of dopamine modulation on placebo analgesia, that does not mean that the administration cannot affect pain. Thus during the conditioning visit, dopamine administration might affect the calibration procedure. If the calibration procedure is completed prior to drug administration (this is not clear from the design), then it still could affect the extent to which an individual perceives differences in pain between the placebo site and the control site. This wouldn't be due to learning, but due to differences in responses to noxious stimuli. The only way to control for this would be to measure pain sensitivity before and after drug administration. If there is no difference in pain sensitivity but an effect on learning, then this can be attributed to dopamine modulating learning.

Given that other studies suggest links between placebo and other processes that are more commonly linked to dopamine, why don't the authors include a learning or reward task on day 1 alongside conditioning? This would provide a positive control that the pharmacological manipulation is effective.

I also think that the decision to test the magnitude of placebo on day 2 and 8 is not well justified in the paper. If the drug modulates learning and expectations, why can you not test placebo effects on the first visit day? It is not clear to me why these should necessarily be long-lasting effects; there is no specific hypothesis about memory or consolidation offered in the paper.

Finally, there are highlighted sections in the main manuscript that are redundant, and three figures are missing captions. These are also highlighted, which suggests the authors may have turned in an incomplete draft accidentally.

Decision Letter 4

Roland G Roberts

21 Jun 2022

Dear Angelika,

Thank you for your patience while your revised Pre-Registered Research Article "The role of dopamine for positive treatment expectations and placebo analgesia" was re-reviewed for PLOS Biology. Your Stage 1 manuscript has been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editors, and by the Academic Editor and one of reviewers that saw the initial version of your study. Please accept my apologies for the extreme delay incurred; as you know, this was due to serious personal circumstances outside our control.

The reviews of your manuscript are appended below. You will see that the reviewers find that your Stage 1 Protocol meets our criteria for importance of research question and technical soundness of the study proposal. We are thus happy to issue a Stage 1 'in-principle acceptance' decision, with a commitment to publish the final Stage 2 Preregistered Research Article (after revision, if needed), pending successful completion of the study. Please carefully read all the following information.

The study should now be completed according to the Stage 1 approved methods and analytic procedures, and the final manuscript should include an evidence-based interpretation of the results. Please see the review criteria for Stage 2 manuscripts here:

https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/reviewer-guidelines#loc-reviewing-preregistered-research-articles

Subsequent editorial decisions for this study will not be based on the perceived importance or novelty of the results obtained during the data gathering and analysis phase of the work. It is critical however that you adhere to the approved Stage 1 study design when performing the study. Any deviation from these experimental procedures would need to be justified and approved by the editors (and potentially the reviewers), as otherwise it could lead to rejection of the manuscript at Stage 2. Please consult the editors immediately for advice if you need to alter this approved study plan.

**IMPORTANT**: Please follow the link below for important information regarding the Stage 2 manuscript template and review criteria. Please carefully read the guidelines on Stage 2 data collection BEFORE performing your study and completing your Stage 2 manuscript.

AUTHOR GUIDELINES: https://plos.io/AuthorGuidelines

*Depositing this Stage 1 Protocol*

PLOS Biology does not publish Stage 1 Protocols immediately following an in-principle acceptance. Instead they are held and integrated into a single, completed 'Preregistered Research Article' following review and acceptance of the final Stage 2 manuscript. You are however required to register this approved Stage 1 Protocol with the Center for Open Science (https://cos.io/prereg/) or another recognised repository. This may be done publicly or under private embargo until submission of the Stage 2 manuscript. Stage 1 Protocols can be quickly and easily registered using a tailored mechanism for Registered Reports (https://osf.io/rr/). Please do this now. You will need to include the URL to this deposited protocol in your Stage 2 manuscript.

*Timeline*

We understand that carrying out the study will require a significant length of time and are willing to allow you 18 months to perform the study. Please email us at plosbiology@plos.org to discuss this if you have any questions or concerns, or to discuss an alternate timeline.

At this stage, your manuscript remains formally under active consideration at our journal. Please notify us by email if you do not wish to submit a Stage 2 manuscript or wish to pursue publication elsewhere, so that we may withdraw your manuscript.

*Resubmission Checklist*

Before submitting the Stage 2 manuscript, please review the following resubmission checklist: https://plos.io/Biology_Checklist

Please note that for PRA stage 2, the response to reviewers file does not follow the standard format, but should rather be a document for the reviewers detailing the changes made to the manuscript since the stage 1 accept.

To submit a revised version of your manuscript, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' where you will find your submission record.

Please make sure to read the following important policies and guidelines while preparing your revision:

*Published Peer Review*

Please note while forming your response, if your article is accepted, you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*PLOS Data Policy*

Please note that as a condition of publication, PLOS' data policy (http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability) requires that you make available all data used to draw the conclusions arrived at in your manuscript. Please note that for this article type, the raw data itself should be archived and made freely available in a public repository rather than submitted as supplementary material. Please make sure to read the Stage 2 submission guidelines online regarding how this data should be annotated and appropriately time stamped to show that data was collected after this Stage 1 in-principle acceptance and not before.

*Blot and Gel Data Policy*

We require the original, uncropped and minimally adjusted images supporting all blot and gel results reported in an article's figures or Supporting Information files. We will require these files before a manuscript can be accepted so please prepare them now, if you have not already uploaded them. Please carefully read our guidelines for how to prepare and upload this data: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/figures#loc-blot-and-gel-reporting-requirements

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that, if applicable, you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/submission-guidelines#loc-materials-and-methods

Thank you again for your submission to PLOS Biology. We hope that our editorial process has been constructive thus far, and we welcome your feedback at any time. Please don't hesitate to contact us if you have any questions or comments.

Sincerely,

Roli

Roland Roberts, PhD

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

rroberts@plos.org

--------------------------------------

REVIEWER'S COMMENTS:

Reviewer #3:

The authors have explained their decisions and updated study design to address the impact of dopamine on pain, separate from its impact on placebo analgesia. The study is sound and although I think there will be outstanding questions after this paper, they are circumspect with respect to the hypotheses they set out to test, which the study will be able to address.

Decision Letter 5

Christian Schnell, PhD

18 Jun 2024

Dear Dr Kunkel,

Thank you for your patience while we considered your revised manuscript "The role of dopamine in positive treatment expectations and placebo analgesia" for publication as a Preregistered Research Article at PLOS Biology. This revised version of your manuscript has been evaluated by the PLOS Biology editors, the academic editors and the original reviewers.

Based on the reviews, we are likely to accept this manuscript for publication, provided you satisfactorily address the remaining points raised by the reviewers. Please also make sure to address the following data and other policy-related requests.

* Please update the abstract to report the main conclusions (based on the preregistered confirmatory hypotheses).

* Please add column on the right of Table 1 that includes the actual (i.e. observed) outcome of each hypothesis test (confirmed or disconfirmed).

* We would like to suggest a different title to improve its accessibility: "Dopamine has no direct causal role in treatment expectations and placebo analgesia in humans"

* DATA POLICY:

You may be aware of the PLOS Data Policy, which requires that all data be made available without restriction: http://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/data-availability. For more information, please also see this editorial: http://dx.doi.org/10.1371/journal.pbio.1001797

Note that we do not require all raw data. Rather, we ask that all individual quantitative observations that underlie the data summarized in the figures and results of your paper be made available in one of the following forms:

1) Supplementary files (e.g., excel). Please ensure that all data files are uploaded as 'Supporting Information' and are invariably referred to (in the manuscript, figure legends, and the Description field when uploading your files) using the following format verbatim: S1 Data, S2 Data, etc. Multiple panels of a single or even several figures can be included as multiple sheets in one excel file that is saved using exactly the following convention: S1_Data.xlsx (using an underscore).

2) Deposition in a publicly available repository. Please also provide the accession code or a reviewer link so that we may view your data before publication.

Regardless of the method selected, please ensure that you provide the individual numerical values that underlie the summary data displayed in the following figure panels as they are essential for readers to assess your analysis and to reproduce it: Figure 5 and 6

NOTE: the numerical data provided should include all replicates AND the way in which the plotted mean and errors were derived (it should not present only the mean/average values).

Please also ensure that figure legends in your manuscript include information on where the underlying data can be found, and ensure your supplemental data file/s has a legend.

Please ensure that your Data Statement in the submission system accurately describes where your data can be found.

* CODE POLICY

Per journal policy, if you have generated any custom code during the course of this investigation, please make it available without restrictions. Please ensure that the code is sufficiently well documented and reusable, and that your Data Statement in the Editorial Manager submission system accurately describes where your code can be found.

Please note that we cannot accept sole deposition of code in GitHub, as this could be changed after publication. However, you can archive this version of your publicly available GitHub code to Zenodo. Once you do this, it will generate a DOI number, which you will need to provide in the Data Accessibility Statement (you are welcome to also provide the GitHub access information). See the process for doing this here: https://docs.github.com/en/repositories/archiving-a-github-repository/referencing-and-citing-content

As you address these items, please take this last chance to review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the cover letter that accompanies your revised manuscript.

We expect to receive your revised manuscript within two weeks.

To submit your revision, please go to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/ and log in as an Author. Click the link labelled 'Submissions Needing Revision' to find your submission record. Your revised submission must include the following:

- a cover letter that should detail your responses to any editorial requests, if applicable, and whether changes have been made to the reference list

- a Response to Reviewers file that provides a detailed response to the reviewers' comments (if applicable, if not applicable please do not delete your existing 'Response to Reviewers' file.)

- a track-changes file indicating any changes that you have made to the manuscript.

NOTE: If Supporting Information files are included with your article, note that these are not copyedited and will be published as they are submitted. Please ensure that these files are legible and of high quality (at least 300 dpi) in an easily accessible file format. For this reason, please be aware that any references listed in an SI file will not be indexed. For more information, see our Supporting Information guidelines:

https://journals.plos.org/plosbiology/s/supporting-information

*Published Peer Review History*

Please note that you may have the opportunity to make the peer review history publicly available. The record will include editor decision letters (with reviews) and your responses to reviewer comments. If eligible, we will contact you to opt in or out. Please see here for more details:

https://blogs.plos.org/plos/2019/05/plos-journals-now-open-for-published-peer-review/

*Press*

Should you, your institution's press office or the journal office choose to press release your paper, please ensure you have opted out of Early Article Posting on the submission form. We ask that you notify us as soon as possible if you or your institution is planning to press release the article.

*Protocols deposition*

To enhance the reproducibility of your results, we recommend that if applicable you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io, where a protocol can be assigned its own identifier (DOI) such that it can be cited independently in the future. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols

Please do not hesitate to contact me should you have any questions.

Sincerely,

Christian

Christian Schnell, PhD

Senior Editor

cschnell@plos.org

PLOS Biology

------------------------------------------------------------------------

Reviewer remarks:

Reviewer #2: The article is a clear report of the results from the pre-registered study on the role of dopamine in placebo analgesia. The authors have made a throrough analysis of the data and, perhaps due to the negative finding, performed many sub-analyses to explore the data extensively. The conclusions are in line with the results and are not over-reaching. I find the manuscript suitable for publication. I would like to congratulate the authors to this large undertaking, and well-balanced discussion of the results.

Reviewer #3: I am really glad the authors conducted this study, as the field has speculated for some time about the role of dopamine in placebo effects due to a single paper and no comprehensive tests of the hypotheses. Doing this through a registered report really provides definitive evidence of there being no substantial relationship between placebo analgesia and dopamine, building on earlier findings from the team (Wrobel et al.). The authors followed their preregistered analyses and study designs, and conducted appropriate additional exploratory analyses that are important for any readers who may still believe that there is a hidden impact of dopamine.

My only minor comment - and it is truly minor - is that there are post-hoc interpretations in the section under "Considering absolute pain intensity" that imply a medication x mean pain interaction, despite the fact that the interaction was not significant (lines 625-630, figure 7E). Given that there is no interaction with medication, it seems unnecessary to explore the slopes for the different medication groups. I recognize that the frequentist p-value is .06, so perhaps they felt a need to explore the potential interaction, but Bayesian models could also be used to see whether the interaction is meaningful.

Thank you again for the opportunity to review this paper, and for your contribution to the field.

-Lauren Atlas

Decision Letter 6

Christian Schnell, PhD

29 Jul 2024

Dear Dr Bingel,

Thank you for the submission of your revised Preregistered Research Article "Dopamine has no direct causal role in the formation of treatment expectations and placebo analgesia in humans" for publication in PLOS Biology. On behalf of my colleagues and the academic editors, Ben Seymour and Chris Chambers, I am pleased to say that we can in principle accept your manuscript for publication, provided you address any remaining formatting and reporting issues. These will be detailed in an email you should receive within 2-3 business days from our colleagues in the journal operations team; no action is required from you until then. Please note that we will not be able to formally accept your manuscript and schedule it for publication until you have completed any requested changes.

Please take a minute to log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pbiology/, click the "Update My Information" link at the top of the page, and update your user information to ensure an efficient production process.

PRESS

We frequently collaborate with press offices. If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper at this point, to enable them to help maximise its impact. If the press office is planning to promote your findings, we would be grateful if they could coordinate with biologypress@plos.org. If you have previously opted in to the early version process, we ask that you notify us immediately of any press plans so that we may opt out on your behalf.

We also ask that you take this opportunity to read our Embargo Policy regarding the discussion, promotion and media coverage of work that is yet to be published by PLOS. As your manuscript is not yet published, it is bound by the conditions of our Embargo Policy. Please be aware that this policy is in place both to ensure that any press coverage of your article is fully substantiated and to provide a direct link between such coverage and the published work. For full details of our Embargo Policy, please visit http://www.plos.org/about/media-inquiries/embargo-policy/.

Thank you again for choosing PLOS Biology for publication and supporting Open Access publishing. We look forward to publishing your study. 

Sincerely,

Christian 

Christian Schnell, PhD

Senior Editor

PLOS Biology

cschnell@plos.org

Associated Data

    This section collects any data citations, data availability statements, or supplementary materials included in this article.

    Supplementary Materials

    S1 Text. Overview of questionnaires with references and verbal placebo instructions.

    (DOCX)

    pbio.3002772.s001.docx (21KB, docx)
    S1 Table. Detailed results of Bayesian quantification of evidence for Hypothesis 1 from JASP output.

    (A) Model comparison. Note that models include subject and random slopes for all repeated measures factors. (B) Analysis of effects showing the inclusion Bayes factors (BFincl) of the model terms. (C) Summary of model averaged posteriors showing estimated marginal means, standard deviations, and 95% credible intervals for all factor levels.

    (DOCX)

    pbio.3002772.s002.docx (23.5KB, docx)
    S2 Table. Detailed results of Bayesian quantification of evidence for Hypothesis 2 from JASP output.

    (A) Model comparison. Note that models include subject and random slopes for all repeated measures factors. (B) Analysis of effects showing the inclusion Bayes factors (BFincl) of the model terms. (C) Summary of model averaged posteriors showing estimated marginal means, standard deviations, and 95% credible intervals for all factor levels.

    (DOCX)

    pbio.3002772.s003.docx (19.8KB, docx)
    S3 Table. Detailed results of Bayesian quantification of evidence for Hypothesis 3 from JASP output.

    (A) Model comparison. Note that models include subject and random slopes for all repeated measures factors. (B) Analysis of effects showing the inclusion Bayes factors (BFincl) of the model terms. (C) Summary of model averaged posteriors showing estimated marginal means, standard deviations, and 95% credible intervals for all factor levels.

    (DOCX)

    pbio.3002772.s004.docx (19.9KB, docx)
    S1 Data. Individual quantitative observations that underlie the data summarized in Fig 5.

    Excel file with single sheets for each figure panel (Fig 5A–5E). Abbreviations: “participant”: participant ID. “blood_Dopa”: serum L-dopa levels in μg/ml. “blood_Prolactin”: prolactin serum levels in ng/ml. “group”: medication group. “expectation”: EXPECT score from 0–10. “expectation_timepoint”: rating time point for EXPECT score. “Expectation_BL”: EXPECT score at baseline at day 1 (BL). “Expectation_day1”: EXPECT score before conditioning at day 1 (preCOND). “Expectation_day2”: EXPECT score at day 2 (preT1); “Expectation_day8”: EXPECT score at day 8 (preT2). “INA”: inactive control group, “DOPA”: L-dopa group, “SUL”: sulpiride group. “Delta_VAS_Day2”: VAS mean difference pain rating between control condition minus placebo condition on day 2 (test session 1). “Delta_VAS_Day8”: VAS mean difference pain rating between control condition minus placebo condition on day 8 (test session 2).

    (XLSX)

    pbio.3002772.s005.xlsx (31.8KB, xlsx)
    S2 Data. Individual quantitative observations that underlie the data summarized in Fig 6.

    Excel file with single sheets for each figure panel (Fig 6A–6F). Abbreviations: “participant”: participant ID. “blood_Dopa”: serum L-dopa levels in μg/ml. “gender”: participant sex. “blood_Prolactin”: prolactin serum levels in ng/ml. “group”: medication group. “Delta_Expect_day2minusday1”: mean difference between EXPECT scores at preT1 (before test session 1 on day 2) vs. preCOND (before conditioning at day 1). “INA”: inactive control group, “DOPA”: L-dopa group, “SUL”: sulpiride group. “Delta_VAS_Day2”: VAS mean difference in pain rating between control condition vs. placebo condition on day 2 (test session 1).

    (XLSX)

    pbio.3002772.s006.xlsx (20.5KB, xlsx)
    S3 Data. Individual quantitative observations that underlie the data summarized in Fig 7.

    Excel file with single sheets for each figure panel (Fig 7A–7E). “Trial”: individual trial number. “group”: medication group. “INA”: inactive control group, “DOPA”: L-dopa group, “SUL”: sulpiride group. “n” = n-number. “sd” = standard deviation. “ControlCream_Conditioning_VAS”: VAS pain rating score in control condition during conditioning (day 1). “PlaceboCream_Conditioning_VAS”: VAS pain rating score in placebo condition during conditioning (day 1). “ControlCream_Day2_VAS”: VAS pain rating score in control condition during test session 1 (day 2). “PlaceboCream_Day2_VAS”: VAS pain rating score in placebo condition during test session 1 (day 2). “Delta_VAS_Cond”: mean VAS pain rating difference between control and placebo condition during conditioning (day 1). “Delta_VAS_Day2”: mean VAS pain rating difference between control condition vs. placebo condition on day 2 (test session 1). “Delta_Expect_day2minusday1”: mean difference between EXPECT scores at preT1 (before test session 1 on day 2) vs. preCOND (before conditioning on day 1). “Day2_VAS_ControlCream”: mean VAS pain rating of the control condition during test session 1 (day 2).

    (XLSX)

    pbio.3002772.s007.xlsx (26.8KB, xlsx)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Response to editor_RR_Stage1_211203.pdf

    pbio.3002772.s008.pdf (665.2KB, pdf)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: ResponseLetter_final_220321.pdf

    pbio.3002772.s009.pdf (129.2KB, pdf)
    Attachment

    Submitted filename: Responses to Reviewer Comments_submitted.docx

    pbio.3002772.s010.docx (15.1KB, docx)

    Data Availability Statement

    Data and code to reproduce all results are publicly shared on the Open Science Framework (OSF) (https://osf.io/f49dt/). R code and input data to perform frequentist statistics are shared, as well as the JASP file for Bayesian analyses. The complete dataset is additionally provided in Brain Imaging Data Structure (BIDS) format. The in-principle-accepted Stage 1 manuscript of this Preregistered Research Article was deposited as public registration in OSF (https://osf.io/py8b3).


    Articles from PLOS Biology are provided here courtesy of PLOS

    RESOURCES